Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T12:27:04.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early motor developmental milestones and level of neuroticism in young adulthood: a 23-year follow-up study of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2012

T. Flensborg-Madsen*
Affiliation:
National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark
H. J. Sørensen
Affiliation:
Psychiatric Center, Copenhagen, Denmark Institute of Preventive Medicine, Bispebjerg Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark
R. Revsbech
Affiliation:
Psychiatric Center Hvidovre, Cognitive Research Unit, Brondby, Denmark
E. L. Mortensen
Affiliation:
Institute of Preventive Medicine, Bispebjerg Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark Institute of Public Health and Center for Healthy Aging, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
*
*Address for correspondence: T. Flensborg-Madsen, National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Øster Farimagsgade 5,2, 1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark. (Email: tfm@niph.dk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Studies investigating early developmental factors in relation to psychopathology have mainly focused on schizophrenia. The personality dimension of neuroticism seems to be a general risk factor for psychopathology, but evidence on associations between early developmental precursors and personality traits is almost non-existent. This study is therefore the first to investigate associations between early motor developmental milestones and neuroticism in adulthood.

Method

Mothers of 9125 children of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort recorded 12 developmental milestones during the child's first year of life. A subsample of the cohort comprising 1182 individuals participated in a follow-up when they were aged 20–34 years and were administered the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ). Associations between motor developmental milestones and level of neuroticism, extraversion and psychoticism were analysed by multiple linear regression adjusting for for sex, single-mother status, parity, mother's age, father's age, parental social status and birth weight.

Results

Among the 1182 participants with information on the EPQ, information on milestones was available for 968 participants. Infants who developed high levels of neuroticism as adults tended to sit without support, crawl, and walk with and without support significantly later than individuals with low levels of neuroticism (p values <0.05). These results remained significant after adjustment for the included covariates and for adult intelligence.

Conclusions

The findings are the first of their kind and suggest that delays in early motor development may not only characterize psychopathological disorders such as schizophrenia, but may also be associated with the personality dimension of neuroticism in adulthood.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Introduction

Several theorists, including Sigmund Freud, have stressed the importance of early life circumstances and early development in the growth and development of personality. While the idea that adult personality originates in early development has been discussed in many contexts (e.g. attachment theory), there is still a limited amount of empirical documentation on the importance of early developmental precursors of personality. This lack of documentation is mainly due to a lack of the complex longitudinal data that would be suitable to perform such analyses.

The hypothesis of neurodevelopmental precursors of personality traits has primarily been explored by researchers using The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study 1934–1944. Analyses based on these data have shown that especially birth weight, length and head circumference are associated with cognitive abilities, temperament, hostility, trait anxiety, depression and attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms later in life (Raikkonen et al. Reference Raikkonen, Pesonen, Kajantie, Heinonen, Forsen, Phillips, Osmond, Barker and Eriksson2007, Reference Raikkonen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Kajantie, Hovi, Jarvenpaa, Eriksson and Andersson2008; Raikkonen & Pesonen, Reference Raikkonen and Pesonen2009; Lahti et al. Reference Lahti, Raikkonen, Wahlbeck, Heinonen, Forsen, Kajantie, Pesonen, Osmond, Barker and Eriksson2010). Data are, however, lacking on the association of developmental milestones during the first years of life with comprehensive follow-up measures on personality and cognition in adulthood.

Studies investigating early developmental factors in relation to mental outcomes have mainly concentrated on psychopathology, especially schizophrenia and alcohol-related disorders, with focus on early weaning (Sørensen et al. Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Reinisch and Mednick2006), later attainment of motor developmental milestones (Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor, Murray and Poulton2002; Manzardo et al. Reference Manzardo, Penick, Knop, Nickel, Hall, Jensen and Gabrielli2005; Sørensen et al. Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Schiffman, Reinisch, Maeda and Mednick2010b), childhood motor coordination (Schiffman et al. Reference Schiffman, Walker, Ekstrom, Schulsinger, Sorensen and Mednick2004, Reference Schiffman, Sorensen, Maeda, Mortensen, Victoroff, Hayashi, Michelsen, Ekstrom and Mednick2009), physical anomalies (Schiffman et al. Reference Schiffman, Ekstrom, LaBrie, Schulsinger, Sorensen and Mednick2002, Reference Schiffman, Maeda, Hayashi, Michelsen, Sorensen, Ekstrom, Abe, Chronicle and Mednick2006), laterality deviance (Schiffman et al. Reference Schiffman, Pestle, Mednick, Ekstrom, Sorensen and Mednick2005) and general lowering of cognitive ability preceding the disorder (Batty et al. Reference Batty, Mortensen and Osler2005; Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen, Sørensen, Jensen, Reinisch and Mednick2005; Sørensen et al. Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Schiffman, Ekstrøm, Denenney and Mednick2010a). The personality dimension of neuroticism seems to be a general risk factor for psychopathology (Boyce & Parker, Reference Boyce and Parker1985; Krabbendam et al. Reference Krabbendam, Janssen, Bak, Bijl, de Graaf and van Os2002; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Fergusson and Horwood2003; Hettema et al. Reference Hettema, Neale, Myers, Prescott and Kendler2006; Barrantes-Vidal et al. Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Ros-Morente and Kwapil2009), but evidence on associations between early developmental precursors and personality traits is almost non-existent. Since later attainment of milestones has been demonstrated to be associated with psychopathology, and the personality trait of neuroticism has been shown to be a general risk factor for psychopathology (Ormel et al. Reference Ormel, Rosmalen and Farmer2004; Griffith et al. Reference Griffith, Zinbarg, Craske, Mineka, Rose, Waters and Sutton2010), it was hypothesized that early developmental milestones would be associated with this trait. The aim of this study was to investigate whether early motor developmental milestones in the first year of life are associated with adult personality, in particular neuroticism.

Method

Study population

The study objectives were investigated using data from the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort (CPC) and from a follow-up study of this birth cohort: The Prenatal Development Project. The CPC was initially established with data on 8949 mothers and their 9125 consecutive deliveries born at the University Hospital in Copenhagen during the period 1959–1961. Information on demographic, socio-economic, prenatal and postnatal factors were recorded prospectively during pregnancy, at delivery, and at a 1-year examination (Zachau-Christiansen & Ross, Reference Zachau-Christiansen and Ross1975). The mothers were mainly residents in Copenhagen, but some were admitted due to obstetrical complications or because of single-mother status (Villumsen, Reference Villumsen1970). A total of 8400 infants survived the first month after birth. On the basis of pre- and perinatal records, a subsample comprising 1575 members of the CPC was selected for participation in The Prenatal Development Project between 1982 and 1994 (Reinisch et al. Reference Reinisch, Mortensen and Sanders1993, Reference Reinisch, Sanders, Mortensen and Rubin1995). In this subsample, data on personality were available for 1182 individuals, and among these participants information on at least one developmental milestone was available for 968 individuals who comprise the present study sample.

Developmental milestones

Developmental milestones were obtained from the mothers who were instructed to use a standardized diary to record the ages in weeks or months at which the child reached 12 developmental milestones. At a 1-year follow-up examination, the diary was brought to the hospital (Zachau-Christiansen & Ross, Reference Zachau-Christiansen and Ross1975). If the mother did not return the diary, an effort was made to obtain retrospective data on milestones. Table 1 shows the recorded milestones during the first year of life.

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of milestones

Data are given as mean (standard deviation; range).

Adult follow-up

The adult follow-up took place when the participants were at the age of 20.3–34.5 years (s.d. = 4.3). The full test battery included a 2–4 h home visit by a social worker and an 8–11 h psychological evaluation conducted at the Institute of Preventive Medicine (Reinisch et al. Reference Reinisch, Mortensen and Sanders1993). Several personality inventories were administered as part of the follow-up, but only the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) (Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1975) will be analysed in this paper. The EPQ was developed in 1975 (Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1975), and the Danish version comprises 101 binary ‘yes’ or ‘no’ questions from which scores on the personality traits of neuroticism, extraversion and psychoticism are derived. Neuroticism as measured by the Danish EPQ has been shown to correlate 0.76 with neuroticism measured with the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (Hansen & Mortensen, Reference Hansen and Mortensen2004). According to Eysenck, the traits associated with neuroticism are: a tendency to be anxious, depressed, tense, irrational, shy, moody, emotional and to suffer from guilt feelings and low self-esteem (Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1985). At the adult follow-up, the complete Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale was also administered (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1958). Since intelligence may mediate associations between milestone attainment and personality traits, the full-scale intelligence quotient (IQ) was included in a separate statistical model.

Covariates

Of the originally registered variables for the CPC, sex of the child, single-mother status, parity, mother's age, father's age, parental social status and birth weight were included in statistical models to adjust for sex and family background. Parity was included as a binary variable coding whether this was the mother's first pregnancy or not. Parental social status coded on an eight-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher status, was included as a linear continuous variable together with birth weight and parental age (preliminary analyses showed no significant quadratic trend for these variables). There were no missing data on sex of the child and birth weight, while the missing data rate was less than 1% for single-mother status, parity and mother's age. However, missing data were 1.2% for father's age and 9.6% for parental social status. The EM (expectation–maximization) algorithm was used to impute values replacing missing data for the covariates to avoid diluting the sample in regression analyses (Schafer, Reference Schafer1997).

Statistical analyses

The rate of missing data on individual milestones varied from 8.0% to 65.0% depending on the recorded milestone. In addition to the individual milestones, the overall mean of the 12 milestones and the three factors derived from a principal components analysis by Sørensen et al. (Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Schiffman, Reinisch, Maeda and Mednick2010b) were also analysed: the smiling and lifting head factor comprised milestones 1–3 (see Table 1), the rolling, crawling, sitting and grabbing factor milestones 4–8, and the standing and walking factor milestones 9–12 (for details, see Sørensen et al. Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Schiffman, Reinisch, Maeda and Mednick2010b). To derive composite scores on the overall mean and on the three factors, the individual milestones were standardized to a mean of 0 and s.d. of 1, and the composite scores were calculated as the mean of the included individual milestone scores. For the present study these composite scores were restandardized to a mean of 0 and s.d. of 1. If data were missing on one or more milestones included in a composite mean, the mean of the available milestone scores was calculated. Since data were available for at least one milestone for all participants, there were no missing data on the overall mean of the 12 milestones, while the percentages of missing data were 4.0, 8.7 and 3.0 for the factor means respectively. Thus, the sample sizes were 968 for the overall mean and 928, 884 and 939 for the three factors.

Participants with incomplete information on a milestone were excluded from the analysis of this particular milestone. We used χ2 tests and analysis of variance to evaluate associations of potential confounding factors with the mean age of attainment of all milestones and the level of neuroticism (Table 2). Estimates of the associations between age of attainment of milestones and level of neuroticism in adulthood were estimated in several multiple linear regression models (Rothman, Reference Rothman1998). The unadjusted model only included the relevant milestone score, another model also included sex, and the fully adjusted model additionally included single-mother status, parity, mother's age, father's age, parental social status, birth weight and age at follow-up. In separate analyses, adult intelligence was added to the fully adjusted model to evaluate whether intelligence mediated the association between milestone attainment and neuroticism.

Table 2. Association of possible confounding factors with the overall mean of milestone scores and neuroticism

a t Test.

b Exposure variables were divided to achieve approximately 50% in each category.

To evaluate the influence of missing data on the milestone variables, we used the structural equation modelling facilities of Stata 12 (StataCorp LP, USA) to repeat all regression analyses using full information maximum likelihood (FIML; Graham, Reference Graham2009). These analyses use all available information, including information on covariates and neuroticism for participants without information on the milestone variable included in a particular analysis. The results for the fully adjusted FIML model will be presented together with the corresponding model based on the subsample of participants with data on the milestone being analysed (Table 3).

Table 3. Regression coefficients for milestones in models predicting level of neuroticism

FIML, Full information maximum likelihood; IQ, intelligence quotient.

a Sex, single-mother status, parity, mother's age, father's age, parental social status, age at home visit and birth weight.

b Sex, single-mother status, parity, mother's age, father's age, parental social status, age at home visit, birth weight and adult intelligence.

* p ⩽ 0.05, ** p ⩽ 0.01, *** p ⩽ 0.001.

The assumption of normal distribution of residuals of the outcome variable was evaluated graphically and only showed minor deviations, which should not bias estimates or standard errors. The assumption of linearity was evaluated graphically and showed no deviations. Additionally, linearity of regression was analysed by testing the significance of a quadratic component for all milestones. This component was not found significant for any the individual milestones or the composite milestones variables. Finally, analyses were conducted to evaluate whether binary milestone attainment scores showed stronger associations with neuroticism. For these analyses the 85% percentiles were used as cut-offs for deriving the binary milestone score.

Since the sample consisted of an index group of individuals exposed to steroid hormones and psychoactive drugs and matched controls, the interaction of prenatal exposure status and each milestone was tested, but not found significant in supplementary analyses (data not shown).

All statistical analyses were conducted by means of the statistical software package Stata 12 (StataCorp LP, USA).

Results

Table 1 shows the number of observations, mean age of attainment, standard deviation and range for the 12 milestones. Table 2 shows associations between the possible confounding factors and the mean of all milestones and scores on neuroticism. Parity, mother's age, father's age and birth weight were all significantly associated with the overall mean of the 12 milestones. Sex, parental social status and adult intelligence were associated with level of neuroticism in adulthood.

Table 3 presents the results of the main analyses. Both unadjusted and adjusted analyses showed patterns of significant associations between high neuroticism in adulthood and the overall mean of the 12 milestones as well as of two of the factor scores (the rolling, crawling, sitting and grabbing factor and the standing and walking factor). The early development factor (smiling and lifting head) was unrelated to adult neuroticism.

The significant associations for individual milestones reflected the pattern of significant associations for the composite milestones scores. The unadjusted analyses showed significant associations between high neuroticism in adulthood and late attainment of standing with support, crawling short and long distances, and walking with support and without support. Adjusting for all covariates did not change the results substantially. Sitting without support became significant in the adjusted analyses, while standing with support became non-significant. The results of the FIML analyses incorporating all available information were essentially the same as those of the regression analyses based on subsamples with information on the relevant milestone outcome variable. Analyses based on binary milestone scores showed a similar pattern of results as analyses based on continuous milestone scores, but a number of non-significant trends seem to reflect lack of statistical power because of the dichotomization (data not shown).

Analyses including adult IQ as covariate showed the same pattern of significant associations as the fully adjusted model.

Results for extraversion and psychoticism mainly showed insignificant associations with attainment of milestones. Crawling a short distance (β = –0.26, p = 0.022) was significant for extroversion and so was the factor rolling, crawling, sitting and grabbing (β = 0.33, p = 0.025). No significant associations were observed between the psychoticism scale and any of the milestone variables.

Discussion

Main findings

Our analyses reveal associations between delays in early developmental milestone attainment and a higher level of neuroticism in adulthood. Infants who grew up to have high scores on neuroticism tended to sit without support, crawl, as well as walk with and without support later than individuals with low scores on neuroticism. The findings for the individual milestones were reflected in the overall mean milestone score and two composite factor scores. Furthermore, these associations were independent of a wide range of potentially confounding factors, and they were apparently not mediated by associations between milestone attainment and adult intelligence. The personality traits of extraversion and psychoticism were generally not associated with later attainment of motor developmental milestones.

Methodological issues

The main advantage of this study is the prospective design including real-time documentation of milestone attainment by mothers. Nevertheless, the frequency of missing data tended to be high, especially for standing and walking without support. The most likely explanation for this is that these milestones may not have been attained by some children at the time of the 1-year follow-up and therefore were not recorded. Since this source of missing data primarily concerns late-developing individuals, systematic selection bias may have attenuated the observed associations between milestones and neuroticism, with the present results most probably underestimating the associations. A further limitation of the study is that type I error is a possibility, as we conducted tests on 12 milestones in addition to the composite means of milestone scores. However, the fact that only ‘late milestones’ were associated with level of neuroticism seems to point towards a consistent pattern and thus a non-incidental finding. The late milestones had most missing data, but significant results were apparently independent of attrition in milestone data (see Table 3), which strengthens the interpretation of the findings.

As in all observational studies, there may be unrecognized confounding. Sex, single-mother status, parity, age of parents at delivery, parental social status and birth weight were considered confounding factors in the linear regression models. However, there may be other unobserved confounding factors. Factors associated with both age of attainment of motor developmental milestones and level of neuroticism could be genetic factors influencing brain development, in particular development of the cerebellum. Environmental factors such as attachment to the primary caregiver, early neglect, or reduced stimulation in the first months, but also biophysical factors such as nutrition, including breastfeeding, could also influence mother motor and personality development. These factors were not included in the analyses, as appropriate data were not available, which makes the interpretation of the present results complex. Thus, it remains an open question whether late attainment of motor developmental milestones reflects direct influences of early motor development on neuroticism in adulthood, or whether milestones are markers of some underlying factors influencing both motor development and personality. The present study sample was selected based on perinatal records, but statistical tests of interactions did not indicate that prenatal exposure status influenced the associations between milestone attainment and adult neuroticism.

Previous studies

One of the most consistent findings from previous longitudinal studies investigating motor development in relation to psychopathology is that later age when first standing and walking is related to the risk of schizophrenia (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Rodgers, Murray and Marmot1994; Walker et al. Reference Walker, Savoie and Davis1994; Jones & Tarrant, Reference Jones and Tarrant2000; Isohanni et al. Reference Isohanni, Jones, Moilanen, Rantakallio, Veijola, Oja, Koiranen, Jokelainen, Croudace and Jarvelin2001; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor, Murray and Poulton2002). Results based on the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort found that non-psychotic disorders were not related to milestones (Isohanni et al. Reference Isohanni, Jones, Moilanen, Rantakallio, Veijola, Oja, Koiranen, Jokelainen, Croudace and Jarvelin2001, Reference Isohanni, Murray, Jokelainen, Croudace and Jones2004), but the present results suggest that late attainment of these milestones is not necessarily specific to the psychopathology of schizophrenia, but is also associated with the broad personality dimension of neuroticism. Neuroticism has been linked with depression (Boyce & Parker, Reference Boyce and Parker1985), various anxiety disorders (Hettema et al. Reference Hettema, Neale, Myers, Prescott and Kendler2006), schizophrenia (Krabbendam et al. Reference Krabbendam, Janssen, Bak, Bijl, de Graaf and van Os2002; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Fergusson and Horwood2003; Barrantes-Vidal et al. Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Ros-Morente and Kwapil2009) and somatoform disorders (Hankin & Abela, Reference Hankin and Abela2005). Thus, neuroticism seems to be an indicator of risk of psychopathology in general, and the present findings may be of interest for a broad spectrum of psychiatric disorders. Similarly, developmental milestones seem to be general risk factors of psychopathology. Thus, Sørensen et al. (Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Schiffman, Reinisch, Maeda and Mednick2010b) found that the association with delayed attainment of developmental milestones was not specific to schizophrenia, since other psychiatric disorders were also associated with late motor development. These results are supported by other studies finding that late attainment of motor developmental milestones is associated with both low cognitive functioning (Taanila et al. Reference Taanila, Murray, Jokelainen, Isohanni and Rantakallio2005) and alcohol dependence (Manzardo et al. Reference Manzardo, Penick, Knop, Nickel, Hall, Jensen and Gabrielli2005). The current findings suggest that delayed milestones may be an early key phenotype associated with both personality development and adult psychopathology or that personality development, in particular development of neuroticism, may mediate the link between milestones and psychopathology. Thus, the present findings support a neurodevelopmental model of psychopathology.

No other studies have found motor developmental milestones to be associated with personality traits or found factors measurable during the first year of life to be associated with adult neuroticism, but other indicators of early development have been associated with personality development. Thus, the Helsinki Birth Cohort found birth weight, length and head circumference to be associated with cognitive abilities, temperament, hostility, trait anxiety, depression and ADHD symptoms later in life (Raikkonen et al. Reference Raikkonen, Pesonen, Kajantie, Heinonen, Forsen, Phillips, Osmond, Barker and Eriksson2007, Reference Raikkonen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Kajantie, Hovi, Jarvenpaa, Eriksson and Andersson2008; Raikkonen & Pesonen, Reference Raikkonen and Pesonen2009; Lahti et al. Reference Lahti, Raikkonen, Wahlbeck, Heinonen, Forsen, Kajantie, Pesonen, Osmond, Barker and Eriksson2010).

In the present study, delays in ‘very early’ developmental milestones such as smiling, and lifting and holding the head were not associated with level of neuroticism in adulthood. Except for standing, it was the ‘later’ milestones such as sitting without support, crawling and walking which were significant in the adjusted analyses. These milestones all require complex motor coordination (perhaps more than standing) and only these late milestones reflect development during most of the first year. This may explain the observed significant associations, but the present results may also reflect environmental factors influencing both milestone attainment and personality development. Late milestones to a greater extent than smiling and lifting the head depend on the interaction between the child and the parents (Broberg et al. Reference Broberg, Mothander, Granqvist and Ivarsson2010), as demands for ‘good parenting’ and stabile attachment gradually increase from early infancy (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1988; Rochat & Striano, Reference Rochat and Striano1999; Rochat, Reference Rochat2009; Broberg et al. Reference Broberg, Mothander, Granqvist and Ivarsson2010) parallel to the infant's motor and self development (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1988; Rochat & Striano, Reference Rochat and Striano1999; Rochat, Reference Rochat2009). In early infancy the cues from the child represent needs that are very basic and require less advanced parental judgment and feedback with regard to the infant's mobility and perception of self (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1988; Rochat, Reference Rochat2009; Broberg et al. Reference Broberg, Mothander, Granqvist and Ivarsson2010). The more important socially dependent motor and language development has its onset from 7 months and onwards (Rochat & Striano, Reference Rochat and Striano1999; Rochat, Reference Rochat2009; Broberg et al. Reference Broberg, Mothander, Granqvist and Ivarsson2010). Thus, it is plausible that attachment and the parent–child dynamics are more central to ‘later’ and more motor-advanced neurodevelopment. In accordance with these and the present results, attachment to the mother at 15 months has been demonstrated to be associated with level of neuroticism at 8–9 years of age (Hagekull & Bohlin, Reference Hagekull and Bohlin2003).

The temperament of the child may be another factor contributing to the observed association between developmental delay and higher risk of neuroticism (Molfese & Molfese, Reference Molfese and Molfese2000). Early attainment of milestones might be related to temperamental traits such as high activity level and lack of inhibition, and late attainment might be related to low activity and inhibition. While a recent study was unable to demonstrate associations between motor competences and measures of temperament (DeSantis et al. Reference DeSantis, Harkins, Tronick, Kaplan and Beeghly2011), another found associations between motor regulation and self-regulation (DeGangi et al. Reference DeGangi, Breinbauer, Roosevelt, Porges and Greenspan2000). In addition it has been found that lower motor performance at 3 and 30 days was associated with negative affectivity at 4 months (Canals et al. Reference Canals, Hernandez-Martinez and Fernandez-Ballart2011), suggesting a possible causal pathway from late motor development to adult neuroticism (Watson & Clark, Reference Watson and Clark1984).

Conclusion

The results suggest that the personality trait of neuroticism may be associated with delayed attainment of developmental milestones requiring complex motor coordination and developing during the second half of the first year of life such as sitting without support, crawling and walking. As this is the first study to investigate motor developmental milestones in relation to adult personality traits, more research should be conducted in the area. In particular, with neuroticism being an indicator of risk of psychopathology in general, we emphasize the importance of investigating delayed milestone attainment as an early precursor of adult neuroticism. Future research should obtain reliable measures of specific milestones and early temperament and should investigate a broader range of personality traits and include measures of psychopathology.

Acknowledgements

The present study was supported by a grant to T.F-M. from the IMK Almene Fond. In addition, the study was supported by grants no. HD-17655 and no. HD-20263 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to June Reinisch, grant no. DA-05056 from the National Institute of Drug Abuse to June Reinisch, grant no. 9700093 to E.L.M. from the Danish Research Council and grant no. 1400/2-4-1997 to E.L.M. from the Danish National Board of Health.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Barrantes-Vidal, N, Ros-Morente, A, Kwapil, TR (2009). An examination of neuroticism as a moderating factor in the association of positive and negative schizotypy with psychopathology in a nonclinical sample. Schizophrenia Research 115, 303309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batty, GD, Mortensen, EL, Osler, M (2005). Childhood IQ in relation to later psychiatric disorder – evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 180181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J (1988). A Secure Base. Routledge: London.Google Scholar
Boyce, P, Parker, G (1985). Neuroticism as a predictor of outcome in depression. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 173, 685688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broberg, A, Mothander, PR, Granqvist, P, Ivarsson, T (2010). Tilknytning i praksis – Tilknytningsteoriens anvendelse i forskning og klinisk arbejde (Attachment in practice – Attachment theory use in research and clinical work). Hans Reitzels Forlag: Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Canals, J, Hernandez-Martinez, C, Fernandez-Ballart, JD (2011). Relationships between early behavioural characteristics and temperament at 6 years. Infant Behavior and Development 34, 152160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, M, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Harrington, HL, Taylor, A, Murray, M, Poulton, R (2002). Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 449456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeGangi, GA, Breinbauer, C, Roosevelt, JD, Porges, S, Greenspan, S (2000). Prediction of childhood problems at three years in children experiencing disorders of regulation during infancy. Infant Mental Health Journal 21, 156175.3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeSantis, A, Harkins, D, Tronick, E, Kaplan, E, Beeghly, M (2011). Exploring an integrative model of infant behavior: What is the relationship among temperament, sensory processing, and neurobehavioral measures? Infant Behavior and Development 34, 280292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, HJ, Eysenck, MW (1985). Personality and Individual Differences: A Natural Science Approach. Plenum Press: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, HJ, Eysenck, SBG (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Hodder & Stoughton Educational: Sevenoaks.Google Scholar
Goodwin, RD, Fergusson, DM, Horwood, LJ (2003). Neuroticism in adolescence and psychotic symptoms in adulthood. Psychological Medicine 33, 10891097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, JW (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology 60, 549576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, JW, Zinbarg, RE, Craske, MG, Mineka, S, Rose, RD, Waters, AM, Sutton, JM (2010). Neuroticism as a common dimension in the internalizing disorders. Psychological Medicine 40, 11251136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagekull, B, Bohlin, G (2003). Early temperament and attachment as predictors of the Five Factor Model of personality. Attachment and Human Development 5, 218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, BL, Abela, JRZ (2005). Development of Psychopathology: A Vulnerability-Stress Perspective. Sage Publications: London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, HS, Mortensen, EL (2004). Dokumentation for den danske udgave af NEO-PI-R Kort version [Documentation of the Danish version of the NEO-PI-R Short version]. Psykologisk Forlag A/S: Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Hettema, JM, Neale, MC, Myers, JM, Prescott, CA, Kendler, KS (2006). A population-based twin study of the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 857864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isohanni, M, Jones, PB, Moilanen, K, Rantakallio, P, Veijola, J, Oja, H, Koiranen, M, Jokelainen, J, Croudace, T, Jarvelin, M (2001). Early developmental milestones in adult schizophrenia and other psychoses. A 31-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Schizophrenia Research 52, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isohanni, M, Murray, GK, Jokelainen, J, Croudace, T, Jones, PB (2004). The persistence of developmental markers in childhood and adolescence and risk for schizophrenic psychoses in adult life. A 34-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort. Schizophrenia Research 71, 213225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P, Rodgers, B, Murray, R, Marmot, M (1994). Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344, 13981402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, PB, Tarrant, CJ (2000). Developmental precursors and biological markers for schizophrenia and affective disorders: specificity and public health implications. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 250, 286291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krabbendam, L, Janssen, I, Bak, M, Bijl, RV, de Graaf, R, van Os, J (2002). Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for psychosis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 37, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahti, M, Raikkonen, K, Wahlbeck, K, Heinonen, K, Forsen, T, Kajantie, E, Pesonen, AK, Osmond, C, Barker, DJ, Eriksson, JG (2010). Prenatal origins of hospitalization for personality disorders: the Helsinki birth cohort study. Psychiatry Research 179, 226230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manzardo, AM, Penick, EC, Knop, J, Nickel, EJ, Hall, S, Jensen, P, Gabrielli, WF (2005). Developmental differences in childhood motor coordination predict adult alcohol dependence: proposed role for the cerebellum in alcoholism. Alcoholism – Clinical and Experimental Research 29, 353357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molfese, VJ, Molfese, DL (editors) (2000). Temperament and Personality Across the Life Span. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mortensen, EL, Sørensen, HJ, Jensen, HH, Reinisch, JM, Mednick, SA (2005). IQ and mental disorder in young men. British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 407415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ormel, J, Rosmalen, J, Farmer, A (2004). Neuroticism: a non-informative marker of vulnerability to psychopathology. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 39, 906912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raikkonen, K, Pesonen, AK (2009). Early life origins of psychological development and mental health. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 50, 583591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raikkonen, K, Pesonen, AK, Heinonen, K, Kajantie, E, Hovi, P, Jarvenpaa, AL, Eriksson, JG, Andersson, S (2008). Depression in young adults with very low birth weight: the Helsinki study of very low-birth-weight adults. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 290296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raikkonen, K, Pesonen, AK, Kajantie, E, Heinonen, K, Forsen, T, Phillips, DI, Osmond, C, Barker, DJ, Eriksson, JG (2007). Length of gestation and depressive symptoms at age 60 years. British Journal of Psychiatry 190, 469474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinisch, JM, Mortensen, EL, Sanders, SA (1993). The Prenatal Development Project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 87, 5461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinisch, JM, Sanders, SA, Mortensen, EL, Rubin, DB (1995). In utero exposure to phenobarbital and intelligence deficits in adult men. JAMA 274, 15181525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochat, P (2009). Others in Mind. Cambridge University Press: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P, Striano, T (1999). Emerging self-exploration by 2-month-old infants. Developmental Science 2, 206218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, KJ (1998). Modern Epidemiology. Lippincott-Raven Publishers: Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Schafer, JL (1997). Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data. Chapman and Hall: London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffman, J, Ekstrom, M, LaBrie, J, Schulsinger, F, Sorensen, H, Mednick, S (2002). Minor physical anomalies and schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 238243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiffman, J, Maeda, JA, Hayashi, K, Michelsen, N, Sorensen, HJ, Ekstrom, M, Abe, KA, Chronicle, EP, Mednick, SA (2006). Premorbid childhood ocular alignment abnormalities and adult schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. Schizophrenia Research 81, 253260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiffman, J, Pestle, S, Mednick, S, Ekstrom, M, Sorensen, H, Mednick, S (2005). Childhood laterality and adult schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective investigation. Schizophrenia Research 72, 151160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiffman, J, Sorensen, HJ, Maeda, J, Mortensen, EL, Victoroff, J, Hayashi, K, Michelsen, NM, Ekstrom, M, Mednick, S (2009). Childhood motor coordination and adult schizophrenia spectrum disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 10411047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiffman, J, Walker, E, Ekstrom, M, Schulsinger, F, Sorensen, H, Mednick, S (2004). Childhood videotaped social and neuromotor precursors of schizophrenia: a prospective investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 20212027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, HJ, Mortensen, EL, Reinisch, JM, Mednick, SA (2006). Early weaning and hospitalization with alcohol-related diagnoses in adult life. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 704709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, HJ, Mortensen, EL, Schiffman, J, Ekstrøm, M, Denenney, D, Mednick, SA (2010 a). Premorbid IQ and adult schizophrenia spectrum disorder: Verbal Performance subtests. Psychiatry Research 178, 2326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, HJ, Mortensen, EL, Schiffman, J, Reinisch, JM, Maeda, J, Mednick, SA (2010 b). Early developmental milestones and risk of schizophrenia: a 45-year follow-up of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort. Schizophrenia Research 118, 4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taanila, A, Murray, GK, Jokelainen, J, Isohanni, M, Rantakallio, P (2005). Infant developmental milestones: a 31-year follow-up. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 47, 581586.Google ScholarPubMed
Villumsen, AL (1970). Environmental Factors in Congenital Malformations: A Prospective Study of 9006 Human Pregnancies. FADL's Forlag: Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Walker, EF, Savoie, T, Davis, D (1994). Neuromotor precursors of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 20, 441451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D, Clark, LA (1984). Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin 96, 465490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1958). The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. Williams and Wilkins: Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Zachau-Christiansen, B, Ross, EM (1975). Babies: Human Development During the First Year. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of milestones

Figure 1

Table 2. Association of possible confounding factors with the overall mean of milestone scores and neuroticism

Figure 2

Table 3. Regression coefficients for milestones in models predicting level of neuroticism