Introduction
Physical (e.g. pain) and psychological (e.g. depression, anxiety) health are closely associated with one another: people in worse physical health often report greater levels of psychological distress, and people reporting poorer mental health tend to report poorer physical health (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Clauw and Keefe2003). Research suggests that pain is associated with a more rapid decline in cognitive function in older adults (Berryman et al., Reference Berryman, Stanton, Bowering, Tabor, McFarlane and Moseley2013), while a separate literature argues that greater cognitive function earlier on in life is related to better physical and mental health in senescence (Gale et al., Reference Gale, Deary, Cooper and Batty2012b). This study uses data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) to untangle the relationship between these three variables by using a cross-lagged panel model to look at the prospective effects of these factors on each other in a sample of respondents diagnosed with arthritis at the second wave of the ELSA. Arthritis is a condition commonly associated with chronic pain, and potentially with cognitive decline (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Wang, Chou, Liao, Liou and Lin2015). We modelled the relationship between these variables at the wave before diagnosis, the wave of diagnosis, and the wave after diagnosis with arthritis.
Disordered mood and pain
The biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain holds that pain is not simply caused by damage to the body, but due to a range of cognitive and affective individual differences alongside the wider social context people live in (Gatchel et al., Reference Gatchel, Peng, Peters, Fuchs and Turk2007; Quartana et al., Reference Quartana, Campbell and Edwards2009). It has been established that pain is moderated by a constellation of individual differences that focus around negative affect and mood, such as psychological distress (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Lewis, Papageorgiou, Thomas, Jayson, Macfarlane and Silman2001; Hurwitz et al., Reference Hurwitz, Morgenstern and Yu2003), depression (Geerlings et al., Reference Geerlings, Twisk, Beekman, Deeg and van Tilburg2002; Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Wu, Bair, Krebs, Damush and Tu2011), anxiety (McWilliams et al., Reference McWilliams, Goodwin and Cox2004; Castillo et al., Reference Castillo, Wegener, Heins, Haythornthwaite, MacKenzie and Bosse2013), catastrophizing (Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Brown, Wallston and Caldwell1989; Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Cahalan, Mensing, Smith and Haythornthwaite2011) and neuroticism (Costa, Reference Costa1987), which all lead to greater subjective pain. However, the interplay between these factors has only been partially explored, especially when considering how pain may change over the course of a chronic disease.
Cognitive function, pain and depression
While the relationship between pain, negative affect and cognition is well established, both cross-sectionally (Lépine and Briley, Reference Lépine and Briley2004; McWilliams et al., Reference McWilliams, Goodwin and Cox2004; Stubbs et al., Reference Stubbs, Vancampfort, Veronese, Thompson, Fornaro, Schofield and Koyanagi2017) and longitudinally (Geerlings et al., Reference Geerlings, Twisk, Beekman, Deeg and van Tilburg2002; Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Wu, Bair, Krebs, Damush and Tu2011; Gerrits et al., Reference Gerrits, van Marwijk, van Oppen, van der Horst and Penninx2015), less is known about their dynamic inter-play. Amongst older adults, it has been observed that the experience of pain appears to be associated with a reduction in cognitive function, which is thought to be because performance on cognitive tasks is impeded due to resources instead being used to respond to the experience of pain (Moriarty et al., Reference Moriarty, McGuire and Finn2011). Indeed, there is evidence that pain appears to interfere with executive functions such as working memory (Berryman et al., Reference Berryman, Stanton, Bowering, Tabor, McFarlane and Moseley2013). At the same time, other evidence suggests that cognitive function acts as a protective factor against the emergence of disease and symptoms of disease such as pain, particularly chronic widespread pain (Gale et al., Reference Gale, Deary, Cooper and Batty2012b). This latter research has identified the importance of cognitive function earlier on in life upon the development of diseases across the lifespan (Deary et al., Reference Deary, Weiss and Batty2010).
Previous studies using data from the ELSA have found that pain does not cause cognitive decline (Veronese et al., Reference Veronese, Koyanagi, Solmi, Thompson, Maggi, Schofield and Stubbs2018). However, while this study controlled for comorbidities [including cancer, heart disease and arthritis (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Wang, Chou, Liao, Liou and Lin2015)], simply grouping people together with a highly prevalent disease like arthritis (affecting around 40% of the ELSA cohort), is a concern as the length of time they have had arthritis for varies considerably, from a few months to several decades and as the disease progresses, the differential effects of cognition, affect and pain may become too comorbid to differentiate [e.g. Hawker et al. (Reference Hawker, Gignac, Badley, Davis, French, Li and Lou2011); Huang et al. (Reference Huang, Wang, Chou, Liao, Liou and Lin2015)]. As such, it is necessary to study the dynamics of pain, affect and cognitive function across the early course of disease. Therefore, this study examines how cognition and affect assessed prior to disease diagnosis affects subsequent pain, and how this subsequent pain influences cognition and affect. Thus, as a disease becomes established we can explore the early inter-play of cognition, affect and pain at the onset when their impact is likely to be more apparent and clearly differentiated (Gerrits et al., Reference Gerrits, van Marwijk, van Oppen, van der Horst and Penninx2015).
Similar to pain, there is a literature that has found that greater depression severity is associated with poorer cognitive function (McDermott and Ebmeier, Reference McDermott and Ebmeier2009), including among older adults. It has been noted that the majority of this literature has looked at the effect of depression and poor mood on cognitive function, rather than the effect of cognitive function on poor mood (Gale et al., Reference Gale, Allerhand and Deary2012a). Gale et al. (Reference Gale, Allerhand and Deary2012a) looked at the dynamics of the relationship between depression and cognitive function using data from the ELSA, finding that although depression and cognitive function were associated with each other in older adults under the age of 80, there was limited evidence either was related to the rate of change in the other. Further, there is emerging evidence suggesting an association between depression and inflammation (Maier and Watkins, Reference Maier and Watkins1998). These studies report that increased levels of cytokines are associated with increased levels of depression (Valkanova et al., Reference Valkanova, Ebmeier and Allan2013). There is also reason to hypothesise an overlap with pain, as inflammation is linked to both pain (de Goeij et al., Reference de Goeij, van Eijk, Vanelderen, Wilder-Smith, Vissers, van der Hoeven and Pickkers2013) and cognitive function, where in the latter case inflammation appears to be a marker of cognitive decline (Yaffe et al., Reference Yaffe, Kanaya, Lindquist, Simonsick, Harris, Shorr and Newman2004).
Arthritis, pain and psychological distress
Arthritis is a cause of chronic pain in older adults. It is estimated that around 15% of the adult population have osteoarthritis (Neogi, Reference Neogi2013; Johnson and Hunter, Reference Johnson and Hunter2014), and a further 1% has rheumatoid arthritis (Alamanos and Drosos, Reference Alamanos and Drosos2005), both of which become more common with advancing age. Osteoarthritis is one of the commonest causes of working age disability and a source of distress for many people who suffer from it (O'Reilly et al., Reference O'Reilly, Muir and Doherty1998). Physically, arthritis typically involves stiffness, inflammation and soreness of joints in the body, most commonly in the hip or knee, which is associated with chronic pain and disability. While arthritis is thought to be an important cause of distress, there is also evidence from other studies using the ELSA (Chou, Reference Chou2007), that there is a reciprocal relationship between pain and distress; pain is predictive of future distress and vice versa. A small number of studies have looked at the relationship between individual differences and pain in regard to arthritis. Hawker et al. (Reference Hawker, Gignac, Badley, Davis, French, Li and Lou2011) found in an arthritis cohort that the experience of pain predicted future reports of depression and disordered mood.
Our approach has a number of advantages over the previous literature on the longitudinal relationship between pain in arthritis patients and depression. We examine the early onset of the disease rather than grouping arthritis patients together, which weakly controls for disease onset (Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Lefebvre, Egert, Affleck, Sullivan and Caldwell2000; Sturgeon and Zautra, Reference Sturgeon and Zautra2013). While these studies identify associations between affect and pain, there is a clinical need to further understand for purposes of early treatment and management which to prioritise. It is also the case that studies often look at how pain predicts psychological distress, or vice versa, without controlling for the outcome variables at baseline. Because studies of ageing have respondents that report incidence of arthritis at different measurement points, it is possible to model the relationship between relatively recently emerging pain and psychological distress.
Consequently, this study further aims to tease out the relationship between pain, affect and cognitive decline. While one study has found that arthritis is related to cognitive decline (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Wang, Chou, Liao, Liou and Lin2015), this finding has been disputed in longitudinal ageing studies (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Barbour, Helmick, Zack and Al Snih2017). Therefore, we longitudinally model the relationship between pain and cognitive function in arthritis, to further understand whether the pain is the driving factor in cognitive decline among people with arthritis. This analysis utilises a subsample of respondents to the ELSA that participated in wave 1 of the ELSA and reported a diagnosis of arthritis at wave 2.
Method
Sample
Data were taken from a subsample of 516 respondents who participated in the ELSA (Marmot et al., Reference Marmot, Oldfield, Clemens, Blake, Phelps, Nazroo, Steptoe, Rogers, Banks and Oskala2016). At present the ELSA consists of eight waves of data, beginning in 2002 and separated by approximately 2 years. Respondents were assigned to the subsample depending on whether they had participated in wave 1 of the ELSA (n = 12 099), reported a diagnosis of arthritis between waves 1 (2002–2003) and waves 2 (2004–2005) (n = 596), did not report a diagnosis of arthritis at wave 1 (n = 540), and further reported their arthritis diagnosis did not fall in the five years running up to the beginning of the ELSA study (n = 519). Of those 519, three had missing data on all of the pain, depression and cognitive function measurements and were removed from the analysis, leaving 516 respondents (see Fig. 1 for details), of who 420 participated in wave 3 as well; missing data were accounted for using a full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation.
Of the 519 eligible for the subsample, 470 reported being diagnosed in the period 2002–2005 (i.e. since their previous ELSA interview), or whom 71 were diagnosed in 2002, 191 in 2003, 194 in 2004 and 14 in 2005. Twenty-eight individuals either refused to answer this question or did not know when they had been diagnosed. Ethical review for the data collection was obtained from the NHS Research Ethics Committees service. The anonymised data were made publicly available by NatCen to download from the UK Data Archive.
Measures
Pain was assessed using two questions that were combined. The first asked (yes/no) whether the respondent has been often troubled by pain. For respondents who affirmed this was the case, they were then asked to rate how bad the pain was (either mild, moderate or severe). These questions were asked as part of the main ELSA interview at each of the eight waves. This was combined into a score from 0 (not troubled by pain) to 3 (troubled by severe pain), representing whether they were troubled by pain, and how severe it was, at each wave. These options, as verbal rating scales, have been used widely in the pain literature (Stubhaug et al., Reference Stubhaug, Breivik, Kvarstein, Romundstad, Rosseland, Allen and Borchgrevink2008). These verbal rating scales are known to be valid indicators of pain, performing extremely similarly to other, more elaborate pain measurements (e.g. visual analogue scales or numeric rating scales which are continuous or with a greater number of ordinal responses), and are responsive to the introduction of pain, such as through a cold pressor paradigm (Ferreira-Valente et al., Reference Ferreira-Valente, Pais-Ribeiro and Jensen2011).
Depression was measured using a dichotomous eight-item variation of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977), which has been used widely to assess depression. This variant of the CES-D was administered to respondents at each wave as part of the main ELSA interview.
Cognitive function was assessed using principal component scores from five tasks in the main ELSA interview. At the start of the cognitive function module, participants were instructed that they would be given a clipboard and a pencil later in the module, and when they were presented with them they should write their initials in the top left-hand corner of the paper attached to the clipboard (prospective memory). Performance on this test was scored from 0 to 5 (5 = completed task correctly without prompting, 4 = partially completed the task (either wrote initials elsewhere or something in top left corner) without prompting, 3 = did something else, or declared they did not remember what to do without prompting, 2 = completed task after prompting, 1 = partially completed task after prompting, 0 = did nothing or failed to remember after prompting). Participants were randomly allocated to receive one of four lists of 10 words, presented to them verbally by the interviewer. They were then asked to immediately recall as many of them as possible (immediate recall) and were asked to recall again the list of words again at the end of the cognitive function module (delayed recall). They were also asked to list as many animals as they could within 1 min (fluency), and complete a letter cancellation task to index attention. These five measures were then entered into a principal component analysis, extracting a single factor which all items loaded strongly onto (parallel analyses indicated a single component model was also the best fit of the data). From this, factor scores were used as a measure of cognitive function. This was calculated at each wave, as the cognitive function module included each of these tasks at waves 1, 2 and 3 (online Supplementary Table S1).
Modelling
A cross-lagged panel model was estimated using the pain, depression and cognitive function measurements at waves 1, 2 and 3. It has been previously noted that the use of cross-lagged models with two time points is problematic (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). To overcome this, we used three time points, and modelled the within-participant variance in each measure using a simplex. At each wave, the covariance between the three pain measures was modelled. A maximal model was used, with pain, depression and cognitive function each predicting all three at the following wave. A simplex was modelled to account for the autoregressive relationship between each variable and measurements of it at earlier waves (i.e. wave 3 pain was regressed on both wave 2 and wave 1 pain, as opposed to just wave 2 pain as is common in many cross-lagged panel models). The model was adjusted for age (year of birth, z-scored) and sex (0 = male, 1 = female), as both are known to be associated with the variables in the model (e.g. sex and depression, age and cognitive function). The path model included the mean structure.
Model fit was assessed using the χ2 test of model fit, the Comparative Fit Index, the Tucker Lewis Index, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and the Standardised Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). Decisions on the cut-off for acceptable fit were determined using the guidelines suggested by Hu and Bentler (Reference Hu and Bentler1999), who advise the use of combinatorial rules to reduce the likelihood of accepting a poor fitting model.
Modelling was conducted in MPlus v.7.4 (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2015) with a maximum likelihood estimation. Missing data were handled using the FIML, as the ELSA data are assumed to be missing at random and the level of dropout in this subsample is small (19.8%). Sensitivity analyses conducted using a listwise deletion, to test if the missing data approach biased the findings in any way, did not find substantial differences between the different approaches (see online Supplementary Materials).
Results
Table 1 reports the descriptive statistics for the sample. Online Supplementary Table S2 reports the bivariate correlations between the variables in the model. All of the fit indices suggested the model was an adequate fit (RMSEA = 0.090, 95% CI 0.073–0.109, SRMR = 0.038, CFI = 0.950, TLI = 0.850). Some of the indices (SRMR < 0.04, CFI > 0.95) identified the model was a good fit, others did not (TLI < 0.95), and others suggested the model the model was adequate (RMSEA > 0.06) (Table 2). Using the combinatorial rules that have been previously suggested for CFI and SRMR (Hu and Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999), we chose this model as suitable.
As most ELSA wave 2 interviews were completed in 2004, this would give the sample an average age of 66–67. This compares closely with the average age in the ELSA at wave 1 (64.19). Regular drinking is defined as reporting they drank on a more frequent basis than ‘never’ or ‘on special occasions’.
The model (Fig. 2, Table 3) shows that pain at wave 2 is predicted by prior pain, depression and cognitive function. Higher levels of depression and pain were associated with greater pain at the wave where respondents reported begin diagnosed with arthritis, and higher cognitive function was associated with less pain, providing support for the idea that cognitive function is a protective factor. Depression at wave 2 was also predicted by pain at wave 1, with greater pain being associated with higher depression scores. Moreover, cognitive function at wave 3 was also predicted by pain and depression, with greater depression and pain being associated with lower subsequent cognitive function. This provides support for the cognitive resources account of cognitive function. We further tested whether treatment engagement mediated the relationship between cognitive function and pain, finding it did not (online Supplementary Table S3).
C.F., cognitive function; W, wave. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001
At wave 1, cognitive function was significantly associated with pain and depression, and pain and depression were significantly associated with each other at all three waves (online Supplementary Table S4). Looking at the indirect relationships between the different measurements of the same constructs (Table 4) showed strong indirect relationships across the three waves (i.e. wave 1 pain -> wave 2 pain -> wave 3 pain). There was also evidence of pain and depression at wave 1 predicting depression and pain at waves 2 and 3, further confirming the bidirectional association between pain and depression. There was also evidence that depression at waves 1 and 2 predict subsequent measures of cognitive function. There was also an indirect relationship between cognitive function at wave 1, pain at wave 2 and pain at wave 3, further supporting that higher cognitive function was protective of future pain.
W, wave; D, depression; P, pain; CF, cognitive function. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001
Discussion
The findings reported in this study demonstrates how cognitive function acts as a protective factor against the experience of pain (including an indirect effect via wave 2 pain) when arthritis emerges, but is impaired by pain when arthritis worsens over time. Thus, both the protective function and resource depletion accounts of cognitive ability are supported when considering the dynamic change of the experience of pain. At least in the early stages of arthritis, cognitive ability is associated with greater future well-being, insofar as it is protective against the emergence of pain. At the same time, pain at the point of arthritis diagnosis was predictive of a decline in subsequent cognitive function.
Greater levels of pain and depression were associated with poorer cognitive function at the wave following arthritis diagnosis. It is not clear whether this is driven by disruption to performance on cognitive tests, or cognitive decline (Sofi et al., Reference Sofi, Valecchi, Bacci, Abbate, Gensini, Casini and Macchi2011). The influence pain has on attention (Eccleston and Crombez, Reference Eccleston and Crombez1999), and depression, means that it is liable to reduce performance on cognitive function tests. There are also other factors that might mediate the relationship between pain and reduced cognitive function. For example, increased sleeping difficulties (also a symptom of depression), are often cited as a consequence of pain, and thus may also be a contributing factor to worsening cognitive difficulties (McCracken and Iverson, Reference McCracken and Iverson2001; Roach et al., Reference Roach, Solberg Nes and Segerstrom2009). The relationship between pain and depression might similarly be mediated. Previous research looking at the impact of pain trajectories over time shows how these impact on engagement in wider social and societal engagement, with those in worsening pain less likely to engage in these activities (James et al., Reference James, Walsh and Ferguson2018). Such a lack of social engagement is linked to social isolation and loneliness and thus potentially depression (Nicholson, Reference Nicholson2012; Courtin and Knapp, Reference Courtin and Knapp2017). Moreover, pain is a frequently cited cause of difficulties in activities of daily living (Verbrugge and Juarez, Reference Verbrugge and Juarez2006), and limitations to such activities, which may not recede when pain improves (James et al., Reference James, Walsh and Ferguson2019). This may in turn reduce social contact (Drageset, Reference Drageset2004), as well as individual levels of self-efficacy (Salkeld et al., Reference Salkeld, Cumming, Thomas and Szonyi2000) and perceived control (Bowling et al., Reference Bowling, Seetai, Morris and Ebrahim2007), which both increase the risk of depression (Holahan and Holahan, Reference Holahan and Holahan1987). While these identify that pain is the overriding cause of these outcomes, it may also be the case that inflammation is a mediating factor between pain and depression as well (see below).
Similar to the previous literature, we found that depression and cognitive function were associated with each other. Depression at waves 1 and 2 was predictive of subsequent cognitive function; in both cases, greater levels of depression were associated with poorer subsequent cognitive function (McDermott and Ebmeier, Reference McDermott and Ebmeier2009). There is increasing evidence to support the role of inflammation in depression, with meta-analyses indicating depressed people have higher levels of inflammatory cytokines (i.e. IL-1, IL-6, TNF-α and CRP) (Dowlati et al., Reference Dowlati, Herrmann, Swardfager, Liu, Sham, Reim and Lanctôt2010; Valkanova et al., Reference Valkanova, Ebmeier and Allan2013). Furthermore, some of these markers, alongside being associated with depression, are also associated with cognitive decline (Yaffe et al., Reference Yaffe, Kanaya, Lindquist, Simonsick, Harris, Shorr and Newman2004; Leonard, Reference Leonard2007). In addition to being an important consideration for arthritis generally, through the potential role of depression on cognitive decline, this is also of particular relevance for rheumatoid arthritis, which is characterised by chronic inflammation (McInnes and Schett, Reference McInnes and Schett2011).
In the run up to a diagnosis of arthritis, both prior pain and depression are strongly related to the future experience of pain and depression, and each other, as wave 1 pain and depression were positive associated with wave 2 depression and pain. This suggests there is a positive feedback loop between pain and disordered mood; people who are troubled by pain become more depressed, and feel more pain. This finding is similar to other studies that have shown how constructs such as catastrophizing are related to the experience of pain (Goubert et al., Reference Goubert, Crombez and Van Damme2004; Sturgeon and Zautra, Reference Sturgeon and Zautra2013). Where this study goes further is to show how both of these impacts subsequent performance on tests of cognitive function. As cognitive function is used in a range of activities of daily living vital to independence in old age, this shows how subjective perceptions of pain and affect subsequently affect processes that underpin activities that constitute independence and self-care. Therefore, intervening upon these early in the disease course has a clear clinical utility.
There are a number of limitations to this analysis. There was some attrition in the study, although we conducted sensitivity analyses using all cases with full data to test whether treating the data as missing at random (as we did using a FIML approach) was appropriate. Although dropout was low, restricting the model to cases with full data showed minimal differences with the model used. This is a problem general longitudinal surveys in the first few waves (Banks et al., Reference Banks, Muriel and Smith2011), and is especially pertinent to an ageing study where one might expect additional dropout due to infirmity and mortality. While this study shows how cognitive function is impaired by pain and depression, further work ought to be conducted to determine whether this affects all areas of cognitive function equally. This has an applied purpose as some aspects of daily living, especially instrumental activities of daily living such as remembering to take medications, shopping for groceries, or managing money will rely on certain aspects of cognitive function more than others. The pain measure in this study is of generalised rather than arthritis specific pain, and respondents may well have other conditions causing them pain. However, studies that have looked at longitudinal trajectories of pain using these measures of pain show that people with arthritis and those with cancer, in the ELSA cohort, show different pain trajectories (James et al., Reference James, Walsh and Ferguson2018). Thus, the pattern of pain experience reported using these general pain questions does seem to be disease specific. Therefore, while other factors may be contributing to the respondents' reporting of pain, they are mostly likely reporting arthritic focused pain. The ELSA did not have information about arthritis severity; although the sample was controlled on disease duration, respondents may have differed on the extent to which they were affected (i.e. level of OA pathology, inflammation and flares).
These analyses indicate there is a clear clinical utility to intervening upon pain and especially depression early after arthritis diagnosis, as this has the potential to limit the quality of life for older adults with arthritis.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719001673
Financial support
This work was supported by Versus Arthritis (grant number 20777), formerly Arthritis Research UK, and the National Institute of Health Research via the NIHR Nottingham Biomedical Research Centre.