Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T07:03:44.277Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The gene–environmental architecture of the development of adolescent substance use

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2018

Frank Vitaro*
Affiliation:
School of Psycho-Education, University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada
Daniel J. Dickson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, USA
Mara Brendgen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Quebec at Montreal, Montreal, Canada
Brett Laursen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, USA
Ginette Dionne
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Laval University, Quebec City, Canada
Michel Boivin
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Laval University, Quebec City, Canada
*
Author for correspondence: Frank Vitaro, E-mail: frank.vitaro@umontreal.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Using a longitudinal twin design and a latent growth curve/autoregressive approach, this study examined the genetic–environmental architecture of substance use across adolescence.

Methods

Self-reports of substance use (i.e. alcohol, marijuana) were collected at ages 13, 14, 15, and 17 years from 476 twin pairs (475 boys, 477 girls) living in the Province of Quebec, Canada. Substance use increased linearly across the adolescent years.

Results

ACE modeling revealed that genetic, as well as shared and non-shared environmental factors explained the overall level of substance use and that these same factors also partly accounted for growth in substance use from age 13 to 17. Additional genetic factors predicted the growth in substance use. Finally, autoregressive effects revealed age-specific non-shared environmental influences and, to a lesser degree, age-specific genetic influences, which together accounted for the stability of substance use across adolescence.

Conclusions

The results support and expand the notion that genetic and environmental influences on substance use during adolescence are both developmentally stable and developmentally dynamic.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Introduction

Although adolescent substance use has declined modestly during the past decade, it remains a serious public health concern in most Western nations. Substance use typically emerges during early adolescence and linearly increases – both in terms of prevalence rates and frequency of use – until late adolescence, when a majority of adolescents report prior use of alcohol or illicit drugs (Duncan & Duncan, Reference Duncan and Duncan1996; Miech et al. Reference Miech, Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, Schulenberg and Patrick2017). Although some degree of experimentation with alcohol and marijuana may be considered normative (Johnston et al. Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman and Schulenberg2015), there is evidence that high levels (i.e. quantity or frequency) of substance use or a sharp increase in substance use during adolescence are associated with adverse social, academic, behavioral, and mental health outcomes (Hingson et al. Reference Hingson, Heeren and Winter2006; Jacobus et al. Reference Jacobus, Bava, Cohen-Zion, Mahmood and Tapert2009; Peleg-Oren et al. Reference Peleg-Oren, Saint-Jean, Cardenas, Tammara and Pierre2009).

Behavioral genetic research based on twin designs indicates that both genetic and environmental factors influence substance use during adolescence, and that they are, to a large extent, the same across different substances (Han et al. Reference Han, McGue and Iacono1999; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003; Young et al. Reference Young, Rhee, Stallings, Corley and Hewitt2006; Sartor et al. Reference Sartor, Grant, Bucholz, Madden, Heath and Agrawal2010). Nevertheless, the relative contribution of genetic and environmental influences on adolescent substance use seems to differ with age. Substance use during early adolescence seems to be primarily influenced by shared social and familial environmental factors, whereas substance use during middle and late adolescence seems to be primarily influenced by genetic factors (for a review, see Hopfer et al. Reference Hopfer, Crowley and Hewitt2003; Lynskey et al. Reference Lynskey, Agrawal and Heath2010). However, most evidence for age differences rests on (a) studies using retrospective data, which are sensitive to memory problems or reconstruction biases (e.g. Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen and Prescott2008; Long et al. Reference Long, Verhulst, Aggen, Kendler and Gillespie2017) or (b) prospective longitudinal studies with a limited number of data points, often during late adolescence or early adulthood (Koopmans et al. Reference Koopmans, van Doornen and Boomsma1997; Viken et al. Reference Viken, Kaprio, Koskenvuo and Rose1999; Malone et al. Reference Malone, Taylor, Marmorstein, McGue and Iacono2004). Only one prospective study has examined the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors from age 13 to 20 (Baker et al. Reference Baker, Maes, Larsson, Lichtenstein and Kendler2011). Results from a Cholesky decomposition of the longitudinal data indicated that genetic and shared environmental factors on general substance use were substantial and stable with age, although some evidence for genetic innovation emerged. Shared environmental influences were also stable over time, whereas unique environmental influences were largely age-specific. Similar to previous work on the topic, this prospective study examined the extent to which repeated measures of substance use have a shared and/or independent etiology; it did not examine whether the same or different genetic and environmental factors influence the level and the growth of substance use at different ages, which is best addressed with latent growth curve modeling. To address this limitation, the present study examines the extent to which genetic and/or environmental factors contribute to the level and rate of change in substance use from early to late adolescence.

Adopting the recommendations of Gillespie et al. (Gillespie et al. Reference Gillespie, Eaves, Maes and Silberg2015; Long et al. Reference Long, Verhulst, Aggen, Kendler and Gillespie2017), the present study combined a latent growth curve approach with an autoregressive approach, in order to disentangle different mechanisms of change. A latent growth model assumes that the level (i.e. intercept) and rates of change (i.e. linear or non-linear slope) in substance use results from the unfolding of inherent, random effects over time that can be decomposed into genetic and environmental sources of variance. An autoregressive model (also known as a simplex/transmission model) assumes that genetic (and/or environmental) variance increases are due to the progressive accumulation of age-specific genetic influences (Eaves et al. Reference Eaves, Long and Heath1986; Boomsma & Molenaar, Reference Boomsma and Molenaar1987). The two models are not incompatible. For example, genetic influences may account for individual differences in the overall level and rate of change in substance use from early to late adolescence (as indicated by growth models), whereas environmental influences may account for individual differences in the persistence of substance use across different ages (as indicated by autoregressive effects). A mixed-model approach, known as a ‘dual change score model’, integrates a latent growth curve model and an autoregressive model (McArdle, Reference McArdle2009; Eaves et al. Reference Eaves, Gillespie and Verhulst2016).

Objective of the present study

The present study examines the relative role of genetic and environmental sources of influence on individual differences in the development of substance use between early adolescence (i.e. age 13) and late adolescence (i.e. age 17). To this end, an ACE decomposition was applied to a dual change score model to (a) examine genetic and environmental effects on the stability of substance use from early to late adolescence and (b) assess the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the overall level and the rate of change of adolescent substance use from age 13 to 17. We first assessed substance use at age 13, a time considered early onset of use (Swendsen et al. Reference Swendsen, Burstein, Case, Conway, Dierker and He2012), because it is the age when our participants entered high school. With the exception of age 16, we tracked substance use annually up to age 17, an age when most adolescents report at least some substance use (Martino et al. Reference Martino, Ellickson and McCaffrey2008).

Methods

Participants

Participants consisted of 476 twin pairs (475 boys, 477 girls) drawn from an ongoing longitudinal study of a population-based sample of 662 monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs (1324 individuals) recruited from the Québec Newborn Twin Registry, which identified all twin births occurring in the Province of Québec between 1995 and 1998 (Boivin et al. Reference Boivin, Brendgen, Dionne, Dubois, Pérusse and Robaey2013). Zygosity was assessed by genetic marker analysis of 8–10 highly polymorphous genetic markers; twins were diagnosed as MZ when concordant for every genetic marker. When genetic material was insufficient or unavailable (43% of cases), zygosity was determined on the basis of physical resemblance questionnaires at 18 months and age 9 (Goldsmith, Reference Goldsmith1991). The comparison of zygosity based on genotyping with zygosity based on physical resemblance in a subsample of 237 same-sex pairs revealed a 94% correspondence rate, which is similar to rates obtained in other studies (Spitz et al. Reference Spitz, Moutier, Reed, Busnel, Marchaland and Roubertoux1996; Magnusson et al. Reference Magnusson, Almqvist, Rahman, Ganna, Viktorin and Walum2013).

Demographic characteristics of the final sample were similar to those of a representative population-based birth sample of singletons assessed in 1998 by the Quebec Ministry of Health and Social Services. Most participants were of European descent (78.8%; n = 750); the remainder were of African descent (2.3%; n = 22), Asian descent (2.3%; n = 22), Native North American descent (1.2%; n = 10), other descents (2.3%; n = 22), or did not specify their ethnicity (13.2%; n = 126). At the outset, 95% of parents lived together. Roughly 17% of mothers and 14% of fathers had not finished high school; 28% of mothers and 27% of fathers held a university degree; 83% of the parents were employed; and 10% of the families received social welfare or unemployment insurance.

Of the 662 twin pairs recruited at birth, 397 (60.0%), 389 (58.8%), 391 (59.0%), and 368 (55.6%) participated in data collection at ages 13, 14, 15, and 17, respectively. A total of 263 twin pairs participated in four waves of data collection, 114 participated in three waves, 54 participated in two waves, and 45 participated in one wave. The final sample (187 MZ twin pairs, 144 same-sex DZ twin pairs, and 145 mixed-sex DZ twin pairs; M age = 13.1, s.d. = 0.30 years) included dyads with valid data for at least one wave. Compared with those who did not participate at ages 13–17, the final sample reported significantly higher family income [t(613) = 3.67, p < 0.01] and maternal education [t(635) = 2.52, p = 0.01]. They did not differ in terms of paternal education [t(587) = 0.44, p = 0.66], family structure [t(649) = 0.09, p = 0.93], child inattention [t(603) = 0.37, p = 0.72], or child hyperactivity [t(603) = 0.84, p = 0.40] during the preschool period. Both concurrently and over time, same- and mixed-sex DZ twins displayed no differences in their means, variances, and co-variances [χ2(7–20) = 3.48–26.71, p > 0.14], so the 145 mixed-sex DZ twin pairs were included in the analyses.

Procedure

Data collection at each wave was approved by the Sainte-Justine Hospital Research Centre ethics committee. Informed active parental consent and child assent for participation was obtained.

Missing data accounted for an average of 20.8% of reports (range 17.54–24.68%). Missing data were handled with full information maximum-likelihood estimation, which allowed participants with incomplete data to be included in the models. Little's test indicated that data were missing completely at random, χ2(238) = 244.52, p = 0.37.

Measures

Substance use

Frequency of substance use was assessed with the Personal Experience Screening Questionnaire (Henly & Winters, Reference Henly and Winters1989; Winters et al. Reference Winters, Stinchfield, Henly and Schwartz1990–91) which consisted of three items (alcohol use, marijuana use, and binge drinking). Participants rated the frequency of substance use over the past 12 months on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (daily) (mean Cronbach's α = 0.69). Item scores were averaged to create scale scores. Table 1 presents means and standard deviations for annual substance use. Table 1 also presents number of participants who used alcohol or marijuana at least once at each data point. Substance use increased steadily: 21.3% (at age 13) to 82.3% (at age 17) of participants reported experience with at least one substance at least once. The logs of substance use scores were calculated in order to correct for positively skewed distributions.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, item frequencies, and intra-class correlations

Note. N = 717–785. ** p < 0.01.

Plan of analysis

In the first step, we estimated a phenotypic dual change score model (Gillespie et al. Reference Gillespie, Eaves, Maes and Silberg2015; Long et al. Reference Long, Verhulst, Aggen, Kendler and Gillespie2017) to describe the phenotypic development of substance use from age 13 to 17. Dual change score models are well suited for segregating autoregressive effects from overall levels of a variable, and from the linear and non-linear inter-individual changes in a variable over time. A two-group model was estimated with 374 MZ twins and 578 DZ twins. The dual change score model tested whether (a) there were significant linear or non-linear increases in substance use, (b) there were autoregressive effects from one time point to the next, and (c) whether the results differed by zygosity. Both linear and non-linear growth curves were initially explored. However, the latter was omitted because it did not show a significant mean or variance in either MZ or DZ pairs. Factor loadings were orthonormalized. Only significant autoregressions were retained. Non-significant residual variances were fixed to 0. Across-zygosity constraints were retained if they did not significantly worsen model fit.

In the second step, we conducted an ACE decomposition of the dual change score model, which allows for the partitioning of effects that are ‘time-specific’ and that contribute to inter-individual differences in the stability of substance use, from effects that directly contribute to inter-individual differences in ‘unfolding’ growth. A Cholesky decomposition of the growth curve portion of the model allowed us to (a) estimate the relative contribution of genetic factors, shared environmental factors, and non-shared environmental factors to the intercept (i.e. overall level) and the slope (i.e. linear rate of change) of substance use, and (b) determine whether different genetic and environmental factors contributed to the intercept and the slope of substance use.

By comparing within-pair correlations for MZ twins (who are genetically identical) to those of DZ twins (who on average share only half of their genes), sources of variability in the intercept, slope, and time-specific portions of substance use can be estimated as latent additive genetic (A), latent shared environmental (C), and latent non-shared environmental (E) factors (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992). Within-twin pair correlations of the latent genetic factors (A) are fixed to 1.0 for MZ twins and to 0.5 for DZ twins. Within-twin pair correlations of the latent shared environmental factors (C) are fixed to 1.0 for both MZ and DZ twins. Within-twin pair correlations of the latent non-shared environmental factors (E) are fixed to 0 for both MZ and DZ twins. The estimated coefficients a, c, and e are fixed to be equal across the two members of a twin pair and across MZ and DZ twins. The estimated coefficients are factor loadings that provide information about the relative contribution of the latent factors A, C, and E to the total variance V T of each phenotype (V T = a 2 + c 2 + e 2).

Analyses were conducted with MPlus v7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2017), applying maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors procedure. Standardized estimates for factor loadings are presented in text and in figures; 95% confidence intervals are presented in brackets. Proportions of variances explained are calculated by squaring the standardized factor loadings.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Two sets of nested χ2-difference tests examined sex and zygosity differences in substance use. Boys reported higher substance use than girls at age 17 only [χ2(1) = 9.14, p < 0.01]. MZ and DZ twins did not differ on substance use at any age. Table 1 presents intra-class correlations for MZ and DZ twins. At ages 13 and 14, MZ correlations were roughly double those of DZ correlations, suggesting significant additive genetic effects (Falconer, Reference Falconer1960). At ages 15 and 17, MZ correlations were less than double those of DZ correlations, suggesting that shared environmental effects became more prominent at these ages. Table 2 presents phenotypic correlations of adolescent substance use.

Table 2. Phenotypic correlations between variables

Note. N = 601–672. MZ twins are below the diagonal, DZ twins are above the diagonal; 95% confidence intervals are presented in brackets. * p <.05, **p < 0.01.

Univariate ACE models were estimated to decompose the variance of substance use – separately at each age – into additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) factors. Results are presented in Table 3. At each age, significant (p < 0.001) loadings were identified on additive genetic factors [γ A = 0.68–0.72 (0.40–0.98)], and on non-shared environmental factors [γ E = 0.61–0.70 (0.50–0.83)], respectively, accounting for 46–52% and for 37–49% of the variance in substance use. Loadings for shared environmental factors were identified at ages 15 and 17 [γ C15 = 0.34(0.00–0.73), p = 0.091; γ C17 = 0.42 (0.11–0.74), p = 0.009], accounting for 11 and 18% of variances in substance use.

Table 3. Univariate ACE decompositions of substance use at age 13, 14, 15, and 17

Note: N = 370–397 dyads. For all models, df = 5; 95% confidence intervals are presented in brackets. S-B χ2 = Satorra–Bentler scaled χ2; RMSEA, root mean error of approximation; CFI, comparative fit index. †p = 0.09, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

Dual change score model without ACE decomposition

The freely estimated two-group dual change score model was acceptable, χ2(6) = 8.81, N = 945, p = 0.18, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.99, root mean error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.03. A scaled χ2 difference test revealed that constraining the model parameters to be equal across zygosity groups did not significantly worsen model fit [χ2(11) = 9.65, p = 0.56].

The final, constrained two-group dual change score model fit the data, χ2(17) = 18.23, N = 945, p = 0.37, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.01. Results are depicted in Fig. 1. The intercept [I = 2.06 (2.01–2.12)], the slope [S = 0.44 (0.41–0.47)], and their correlation [r IS = 0.85 (0.77–0.93)] were significantly different from zero (p < 0.001). Substance use significantly increased, in a linear fashion, from age 13 to 17. Intra-class correlations for the intercept and slope of substance use were statistically significant (p < 0.001), and were higher for MZ twin pairs [r II = 0.68 (0.58–0.76), r SS = 0.67 (0.56–0.76)] than DZ twin pairs [r II = 0.45 (0.35–0.54), r SS = 0.43 (0.33–0.53)]. There were statistically significant (p < 0.001) autoregressions from substance use at age 13–14 [β = 0.17 (0.12–0.23)], and from age 14 to 15 [β = 0.19 (0.13–0.25)], but not from age 15 to 17.

Fig. 1. Dual change score model of adolescent substance use from age 13 to 17. Note: N = 373 MZ and 572 DZ twins. Unstandardized factor loadings for the intercept and growth terms were orthonormalized. Standardized estimates are presented for autoregressions and for the intercept/slope correlations. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are presented in brackets. Estimates for MZ twins are presented to the left of the slash and estimates for DZ twins are present to the right of the slash. Non-significant autoregressions were omitted from the model. **p < 0.01.

Dual change score model with ACE decomposition

The next step was to conduct an ACE decomposition of the dual change score model. To this end, the variances and the covariance of the intercept and slope of substance use were decomposed into common and unique genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) sources of influence. The model fit the data, χ2(63) = 86.92, N = 476, p = 0.02, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.04. Results are depicted in Fig. 2. Significant genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors were identified for the intercept [γ IAshared = 0.68 (0.43–0.93), γ ICshared = 0.59 (0.36–0.82), γ IEshared = 0.43 (0.29–0.57), p < 0.001], which, respectively, explained 47, 35, and 18% of the variance.

Fig. 2. ACE decomposition of the dual change score model of adolescent substance use from age 13 to 17. Note: N = 187 MZ and 289 DZ twin dyads. Unstandardized factor loadings for the intercept and growth terms were orthonormalized. Standardized estimates are presented for autoregressions and for all additional factor loadings. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are presented in brackets. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Genetic factors also affected the slope of substance use but these genetic factors were partly different from the ones associated with the intercept. More precisely, 24% of the slope variance was explained by genetic factors common to the intercept [γ SAshared = 0.49 (0.14–0.84), p < 0.001], whereas another 34% of the variance was explained by different genetic factors [γ SAunique = 0.58 (0.44–0.72), p = 0.006]. Shared environmental and non-shared environmental effects, respectively, accounted for 30% and 12% of the slope variance [γ SCshared = 0.55 (0.25–0.84), p < 0.001; γ SEshared = 0.35 (0.13–0.58), p = 0.002]. The environmental effects on the slope were the same as those that influenced the intercept of substance use.

The remaining variance in substance use not captured by the intercept and growth functions of the dual change score model was decomposed into latent variables representing age-specific additive genetic (A13–A17), shared environmental (C13–C17), and non-shared environmental influences (EE17). At ages 14 and 15, age-specific genetic factors accounted for 27% and 30% of the variance, respectively, in substance use [γ A14 = 0.52 (0.41–0.63), γ A15 = 0.55 (0.44–0.65)]. At ages 14, 15, 16, and 17, age-specific non-shared environmental factors accounted for 47, 41, 34, and 22% of the variance, respectively, in substance use [γ A13 = 0.69 (0.56–0.81), γ A14 = 0.64 (0.55–0.74), γ A15 = 0.59 (0.49–0.68), γ A17 = 0.47 (0.36–0.58)]. Age-specific genetic factors at ages 14 and 17, as well as age-specific shared environmental factors at all ages, did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance.

The final analyses examined the extent to which age-specific genetic and non-shared environmental sources of influence explained the significant autoregressions identified in the baseline dual change score model. Only autoregressions involving significant age-specific variances were estimated. Results revealed significant associations between the age-specific genetic factors at age 14 and those at age 15, β = 0.38 (0.13–0.64), p = 0.003, between the age-specific non-shared environmental factors at age 13 and those at age 14, β = 0.22 (0.05–0.38), p = 0.010, and between the age-specific non-shared environmental factors at age 14 and those at age 15, β = 0.22 (0.05–0.40), p = 0.014.

Discussion

Consistent with previous studies, alcohol and marijuana use increased in a linear fashion from early to late adolescence (Duncan & Duncan, Reference Duncan and Duncan1996). However, both the general level of use and the rate of growth varied from one individual to another. Genetic as well as shared and non-shared environmental factors explained the general level of substance use; these same factors also partly accounted for inter-individual differences in growth in substance use from age 13 to 17. Importantly, our analyses also revealed genetic influences that are unique to the growth in substance use. These unique genetic effects were not accounted by autoregressive effects, which were also significant from age 13 to 14 and from age 14 to 15. Some of the inter-individual temporal stability of substance use, as indicated by these auto-regressive paths, resulted from carry-over effects of time-specific non-shared environmental influences. However, the stability of substance use from age 14 to 15 was mostly explained by carry-over effects of time-specific genetic influences. Globally, these results are in line with the ‘dual change score model’ (McArdle, Reference McArdle2009; Eaves et al. Reference Eaves, Gillespie and Verhulst2016).

Latent growth effects

Genetic as well as shared and non-shared environmental factors affected both the general level of substance use and rates of change in substance use from age 13 to 17. The genes that might be responsible for this double effect from early to late adolescence may be the same as the genes related to heritable personal characteristics such as sensation seeking or hyperactivity–impulsivity that have been found to predict the amount and the rate of change in substance use during adolescence in non-genetically informed studies (Duncan & Duncan, Reference Duncan and Duncan1996; Colder et al. Reference Colder, Campbell, Ruel, Richardson and Flay2002; Sartor et al. Reference Sartor, Lynskey, Heath, Jacob and True2007). Attitudes toward substance, acquired from parents or the community, may be responsible for shared environmental influences over the level and the growth in substance use across adolescence. In other studies (e.g. Baker et al. Reference Baker, Maes and Kendler2012), perceived household substance use and parent attitudes towards substance use account for a portion of the shared environmental variance in substance use. Finally, idiosyncratic experiences with peers or others could account for non-shared environmental experiences that influence the overall level and the growth in substance use from early to late adolescence.

It is noteworthy that individual rates of change (i.e. the slope) were influenced by the unfolding of genetic factors, some of which differ from the genetic factors that influence the overall level of substance use. A similar finding emerged from the only other study of growth modeling of the development of alcohol use from age 15 to 25 (Long et al. Reference Long, Verhulst, Aggen, Kendler and Gillespie2017). In contrast to that study, however, no quadratic component was found in the present study, possibly because of the younger developmental period under examination here (i.e. the shifting point in genetic influences was age 18 in the Gillespie et al. study). The linear unfolding of genetic influences at different rates across individuals may reflect differential acceleration in the degree of autonomy adolescents gain across adolescence, but it could also reflect differential acceleration in maturational processes.

The possibility that genetic influences unfold at different rates for different individuals has important consequences: first, it suggests that age groups should not be collapsed. Individual growth should be considered apart from stability of substance use. Second, it suggests that prevention programs targeting same-age adolescents should account for variability in rates of change. These tentative conclusions are further accentuated by the fact that new genetic and environmental influences were also found in the autoregressive part of the model.

Autoregressive effects

Autoregressive effects partially accounted for the stability of non-shared environmental effects from age 13 to 14 and from age 14 to 15, but not from age 15 to 17. The latter null effect may have been because of the 2-year time interval. Autoregressive effects also accounted for age-specific effects at each time period. Non-shared environmental influences may be confounded with measurement error, which could explain some of the autoregressive and the time-specific effects. However, time-specific effects may also reflect environmental experiences that differ as adolescents grow older. For example, youth may not necessarily maintain the same friends over the course of adolescence. Changes in the friendship group may be reflected in emerging non-shared environmental influences. Yet despite changes in the composition of the peer network, new friends may nevertheless behave similar to old friends (e.g. they may all show high levels of substance use), which may explain the persistent effects of time-specific non-shared environmental influences that account, at least partly, for the stability of substance use from ages 13 14 and from ages 14 to 15.

Interestingly, new genetic factors emerged to influence the variability in substance use at ages 14 and 15 and they accumulated across the two ages. These new genetic factors seem to be expressed when substance use is on the rise (i.e. ages 14 and 15), but before substance use becomes normative by age 17. As suggested by Edwards & Kendler (Reference Edwards and Kendler2013), these age-specific genetic factors might represent a liability specific to substance use. In contrast, persistent genetic effects on substance use may reflect general dispositions to impulsivity, risk taking and problem behaviors (Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Hicks, Patrick, Carlson, Iacono and McGue2002). Together, these findings help explain the increasing importance of genetic influences from early to late adolescence (e.g. Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Gardner, Gillespie, Aggen and Prescott2007; Baker et al. Reference Baker, Maes, Larsson, Lichtenstein and Kendler2011). Yet, the absence of new genetic effects by age 17 is not in line with earlier studies (i.e. Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Gardner, Gillespie, Aggen and Prescott2007; Baker et al. Reference Baker, Maes, Larsson, Lichtenstein and Kendler2011). These different results could be explained by the use of different instruments to assess substance use or by changing societal norms with respect to substance use. To illustrate, the retrospective assessment of substance use in the Kendler et al. (Reference Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen and Prescott2008) study included substances such as caffeine and tobacco and referred to a period of time between the late 60s and the early 90s. At that time, attitudes toward substances such as alcohol and marijuana were less liberal than today (Miech et al. Reference Miech, Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, Schulenberg and Patrick2017). Different findings for new genetic effects at age 17 could also be explained by the fact that previous studies did not use a model that allowed for the segregation of factors driven by genetic and environmental time-specific effects from those that directly contribute to the unfolding of growth.

Strengths and limitations

The present study offers several important advantages over previous studies, including the use of a prospective longitudinal design that spans a critical period for the development of substance use and the adoption of a dual change model. Despite these merits, the present study also has limitations. First, our focus was on the frequency of substance use and not on substance-related problems. Although use by early adolescence and high involvement throughout adolescence are established precursors of later problems, caution should be exercised in generalizing our findings to the etiology of addictions. Second, we note our exclusive reliance on self-reports to assess substance use. Although adolescents’ self-reports are reliable and valid, a multi-informant approach would have been preferable (Winters et al. Reference Winters, Stinchfield, Henly and Schwartz1990–91). Third, the relatively small sample size precluded the examination of possible sex differences. Finally, results are restricted to twins born in Quebec between 1996 and 1998.

We close with two tentative conclusions. First, it is important to determine whether specific endophenotype(s), which may themselves be heritable, mediate the genetic influences on the general level and the growth in substance use during adolescence. The identification of such phenotypes could be useful both for early screening of at-risk individuals and as a potential target for preventive interventions. As mentioned, sensation seeking/hyperactivity–impulsivity could play a role (Bezdjian et al. Reference Bezdjian, Baker and Tuvblad2011), but other endophenotypes such as reward dependence/delayed discounting (i.e. how much a reward loses value based on its distance in time) (MacKillop, Reference MacKillop2013) or a more general externalizing problem syndrome (Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning and Kramer2007) could be involved. Second, prevention programs in early adolescence that target personal and familial risk or beneficial factors linked to substance use may not be optimally effective, unless they are complemented by modules that specifically target later risk factors reflected in age-specific genetic and non-shared environmental influences.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP 123342), the Fonds de Recherche du Québec (Société et Culture; 2014-JU-172894), the Quebec Ministry of Health and Social Services, the US National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD068421) and the US National Science Foundation (0909733).

Footnotes

None of the authors declares any conflict of interest. Frank Vitaro wrote the first draft of this manuscript. No honorarium, grant, or other form of payment was given to anyone to produce the manuscript.

References

Baker, JH, Maes, HH and Kendler, KS (2012) Shared environmental contributions to substance use. Behavior Genetics 42, 345353.Google Scholar
Baker, JH, Maes, HH, Larsson, H, Lichtenstein, P and Kendler, KS (2011) Sex differences and developmental stability in genetic and environmental influences on psychoactive substance consumption from early adolescence to young adulthood. Psychological Medicine 41, 19071916.Google Scholar
Bezdjian, S, Baker, LA and Tuvblad, C (2011) Genetic and environmental influences on impulsivity: a meta-analysis of twin, family and adoption studies. Clinical Psychology Review 31, 12091223.Google Scholar
Boivin, M, Brendgen, M, Dionne, G, Dubois, L, Pérusse, D, Robaey, P et al. (2013) The Quebec newborn twin study into adolescence: 15 years later. Twin Research and Human Genetics 16, 6469.Google Scholar
Boomsma, DI and Molenaar, PCM (1987) The genetic-analysis of repeated measures. 1. Simplex models. Behavior Genetics 17, 111123.Google Scholar
Colder, CR, Campbell, RT, Ruel, E, Richardson, JL and Flay, BR (2002) A finite mixture model of growth trajectories of adolescent alcohol use: predictors and consequences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 70, 976985.Google Scholar
Duncan, SC and Duncan, TE (1996) A multivariate latent growth curve analysis of adolescent substance Use. Structural Equation Modeling – a Multidisciplinary Journal 3, 323347.Google Scholar
Eaves, LJ, Long, J and Heath, AC (1986) A theory of developmental-change in quantitative phenotypes applied to cognitive-development. Behavior Genetics 16, 143162.Google Scholar
Eaves, LJ, Gillespie, N and Verhulst, B (2016) Developmental Models. Power point presentation. Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics. Available at http://ibg.colorado.edu/cdrom2016/gillespie/Development/Gillespie2016_Developmental_Models.pdf (Accessed 15 September 2017).Google Scholar
Edwards, AC and Kendler, KS (2013) Alcohol consumption in men is influenced by qualitatively different genetic factors in adolescence and adulthood. Psychological Medicine 43, 18571868.Google Scholar
Falconer, DS (1960) Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. New York, NY: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Gillespie, NA, Eaves, LJ, Maes, H and Silberg, JL (2015) Testing models for the contributions of genes and environment to developmental change in adolescent depression. Behavior Genetics 45, 382393.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, HH (1991) A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: a research note. Behavior Genetics 21, 257269.Google Scholar
Han, C, McGue, MK and Iacono, WG (1999) Lifetime tobacco, alcohol and other substance use in adolescent Minnesota twins: univariate and multivariate behavioral genetic analyses. Addiction 94, 981993.Google Scholar
Henly, GA and Winters, KC (1989) Development of psychosocial scales for the assessment of adolescent involved with alcohol and drugs. The International Journal of the Addictions 24, 9731001.Google Scholar
Hingson, RW, Heeren, T and Winter, MR (2006) Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence – age at onset, duration, and severity. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 160, 739746.Google Scholar
Hopfer, CJ, Crowley, TJ and Hewitt, JK (2003) Review of twin and adoption studies of adolescent substance use. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 42, 710719.Google Scholar
Jacobus, J, Bava, S, Cohen-Zion, M, Mahmood, O and Tapert, SF (2009) Functional consequences of marijuana use in adolescents. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 92, 559565.Google Scholar
Johnston, LD, O'Malley, PM, Bachman, JG and Schulenberg, JE (2015) Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug use, 1975–2014. Volume I: Secondary School Students. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Jacobson, KC, Gardner, CO, Gillespie, N, Aggen, SA and Prescott, CA (2007) Creating a social world – a developmental twin study of peer-group deviance. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 958965.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Jacobson, KC, Prescott, CA and Neale, MC (2003) Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 687695.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Schmitt, E, Aggen, SH and Prescott, CA (2008) Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 674682.Google Scholar
Koopmans, JR, van Doornen, LJP and Boomsma, DI (1997) Association between alcohol use and smoking in adolescent and young adult twins: a bivariate genetic analysis. Alcoholism-Clinical and Experimental Research 21, 537546.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, Hicks, BM, Patrick, CJ, Carlson, SR, Iacono, WG and McGue, M (2002) Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111, 411424.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, Markon, KE, Patrick, CJ, Benning, SD and Kramer, MD (2007) Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: an integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 645666.Google Scholar
Long, EC, Verhulst, B, Aggen, SH, Kendler, KS and Gillespie, NA (2017) Contributions of genes and environment to developmental change in alcohol use. Behavior Genetics 47, 498506.Google Scholar
Lynskey, MT, Agrawal, A and Heath, AC (2010) Genetically informative research on adolescent substance use: methods, findings, and challenges. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 12021214.Google Scholar
MacKillop, J (2013) Integrating behavioral economics and behavioral genetics: delayed reward discounting as an endophenotype for addictive disorders. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior 99, 1431.Google Scholar
Magnusson, PKE, Almqvist, C, Rahman, I, Ganna, A, Viktorin, A, Walum, H et al. (2013) The Swedish twin registry: establishment of a biobank and other recent developments. Twin Research and Human Genetics 16, 317329.Google Scholar
Malone, SM, Taylor, J, Marmorstein, NR, McGue, M and Iacono, WG (2004) Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior and alcohol dependence from adolescence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology 16, 943966.Google Scholar
Martino, SC, Ellickson, PL and McCaffrey, DE (2008) Developmental trajectories of substance use from early to late adolescence: a comparison of rural and urban youth. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 69, 430440.Google Scholar
McArdle, JJ (2009) Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology 60, 577605.Google Scholar
Miech, RA, Johnston, LD, O'Malley, PM, Bachman, JG, Schulenberg, JE and Patrick, ME (2017) Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug use, 1975–2016: Volume I, Secondary School Students. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Muthén, LK and Muthén, BO (1998–2017) MPlus User's Guide, 8th edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Neale, MC and Cardon, LR (1992) Methodology for Genetic Studies in Twins and Families. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Peleg-Oren, N, Saint-Jean, G, Cardenas, GA, Tammara, H and Pierre, C (2009) Drinking alcohol before age 13 and negative outcomes in late adolescence. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 33, 19661972.Google Scholar
Sartor, CE, Grant, JD, Bucholz, KK, Madden, PAF, Heath, AC, Agrawal, A et al. (2010) Common genetic contributions to alcohol and cannabis use and dependence symptomatology. Alcoholism-Clinical and Experimental Research 34, 545554.Google Scholar
Sartor, CE, Lynskey, MT, Heath, AC, Jacob, T and True, W (2007) The role of childhood risk factors in initiation of alcohol use and progression to alcohol dependence. Addiction 102, 216225.Google Scholar
Spitz, E, Moutier, R, Reed, T, Busnel, MC, Marchaland, C, Roubertoux, PL et al. (1996) Comparative diagnoses of twin zygosity by SSLP variant analysis, questionnaire, and dermatoglyphic analysis. Behavior Genetics 26, 5563.Google Scholar
Swendsen, J, Burstein, M, Case, B, Conway, KP, Dierker, L, He, JP et al. (2012) Use and abuse of alcohol and illicit drugs in US adolescents results of the national comorbidity survey-adolescent supplement. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 390398.Google Scholar
Viken, RJ, Kaprio, J, Koskenvuo, M, and Rose, RJ (1999) Longitudinal analyses of the determinants of drinking and of drinking to intoxication in adolescent twins. Behavior Genetics 29, 455461.Google Scholar
Winters, KC, Stinchfield, RD, Henly, GA and Schwartz, RH (1990–91) Validity of adolescent self-report of alcohol and other drug involvement. The International Journal of the Addictions 25, 13791395.Google Scholar
Young, SE, Rhee, SH, Stallings, MC, Corley, RP and Hewitt, JK (2006) Genetic and environmental vulnerabilities underlying adolescent substance use and problem use: general or specific? Behavior Genetics 36, 603615.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, item frequencies, and intra-class correlations

Figure 1

Table 2. Phenotypic correlations between variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Univariate ACE decompositions of substance use at age 13, 14, 15, and 17

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Dual change score model of adolescent substance use from age 13 to 17. Note: N = 373 MZ and 572 DZ twins. Unstandardized factor loadings for the intercept and growth terms were orthonormalized. Standardized estimates are presented for autoregressions and for the intercept/slope correlations. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are presented in brackets. Estimates for MZ twins are presented to the left of the slash and estimates for DZ twins are present to the right of the slash. Non-significant autoregressions were omitted from the model. **p < 0.01.

Figure 4

Fig. 2. ACE decomposition of the dual change score model of adolescent substance use from age 13 to 17. Note: N = 187 MZ and 289 DZ twin dyads. Unstandardized factor loadings for the intercept and growth terms were orthonormalized. Standardized estimates are presented for autoregressions and for all additional factor loadings. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are presented in brackets. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.