Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:50:07.960Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Directed threat imagery in generalized anxiety disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2017

C. Buff*
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52, 48149 Muenster, Germany
C. Schmidt
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52, 48149 Muenster, Germany
L. Brinkmann
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52, 48149 Muenster, Germany
B. Gathmann
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52, 48149 Muenster, Germany
S. Tupak
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52, 48149 Muenster, Germany
T. Straube
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52, 48149 Muenster, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: C. Buff, MSc, Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Von-Esmarch-Str. 52; 48149 Muenster, Germany. (E-mail: christine.buff@uni-muenster.de)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Worrying has been suggested to prevent emotional and elaborative processing of fears. In cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) patients are exposed to their fears during the method of directed threat imagery by inducing emotional reactivity. However, studies investigating neural correlates of directed threat imagery and emotional reactivity in GAD patients are lacking. The present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study aimed at delineating neural correlates of directed threat imagery in GAD patients.

Method

Nineteen GAD patients and 19 healthy controls (HC) were exposed to narrative scripts of either disorder-related or neutral content and were encouraged to imagine it as vividly as possible.

Results

Rating results showed that GAD patients experienced disorder-related scripts as more anxiety inducing and arousing than HC. These results were also reflected in fMRI data: Disorder-related v. neutral scripts elicited elevated activity in the amygdala, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the thalamus as well as reduced activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex/subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in GAD patients relative to HC.

Conclusion

The present study presents the first behavioral and neural evidence for emotional reactivity during directed threat imagery in GAD. The brain activity pattern suggests an involvement of a fear processing network as a neural correlate of initial exposure during directed imagery in CBT in GAD.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) patients are characterized by excessive and uncontrollable worry and anxiety and assumed to be hyper-reactive to threat (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Hayes & Hirsch, Reference Hayes and Hirsch2007; Newman et al. Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013; Mochcovitch et al. Reference Mochcovitch, Da Rocha Freire, Garcia and Nardi2014; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). According to cognitive models of GAD, patients have brief images of their fears and respond to them by thinking about their worries in verbal form (Hirsch & Holmes, Reference Hirsch and Holmes2007). Verbal thinking prevents full emotional and elaborative processing of fears, resulting in prolonged bouts of distress (Hirsch & Holmes, Reference Hirsch and Holmes2007). Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) aims at preventing GAD patients from thinking about their fears in verbal form by confronting them with their worries through the method of directed imagery (Becker & Margraf, Reference Becker and Margraf2016). During directed imagery, GAD patients imagine feared situation as vividly as possible. Facing their fears through exposure triggers emotional reactivity and thereby allows for elaborative and emotional processing, resulting in habituation and extinction (Foa & Kozak, Reference Foa and Kozak1986). To date, directed imagery is incorporated in established treatments of fear and anxiety in various psychiatric disorders and has also been shown to be a productive tool in research (Hoyer et al. Reference Hoyer, Beesdo, Gloster, Runge, Hofler and Becker2009; McTeague & Lang, Reference Mcteague and Lang2012).

A number of studies have applied the method of directed imagery to delineate the psychophysiology of various anxiety/stress disorders, such as post-traumatic stress disorder, panic disorder, or social anxiety disorder (Bystritsky et al. Reference Bystritsky, Pontillo, Powers, Sabb, Craske and Bookheimer2001; McTeague et al. Reference Mcteague, Lang, Laplante, Cuthbert, Strauss and Bradley2009; McTeague et al. Reference Mcteague, Lang, Laplante, Cuthbert, Shumen and Bradley2010; McTeague et al. Reference Mcteague, Lang, Laplante and Bradley2011). It proved to be a productive means of eliciting emotions and to evoke disorder-related symptoms both in clinical practice and in research (Frewen et al. Reference Frewen, Dozois, Neufeld, Densmore, Stevens and Lanius2011; McTeague & Lang, Reference Mcteague and Lang2012). During directed imagery, participants typically listen to an affectively laden narrative script and imagine the engaging role of the protagonist. Guided by the bio-informational view of affective imagery (McTeague & Lang, Reference Mcteague and Lang2012), narrative scripts typically include information on stimulus events (who, what, where), reported feelings, interpretations, and responses, such as expressive physiology and actions coded in fear (Cuthbert et al. Reference Cuthbert, Lang, Strauss, Drobes, Patrick and Bradley2003). It appears that mental imagery elicits stronger emotions as compared to verbal processing of the same content (Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes and Kosslyn2015). Findings suggest that directed threat imagery parallels reactions to in vivo threat and results in potentiation of startle responses, increased autonomic arousal (heart rate, skin conductance) and the recruitment of a fear-related brain circuitry including elevated activity in the amygdala, insula, cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the hippocampus, as well as reduced activity in the thalamus and the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) in patients (Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Van Der Kolk, Fisler, Alpert, Orr, Savage, Fischman, Jenike and Pitman1996; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Kosslyn, Mcnally, Alpert, Thompson, Rauch, Macklin and Pitman1997; Liberzon et al. Reference Liberzon, Taylor, Amdur, Jung, Chamberlain, Minoshima, Koeppe and Fig1999; Bystritsky et al. Reference Bystritsky, Pontillo, Powers, Sabb, Craske and Bookheimer2001; Lanius et al. Reference Lanius, Williamson, Boksman, Densmore, Gupta, Neufeld, Gati and Menon2002; Lanius et al. Reference Lanius, Williamson, Hopper, Densmore, Boksman, Gupta, Neufeld, Gati and Menon2003; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Orr, Carson, Rauch, Macklin, Lasko, Peters, Metzger, Dougherty, Cannistraro, Alpert, Fischman and Pitman2004; Britton et al. Reference Britton, Phan, Taylor, Fig and Liberzon2005; Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Shin and Phelps2006; Etkin & Wager, Reference Etkin and Wager2007; Shin & Liberzon, Reference Shin and Liberzon2010; Ji et al. Reference Ji, Heyes, Macleod and Holmes2016). The amygdala plays a pivotal role in attention-vigilance aspects of threat-related processing and hypersensitive responding is one of the most common findings in symptom provocation studies in anxiety/stress disorder patients (Etkin, Reference Etkin2010; Holzschneider & Mulert, Reference Holzschneider and Mulert2011; Feldker et al. Reference Feldker, Heitmann, Neumeister, Tupak, Schrammen, Moeck, Zwitserlood, Bruchmann and Straube2017). Amygdala-driven fear expression is proposed to be regulated through PFC inhibitory mechanisms, for which both lateral and medial PFC (mPFC) appear essential (Hartley & Phelps, Reference Hartley and Phelps2010). Lateral PFC is implicated in attention control, as well as in emotion modulation processes (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). VmPFC is implicated in emotion modulation through inhibiting the amygdala and reduced vmPFC activity in anxiety/stress disorder patients is one of the most widely reported findings during symptom provocation studies (Frewen & Lanius, Reference Frewen and Lanius2010; Motzkin et al. Reference Motzkin, Philippi, Wolf, Baskaya and Koenigs2015). Dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) are related to threat-related appraisal, monitoring and expression (Etkin, Reference Etkin2010; Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011; Kalisch & Gerlicher, Reference Kalisch and Gerlicher2014). The experience of negative affect, cognitive control and memory processes are related to mid-cingulate and posterior cingulate cortex (Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011; Leech & Sharp, Reference Leech and Sharp2014). The insula is involved in interoceptive processing and the generation of subjective feelings (Lindquist & Barrett, Reference Lindquist and Barrett2012; Gasquoine, Reference Gasquoine2014). The hippocampus is primarily involved in processing of contextual information and fear generalization (Deckersbach et al. Reference Deckersbach, Dougherty and Rauch2006; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). An important relay between peripheral and cortical sensory signaling is the thalamus that is linked to arousal (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). One directed-threat imagery study detected reduced thalamus activity (Lanius et al. Reference Lanius, Williamson, Hopper, Densmore, Boksman, Gupta, Neufeld, Gati and Menon2003), which is in contrast to the majority of symptom provocation studies revealing increased thalamus activity in patients (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015; Feldker et al. Reference Feldker, Heitmann, Neumeister, Tupak, Schrammen, Moeck, Zwitserlood, Bruchmann and Straube2017). Overall, these brain regions constitute key components during fear generation and modulation in healthy subjects (HC) and patients with anxiety/stress disorders (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015).

Surprisingly, to date, no study implemented directed threat imagery to explore neural correlates of threat-related processing in GAD. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have investigated brain activity patterns in GAD patients in response to several classes of aversive stimuli (e.g. facial expressions, verbal stimuli, pictures or sounds) (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013; Hilbert et al. Reference Hilbert, Lueken and Beesdo-Baum2014; Mochcovitch et al. Reference Mochcovitch, Da Rocha Freire, Garcia and Nardi2014) and results appear inconclusive: Amygdala activity in GAD patients tended to be elevated when anticipating aversive or neutral stimuli (Nitschke et al. Reference Nitschke, Sarinopoulos, Oathes, Johnstone, Whalen, Davidson and Kalin2009) and in response to ambiguous stimuli (Hölzel et al. Reference Hölzel, Hoge, Greve, Gard, Creswell, Brown, Barrett, Schwartz, Vaitl and Lazar2013), but not or to a lesser degree during picture processing (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Shaywitz, Smith, Rhodes, Geraci, Jones, Mccaffrey, Vythilingam, Finger, Mondillo, Jacobs, Charney, Blair, Drevets and Pine2008; Whalen et al. Reference Whalen, Johnstone, Somerville, Nitschke, Polis, Alexander, Davidson and Kalin2008; Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Prater, Hoeft, Menon and Schatzberg2010; Palm et al. Reference Palm, Elliott, Mckie, Deakin and Anderson2011; Fonzo et al. Reference Fonzo, Ramsawh, Flagan, Sullivan, Letamendi, Simmons, Paulus and Stein2015; Buff et al. Reference Buff, Brinkmann, Neumeister, Feldker, Heitmann, Gathmann, Andor and Straube2016). Decreased ACC/mPFC activity in GAD patients was observed in emotion regulation tasks or in response to affective facial expressions (Etkin & Schatzberg, Reference Etkin and Schatzberg2011; Palm et al. Reference Palm, Elliott, Mckie, Deakin and Anderson2011; Ball et al. Reference Ball, Ramsawh, Campbell-Sills, Paulus and Stein2013; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015), while increased ACC/mPFC activity was reported for worry induction tasks and in response to aversive pictures (Hoehn-Saric et al. Reference Hoehn-Saric, Schlund and Wong2004; Paulesu et al. Reference Paulesu, Sambugaro, Torti, Danelli, Ferri, Scialfa, Sberna, Ruggiero, Bottini and Sassaroli2010; Buff et al. Reference Buff, Brinkmann, Neumeister, Feldker, Heitmann, Gathmann, Andor and Straube2016). Notably, accumulating evidence from treatment studies and a recent transdiagnostic study suggests that ACC/mPFC may play an essential role in the pathophysiology of GAD (Hoehn-Saric et al. Reference Hoehn-Saric, Schlund and Wong2004; Whalen et al. Reference Whalen, Johnstone, Somerville, Nitschke, Polis, Alexander, Davidson and Kalin2008; Nitschke et al. Reference Nitschke, Sarinopoulos, Oathes, Johnstone, Whalen, Davidson and Kalin2009; Buff et al. Reference Buff, Brinkmann, Neumeister, Feldker, Heitmann, Gathmann, Andor and Straube2016). Furthermore, increased lateral PFC activity to aversive pictures was detected in GAD patients in a recent transdiagnostic study (Buff et al. Reference Buff, Brinkmann, Neumeister, Feldker, Heitmann, Gathmann, Andor and Straube2016). Hyperactive lateral PFC activity is speculated to serve as a compensatory response to regulate abnormal function in GAD patients (Hölzel et al. Reference Hölzel, Hoge, Greve, Gard, Creswell, Brown, Barrett, Schwartz, Vaitl and Lazar2013). Other studies detected hyper- and hypoactivity of lateral PFC in GAD patients in response to affective stimuli (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Shaywitz, Smith, Rhodes, Geraci, Jones, Mccaffrey, Vythilingam, Finger, Mondillo, Jacobs, Charney, Blair, Drevets and Pine2008; Palm et al. Reference Palm, Elliott, Mckie, Deakin and Anderson2011; Moon & Jeong, Reference Moon and Jeong2015; Moon et al. Reference Moon, Yang and Jeong2015; Moon et al. Reference Moon, Sundaram, Choi and Jeong2016; Park et al. Reference Park, Kim, Jeong, Chung and Yang2016). In sum, the neural correlates of threat-related processing in GAD remain inconclusive, but tend to concentrate around dysfunctions within the amygdala–PFC circuitry (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013; Hilbert et al. Reference Hilbert, Lueken and Beesdo-Baum2014; Mochcovitch et al. Reference Mochcovitch, Da Rocha Freire, Garcia and Nardi2014).

A possible explanation for these mixed findings may be that most GAD patients report to fear hypothetical threatening situations rather than specifically anxiety-provoking stimuli (Hoyer et al. Reference Hoyer, Beesdo, Gloster, Runge, Hofler and Becker2009). Along these lines, stimulus material used in previous studies may have been limited in terms of meaning and not deemed personally relevant by GAD patients. Another important aspect to consider is that GAD patients tend to avoid emotional experience (Mennin et al. Reference Mennin, Mclaughlin and Flanagan2009). Thus, in order to investigate neural correlates of threat-related processing in GAD patients, a powerful tool is required to elicit emotional experience. As described above, directed threat imagery proved a productive means to elicit disorder-related symptoms in patients and may be suitable to trigger threat-related processing in GAD patients, since it allows to present hypothetical threatening situations in form of narrative scripts. Overall, directed threat imagery allows us to investigate threat-related processing in GAD patients and to resolve whether directed threat imagery actually results in emotional reactivity, which is a prerequisite for elaborative processing, habituation and extinction during imagery in CBT (Foa & Kozak, Reference Foa and Kozak1986).

The goal of the present study was to elucidate brain activity patterns underlying fear processing during directed threat imagery in GAD patients. Since as yet, no study has used the method of directed imagery in GAD patients, we focused on brain regions that constitute key components of the circuitry involved in (altered) processing of threat-related stimuli, including the amygdala, insula, PFC, cingulate cortex, thalamus and the hippocampus (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). We expected increased activity in all regions except in the vmPFC to disorder-related v. neutral scripts in GAD patients v. HC, since this is commonly detected in anxiety/stress disorder patients during symptom provocation studies (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). With regard to behavioral results, disorder-related scripts were expected to be experienced as more anxiety-inducing by GAD patients relative to HC, while no group differences were expected for neutral scripts.

Methods and materials

Subjects

Nineteen GAD patients and 19 HC matched for age, years of education, school-leaving certificate and gender were recruited through public advertisements and an outpatient clinic (Table 1). An experienced clinical psychologist diagnosed participants by means of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis-I Disorders (Wittchen et al. Reference Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz and Zaudig1997). GAD patients met criteria for GAD as primary diagnosis. GAD patients showed mild depressive symptoms (see Table 1). The following axis I comorbidities were further identified in patients: Recurrent major depressive disorder (n = 3), posttraumatic stress disorder (n = 1), specific phobia (n = 1), eating disorder (n = 1). Seven GAD patients took long-term medication (antidepressant medication, one GAD patient used Pregabalin). All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and gave written informed consent. Exclusion criteria were neurological disorders, presence or history of psychotic or bipolar disorders, current drug abuse or dependence and fMRI contraindications. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Muenster and conformed to the latest declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characterization of GAD patients and HC

GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; HC, healthy controls; BDI, Beck-Depression Inventory (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996); PSWQ, Penn State Worry Questionnaire (Meyer et al. Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990); MCQ, Metacognition questionnaire (Cartwright-Hatton & Wells, Reference Cartwright-Hatton and Wells1997); GAD-Q-IV, Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Zuellig, Kachin, Constantino, Przeworski, Erickson and Cashman-Mcgrath2002)

Measures

The self-report measure Beck-Depression inventory-II (BDI) was used to assess level of depressive symptoms (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996). The BDI demonstrated good internal consistency (α = 0.84), sensitivity (81%), specificity (92%) and test–retest reliability (r = 0.75) (Dozois et al. Reference Dozois, Dobson and Ahnberg1998; Kühner et al. Reference Kühner, Bürger, Keller and Hautzinger2007; Herzberg & Goldschmidt, Reference Herzberg and Goldschmidt2008). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ) is one of the most commonly used measures of worry severity in GAD patients (Meyer et al. Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990) with good internal consistency (α = 0.90), test–retest reliability (r = 0.75–93), good sensitivity (99%) and specificity (98%) (Behar et al. Reference Behar, Alcaine, Zuellig and Borkovec2003; Hoyer & Margraf, Reference Hoyer and Margraf2013). The Meta-Cognition-Questionnaire (MCQ) is a trait measure assessing beliefs about worry and intrusive thoughts and showed good internal consistency (α = 0.72–89) for subscales and test–retest reliability (r = 0.94) (Cartwright-Hatton & Wells, Reference Cartwright-Hatton and Wells1997; Wells & Cartwright-Hatton, Reference Wells and Cartwright-Hatton2004; Hoyer & Margraf, Reference Hoyer and Margraf2013). The Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV (GAD-Q-IV) is a self-report measure, capturing both primary and associated symptoms of GAD and demonstrated to have good internal consistency (α = 0.84), test–retest reliability (r = 0.81), sensitivity (69%) and specificity (97%) (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Zuellig, Kachin, Constantino, Przeworski, Erickson and Cashman-Mcgrath2002; Hoyer & Margraf, Reference Hoyer and Margraf2013). Compared with the PSWQ and MCQ, the GAD-Q-IV is the only self-report measure that endeavors to assess the entire clinical syndrome of GAD. The PSWQ, GAD-Q-IV and MCQ supported the diagnosis of GAD in the current patient sample.

Stimuli

Stimulus material consisted of standardized disorder-related (n = 5) and neutral (n = 5) scripts, matched for number of words (t [8] = 1.25, p = 0.248), and number of sentences (t [8] = 0.89, p = 0.397). All scripts were written in third person and present tense to reflect active participation and lasted exactly 30 s. The scripts included details of stimulus events (who, what, where), meaning, and response information, based on the bio-informational theory of emotional imagery (McTeague & Lang, Reference Mcteague and Lang2012). The disorder-related scripts involved threatening themes, such as the wellbeing of a relative or partner, one's own health, but also about miscellaneous topics (Olatunji et al. Reference Olatunji, Wolitzky-Taylor, Sawchuk and Ciesielski2010), such as being late for an appointment or a talk with the superior. Neutral scripts described various daily routines (e.g. reading a book, crossing a street). Scripts were tape-recorded and read by a female voice in a neutral tone, with the reader blind to the study's hypotheses.

Experimental design

The scanning session lasted approximately 9 min. Scripts were presented to the participants via headphones and in pseudo-randomized order by means of Presentation Software (v17.2, Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, California, USA). Participants received instructions at the beginning in written form on a black screen and after practice trials via headphones. They were instructed to listen to the scripts with their eyes closed and to vividly imagine the described scene as being actively involved. A brief sound was played before and after each script to indicate the beginning and end (400 Hz, 500 ms). Participants were instructed to stop imagining the scripted scene as soon as they heard the sound at the end of each script, to keep their eyes closed and to pay attention to the surrounding sounds of the scanner during the 20 s inter-stimulus intervals.

Participants received standardized instructions and training both outside and inside the scanner. Practice trials consisted of two neutral scripts that were not presented in the actual experiment. The experimenter asked participants after each script how well they felt they had been able to imagine the scene and encouraged them to pay attention to auditory, tactile, olfactory, gustatory and visual aspects of the imagined scene. If participants were able to vividly imagine on the practice trials, the scanning procedure was started.

Following fMRI scanning, participants were re-exposed to the scripts. Participants listened to the first 10 s of each script minimum and could terminate the presentation through a button press if they remembered the script. If they experienced difficulties remembering, they could listen to the full script. Participants were asked to rate their emotional responses to each script in terms of valence (1 = very unpleasant to 9 = very pleasant), emotional arousal (1 = not arousing to 9 = very arousing) and experienced anxiety (1 = not anxiety inducing to 9 = very anxiety inducing) using a nine-point Self-Assessment Manekin scale (Bradley & Lang, Reference Bradley and Lang1994). Participants also rated their ability to imagine each script (measured in percent).

Analysis of sociodemographic clinical questionnaire and rating data

Sociodemographic clinical questionnaire and rating data were analyzed using IBM SPSS software (v22, Armonk, New York, USA). Rating data for anxiety, valence, arousal and ability to imagine each script were subjected to separate 2 (script valence: disorder-related, neutral) by 2 (group: GAD, HC) mixed model analyses of variance (ANOVA). A probability level of p ⩽ 0.05 was considered as statistically significant. Bonferroni-corrected t tests were applied to resolve interaction effects (corrected significance level p ⩽ 0.008). We report Cohen's d (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988) as a measure of effect size for rating data.

fMRI acquisition and analysis

Anatomical and functional data were acquired with a 3 T magnetic resonance scanner (‘Magnetom PRISMA’, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a 20 channel head–neck coil. After acquiring a high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical scan with 192 slices, functional data were recorded with a T2*-weighted echo-planar sequence (TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix = 92 × 92 voxels, FOV = 208 mm2, TR = 2080 ms); and 260 volumes consisting of 36 axial slices (thickness = 3 mm, 0.3 mm gap, in plane resolution = 2.26 × 2.26 mm2) were acquired.

FMRI data were preprocessed and analyzed using BrainVoyager QX (BVQX, v2.8, Brain Innovation, Maastricht, Netherlands). To ensure steady-state tissue magnetization, the first four volumes were discarded. Data were corrected for slice time errors. Anatomical and functional data were co-registered and normalized to Talairach space (Talairach & Tournoux, Reference Talairach and Tournoux1988). Subsequently, data were smoothed spatially [6 mm FWHM (full-width at half-maximum) Gaussian kernel] and temporally (high-pass filter: 10 cycles per run; low-pass filter: 2.8 s; linear trend removal). Volumes were resampled 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 voxel size.

A canonical double-gamma HRF (hemodynamic response function) modeled the expected BOLD (blood oxygen level-dependent) signal for each predictor. Predictors of interest were disorder-related and neutral scripts, while sounds and six movement parameters were defined as predictors of no interest. Predictor estimates based on z-standardized time course data were calculated, with adjustment for autocorrelation following a global AR(1) model.

Analysis was conducted for a priori defined regions of interest (ROIs) that constitute key components during (altered) processing of fear-related stimuli, including the amygdala, insula, PFC, cingulate cortex, thalamus and the hippocampus (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). ROIs were defined based on the AAL (Automated Anatomical Labeling) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. Reference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landeau, Papathanassiou, Crivello, Etard, Delcroix, Mazoyer and Joliot2002; Maldjian et al. Reference Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft and Burdette2003; Maldjian et al. Reference Maldjian, Laurienti and Burdette2004). These were transformed into Talairach space (Lancaster et al. Reference Lancaster, Tordesillas-GUTIÉRREZ, Martinez, Salinas, Evans, Zilles, Mazziotta and Fox2007) using ICBM2TAL in Matlab (v8.2, The MathWorks Inc, Natick, Massachusetts, USA).

Statistical parametric maps derived from voxel-wise analyses were considered significant for clusters that survived cluster-based correction for multiple comparisons. The voxel-level threshold was set to an uncorrected statistical threshold of p ⩽ 0.005 (Lieberman & Cunningham, Reference Lieberman and Cunningham2009). Using the cluster-level statistical threshold estimator plugin for BVQX (Goebel et al. Reference Goebel, Esposito and Formisano2006), a mask consisting of all ROIs was applied to the thresholded maps. ROI-specific correction criteria were based on the estimates of the maps’ spatial smoothness and on an iterative procedure (Monte Carlo simulation) applied to estimate cluster-level false-positive rates (Forman et al. Reference Forman, Cohen, Fitzgerald, Eddy, Mintun and Noll1995). After 1000 iterations, this procedure yielded a minimum cluster size (k = 11 resampled voxel size) to generate a map-wise corrected false positive rate of p ⩽ 0.05. We report effect sizes (Cohen's d; Cohen, Reference Cohen1988) for each significant outcome based on average t values.

To examine the relationship between differential brain responses (disorder-related v. neutral scripts) and behavioral measures, we correlated subjective ratings (arousal, valence, anxiety) and BDI level of patients with mean parameter estimates (disorder-related v. neutral scripts) of significant activation clusters resulting from ROI analysis (Bonferroni-corrected significance level: p ⩽ 0.0125). The influence of medication intake was tested by performing a t test (medicated v. non-medicated patients) on parameter estimates (disorder-related v. neutral scripts) of significant activation clusters resulting from ROI analysis (p ⩽ 0.05 was considered as statistically significant).

Results

Rating data

Mean ratings are provided in Table 2. Analyses of arousal, valence and anxiety ratings each revealed significant main effects of group (arousal: F [1,36] = 9.86, p = 0.003, d = 1.05; valence: F [1,36] = 8.30, p = 0.007, d = 0.96; anxiety: F [1,36] = 21.33, p < 0.001, d = 1.56) and script valence (arousal: F [1,36] = 163.11, p < 0.001, d = 4.26; valence: F [1,36] = 170.73, p < 0.001, d = 4.36; anxiety: F [1,36] = 113.33, p < 0.001, d = 3.55), and significant group by script valence interactions (anxiety: F [1,36] = 13.20, p = 0.001, d = 1.21; arousal F [1,36] = 6.96, p = 0.012, d = 0.88) (see Fig. 1). Disorder-related scripts were rated as more negative, more arousing and more anxiety inducing than neutral scripts and GAD patients generally rated stimuli as more negative, more arousing and more anxiety inducing than HC. Resolving the interaction effects for anxiety and arousal ratings showed that disorder-related scripts were rated as more arousing (t [36] = 3.46, p = 0.001, d = 1.12) and more anxiety inducing (t [36] = 4.65, p < 0.001, d = 1.51) by GAD patients v. HC. Analysis of the participants’ ability to imagine each script yielded no significant effects (all p > 0.184).

Fig. 1. Mean ratings for disorder-related and neutral scripts on a nine-point Self-Assessment Manekin Scale. The 2 (script valence) by 2 (group) mixed model analysis of variance revealed interaction effects for anxiety and arousal ratings. Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) patients relative to healthy controls (HC) rated disorder-related scripts as more anxiety inducing and more arousing.

Table 2. Mean ratings per script valence on the dimensions of arousal, anxiety, valence and ability to imagine the scripts

GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; HC, healthy controls.

a Raw data drawn from a nine-point Self-Assessment Manekin Scale.

b In percent.

ROI analysis

ROI analysis revealed that GAD patients relative to HC responded to disorder-related v. neutral scripts with increased activity in several areas (Fig. 2): lateral amygdala (LA), extending to central amygdala (CeA) (right: peak voxel Talairach coordinates: x = 18, y = −19, z = −10; size: 232 mm3; average t value: 3.01; maximal t value: 3.76; p ⩽ 0.005 uncorrected, p ⩽ 0.05 corrected; d = 0.98), ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC) [(1) left: peak voxel Talairach coordinates: x = −47, y = 35, z = 17; size: 232 mm3; average t value: 2.90; maximal t value: 3.04; p ⩽ 0.005 uncorrected, p ⩽ 0.05 corrected; d = 0.94; (2) left: peak voxel Talairach coordinates: x = −36, y = 32, z = −1; size: 88 mm3; average t value: 2.90; maximal t value: 3.12; p ⩽ 0.005 uncorrected, p ⩽ 0.05 corrected; d = 0.94], dmPFC (left: peak voxel Talairach coordinates: x = −10, y = 37, z = 31; size: 96 mm3; average t value: 2.93; maximal t value: 3.29; p ⩽ 0.005 uncorrected, p ⩽ 0.05 corrected; d = 0.95), and thalamus (right: peak voxel Talairach coordinates: x = 5, y = −13, z = 11; size: 136 mm3; average t value: 2.89; maximal t value: 3.18; p ⩽ 0.005 uncorrected, p ⩽ 0.05 corrected; d = 0.94).

Fig. 2. Brain responses to disorder-related v. neutral scripts. The region of interest analysis revealed for the contrast disorder-related > neutral scripts for generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) patients relative to healthy controls (HC) increased activity in the amygdala, thalamus, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and reduced activity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC)/ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Statistical parametric maps are overlaid on an averaged T1 scan (radiological convention: left = right). Graphs display contrasts of parameter estimates [disorder-related > neutral scripts (mean ± s.e. for activation cluster)] per group.

In addition to increased activity, GAD patients relative to HC showed reduced activity in the vmPFC/subgenual ACC (sgACC) (right/left: peak voxel Talairach coordinates: x = 2, y = 20, z = −6; size: 192 mm3; average t value: 3.38; maximal t value: 4.25; p ⩽ 0.005 uncorrected, p ⩽ 0.05 corrected; d = 1.10) (Fig. 2).

There were no differential brain responses in GAD patients v. HC in the insula or the hippocampus. We conducted exploratory analysis using a voxelwise threshold of p ⩽ 0.05 and detected increased hippocampus and insula activity in GAD as compared to HC to disorder-related v. neutral scripts. It appears that the sensitivity of our study was not high enough to detected effects in these regions. There were no significant correlations between differential activations detected in ROI analysis and BDI level or subjective ratings as well as no effects regarding medication intake.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the current study is the first to investigate brain responses during the well-established method of directed threat imagery in GAD patients using disorder-related and neutral scripts. Notably, the present findings show that directed threat imagery is effective in eliciting emotional reactivity related to fear processing in GAD patients on both behavioral and neural levels. While disorder-related scripts were rated as more negative, arousing and anxiety-inducing than neutral scripts by all participants, crucially, disorder-related scripts were experienced as more anxiety-inducing and more arousing by GAD patients relative to HC. fMRI results yielded a brain activity pattern in GAD patients in response to disorder-related scripts that parallels activations previously reported for fear processing (Shin & Liberzon, Reference Shin and Liberzon2010; Holzschneider & Mulert, Reference Holzschneider and Mulert2011; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). First, GAD patients responded to disorder-related scripts with elevated activity in the amygdala, thalamus and the dmPFC, possibly reflecting hyper-reactive emotional responding (Wilensky et al. Reference Wilensky, Schafe, Kristensen and Ledoux2006; Etkin, Reference Etkin2010; Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011; Kalisch & Gerlicher, Reference Kalisch and Gerlicher2014; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). Second, GAD patients also responded with reduced vmPFC/sgACC and elevated vlPFC activity, which might reflect deficient emotion regulation (Wager et al. Reference Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist and Ochsner2008; Holzschneider & Mulert, Reference Holzschneider and Mulert2011; Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Berkman, Lieberman, Stuss and Knight2013; Greenberg et al. Reference Greenberg, Carlson, Cha, Hajcak and Mujica-Parodi2013; Hölzel et al. Reference Hölzel, Hoge, Greve, Gard, Creswell, Brown, Barrett, Schwartz, Vaitl and Lazar2013; Tupak et al. Reference Tupak, Dresler, Guhn, Ehlis, Fallgatter, Pauli and Herrmann2014; Motzkin et al. Reference Motzkin, Philippi, Wolf, Baskaya and Koenigs2015).

Hyper-reactive emotional responding to disorder-related scripts in GAD patients may be associated with increased amygdala activity, which was detected in a cluster consisting of LA extending to CeA. LA encodes threat value of a stimulus and CeA is linked to the expression of conditioned fear response (Wilensky et al. Reference Wilensky, Schafe, Kristensen and Ledoux2006). LA and CeA appear to play an essential role in attention-vigilance aspects of threat-related processing and abnormalities in these regions have been linked to hyperarousal and/or hypervigilance to threat in anxiety/stress disorders (Etkin, Reference Etkin2010; Holzschneider & Mulert, Reference Holzschneider and Mulert2011; Feldker et al. Reference Feldker, Heitmann, Neumeister, Tupak, Schrammen, Moeck, Zwitserlood, Bruchmann and Straube2017). It seems that while imagining disorder-related scripts, GAD patients experienced, as part of their increased anxiety, ongoing, exaggerated arousal and vigilance towards imagined possible threat (Milad et al. Reference Milad, Pitman, Ellis, Gold, Shin, Lasko, Zeidan, Handwerger, Orr and Rauch2009; Holzschneider & Mulert, Reference Holzschneider and Mulert2011), possibly associated with exaggerated threat detection and expectancies (Grupe & Nitschke, Reference Grupe and Nitschke2013). Our findings are of particular importance in the research of the amygdala involvement in GAD, because findings are inconclusive: Some studies have reported both increased and decreased amygdala responding to affective stimuli (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Shaywitz, Smith, Rhodes, Geraci, Jones, Mccaffrey, Vythilingam, Finger, Mondillo, Jacobs, Charney, Blair, Drevets and Pine2008, Reference Blair, Geraci, Smith, Hollon, Devido, Otero, Blair and Pine2012; Fonzo et al. Reference Fonzo, Ramsawh, Flagan, Sullivan, Simmons, Paulus and Stein2014; Nitschke et al. Reference Nitschke, Sarinopoulos, Oathes, Johnstone, Whalen, Davidson and Kalin2009; Park et al. Reference Park, Kim, Jeong, Chung and Yang2016; Yassa et al. Reference Yassa, Hazlett, Stark and Hoehn-Saric2012), whereas others have failed to reveal alterations in amygdala activity altogether (Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Prater, Hoeft, Menon and Schatzberg2010; Fonzo et al. Reference Fonzo, Ramsawh, Flagan, Sullivan, Letamendi, Simmons, Paulus and Stein2015; Whalen et al. Reference Whalen, Johnstone, Somerville, Nitschke, Polis, Alexander, Davidson and Kalin2008). The present results are relevant for the ongoing discussion of the amygdala involvement in GAD and imply that amygdala functioning is altered in patients.

Apart from increased amygdala activity, disorder-related scripts induced heightened thalamus activity in GAD patients. Similarly, another study showed that listening to worry statements induced elevated thalamus activity in GAD patients (Hoehn-Saric et al. Reference Hoehn-Saric, Schlund and Wong2004). The thalamus has been linked to the integration of sensory information and signals such information to the amygdala (Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). Increased thalamic activation to disorder-related stimuli in anxiety/stress disorder patients has been described in previous studies (Straube et al. Reference Straube, Glauer, Dilger, Mentzel and Miltner2006; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015; Feldker et al. Reference Feldker, Heitmann, Neumeister, Tupak, Schrammen, Moeck, Zwitserlood, Bruchmann and Straube2017), except of in one directed threat imagery study in post-traumatic stress disorder patients (Lanius et al. Reference Lanius, Williamson, Hopper, Densmore, Boksman, Gupta, Neufeld, Gati and Menon2003). The thalamic activation in the current study might indicate an arousal response in GAD patients when imagining disorder-related scripts (Vertes et al. Reference Vertes, Linley and Hoover2015).

Furthermore, GAD-related emotional reactivity in response to disorder-related scripts was reflected in elevated dmPFC activity. This is in accordance with an earlier study reporting increased dmPFC activity in GAD patients during worry induction (Paulesu et al. Reference Paulesu, Sambugaro, Torti, Danelli, Ferri, Scialfa, Sberna, Ruggiero, Bottini and Sassaroli2010). DmPFC activity has been implicated in monitoring, evaluation and appraisal of stimuli as well as in anxiety expression (Etkin, Reference Etkin2010; Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011; Kalisch & Gerlicher, Reference Kalisch and Gerlicher2014) or in the relay of information to vmPFC for regulatory purposes (Etkin, Reference Etkin2010). Given these findings, dmPFC hyper-activation to disorder-related scripts in the present study may indicate that scripts were more negatively appraised by GAD patients, possibly resulting in fear and anxiety expression (Etkin, Reference Etkin2010).

Parallel to hyper-reactive emotional responding reflected in increased activation in the amygdala, thalamus and the dmPFC, GAD patients were also marked by reduced activity in the vmPFC/sgACC and increased activity in the vlPFC. In view of findings that associate decreased vmPFC/sgACC with attenuation of fear responses (Hartley & Phelps, Reference Hartley and Phelps2010; Greenberg et al. Reference Greenberg, Carlson, Cha, Hajcak and Mujica-Parodi2013; Frewen & Lanius, Reference Frewen and Lanius2010; Motzkin et al. Reference Motzkin, Philippi, Wolf, Baskaya and Koenigs2010), this particular result pattern may point to deficient emotion regulation in patients, with reduced vmPFC/sgACC activity reflecting difficulty in dampening fear responses. This interpretation draws upon neurocircuitry-based models of anxiety disorders in which deficient vmPFC activity together with increased amygdala activity in response to threat is assumed to mediate inadequate regulation of fear responses (Hartley & Phelps, Reference Hartley and Phelps2010; Holzschneider & Mulert, Reference Holzschneider and Mulert2011; Greenberg et al. Reference Greenberg, Carlson, Cha, Hajcak and Mujica-Parodi2013; Motzkin et al. Reference Motzkin, Philippi, Wolf, Baskaya and Koenigs2015). Altered emotion regulation linked to deficient vmPFC/sgACC recruitment in GAD patients has been emphasized in previous investigations (Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Prater, Schatzberg, Menon and Greicius2009; Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Prater, Hoeft, Menon and Schatzberg2010; Etkin & Schatzberg, Reference Etkin and Schatzberg2011; Greenberg et al. Reference Greenberg, Carlson, Cha, Hajcak and Mujica-Parodi2013; Wang et al. Reference Wang, Hou, Qian, Liu, Li, Li, Peng, Xin and Sun2016). Difficulty in dampening amygdala activity may also be reflected by increased vlPFC activity in response to disorder-related scripts in GAD patients. Functionally, vlPFC has been linked to inhibitory control or implicit emotion regulation and modulation of amygdala activity, based on findings of vlPFC recruitment during voluntary downregulation of unpleasant emotions in HC (Wager et al. Reference Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist and Ochsner2008; Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Berkman, Lieberman, Stuss and Knight2013; Tupak et al. Reference Tupak, Dresler, Guhn, Ehlis, Fallgatter, Pauli and Herrmann2014). It is speculated that increased activation of the PFC, including the vlPFC, may reflect a compensatory regulation mechanisms in GAD patients (Hölzel et al. Reference Hölzel, Hoge, Greve, Gard, Creswell, Brown, Barrett, Schwartz, Vaitl and Lazar2013). Elevated PFC together with altered amygdala activity hints toward deficient signal transformation that may lead to unsuccessful emotion regulation (Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Prater, Schatzberg, Menon and Greicius2009). Disturbed vlPFC functioning in GAD patients was detected in previous studies revealing either elevated (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Shaywitz, Smith, Rhodes, Geraci, Jones, Mccaffrey, Vythilingam, Finger, Mondillo, Jacobs, Charney, Blair, Drevets and Pine2008; Hölzel et al. Reference Hölzel, Hoge, Greve, Gard, Creswell, Brown, Barrett, Schwartz, Vaitl and Lazar2013; Moon et al. Reference Moon, Yang and Jeong2015; Park et al. Reference Park, Kim, Jeong, Chung and Yang2016) or reduced vlPFC activity (Palm et al. Reference Palm, Elliott, Mckie, Deakin and Anderson2011). Both vmPFC/sgACC and vlPFC possibly failed to regulate emotional reactivity, resulting in prolonged hypervigilance and hyperarousal, both of which are core symptoms of GAD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

Exploratory analyses revealed increased activity in the hippocampus and the insula to disorder-related scripts in GAD patients. This is in line with former investigations showing aberrant hippocampus and insula involvement during threat-related processing in GAD (Hoehn-Saric et al. Reference Hoehn-Saric, Schlund and Wong2004; Hilbert et al. Reference Hilbert, Lueken and Beesdo-Baum2014; Buff et al. Reference Buff, Brinkmann, Neumeister, Feldker, Heitmann, Gathmann, Andor and Straube2016), as well as previous symptom provocation studies in anxiety/stress disorder patients (McTeague & Lang, Reference Mcteague and Lang2012; Duval et al. Reference Duval, Javanbakht and Liberzon2015). It is to be assumed that future directed threat imagery studies in GAD patients with larger samples may detect hippocampus and insula activity alterations.

While not all predicted areas showed increased activation, we suggest that with regard to the aim of the present study, findings confirm that directed threat imagery triggers neural emotional reactivity related to fear processing in GAD patients. Disorder-related scripts appeared to induce heightened registration/reactivity (associated with amygdala activity), arousal (linked to thalamus activity) and negative appraisal (associated with dmPFC activity) in GAD patients. In turn, decreased activation in the vmPFC/sgACC and elevated activation in the vlPFC could be interpreted to reflect deficient downregulation of such emotional reactivity in GAD patients, possibly resulting in prolonged hyperarousal and vigilance. Both hyperarousal and vigilance are DSM-IV-associated symptoms of GAD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Consequently, the present results provide tentative evidence for emotional reactivity during directed imagery in GAD, which is a prerequisite for elaborative processing and ultimately habituation and extinction during successful CBT (Foa & Kozak, Reference Foa and Kozak1986). We infer from our findings that directed threat imagery is a promising tool for exposing patients to their fears and that brain activity patterns arising during directed imagery in CBT may be similar to the activation pattern of the present study.

Of course, this postulation is preliminary and somewhat tentative, given that participants in the present study were exposed to standardized rather than autobiographical scripts and given that the effect of repeated directed threat imagery exposure was not assessed. Autobiographical scripts capturing individual worries as well as effects of repeated exposure may represent two interesting and important avenues for future research. With regard to other limitations, medication needs to be mentioned. Seven GAD patients were medicated at time of testing. However, analyses showed that medication intake showed no effect on the observed results. According to a review psychotropic medication intake seems to have no influence or a normalizing influence on neuroimaging findings (Hafeman et al. Reference Hafeman, Chang, Garrett, Sanders and Phillips2012). Second, patients had comorbidities. Comorbidities constitute a challenge for patient studies and the interpretation of the results. Therefore, we carefully ensured that GAD was the main diagnosis for all patients through diagnosing patients by an experienced clinical psychologist. Furthermore, the exclusion of patients with comorbid diagnoses may have limited the representativeness and generalizability of the current findings, especially because comorbidities frequently occur in GAD (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013). Nevertheless, it would be beneficial to investigate patients without medication or comorbidities in future studies, possibly also accounting for other variables such as personality or genotypes. Third, although we matched for school leaving certificate and years of education, there was no additional measure of intelligence. Future studies should control for level of intelligence.

To conclude, the present study investigated whether directed threat imagery resulted in emotional reactivity in GAD patients, paralleling the method used in CBT. Findings yielded a brain activity pattern in GAD patients marked by elevated amygdala, dmPFC, thalamus, vlPFC and reduced vmPFC/sgACC activity. This pattern may be indicative of hyper-reactive emotional responding together with deficient emotion regulation during imagination of disorder-related scenarios in GAD patients. We suggest that directed threat imagery is a powerful tool both for the investigation of fear processing in GAD patients and for the induction of emotional reactivity in CBT.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG: SFB/TRR 58: C06, C07).

Declaration of Interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed., text revision). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Ball, TM, Ramsawh, HJ, Campbell-Sills, L, Paulus, MP, Stein, MB (2013). Prefrontal dysfunction during emotion regulation in generalized anxiety and panic disorders. Psychological Medicine 43, 14751486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Brown, GK (1996). Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio 78, 490498.Google Scholar
Becker, ES, Margraf, J (2016). Generalisierte Angststörung: Ein Therapieprogramm (3., vollst. überarb). Beltz: Weinheim.Google Scholar
Behar, E, Alcaine, O, Zuellig, AR, Borkovec, TD (2003). Screening for generalized anxiety disorder using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire: a receiver operating characteristic analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 34, 2543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, K, Geraci, M, Smith, BW, Hollon, N, Devido, J, Otero, M, Blair, JR, Pine, DS (2012). Reduced dorsal anterior cingulate cortical activity during emotional regulation and top-down attentional control in generalized social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, and comorbid generalized social phobia/generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry 72, 476482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, K, Shaywitz, J, Smith, BW, Rhodes, R, Geraci, M, Jones, M, Mccaffrey, D, Vythilingam, M, Finger, E, Mondillo, K, Jacobs, M, Charney, DS, Blair, RJ, Drevets, WC, Pine, DS (2008). Response to emotional expressions in generalized social phobia and generalized anxiety disorder: evidence for separate disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 11931202.Google Scholar
Bradley, MM, Lang, PJ (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 25, 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Britton, JC, Phan, KL, Taylor, SF, Fig, LM, Liberzon, I (2005). Corticolimbic blood flow in posttraumatic stress disorder during script-driven imagery. Biological Psychiatry 57, 832840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buff, C, Brinkmann, L, Neumeister, P, Feldker, K, Heitmann, C, Gathmann, B, Andor, T, Straube, T (2016). Specifically altered brain responses to threat in generalized anxiety disorder relative to social anxiety disorder and panic disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical 12, 698706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bystritsky, A, Pontillo, D, Powers, M, Sabb, FW, Craske, MG, Bookheimer, SY (2001). Functional MRI changes during panic anticipation and imagery exposure. Neuroreport 12, 39533957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright-Hatton, S, Wells, A (1997). Beliefs about worry and intrusions: the Meta-Cognitions Questionnaire and its correlates. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 11, 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, pp. 2026. Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Cohen, JR, Berkman, ET, Lieberman, MD (2013). Intentional and Incidental Self-Control in Ventrolateral PFC. In Principles of Frontal Lobe Function (2nd ed.) (Ed. Stuss, D. T. and Knight, R. T.), pp. 417440. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Cuthbert, BN, Lang, PJ, Strauss, C, Drobes, D, Patrick, CJ, Bradley, MM (2003). The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology 40, 407422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckersbach, T, Dougherty, DD, Rauch, SL (2006). Functional imaging of mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Neuroimaging 16, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dozois, DJ, Dobson, KS, Ahnberg, JL (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Assessment 10, 83.Google Scholar
Duval, ER, Javanbakht, A, Liberzon, I (2015). Neural circuits in anxiety and stress disorders: a focused review. Therapeutics and Clinical Risk Management 11, 115126.Google ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A (2010). Functional neuroanatomy of anxiety: a neural circuit perspective. Current Topics in Behavioral Neuroscience 2, 251277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A, Egner, T, Kalisch, R (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 8593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A, Prater, KE, Hoeft, F, Menon, V, Schatzberg, AF (2010). Failure of anterior cingulate activation and connectivity with the amygdala during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 545554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A, Prater, KE, Schatzberg, AF, Menon, V, Greicius, MD (2009). Disrupted amygdalar subregion functional connectivity and evidence of a compensatory network in generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 66, 13611372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A, Schatzberg, AF (2011). Common abnormalities and disorder-specific compensation during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety and major depressive disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 968978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, AW, Wager, TD (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 14761488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldker, K, Heitmann, C, Neumeister, P, Tupak, S, Schrammen, E, Moeck, R, Zwitserlood, P, Bruchmann, M, Straube, T (2017). Transdiagnostic brain responses to disorder-related threat across four psychiatric disorders. Psychological Medicine 47, 730743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, EB, Kozak, MJ (1986). Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin 99, 20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonzo, GA, Ramsawh, HJ, Flagan, TM, Sullivan, SG, Letamendi, A, Simmons, AN, Paulus, MP, Stein, MB (2015). Common and disorder-specific neural responses to emotional faces in generalised anxiety, social anxiety and panic disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry 114, 149880.Google Scholar
Fonzo, GA, Ramsawh, HJ, Flagan, TM, Sullivan, SG, Simmons, AN, Paulus, MP, Stein, MB (2014). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder is associated with attenuation of limbic activation to threat-related facial emotions. Journal of Affective Disorders 169, 7685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forman, SD, Cohen, JD, Fitzgerald, M, Eddy, WF, Mintun, MA, Noll, DC (1995). Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 33, 636647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewen, PA, Dozois, DJA, Neufeld, RWJ, Densmore, M, Stevens, TK, Lanius, RA (2011). Neuroimaging social emotional processing in women: fMRI study of script-driven imagery. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6, 375392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewen, PA, Lanius, RA (2010). Non-idiographic emotional script-driven imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology 218, 8995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasquoine, PG (2014). Contributions of the insula to cognition and emotion. Neuropsychology Review 24, 7787.Google Scholar
Goebel, R, Esposito, F, Formisano, E (2006). Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Human Brain Mapping 27, 392401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, T, Carlson, JM, Cha, J, Hajcak, G, Mujica-Parodi, LR (2013). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex reactivity is altered in generalized anxiety disorder during fear generalization. Depression and Anxiety 30, 242250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grupe, DW, Nitschke, JB (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, 488501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafeman, DM, Chang, KD, Garrett, AS, Sanders, EM, Phillips, ML (2012). Effects of medication on neuroimaging findings in bipolar disorder: an updated review. Bipolar Disorders 14, 375410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartley, CA, Phelps, EA (2010). Changing fear: the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 136146.Google Scholar
Hayes, S, Hirsch, CR (2007). Information processing biases in generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry 6, 176182.Google Scholar
Herzberg, P, Goldschmidt, S (2008). Beck Depressions-Inventar (BDI-II). Revision. Report Psychologie 33, 301302.Google Scholar
Hilbert, K, Lueken, U, Beesdo-Baum, K (2014). Neural structures, functioning and connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder and interaction with neuroendocrine systems: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders 158, 114126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsch, CR, Holmes, EA (2007). Mental imagery in anxiety disorders. Psychiatry 6, 161165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoehn-Saric, R, Schlund, MW, Wong, SH (2004). Effects of citalopram on worry and brain activation in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research 131, 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hölzel, BK, Hoge, EA, Greve, DN, Gard, T, Creswell, JD, Brown, KW, Barrett, LF, Schwartz, C, Vaitl, D, Lazar, SW (2013). Neural mechanisms of symptom improvements in generalized anxiety disorder following mindfulness training. NeuroImage: Clinical 2, 448458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzschneider, K, Mulert, C (2011). Neuroimaging in anxiety disorders. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 13, 453461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyer, J, Beesdo, K, Gloster, AT, Runge, J, Hofler, M, Becker, ES (2009). Worry exposure versus applied relaxation in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 78, 106115.Google Scholar
Hoyer, J, Margraf, J (2013). Angstdiagnostik: Grundlagen und Testverfahren. Springer-Verlag: Berlin Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Ji, JL, Heyes, SB, Macleod, C, Holmes, EA (2016). Emotional mental imagery as simulation of reality: fear and beyond – a tribute to Peter Lang. Behavior Therapy 47, 702719.Google Scholar
Kalisch, R, Gerlicher, AMV (2014). Making a mountain out of a molehill: on the role of the rostral dorsal anterior cingulate and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in conscious threat appraisal, catastrophizing, and worrying. Neuroscience, Biobehavioral Reviews 42, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kühner, C, Bürger, C, Keller, F, Hautzinger, M (2007). Reliabilität und validität des revidierten beck-depressionsinventars (BDI-II). Der Nervenarzt 78, 651656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lancaster, JL, Tordesillas-GUTIÉRREZ, D, Martinez, M, Salinas, F, Evans, A, Zilles, K, Mazziotta, JC, Fox, PT (2007). Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Human Brain Mapping 28, 11941205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanius, RA, Williamson, PC, Boksman, K, Densmore, M, Gupta, M, Neufeld, RW, Gati, JS, Menon, RS (2002). Brain activation during script-driven imagery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biological Psychiatry 52, 305311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanius, RA, Williamson, PC, Hopper, J, Densmore, M, Boksman, K, Gupta, MA, Neufeld, RW, Gati, JS, Menon, RS (2003). Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation. Biological Psychiatry 53, 204210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leech, R, Sharp, DJ (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 137, 1232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberzon, I, Taylor, SF, Amdur, R, Jung, TD, Chamberlain, KR, Minoshima, S, Koeppe, RA, Fig, LM (1999). Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biological Psychiatry 45, 817826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, MD, Cunningham, WA (2009). Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 423428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, KA, Barrett, LF (2012). A functional architecture of the human brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 533540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maldjian, JA, Laurienti, PJ, Burdette, JH (2004). Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas. Neuroimage 21, 450455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maldjian, JA, Laurienti, PJ, Kraft, RA, Burdette, JH (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 12331239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mcteague, LM, Lang, PJ (2012). The anxiety spectrum and the reflex physiology of defense: from circumscribed fear to broad distress. Depression and Anxiety 29, 264281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mcteague, LM, Lang, PJ, Laplante, M-C, Bradley, MM (2011). Aversive imagery in panic disorder: agoraphobia severity, comorbidity, and defensive physiology. Biological Psychiatry 70, 415424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mcteague, LM, Lang, PJ, Laplante, M-C, Cuthbert, BN, Shumen, JR, Bradley, MM (2010). Aversive imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder: trauma recurrence, comorbidity, and physiological reactivity. Biological Psychiatry 67, 346356.Google Scholar
Mcteague, LM, Lang, PJ, Laplante, M-C, Cuthbert, BN, Strauss, CC, Bradley, MM (2009). Fearful imagery in social phobia: generalization, comorbidity, and physiological reactivity. Biological Psychiatry 65, 374382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mennin, DS, Mclaughlin, KA, Flanagan, TJ (2009). Emotion regulation deficits in generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder, and their co-occurrence. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23, 866871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, TJ, Miller, ML, Metzger, RL, Borkovec, TD (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 487495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milad, MR, Pitman, RK, Ellis, CB, Gold, AL, Shin, LM, Lasko, NB, Zeidan, MA, Handwerger, K, Orr, SP, Rauch, SL (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 66, 10751082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mochcovitch, MD, Da Rocha Freire, RC, Garcia, RF, Nardi, AE (2014). A systematic review of fMRI studies in generalized anxiety disorder: evaluating its neural and cognitive basis. Journal of Affective Disorders 167, 336342.Google Scholar
Moon, CM, Jeong, GW (2015). Functional neuroanatomy on the working memory under emotional distraction in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 69, 609619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, C-M, Sundaram, T, Choi, N-G, Jeong, G-W (2016). Working memory dysfunction associated with brain functional deficits and cellular metabolic changes in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 254, 137144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, C-M, Yang, J-C, Jeong, G-W (2015). Explicit verbal memory impairments associated with brain functional deficits and morphological alterations in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 186, 328336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motzkin, JC, Philippi, CL, Wolf, RC, Baskaya, MK, Koenigs, M (2015). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biological Psychiatry 77, 276284.Google Scholar
Newman, MG, Llera, SJ, Erickson, TM, Przeworski, A, Castonguay, LG (2013). Worry and generalized anxiety disorder: a review and theoretical synthesis of evidence on nature, etiology, mechanisms, and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 9, 275297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, MG, Zuellig, AR, Kachin, KE, Constantino, MJ, Przeworski, A, Erickson, T, Cashman-Mcgrath, L (2002). Preliminary reliability and validity of the generalized anxiety disorder questionnaire-IV: a revised self-report diagnostic measure of generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy 33, 215233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nitschke, JB, Sarinopoulos, I, Oathes, DJ, Johnstone, T, Whalen, PJ, Davidson, RJ, Kalin, NH (2009). Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment response. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 302310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, BO, Wolitzky-Taylor, KB, Sawchuk, CN, Ciesielski, BG (2010). Worry and the anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic synthesis of specificity to GAD. Applied and Preventive Psychology 14, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palm, ME, Elliott, R, Mckie, S, Deakin, JF, Anderson, IM (2011). Attenuated responses to emotional expressions in women with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychological Medicine 41, 10091018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J-I, Kim, G-W, Jeong, G-W, Chung, GH, Yang, J-C (2016). Brain activation patterns associated with the effects of emotional distracters during working memory maintenance in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Investigation 13, 152156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E, Sambugaro, E, Torti, T, Danelli, L, Ferri, F, Scialfa, G, Sberna, M, Ruggiero, GM, Bottini, G, Sassaroli, S (2010). Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: a functional MRI study. Psychological Medicine 40, 117124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, J, Naselaris, T, Holmes, EA, Kosslyn, SM (2015). Mental imagery: functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19, 590602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauch, SL, Shin, LM, Phelps, EA (2006). Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research – past, present, and future. Biological Psychiatry 60, 376382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauch, SL, Van Der Kolk, BA, Fisler, RE, Alpert, NM, Orr, SP, Savage, CR, Fischman, AJ, Jenike, MA, Pitman, RK (1996). A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Archives of General Psychiatry 53, 380387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, AJ, Salomons, TV, Slagter, HA, Fox, AS, Winter, JJ, Davidson, RJ (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain, and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 12, 154167.Google Scholar
Shin, LM, Kosslyn, SM, Mcnally, RJ, Alpert, NM, Thompson, WL, Rauch, SL, Macklin, ML, Pitman, RK (1997). Visual imagery and perception in posttraumatic stress disorder. A positron emission tomographic investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 233241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, LM, Liberzon, I (2010). The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 169191.Google Scholar
Shin, LM, Orr, SP, Carson, MA, Rauch, SL, Macklin, ML, Lasko, NB, Peters, PM, Metzger, LJ, Dougherty, DD, Cannistraro, PA, Alpert, NM, Fischman, AJ, Pitman, RK (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontalcortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 168176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straube, T, Glauer, M, Dilger, S, Mentzel, H-J, Miltner, WH (2006). Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on brain activation in specific phobia. Neuroimage 29, 125135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talairach, J, Tournoux, P (1988). Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Thieme: New York.Google Scholar
Tupak, SV, Dresler, T, Guhn, A, Ehlis, A-C, Fallgatter, AJ, Pauli, P, Herrmann, MJ (2014). Implicit emotion regulation in the presence of threat: neural and autonomic correlates. NeuroImage 85(Part 1), 372379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N, Landeau, B, Papathanassiou, D, Crivello, F, Etard, O, Delcroix, N, Mazoyer, B, Joliot, M (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273289.Google Scholar
Vertes, RP, Linley, SB, Hoover, WB (2015). Limbic circuitry of the midline thalamus. Neuroscience, Biobehavioral Reviews 54, 89107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wager, TD, Davidson, ML, Hughes, BL, Lindquist, MA, Ochsner, KN (2008). Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron 59, 10371050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, W, Hou, J, Qian, S, Liu, K, Li, B, Li, M, Peng, Z, Xin, K, Sun, G (2016). Aberrant regional neural fluctuations and functional connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder revealed by resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience Letters 624, 7884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, A, Cartwright-Hatton, S (2004). A short form of the metacognitions questionnaire: properties of the MCQ-30. Behaviour Research and Therapy 42, 385396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalen, PJ, Johnstone, T, Somerville, LH, Nitschke, JB, Polis, S, Alexander, AL, Davidson, RJ, Kalin, NH (2008). A functional magnetic resonance imaging predictor of treatment response to venlafaxine in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry 63, 858863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilensky, AE, Schafe, GE, Kristensen, MP, Ledoux, JE (2006). Rethinking the fear circuit: the central nucleus of the amygdala is required for the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience 26, 1238712396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H-U, Wunderlich, U, Gruschwitz, S, Zaudig, M (1997). SKID I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: psychische Störungen. Interviewheft und Beurteilungsheft. Eine deutschsprachige, erweiterte Bearb. d. amerikanischen Originalversion des SKID I. Z Fur Klin Psychol Psychother 28, 6870.Google Scholar
Yassa, MA, Hazlett, RL, Stark, CEL, Hoehn-Saric, R (2012). Functional MRI of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis during conditions of uncertainty in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 10451052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characterization of GAD patients and HC

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Mean ratings for disorder-related and neutral scripts on a nine-point Self-Assessment Manekin Scale. The 2 (script valence) by 2 (group) mixed model analysis of variance revealed interaction effects for anxiety and arousal ratings. Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) patients relative to healthy controls (HC) rated disorder-related scripts as more anxiety inducing and more arousing.

Figure 2

Table 2. Mean ratings per script valence on the dimensions of arousal, anxiety, valence and ability to imagine the scripts

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Brain responses to disorder-related v. neutral scripts. The region of interest analysis revealed for the contrast disorder-related > neutral scripts for generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) patients relative to healthy controls (HC) increased activity in the amygdala, thalamus, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and reduced activity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC)/ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Statistical parametric maps are overlaid on an averaged T1 scan (radiological convention: left = right). Graphs display contrasts of parameter estimates [disorder-related > neutral scripts (mean ± s.e. for activation cluster)] per group.