Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T11:57:02.102Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Examining the shared and unique relationships among substance use and mental disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 September 2014

M. Sunderland*
Affiliation:
NHMRC Centre for Research Excellence in Mental Health and Substance Use, National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
T. Slade
Affiliation:
NHMRC Centre for Research Excellence in Mental Health and Substance Use, National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
R. F. Krueger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
*
* Address for correspondence: Dr M. Sunderland, Centre for Research Excellence in Mental Health and Substance Use, National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, Randwick Campus, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia. (Email: matthews@unsw.edu.au)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

Co-morbidity among use of different substances can be explained by a shared underlying dimensional factor. What remains unknown is whether the relationship between substance use and various co-morbid mental disorders can be explained solely by the general factor or whether there remain unique contributions of specific substances.

Method.

Data were from the 2007 Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing (NSMHWB). A unidimensional latent factor was constructed that represented general substance use. The shared and specific relationships between lifetime substance use indicators and internalizing disorders, suicidality and psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) were examined using Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) models in the total sample. Additional analyses then examined the shared and specific relationships associated with substance dependence diagnoses as indicators of the latent trait focusing on a subsample of substance users.

Results.

General levels of latent substance use were significantly and positively related to internalizing disorders, suicidality and psychotic-like experiences. Similar results were found when examining general levels of latent substance dependence in a sample of substance users. There were several direct effects between specific substance use/dependence indicators and the mental health correlates that significantly improved the overall model fit but they were small in magnitude and had relatively little impact on the general relationship.

Conclusions.

The majority of pairwise co-morbid relationships between substance use/dependence and mental health correlates can be explained through a general latent factor. Researchers should focus on investigating the commonalities across all substance use and dependence indicators when studying mental health co-morbidity.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Introduction

In the general population substance use disorders are often co-morbid with other mental disorders including anxiety disorders and affective disorders (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Issakidis, Slade, Teesson and Proudfoot2003; Myrick & Brady, Reference Myrick and Brady2003). Understanding co-morbidity, particularly among substance use and mental disorders, is important for a variety of reasons (see Hall et al. Reference Hall, Degenhardt and Teesson2009 for a review). Specifically, co-morbid disorders have been linked to poor treatment response (Schäfer et al. Reference Schäfer, Eiroa-Orosa, Verthein, Dilg, Haasen and Reimer2010), poor long-term prognosis (Proudfoot et al. Reference Proudfoot, Teesson, Brewin, Gournay, Teesson and Proudfood2003), greater impairment and disability (Teesson et al. Reference Teesson, Slade and Mills2009), greater social costs (Dickey & Azeni, Reference Dickey and Azeni1996) and increased mortality rates (Dickey et al. Reference Dickey, Dembling, Azeni and Normand2004), particularly among adolescents and young adults (Teesson & Proudfoot, Reference Teesson, Proudfoot, Teesson and Proudfoot2003). Co-morbidity also adds to the complexity of establishing a formal diagnosis and determining effective treatment options, a task that is particularly challenging given that many existing services and professional guidelines specialize in the diagnosis and treatment of either substance use disorders or mental disorders with little formal overlap (Kavanagh et al. Reference Kavanagh, Mueser, Baker, Teesson and Proudfoot2003).

Confounding the interpretation of co-morbid substance use and mental disorders further is the large degree of co-morbidity that is also observed within substance use (e.g. alcohol use/dependence and stimulant use/dependence). Studies from general population and clinical samples have consistently shown that co-morbidity within substance use disorders is the rule rather than the exception (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Teesson, Lynskey and Degenhardt1999; Stinson et al. Reference Stinson, Grant, Dawson, Ruan, Huang and Saha2005). Indeed, the high level of co-morbidity has led some researchers to assume that these strong relationships may be better explained or conceptualized as the manifest representation of liability factors that can be formally organized into a hierarchical meta-structure (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Goldberg, Krueger, Carpenter, Hyman, Sachdev and Pine2009; Krueger & South, Reference Krueger and South2009). Extensive work by Krueger and colleagues supports this claim. Through the use of latent variable modelling techniques, Krueger and co-workers have demonstrated the importance of an overarching dimensional factor, referred to as externalizing, when explaining the relationships among multiple disorders that involve disinhibition and antagonism (Krueger, Reference Krueger1999; Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, McGue and Iacono2001, Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning and Kramer2007; Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006; Slade & Watson, Reference Slade and Watson2006; Wright et al. Reference Wright, Krueger, Hobbs, Markon, Eaton and Slade2013). Moreover, previous investigations have highlighted the hierarchical nature of externalizing behaviours, which encompass a single overarching externalizing factor and two distinct subfactors that represent callous-aggressive tendencies and substance use. The substance use subfactor can then be defined further as representing several facets that include multiple substance use and substance problems (Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning and Kramer2007; Derringer et al. Reference Derringer, Krueger, Dick, Agrawal, Bucholz, Foroud, Grucza, Hesselbrock, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Nurnberger, Schuckit, Bierut, Iacono and McGue2013). The current analysis focuses on the substance use subfactor of the externalizing spectrum rather than the broad externalizing domain.

There are several notable advantages to using broad dimensional latent traits to understand psychopathology co-morbidity from both psychometric and public health and treatment perspectives. The use of continuous rather than categorical measures of mental disorder offers significant improvements in reliability and validity (Markon et al. Reference Markon, Chmielewski and Miller2011). Moreover, using broad hierarchical traits that capture multiple disorders from traditionally separate disorder clusters may result in improvements in clinical utility by simplifying the complex nature and structure of the existing classification systems (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Goldberg, Krueger, Carpenter, Hyman, Sachdev and Pine2009). Indeed, the recent revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders briefly recognizes the importance of a dimensional and hierarchical structure of mental disorders (APA, 2013, pp. 12–13). From a public health perspective, incorporating higher-order dimensions in the extant nosology could encourage greater prevention programmes targeting the broader psychopathological constructs and thereby reducing the incidence of multiple related disorders (Brown & Barlow, Reference Brown and Barlow2005). In addition, the inclusion of broader traits can facilitate empirical exploration of shared aetiology between putatively distinct disorders as well as incorporating important biological risk factors, including genetic and environmental risk factors, when modelling levels of psychopathology (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Prescott, Myers and Neale2003; Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick and Iacono2005). Finally, from a treatment perspective, incorporating broad traits of psychopathology provides greater credence to unified treatment approaches that seek to treat the commonalities shared by co-morbid mental disorders and polysubstance abuse/dependence (Brown & Barlow, Reference Brown and Barlow2009; Hesse, Reference Hesse2009; Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Daley and Douaihy2012).

Prior evidence has demonstrated the utility and centrality of latent variables when describing co-morbid relationships between substance use and mental disorders. Notably, Kushner et al. (Reference Kushner, Wall, Krueger, Sher, Maurer, Thuras and Lee2012) demonstrated that the majority of the relationships between mood and anxiety disorders could be explained by a common latent trait representing internalizing liability and it was this latent trait that was strongly related to alcohol dependence rather than the influence of specific DSM-IV mood or anxiety diagnoses. Indeed, Kushner et al. (Reference Kushner, Wall, Krueger, Sher, Maurer, Thuras and Lee2012) concluded that knowledge of the overall severity of internalizing psychopathology and the commonalities across mood and anxiety disorders provides more information about whether each individual will also be alcohol dependent than does knowledge about the presence of any single mood or anxiety disorder. Finally, in two studies that investigated the development of co-morbidity across the lifespan, Kessler et al. (Reference Kessler, Cox, Green, Ormel, McLaughlin, Merikangas, Petukhova, Pine, Russo, Swendsen, Wittchen and Zaslavsky2011a , Reference Kessler, Ormel, Petukhova, McLaughlin, Green, Russo, Stein, Zaslavsky, Anguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Andrade, Benjet, de Girolamo, de Graaf, Demyttenaere, Fayyad, Haro, Hu, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Matchsinger, Mihaescu-Pintia, Posada-Villa, Sagar and Ustün b ) found that the majority of the 306 pairwise time-lagged associations among 18 disorders can be explained by a model that assumes the existence of mediating latent internalizing and externalizing variables.

The current study sought to further demonstrate the centrality of latent variables when investigating the mental health correlates of several types of substance use in the Australian general population, along with several types of substance dependence in a substance-using subsample of the population. Primarily, the current study sought to determine whether mental disorders are related generally to substance use and substance dependence or whether there remains a unique relationship between mental disorders and some of the different types of substance use and substance dependence over and above the general relationship. The mental disorders of interest examined in the current study include several mood and anxiety disorders and psychotic-like experiences (PLEs). Suicidality was also examined. Given the high degree of co-morbidity between mood and anxiety disorders, the current study modelled the mood and anxiety disorders as a more parsimonious latent dimension representing internalizing liability rather than modelling several discrete categorical variables separately. It is this internalizing liability and how it relates to general and specific types of substance use and dependence that is of interest in the current study. There may, however, be several noteworthy differences in the contribution of suicidality and PLEs that warrant detailed investigation. Therefore, the differing rates of suicidality and PLEs were modelled as separate observable categorical variables.

Method

Sample

Data for the current study were from the 2007 Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing (NSMHWB), a nationally representative private household survey of the Australian population aged 16–85 years. The survey recruited participants using a multi-stage clustered sampling design to ensure representativeness. Once the sampling framework was established, the survey collected information from one randomly selected household member from each of the 8841 fully-responding households out of a possible 14 805, resulting in a response rate of 60%. Oversampling of the younger (16–24 years old) and older (65–85 years old) household members was used to generate representative estimates from these traditionally under-represented age groups. Data in the current study were weighted to correspond to the sociodemographic characteristics of the Australian population. In brief, 49.6% were male, 57% were married or in a de-facto relationship, 65.2% were employed, 53.9% had received no post-school qualification, and 72.9% were born in Australia. More information on the sample characteristics and design of the NSMHWB are available in Slade et al. (Reference Slade, Johnston, Oakley Browne, Andrews and Whiteford2009).

Measures

Substance use

The current study examined variables that represent the use of five broad types of substances across the respondent's lifetime: binge drinking, cannabis use, sedative use, stimulant use, and opiate use. Binge drinking was classified among the respondents in the current study as consuming five or more standard alcoholic drinks (10 g of alcohol) per day on the days they drank in the past 12 months or the period of time when their drinking was the worst. Cannabis use was classified as using cannabis, marijuana or hashish more than five times. Stimulant use was classified as using stimulants (e.g. amphetamine, speed, ice, methylphenidate, dexamphetamine) without the recommendation of a health professional more than five times. Sedative use was classified as using sedatives (e.g. Valium, Xanax, diazepam) without the recommendation of a health professional more than five times. Finally, opiate use was classified as using opiates (e.g. heroin, opium, Fentanyl, OxyContin, Suboxone) without the recommendation of a health professional more than five times.

Substance dependence

The lifetime presence of DSM-IV substance dependence was assessed using the World Mental Health version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (WMH-CIDI; Kessler & Ustün, Reference Kessler and Ustün2004). The WMH-CIDI has demonstrated sound reliability and has good clinical validity in relation to a clinician-administered semi-structured diagnostic interview, the Structured Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV), which is traditionally considered as the ‘gold standard’ in psychiatric assessments (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Abelson, Demler, Escobar, Gibbon, Guyer, Howes, Jin, Vega, Walters, Wang, Zaslavsky and Zheng2004). The specific substance dependence diagnoses included: alcohol dependence, cannabis dependence, stimulant dependence, sedative dependence and opiate dependence.

Mental health correlates

To assess the presence of lifetime DSM-IV mental disorders, the WMH-CIDI was again used to measure each criterion and apply the diagnostic decision rules. To assess the influence of co-morbid mental disorders in the current study, the diagnostic criteria were applied without hierarchy rules, that is the presence of one disorder is not ruled out over the presence of another related disorder. However, disorders were excluded if they were attributed solely to a physical condition or medication use. Mental disorders examined in the current study included: major depressive episode, dysthymia, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), social phobia, panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

To measure non-specific rates of suicidality, the survey included several questions that were separate from the mood and anxiety modules of the WMH-CIDI. Three questions were used in the current study: the first measured general suicidal thoughts ‘Have you ever seriously thought about committing suicide?’, the second measured suicidal plans ‘Have you ever made a plan for committing suicide?’ and the third measured suicide attempts ‘Have you ever attempted suicide?’ Respondents were only asked about suicidal plans if they had also indicated that they had experienced suicidal thoughts and were only asked about suicide attempts if they had also indicated suicidal plans. The three questions were dummy coded, treating ‘no suicidality’ as the reference category.

To measure the presence of PLEs, a brief self-report screening instrument was used to determine whether the respondent had ever experienced someone or something directly controlling or interfering with their thoughts, that people were too interested in them, or that they had special powers that most people lack. This instrument has been used in previous research to investigate the correlates of PLEs in the Australian general population (Scott et al. Reference Scott, Chant, Andrews, Martin and McGrath2007; Saha et al. Reference Saha, Scott, Varghese and McGrath2011). The presence of PLEs was measured by summing the number of PLEs endorsed by each respondent and then dummy coded to represent the presence of no PLEs (reference category), one PLE, and two or more PLEs. Auditory or visual hallucinations occurring during dreams or half-asleep or under the influence of alcohol or drugs were excluded.

Analysis

The analytic approach was dividing into two stages: the first involved testing the measurement model of substance use, substance dependence and internalizing using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA); and the second involved building two Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) models for substance use in the total sample and substance dependence in a subsample of substance users.

CFA modelling

Separate CFA models were used to confirm the suitability of using a unidimensional structure for substance use, substance dependence and internalizing. Absolute fit indices to measure model fit are not available when using full information maximum likelihood for categorical data. Therefore, the CFA models were estimated using tetrachoric correlation matrices and the weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator. The latent variables were identified by fixing the latent variance to 1.0 and estimating all factor loadings. Model fit was determined using a range of absolute fit indices, including: the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis fit index (TLI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Good model fit was determined using recommended cut-offs established by simulation studies, for example RMSEA < 0.05, CFI > 0.95 and TLI > 0.95 (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1998; Yu, Reference Yu2002).

MIMIC modelling

In brief, MIMIC models consist of three components that are built sequentially (see Gallo et al. Reference Gallo, Anthony and Muthén1994 for more detailed information). The first component involves a measurement model whereby a series of observed categorical indicators for substance use or substance dependence are related to a continuous and normally distributed latent variable. The second component involves a regression model whereby the continuous latent variable is regressed on a series of correlates or background variables of interest. The correlates of interest in the current study include: internalizing (measured as a continuous and normally distributed latent variable), suicidality and PLEs. Sex and age were also included as background variables to account for any differences in the latent variables attributed to these sociodemographic features. These estimates are considered ‘indirect effects’ because the relationships between the indicators and correlates of interest are completely mediated through the general latent trait. The third component involves identifying and estimating direct regression effects between the indicators of substance use/dependence and the mental health correlates of interest in a model that already contains the indirect effects. These ‘direct effects’ can be interpreted as indicating that a specific relationship exists between some indicators and the mental health correlates while controlling for mean differences in the latent trait attributed to the correlates. As the aim of the current study was to identify the specific relationships between substance indicators and mental health correlates, only the direct effects associated with internalizing, suicidality and PLEs were examined in this analysis. All MIMIC models were parameterized as two-parameter logistic item response models and fitted to the data using a full information robust maximum likelihood estimator for categorical data as implemented by Mplus version 7.2 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2010).

The current study used a strategy developed by Woods et al. (Reference Woods, Oltmanns and Turkheimer2009). This approach has been used extensively in the prior literature to detect differential item functioning as part of routine psychometric testing. This strategy begins by identifying indicators that do not exhibit any direct effects with the correlates to form a set of ‘anchors’ that identify the subsequent MIMIC models (Woods, Reference Woods2009). This was achieved by estimating the direct effects, in five separate models, associated with one indicator at a time while the direct effects associated with the other indicators were fixed to zero. Indicators that exhibited non-significant direct effects across all the mental health correlates of interest were allocated to the anchor set whereas indicators that had significant direct effects were allocated to the study set and were examined in subsequent MIMIC modelling.

The direct effects associated with the indicators of substance use/dependence that were assigned to the study set were then tested individually using scale-corrected likelihood ratio (LR) difference tests for nested models. For example, to test the significance of the direct effects associated with alcohol dependence, a model that estimated the direct effects for all the studied indicators was compared to a model that fixed the direct effects associated with alcohol dependence to zero. If removing the direct effects associated with alcohol dependence resulted in a significant decrease in model fit, then this would provide evidence that a specific relationship between alcohol dependence and the mental health correlates is present over and above the general relationship. Bonferroni adjustments were made to the p value to account for multiple LR difference tests. LR difference tests for nested models have been criticized for being overly sensitive to trivial effects, particularly in large samples, and therefore the difference in Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) values between the fitted models is also presented. Lower AIC and BIC values indicate a better model fit when comparing two competing models (Burnham & Anderson, Reference Burnham and Anderson2002). Therefore, positive ∆AIC and ∆BIC values indicate that the model with all the estimated direct effects provides a better fit whereas negative values indicate that the competing model without the direct effects provides a better fit.

Final MIMIC models for the substance use indicators in the total sample and substance dependence indicators in a sample of substance users were then estimated that included all the significant direct effects identified in the final step. The direct effects are presented as odds ratios (ORs) (exponentiated regression coefficients) and associated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) to assist with interpretation. Finally, the regression coefficients associated with the latent variables and the correlates of interest were estimated and compared using two models (one that did not adjust for any direct effects, that is an indirect effects model, and the final MIMIC model that adjusted for significant direct effects). Comparing these regression coefficients determines the overall impact of the direct effects on the indirect effects and provides some indication of the overall validity and utility of the general latent variables when describing the multiple relationships between substance use/dependence and mental disorders.

Results

CFA

The frequencies associated with each of the substance use, substance dependence and mental disorder indicators used in the subsequent factor models are provided in Table 1. The CFA models supported a unidimensional structure of substance use with excellent fit according to all three fit statistics (CFI = 0.989, TLI = 0.977, RMSEA = 0.049). Similarly, the CFA supported a unidimenisonal structure of substance dependence in a sample of substance users (CFI = 0.994, TLI = 0.987, RMSEA = 0.025). This confirms the use of a unidimensional measurement model to describe latent levels of general substance use and general substance dependence in subsequent analyses. A unidimensional structure of mood and anxiety disorders was also fit to both the total sample (CFI = 0.994, TLI = 0.991, RMSEA = 0.022) and a sample of substance users (CFI = 0.995, TLI = 0.992, RMSEA = 0.022), with model fit statistics indicating excellent fit.

Table 1. Weighted frequencies (%) for substance use and mental disorders in the total sample and among substance users

MIMIC modelling

Table 2 provides the results of the model fit comparisons testing the significance of the direct effects of the mental health correlates of interest on the substance use and substance dependence indicators respectively. For the substance use indicators, opiate use formed the anchor set and was assumed to have no direct effects with the correlates of interest. After adjusting the p value for multiple comparisons, the removal of the direct effects associated with binge drinking resulted in a significant decrease in the model fit. For substance dependence indicators, stimulant dependence and opiate dependence formed the anchor set whereas the removal of the direct effects associated with alcohol dependence and sedative dependence resulted in a significance decrease in model fit. The ∆AIC values suggested that the removal of direct effects associated with cannabis use and stimulant use resulted in a minor decrease in model fit whereas the values for the remaining substances confirmed the LR difference tests. By contrast, the ∆BIC values suggested that the removal of the direct effects associated with only alcohol dependence resulted in a decrease in model fit.

Table 2. Model fit comparisons testing for the direct effect between substance use and substance dependence indicators and mental health correlates in the total sample and a sample of substance users

df, Degrees of freedom; ∆AIC, difference in the Akaike information criterion between a model with all direct effects included and a model with the direct effects of the studied indicator fixed to zero; ∆BIC, difference in the Bayesian information criterion between a model with all direct effects included and a model with the direct effects of the studied indicator fixed to zero; ∆χ2, scaled χ2 difference values of model fit between a model with all direct effects included and a model with the direct effects of the studied indicator fixed to zero.

Bold type indicates significant ∆χ2 test of model fit after Bonferroni adjustments to the critical p value.

a Treated as an empirically selected anchor item with all direct effects fixed to zero to identify the model.

The path diagrams of the final two MIMIC models are provided in Fig. 1 with direct effects estimated for binge drinking only in the substance use model whereas direct effects for alcohol dependence and sedative dependence were estimated in the substance dependence model. ORs and 95% CIs associated with the significant direct effects (i.e. the broken arrows in Fig. 1) for both models are provided in Table 3. Controlling for the other correlates and for mean differences in latent substance use, people with higher rates of internalizing had a significantly lower probability of binge drinking (OR 0.82). Likewise, respondents with suicidal plans had a lower probability of binge drinking than people without suicidality (OR 0.64). Controlling for the other correlates and for mean differences in latent substance dependence, people with higher rates of internalizing were more likely to experience alcohol dependence (OR 1.75) and sedative dependence (OR 2.89). Those with suicidal plans were almost four times more likely to experience sedative dependence (OR 3.96) whereas those with suicide attempts were almost three times more likely to experience alcohol dependence (OR 2.89) compared to those without suicidality. Finally, respondents who report two or more PLEs were less likely to experience sedative dependence in their lifetime compared to those with no PLEs (OR 0.11).

Fig. 1. Path diagrams for final Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) models for substance use and substance dependence among a sample of substance users. Top diagram represents substance use among the total sample. Bottom diagram represents substance dependence among a sample of substance users. Reference categories: no suicidality, no psychotic-like experiences (PLEs), and male. Age and internalizing entered as continuous variables.

Table 3. Odds ratios (95% confidence intervals) for direct effects of substance use and substance dependence indicators on mental health correlates in the final estimated MIMIC models

MIMIC, Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes; PLE, psychotic-like experience.

Bold type indicates significant at the p < 0.05 level.

The unadjusted and adjusted regression coefficients associated with differences in the mean levels of general substance use and substance dependence across the correlates are provided in Table 4. Prior to adjusting for direct effects, internalizing, aspects of suicidality, two or more PLEs, sex and age demonstrated significant positive associations with general substance use and substance dependence. The presence of two or more PLEs was related to higher rates of general substance dependence than to general substance use whereas the presence of suicidal thoughts was strongly associated to general substance use and not general substance dependence. After adjusting for the significant direct effects, the overall pattern of significance regarding the indirect effects was similar to the unadjusted models with relatively minor differences in the magnitude associated with the regression coefficients. A notable exception involved the regression coefficient associated with suicide attempts and general substance dependence. After adjusting for direct effects, the effect of suicide attempts diminished to a large extent. Similarly, but to a lesser extent, the indirect effect associated with internalizing and general substance dependence diminished after adjusting for direct effects although remained highly significant.

Table 4. Regression coefficients for MIMIC models examining differences in mean latent substance use and substance dependence across levels of the correlates with and without adjusting for significant direct effects

MIMIC, Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes; PLE, psychotic-like experience.

Unadjusted model fixes all direct effects between substance use/dependence indicators and mental health correlates to zero. Adjusted models include significant direct effects described in Fig. 1.

***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

Discussion

The current study suggests that, for the most part, the relationship between the use of various substances and co-occurring mental disorders is shared and cumulative. Several significant direct effects associated with specific substance use and substance dependence indicators and the mental health correlates of interest were identified. However, there were inconsistencies in the model fit statistics, with the difference in BIC values suggesting that the removal of the majority of the direct effects led to significant improvements in model fit. Furthermore, the overall impact of these direct effects on the utility and validity of the general latent variables to describe differences in substance use and dependence between respondents with various mental disorders seemed to be minimal. There were two notable exceptions to this conclusion. The relatively large direct effect associated with alcohol dependence and suicide attempts sufficiently diminished the indirect relationship between general substance dependence and suicide attempts so that it was no longer significant. Therefore, a substantial proportion of the relationship between general substance dependence and suicide attempts can be explained by mechanisms that are specific to alcohol dependence. Similarly, but to a lesser extent, the direct effects associated with alcohol dependence and sedative dependence diminished the indirect relationship between internalizing and general substance dependence; however, this relationship remained significant.

It is possible to speculate on the mechanisms that might be driving the specific direct effects between alcohol dependence and suicide attempts and also those between alcohol dependence, sedative dependence and internalizing. However, these conclusions should be thought of as preliminary as more detailed research is required. These direct effects might be plausibly explained by a ‘self-medication’ hypothesis, which assumes that the maladaptive use of alcohol and/or sedatives is the result of a coping mechanism to reduce the symptoms and severity of suicidality and internalizing. Indeed, previous research has linked the use of alcohol and sedatives as a maladaptive coping mechanism for a range of suicidality, mood and anxiety disorders (Bolton et al. Reference Bolton, Cox, Clara and Sareen2006; Robinson et al. Reference Robinson, Sareen, Cox and Bolton2009). Such reliance on the use of these substances to reduce the severity of suicidality and internalizing may evolve over time into dependence for some individuals (Swendsen et al. Reference Swendsen, Conway, Degenhardt, Glantz, Jin, Merikangas, Sampson and Kessler2010). This hypothesis does assume a one-way direction whereby mental disorders are present prior to the emergence of substance dependence. It is possible, however, that the relationship could emerge in the other direction. Therefore, the specific mechanisms that attempt to explain the increased onset of suicidality and internalizing as a result of primarily alcohol or sedative dependence, including the associated biochemical changes that result from the extended use of these substances, warrants further investigation to determine the nature of the relationship between these specific substance and mental disorder co-morbidities.

There are several notable strengths of the current study, including the comprehensive assessment of DSM-IV criteria for multiple substance use and mental disorders, the use of a large representative sample of the Australian population, and the use of a latent variable approach to parse the general and specific relationships among substance use and mental disorders. However, these findings should also be interpreted with respect to some limitations. First, substance use and mental disorders were assessed using self-report face-to-face structured interviews. Because of the sensitive nature of the assessment, these results could have been biased due to social desirability, particularly when assessing the use of illicit substances. Furthermore, illicit substance-using individuals often represent a hidden or difficult to assess subgroup of the population and therefore these groups may be under-represented in a household survey. Consequently, the prevalence of some substance use disorders is relatively low, particularly for sedative dependence and opiate dependence. These results need to be replicated in various settings, particularly in large clinical and substance-using samples, to ensure reliability and robustness. Second, the current study was restricted to using drug and alcohol disorders to form the latent variables under investigation. Therefore, the current study was only able to examine the constituent facets of the substance use subfactor of the broader externalizing spectrum (Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning and Kramer2007). The current findings need to be extended by including various externalizing facets and behaviours to cover the full spectrum of externalizing. Third, because of the cross-sectional nature of the survey used by the current study, causality and the interaction among various specific co-morbidities over time in conjunction with the general latent factors were not investigated in this study. Interpretation of the general and specific relationships between substance use and mental disorders will be aided by future research that uses longitudinal or repeated measures designs.

The current study further highlights the utility of a ‘macroscopic’ view of disorders when predicting co-morbidity and a range of clinically relevant factors (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Ormel, Petukhova, McLaughlin, Green, Russo, Stein, Zaslavsky, Anguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Andrade, Benjet, de Girolamo, de Graaf, Demyttenaere, Fayyad, Haro, Hu, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Matchsinger, Mihaescu-Pintia, Posada-Villa, Sagar and Ustün2011b ; Kushner et al. Reference Kushner, Wall, Krueger, Sher, Maurer, Thuras and Lee2012; Eaton et al. Reference Eaton, Krueger, Markon, Keyes, Skodol, Wall, Hasin and Grant2013). Our results extend previous findings and suggest that, once the commonalities across various types of substance use and substance dependence have been entered into the model, knowledge of the specific type or types of substances present offers relatively little to our understanding of the relationship between substance use and mental disorders. These results should direct researchers and clinicians to focus on elucidating the commonalities or shared mechanisms across all the substance use disorders and determining how these commonalities are associated with mental disorders. These findings give further credence to the use of assessment tools that measure these broad dimensional constructs using item response theory (Krueger & Finger, Reference Krueger and Finger2001; Patrick et al. Reference Patrick, Kramer, Krueger and Markon2013) in addition to the use of transdiagnostic treatments that focus on treating the commonalities across putatively distinct disorders (Brown & Barlow, Reference Brown and Barlow2009; Hesse, Reference Hesse2009; Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Daley and Douaihy2012). One exception to this rule was the influence of the specific relationship between alcohol dependence and suicide attempts over and above the general relationship. Future studies should also seek to identify why the probability of alcohol dependence is so high among people with suicidality in comparison to the other types of substances examined in the current study.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Early Career Fellowship (No. 1052327). The 2007 Australian NSMHWB was funded by the Australian Government Department of Health and Ageing.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Andrews, G, Goldberg, D, Krueger, R, Carpenter, W, Hyman, S, Sachdev, P, Pine, D (2009). Exploring the feasibility of a meta-structure for DSM-V and ICD-11: could it improve utility and validity? Psychological Medicine 39, 19932000.Google Scholar
Andrews, G, Issakidis, C, Slade, T (2003). How common is comorbidity? In Comorbid Mental Disorders and Substance Use Disorders: Epidemiology, Prevention, and Treatment (ed. Teesson, M. and Proudfoot, H.), pp. 2641. Department of Health and Ageing: Canberra, ACT.Google Scholar
APA (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Bolton, J, Cox, B, Clara, I, Sareen, J (2006). Use of alcohol and drugs to self-medicate anxiety disorders in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 194, 818825.Google Scholar
Brown, TA, Barlow, DH (2005). Dimensional versus categorical classification of mental disorder in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders and beyond: comment on the special section. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 551556.Google Scholar
Brown, TA, Barlow, DH (2009). A proposal for a dimensional classification system based on the shared features of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: implications for assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment 21, 256271.Google Scholar
Burnham, KP, Anderson, DR (2002). Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach, 2nd edn. Springer-Verlag: New York, NY.Google Scholar
Derringer, J, Krueger, RF, Dick, DM, Agrawal, A, Bucholz, KK, Foroud, T, Grucza, RA, Hesselbrock, MN, Hesselbrock, V, Kramer, J, Nurnberger, JI, Schuckit, M, Bierut, LJ, Iacono, WG, McGue, M (2013). Measurement invariance of DSM-IV alcohol, marijuana and cocaine dependence between community-sampled and clinically overselected studies. Addiction 108, 17671776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickey, B, Azeni, H (1996). Persons with dual diagnoses of substance abuse and major mental illness: their excess costs of psychiatric care. American Journal of Public Health 86, 973977.Google Scholar
Dickey, B, Dembling, B, Azeni, H, Normand, S-L (2004). Externally caused deaths for adults with substance use and mental disorders. Journal of Behavioral Health Services and Research 31, 7585.Google Scholar
Eaton, NR, Krueger, RF, Markon, KE, Keyes, KM, Skodol, AE, Wall, M, Hasin, DS, Grant, BF (2013). The structure and predictive validity of the internalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 8692.Google Scholar
Gallo, JJ, Anthony, JC, Muthén, BO (1994). Age differences in the symptoms of depression: a latent trait analysis. Journal of Gerontology 49, P251P264.Google Scholar
Hall, W, Degenhardt, L, Teesson, M (2009). Understanding comorbidity between substance use, anxiety and affective disorders: broadening the research base. Addictive Behaviors 34, 526530.Google Scholar
Hall, W, Teesson, M, Lynskey, M, Degenhardt, L (1999). The 12-month prevalence of substance use and ICD-10 substance use disorders in Australian adults: findings from the National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being. Addiction 94, 15411550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesse, M (2009). Integrated psychological treatment for substance use and co-morbid anxiety or depression vs. treatment for substance use alone. A systematic review of the published literature. BMC Psychiatry 9, 6.Google Scholar
Hu, LT, Bentler, PM (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods 3, 424453.Google Scholar
Kavanagh, DJ, Mueser, KT, Baker, A (2003). Management of comorbidity. In Comorbid Mental Disorders and Substance Use Disorders: Epidemiology, Prevention, and Treatment (ed. Teesson, M. and Proudfoot, H.), pp. 78120. Department of Health and Ageing: Canberra, ACT.Google Scholar
Kelly, TM, Daley, DC, Douaihy, AB (2012). Treatment of substance abusing patients with comorbid psychiatric disorders. Addictive Behaviors 37, 1124.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Prescott, CA, Myers, J, Neale, MC (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 929937.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Abelson, J, Demler, O, Escobar, JI, Gibbon, M, Guyer, ME, Howes, MJ, Jin, R, Vega, WA, Walters, EE, Wang, P, Zaslavsky, A, Zheng, H (2004). Clinical calibration of DSM-IV diagnoses in the World Mental Health (WMH) version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (WMH-CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 13, 122139.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Cox, BJ, Green, JG, Ormel, J, McLaughlin, KA, Merikangas, KR, Petukhova, M, Pine, DS, Russo, LJ, Swendsen, J, Wittchen, HU, Zaslavsky, AM (2011 a). The effects of latent variables in the development of comorbidity among common mental disorders. Depression and Anxiety 28, 2939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Ormel, J, Petukhova, M, McLaughlin, KA, Green, JG, Russo, LJ, Stein, DJ, Zaslavsky, AM, Anguilar-Gaxiola, S, Alonso, J, Andrade, L, Benjet, C, de Girolamo, G, de Graaf, R, Demyttenaere, K, Fayyad, J, Haro, JM, Hu, CY, Karam, A, Lee, S, Lepine, J-P, Matchsinger, H, Mihaescu-Pintia, C, Posada-Villa, J, Sagar, R, Ustün, TB (2011 b). Development of lifetime comorbidity in the World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 90100.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Ustün, TB (2004). The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 13, 93121.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 921926.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, Finger, MS (2001). Using item response theory to understand comorbidity among anxiety and unipolar mood disorder. Psychological Assessment 13, 140151.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, Markon, KE (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: a model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Reviews in Clinical Psychology 2, 111133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, RF, Markon, KE, Patrick, CJ, Benning, SD, Kramer, MD (2007). Linking antisocial behaviour, substance use, and personality: an integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 645666.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, Markon, KE, Patrick, CJ, Iacono, WG (2005). Externalizing psychopathology in adulthood: a dimensional-spectrum conceptualization and its implication for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 537550.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, McGue, M, Iacono, WG (2001). The higher-order structure of common DSM mental disorders: internalization, externalization, and their connections to personality. Personality and Individual Differences 30, 12451259.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, South, S (2009). Externalizing disorders: cluster 5 of the proposed meta-structure for DSM-V and ICD-11. Psychological Medicine 39, 20612070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushner, MG, Wall, MM, Krueger, RF, Sher, KJ, Maurer, E, Thuras, P, Lee, S (2012). Alcohol dependence is related to overall internalizing psychopathology load rather than to particular internalizing disorders: evidence from a national sample. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 36, 325331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markon, KE, Chmielewski, M, Miller, CJ (2011). The reliability and validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopathology: a quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin 137, 856879.Google Scholar
Muthén, LK, Muthén, BO (2010). Mplus User's Guide. Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Myrick, H, Brady, K (2003). Current review of the comorbidity of affective, anxiety, and substance use disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 16, 261270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, CJ, Kramer, MD, Krueger, RF, Markon, KE (2013). Optimizing efficiency of psychopathology assessment through quantitative modelling: development of a brief form of the Externalizing Spectrum Inventory. Psychological Assessment 25, 13321348.Google Scholar
Proudfoot, H, Teesson, M, Brewin, E, Gournay, K (2003). Comorbidity and delivery of services. In Comorbid Mental Disorders and Substance Use Disorders: Epidemiology, Prevention, and Treatment (ed. Teesson, M. and Proudfood, H.), pp. 121142. Department of Health and Ageing: Canberra, ACT.Google Scholar
Robinson, J, Sareen, J, Cox, BJ, Bolton, J (2009). Self-medication of anxiety disorders with alcohol and drugs: results from a nationally representative sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23, 3845.Google Scholar
Saha, S, Scott, J, Varghese, D, McGrath, J (2011). The association between general psychological distress and delusional-like experiences: a large population-based study. Schizophrenia Research 127, 246251.Google Scholar
Schäfer, I, Eiroa-Orosa, FJ, Verthein, U, Dilg, C, Haasen, C, Reimer, J (2010). Effects of psychiatric comorbidity on treatment outcome in patients undergoing diamorphine or methadone maintenance treatment. Psychopathology 43, 8895.Google Scholar
Scott, J, Chant, D, Andrews, G, Martin, G, McGrath, J (2007). Association between trauma exposure and delusional experiences in a large community-based sample. British Journal of Psychiatry 190, 339343.Google Scholar
Slade, T, Johnston, A, Oakley Browne, MA, Andrews, G, Whiteford, H (2009). 2007 National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing: methods and key findings. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 43, 594605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, T, Watson, D (2006). The structure of common DSM-IV and ICD-10 mental disorders in the Australian general population. Psychological Medicine 36, 15931600.Google Scholar
Stinson, FS, Grant, BF, Dawson, DA, Ruan, WJ, Huang, B, Saha, T (2005). Comorbidity between DSM-IV alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 80, 105116.Google Scholar
Swendsen, J, Conway, KP, Degenhardt, L, Glantz, M, Jin, R, Merikangas, KR, Sampson, N, Kessler, RC (2010). Mental disorders as risk factors for substance use, abuse and dependence: results from the 10-year follow-up of the National Comorbidity Survey. Addiction 105, 11171128.Google Scholar
Teesson, M, Proudfoot, H (2003). Responding to comorbid mental disorders and substance use disorders. In Comorbid Mental Disorders and Substance Use Disorders: Epidemiology, Prevention, and Treatment (ed. Teesson, M. and Proudfoot, H.), pp. 18. Department of Health and Ageing: Canberra, ACT.Google Scholar
Teesson, M, Slade, T, Mills, K (2009). Comorbidity in Australia: findings of the 2007 National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 43, 606614.Google Scholar
Woods, CM (2009). Evaluation of MIMIC-model methods for DIF testing with comparison to two-group analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research 44, 127.Google Scholar
Woods, CM, Oltmanns, TF, Turkheimer, E (2009). Illustration of MIMIC-model DIF testing with the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioural Assessment 31, 320330.Google Scholar
Wright, AG, Krueger, RF, Hobbs, MJ, Markon, KE, Eaton, NR, Slade, T (2013). The structure of psychopathology: toward an expanded quantitative empirical model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 281294.Google Scholar
Yu, C-Y (2002). Evaluating Cutoff Criteria of Model Fit Indices for Latent Variable Models with Binary and Continuous Outcomes. University of California: Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Weighted frequencies (%) for substance use and mental disorders in the total sample and among substance users

Figure 1

Table 2. Model fit comparisons testing for the direct effect between substance use and substance dependence indicators and mental health correlates in the total sample and a sample of substance users

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Path diagrams for final Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) models for substance use and substance dependence among a sample of substance users. Top diagram represents substance use among the total sample. Bottom diagram represents substance dependence among a sample of substance users. Reference categories: no suicidality, no psychotic-like experiences (PLEs), and male. Age and internalizing entered as continuous variables.

Figure 3

Table 3. Odds ratios (95% confidence intervals) for direct effects of substance use and substance dependence indicators on mental health correlates in the final estimated MIMIC models

Figure 4

Table 4. Regression coefficients for MIMIC models examining differences in mean latent substance use and substance dependence across levels of the correlates with and without adjusting for significant direct effects