Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:49:27.482Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The dice are rolling for schizophrenia genetics

A commentary on ‘The emperors of the schizophrenia polygene have no clothes’ by Crow (2008)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 June 2008

P. F. Sullivan*
Affiliation:
Departments of Genetics, Psychiatry, & Epidemiology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, NC, USA Department of Medical Epidemiology & Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
*
*Address for correspondence: P. F. Sullivan, M.D., FRANZCP, Department of Genetics, CB#7264, 4109D Neurosciences Research Building, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, 27599-7264, USA. (Email: pfsulliv@med.unc.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Professor Tim Crow's impassioned review (Crow, Reference Crow2008) addresses the question at the heart of schizophrenia (Scz) genetics: a decade into the genomic era and after at least 28 genome-wide linkage studies (Konneker et al. in press), ~1300 association studies of ~700 candidate genes (Allen et al. in press), and three published genome-wide association studies (GWAS) (Lencz et al. Reference Lencz, Morgan, Athanasiou, Dain, Reed, Kane, Kucherlapati and Malhotra2007, Shifman et al. Reference Shifman, Johannesson, Bronstein, Chen, Collier, Craddock, Kendler, Li, O'Donovan, O'Neill, Owen, Walsh, Weinberger, Sun, Flint and Darvasi2008, Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Lin, Tzeng, van den Oord, Perkins, Stroup, Wagner, Lee, Wright, Zou, Liu, Downing, Lieberman and Close2008), what do we truly know about the genetics of Scz?

The initial portion of Crow's review is reasonable, but the latter portion is premature. As GWAS data for ~12 000 subjects with Scz and ~14 000 controls will become available for meta-analysis by the end of 2008, it is far preferable for these data to be interpreted within a logical and systematic framework [Psychiatric GWAS Consortium (PGC), in press]. It is crucial that this passionately debated area regains an element of dispassionate logic instead of serving as a projective canvas for belief, pet theories, and musings about the nature of Scz. In effect, the GWAS dice are rolling and dice are famously disinterested in the cogitations of punters.

I agree with Crow that the current short answer about what we know for certain about the genetics of Scz is ‘not much’. (I would take exception with some of Crow's scholarship – some reviews cited as examples of hype are more balanced and careful than indicated.) At present, based on published data, there are no genomic regions implicated by linkage or association with clean, clear, and cogent replication evidence for Scz (particularly when negative results for 14 prominent Scz candidate genes are included) (Sanders et al. Reference Sanders, Duan, Levinson, Shi, He, Hou, Burrell, Rice, Nertney, Olincy, Rozic, Buccola, Mowry, Freedman, Amin, Black, Silverman, Byerley, Crowe, Cloninger, Martinez and Gejman2008). The only sure way to make the existing Scz association results consistent across studies is to invoke more complex models of disease. It is possible that empirical evidence will eventually demonstrate that such models are operative for Scz. However, these more complex models are hardly parsimonious and are inconsistent with over 100 findings in human complex trait genetics where simple models were confirmed. The possibility that all reported associations for Scz are false has not been excluded.

I agree with Crow the evidential standard used by many in the field of Scz genetics is inadequate: the all-too-common use of lax standards of evidence has led to confusion rather than clarity. A standard for ‘replication’ has been defined for association studies (Chanock et al. Reference Chanock, Manolio, Boehnke, Boerwinkle, Hunter, Thomas, Hirschhorn, Abecasis, Altshuler, Bailey-Wilson, Brooks, Cardon, Daly, Donnelly, Fraumeni, Freimer, Gerhard, Gunter, Guttmacher, Guyer, Harris, Hoh, Hoover, Kong, Merikangas, Morton, Palmer, Phimister, Rice, Roberts, Rotimi, Tucker, Vogan, Wacholder, Wijsman, Winn and Collins2007, Box 3) and appears to be widely used in human genetics. The poster child for replication in human genetics is the FTO–body mass index association in which replication was reported in 13 cohorts with 38 759 participants, all in the same paper (Frayling et al. Reference Frayling, Timpson, Weedon, Zeggini, Freathy, Lindgren, Perry, Elliott, Lango, Rayner, Shields, Harries, Barrett, Ellard, Groves, Knight, Patch, Ness, Ebrahim, Lawlor, Ring, Ben-Shlomo, Jarvelin, Sovio, Bennett, Melzer, Ferrucci, Loos, Barroso, Wareham, Karpe, Owen, Cardon, Walker, Hitman, Palmer, Doney, Morris, Davey-Smith, Hattersley and McCarthy2007). Obviously, the standard for replication in Scz genetics should conform to that used in human genetics. The definition of replication in human genetic association studies (Chanock et al. Reference Chanock, Manolio, Boehnke, Boerwinkle, Hunter, Thomas, Hirschhorn, Abecasis, Altshuler, Bailey-Wilson, Brooks, Cardon, Daly, Donnelly, Fraumeni, Freimer, Gerhard, Gunter, Guttmacher, Guyer, Harris, Hoh, Hoover, Kong, Merikangas, Morton, Palmer, Phimister, Rice, Roberts, Rotimi, Tucker, Vogan, Wacholder, Wijsman, Winn and Collins2007) is akin to the definition of guilt in a criminal trial in US jurisprudence (‘proof beyond a reasonable doubt’, PBARD). The definition often applied in psychiatric genetics (and by some of the reviewers Crow pillories) is lesser, and akin that used in a civil case (‘preponderance of evidence’, POE). This distinction resolves the ‘OJ Paradox’ – the US gridiron star Orenthal James Simpson was found not guilty of the murder of his wife and her lover but liable for wrongful death because the POE standard was met but not PBARD.

Researchers who apply POE-like standards of evidence often have the best of intentions. The rationale usually has two components: (a) due to inadequate sample sizes, signals that do not quite meet a PBARD-like standard must be taken seriously to avoid erroneous rejection of true positive signals, or (b) the informal Bayesian argument that signals that do not meet a PBARD-like standard but which have a priori evidence of involvement in Scz (e.g. via neuroscience or molecular biology) should be elevated in importance. A straightforward simulation study has highlighted the perils of these strategies (Sullivan, Reference Sullivan2007). For Scz genetics to move forward, genomic regions associated with Scz must be ‘convicted’ at a PBARD-like level of evidence. Use of POE-like standards simply has not worked. For example, the initial descriptions of the associations of DTNBP1 (Straub et al. Reference Straub, Jiang, MacLean, Ma, Webb, Myakishev, Harris-Kerr, Wormley, Sadek, Kadambi, Cesare, Gibberman, Wang, O'Neill, Walsh and Kendler2002) and NRG1 (Stefansson et al. Reference Stefansson, Sigurdsson, Steinthorsdottir, Bjornsdottir, Sigmundsson, Ghosh, Brynjolfsson, Gunnarsdottir, Ivarsson, Chou, Hjaltason, Birgisdottir, Jonsson, Gudnadottir, Gudmundsdottir, Bjornsson, Ingvarsson, Ingason, Sigfusson, Hardardottir, Harvey, Lai, Zhou, Brunner, Mutel, Gonzalo, Lemke, Sainz, Johannesson, Andresson, Gudbjartsson, Manolescu, Frigge, Gurney, Kong, Gulcher, Petursson and Stefansson2002) with Scz were published 6 years ago – it is highly disappointing that neither association has been compellingly proven or disproved and that belief-influenced arguments are required rather than reliance on empirical data.

Two of Crow's conclusions are unquestionably premature. First, based on a small number of talks given at the World Congress of Psychiatric Genetics meeting in October 2007, Crow appears to conclude that GWAS has not and will not work for Scz. This conclusion is difficult to understand given that GWAS have yielded over 100 confirmed findings in cardiology, endocrinology, gastroenterology, hepatology, infectious disease, oncology, ophthalmology, neurology, pulmonology, and rheumatology (usually at PBARD-like levels of evidence). GWAS unquestionably can identify confirmed associations for human complex traits; the question is will GWAS ‘work’ for psychiatry.

The disease that arguably has benefited most in the GWAS era is type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM) where there are now around 15 confirmed associations (up from three such associations in 2006). Two of the initial T2DM GWAS were completely unremarkable (Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Voight, Lyssenko, Burtt, de Bakker, Chen, Roix, Kathiresan, Hirschhorn, Daly, Hughes, Groop, Altshuler, Almgren, Florez, Meyer, Ardlie, Bengtsson, Isomaa, Lettre, Lindblad, Lyon, Melander, Newton-Cheh, Nilsson, Orho-Melander, Rastam, Speliotes, Taskinen, Tuomi, Guiducci, Berglund, Carlson, Gianniny, Hackett, Hall, Holmkvist, Laurila, Sjogren, Sterner, Surti, Svensson, Tewhey, Blumenstiel, Parkin, Defelice, Barry, Brodeur, Camarata, Chia, Fava, Gibbons, Handsaker, Healy, Nguyen, Gates, Sougnez, Gage, Nizzari, Gabriel, Chirn, Ma, Parikh, Richardson, Ricke and Purcell2007; Scott et al. Reference Scott, Mohlke, Bonnycastle, Willer, Li, Duren, Erdos, Stringham, Chines, Jackson, Prokunina-Olsson, Ding, Swift, Narisu, Hu, Pruim, Xiao, Li, Conneely, Riebow, Sprau, Tong, White, Hetrick, Barnhart, Bark, Goldstein, Watkins, Xiang, Saramies, Buchanan, Watanabe, Valle, Kinnunen, Abecasis, Pugh, Doheny, Bergman, Tuomilehto, Collins and Boehnke2007), and only with meta-analysis did numerous positive findings emerge (indeed, multiple associations that ultimately replicated well were not in the top 1000 signals in the initial study) (Frayling, Reference Frayling2007). Crow's conclusion is premature until meta-analyses have been done.

The PGC was formed in early 2007 to conduct GWAS meta-analyses and now has 101 investigators from 48 institutions in 11 countries. There are 47 GWAS samples that, taken together, constitute the largest biological experiment ever conducted in psychiatry – over 80 000 subjects (59 000 independent cases/controls and over 7700 family trios), ~50 0000 SNP genotypes per subject, and ~40 billion total genotypes. The overall philosophy of the PGC is to be as inclusive, democratic, and rapid as possible. The PGC is well underway with a coordinating committee, five disease working groups, a cross-disorder group, a statistical analysis and computation group, and a cluster computer for data warehousing and statistical analysis. There are two main specific aims: (1) to conduct within-disorder meta-analyses (i.e. separately on all available GWAS data for ADHD, autism, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, and Scz) to attempt to identify convincing genotype–phenotype associations, and (2) to conduct cross-disorder analyses to identify genetic associations that cut across the clinically derived DSM/ICD disease boundaries. The analytical plans conform to current best practices for GWAS quality control and meta-analysis, particularly in attention to sources of heterogeneity. Statistical power will be superior to any prior study in psychiatric genetics. Results will be made available as soon as possible, probably in late 2008 or early 2009 (http://pgc.unc.edu).

Second, Crow uses his impression of the ‘failure’ of Scz GWAS efforts as supporting a theory of an epigenetic basis for Scz. This conclusion is doubly premature as there are multiple genetic models that might be operative should a polygenic model be rigorously shown not to apply to Scz. Ironically, an epigenetic hypothesis for Scz may be difficult to falsify due to technical limitations in assessing epigenetic marks on a genomic scale and because of the plausible requirement for inaccessible tissue (i.e. discrete portions of the brain) sampled at critical developmental periods well before a psychotic prodrome becomes evident clinically.

As articulated more fully elsewhere (PGC, in press), debates about the ‘success’ or ‘failure’ of GWAS for Scz miss the point. Indeed, the PGC is an exceptional opportunity in the history of psychiatry. No matter what the findings, we should uncover some certainties about the genetic architecture of Scz. Formulated differently, the massive efforts that comprise the PGC are a large-scale test of an over-arching ‘meta-hypothesis’ about the etiology of the most important psychiatric disorders. This latter point is exceptional – both positive and negative results will enable us to learn hard facts about ADHD, autism, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, and Scz.

The potential outcomes of PGC meta-analyses for Scz can logically be viewed as a set of sequential hypotheses. (1) It is possible that GWAS meta-analysis for Scz identifies a genomic region that is strongly significant after careful consideration of technical artifacts and correction for multiple comparisons. This would constitute an historical advance, and it is difficult to over-emphasize how important such a landmark would be to patients, families, clinicians, and researchers. As GWAS power is optimal under a polygenic model, it is most likely that any such identified variant would be relatively common and confer modest risk. (2) It is possible that rarer copy number variants of strong effect could be identified; indeed, there are some early data that these may be the first fruits of the GWAS era for psychiatric genetics (Walsh et al. Reference Walsh, McClellan, McCarthy, Addington, Pierce, Cooper, Nord, Kusenda, Malhotra, Bhandari, Stray, Rippey, Roccanova, Makarov, Lakshmi, Findling, Sikich, Stromberg, Merriman, Gogtay, Butler, Eckstrand, Noory, Gochman, Long, Chen, Davis, Baker, Eichler, Meltzer, Nelson, Singleton, Lee, Rapoport, King and Sebat2008; Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Shen, Korn, Arking, Miller, Fossdal, Saemundsen, Stefansson, Ferreira, Green, Platt, Ruderfer, Walsh, Altshuler, Chakravarti, Tanzi, Stefansson, Santangelo, Gusella, Sklar, Wu and Daly2008). These first two options are not mutually exclusive. (3) If no replicated associations are found, the available sample sizes may be too small – a true genetic effect may be present but statistical power is insufficient. Efforts to efficiently obtain considerably larger samples for Scz are underway as there currently are too few Scz samples in the world. (4) Assumptions that Scz is a distinct disease and that clinical heterogeneity is ignorable may be incorrect, and careful cross-disorder analyses could prove informative (as are planned in the PGC). (5) A polygenic model may be correct for Scz, but the salient regions of the genome may not be adequately covered in a GWAS. For example, after quality control, one GWAS had no genetic markers in several key genes important in dopaminergic neurotransmission (DRD1, DRD4, and TH) (Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Lin, Tzeng, van den Oord, Perkins, Stroup, Wagner, Lee, Wright, Zou, Liu, Downing, Lieberman and Close2008). More complete coverage requires the next generation of GWAS platforms. (6) Genetic variation might function solely via a more complex genetic model – higher order interactions (e.g. gene–gene or gene–environment interactions) instead of main effects. Explicitly modeling interactions might prove informative. (7) As Crow believes, a polygenic model might be incorrect for Scz and there might be a large number of different but individually rare risk variants each with strong effect. The next generation of genomic technology (individual genome sequencing) might be needed to resolve this model. (8) Finally, GWAS tests the simplest conceivable model of how a genetic variant might cause a phenotype like Scz. Alternative models are certainly possible – these include the epigenetic model favored by Crow and perhaps even mechanisms not yet described.

It is possible that Crow's assessment of the dire state of Scz genetics is correct. However, elements of his argument have not been rigorously evaluated. It is sensible to thoroughly test and reject simple models before moving to more complex possibilities. Within the year, a massive bolus of data should be available for GWAS meta-analysis. Evidence from these meta-analyses could transform the field or add more modestly to our knowledge base about Scz. Either way, we will learn.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Allen, N, Bagade, S, McQueen, M, Ioannidis, J, Kavvoura, F, Khoury, M, Tanzi, R, Bertram, L (in press). Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies in schizophrenia: the szgene database. Nature Genetics.Google Scholar
Chanock, SJ, Manolio, T, Boehnke, M, Boerwinkle, E, Hunter, DJ, Thomas, G, Hirschhorn, JN, Abecasis, G, Altshuler, D, Bailey-Wilson, JE, Brooks, LD, Cardon, LR, Daly, M, Donnelly, P, Fraumeni, JF Jr., Freimer, NB, Gerhard, DS, Gunter, C, Guttmacher, AE, Guyer, MS, Harris, EL, Hoh, J, Hoover, R, Kong, CA, Merikangas, KR, Morton, CC, Palmer, LJ, Phimister, EG, Rice, JP, Roberts, J, Rotimi, C, Tucker, MA, Vogan, KJ, Wacholder, S, Wijsman, EM, Winn, DM, Collins, FS (2007). Replicating genotype-phenotype associations. Nature 447, 655660.Google ScholarPubMed
Crow, TJ (2008). The emperors of the schizophrenia polygene have no clothes. Psychological Medicine. Published online: 21 April 2008. doi:10.1017/S0033291708003395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frayling, TM (2007). Genome-wide association studies provide new insights into type 2 diabetes aetiology. Nature Reviews Genetics 8, 657662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frayling, TM, Timpson, NJ, Weedon, MN, Zeggini, E, Freathy, RM, Lindgren, CM, Perry, JR, Elliott, KS, Lango, H, Rayner, NW, Shields, B, Harries, LW, Barrett, JC, Ellard, S, Groves, CJ, Knight, B, Patch, AM, Ness, AR, Ebrahim, S, Lawlor, DA, Ring, SM, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Jarvelin, MR, Sovio, U, Bennett, AJ, Melzer, D, Ferrucci, L, Loos, RJ, Barroso, I, Wareham, NJ, Karpe, F, Owen, KR, Cardon, LR, Walker, M, Hitman, GA, Palmer, CN, Doney, AS, Morris, AD, Davey-Smith, G, Hattersley, AT, McCarthy, MI (2007). A common variant in the fto gene is associated with body mass index and predisposes to childhood and adult obesity. Science 316, 889894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konneker, T, Barnes, T, Furberg, H, Losh, M, Bulik, CM, Sullivan, PF (in press). A searchable database of genetic evidence for psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics).Google Scholar
Lencz, T, Morgan, TV, Athanasiou, M, Dain, B, Reed, CR, Kane, JM, Kucherlapati, R, Malhotra, AK (2007). Converging evidence for a pseudoautosomal cytokine receptor gene locus in schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 12, 572580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychiatric GWAS Consortium (PGC) (in press). A framework for interpreting genomewide association studies of psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Sanders, A, Duan, J, Levinson, DF, Shi, J, He, D, Hou, C, Burrell, GJ, Rice, JP, Nertney, DA, Olincy, A, Rozic, P, Buccola, NG, Mowry, BJ, Freedman, R, Amin, F, Black, DW, Silverman, JM, Byerley, WF, Crowe, RR, Cloninger, CR, Martinez, M, Gejman, P (2008). No significant association of fourteen candidate genes to schizophrenia in a large european-ancestry sample: implications for the field. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 497506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxena, R, Voight, BF, Lyssenko, V, Burtt, NP, de Bakker, PI, Chen, H, Roix, JJ, Kathiresan, S, Hirschhorn, JN, Daly, MJ, Hughes, TE, Groop, L, Altshuler, D, Almgren, P, Florez, JC, Meyer, J, Ardlie, K, Bengtsson, K, Isomaa, B, Lettre, G, Lindblad, U, Lyon, HN, Melander, O, Newton-Cheh, C, Nilsson, P, Orho-Melander, M, Rastam, L, Speliotes, EK, Taskinen, MR, Tuomi, T, Guiducci, C, Berglund, A, Carlson, J, Gianniny, L, Hackett, R, Hall, L, Holmkvist, J, Laurila, E, Sjogren, M, Sterner, M, Surti, A, Svensson, M, Tewhey, R, Blumenstiel, B, Parkin, M, Defelice, M, Barry, R, Brodeur, W, Camarata, J, Chia, N, Fava, M, Gibbons, J, Handsaker, B, Healy, C, Nguyen, K, Gates, C, Sougnez, C, Gage, D, Nizzari, M, Gabriel, SB, Chirn, GW, Ma, Q, Parikh, H, Richardson, D, Ricke, D, Purcell, S (2007). Genome-wide association analysis identifies loci for type 2 diabetes and triglyceride levels. Science 316, 13311336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, LJ, Mohlke, KL, Bonnycastle, LL, Willer, CJ, Li, Y, Duren, WL, Erdos, MR, Stringham, HM, Chines, PS, Jackson, AU, Prokunina-Olsson, L, Ding, CJ, Swift, AJ, Narisu, N, Hu, T, Pruim, R, Xiao, R, Li, XY, Conneely, KN, Riebow, NL, Sprau, AG, Tong, M, White, PP, Hetrick, KN, Barnhart, MW, Bark, CW, Goldstein, JL, Watkins, L, Xiang, F, Saramies, J, Buchanan, TA, Watanabe, RM, Valle, TT, Kinnunen, L, Abecasis, GR, Pugh, EW, Doheny, KF, Bergman, RN, Tuomilehto, J, Collins, FS, Boehnke, M (2007). A genome-wide association study of type 2 diabetes in Finns detects multiple susceptibility variants. Science 316, 13411345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shifman, S, Johannesson, M, Bronstein, M, Chen, SX, Collier, DA, Craddock, NJ, Kendler, KS, Li, T, O'Donovan, M, O'Neill, FA, Owen, MJ, Walsh, D, Weinberger, DR, Sun, C, Flint, J, Darvasi, A (2008). Genome-wide association identifies a common variant in the reelin gene that increases the risk of schizophrenia only in women. PLoS Genetics 4, e28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefansson, H, Sigurdsson, E, Steinthorsdottir, V, Bjornsdottir, S, Sigmundsson, T, Ghosh, S, Brynjolfsson, J, Gunnarsdottir, S, Ivarsson, O, Chou, TT, Hjaltason, O, Birgisdottir, B, Jonsson, H, Gudnadottir, VG, Gudmundsdottir, E, Bjornsson, A, Ingvarsson, B, Ingason, A, Sigfusson, S, Hardardottir, H, Harvey, RP, Lai, D, Zhou, M, Brunner, D, Mutel, V, Gonzalo, A, Lemke, G, Sainz, J, Johannesson, G, Andresson, T, Gudbjartsson, D, Manolescu, A, Frigge, ML, Gurney, ME, Kong, A, Gulcher, JR, Petursson, H, Stefansson, K (2002). Neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to schizophrenia. American Journal of Human Genetics 71, 877892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Straub, RE, Jiang, Y, MacLean, CJ, Ma, Y, Webb, BT, Myakishev, MV, Harris-Kerr, C, Wormley, B, Sadek, H, Kadambi, B, Cesare, AJ, Gibberman, A, Wang, X, O'Neill, FA, Walsh, D, Kendler, KS (2002). Genetic variation in the 6p22.3 gene dtnbp1, the human ortholog of the mouse dysbindin gene, is associated with schizophrenia. American Journal of Human Genetics 71, 337348. [Erratum in American Journal of Human Genetics (2002), 72, 1007.]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, PF (2007). Spurious genetic associations. Biological Psychiatry 61, 11211126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, PF, Lin, D, Tzeng, JY, van den Oord, EJCG, Perkins, D, Stroup, TS, Wagner, M, Lee, S, Wright, FA, Zou, F, Liu, W, Downing, AM, Lieberman, JA, Close, SL (2008). Genomewide association for schizophrenia in the CATIE study: results of stage 1. Molecular Psychiatry 13, 570584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, T, McClellan, JM, McCarthy, SE, Addington, AM, Pierce, SB, Cooper, GM, Nord, AS, Kusenda, M, Malhotra, D, Bhandari, A, Stray, SM, Rippey, CF, Roccanova, P, Makarov, V, Lakshmi, B, Findling, RL, Sikich, L, Stromberg, T, Merriman, B, Gogtay, N, Butler, P, Eckstrand, K, Noory, L, Gochman, P, Long, R, Chen, Z, Davis, S, Baker, C, Eichler, EE, Meltzer, PS, Nelson, SF, Singleton, AB, Lee, MK, Rapoport, JL, King, MC, Sebat, J (2008). Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science 320, 539543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, LA, Shen, Y, Korn, JM, Arking, DE, Miller, DT, Fossdal, R, Saemundsen, E, Stefansson, H, Ferreira, MA, Green, T, Platt, OS, Ruderfer, DM, Walsh, CA, Altshuler, D, Chakravarti, A, Tanzi, RE, Stefansson, K, Santangelo, SL, Gusella, JF, Sklar, P, Wu, BL, Daly, MJ (2008). Association between microdeletion and microduplication at 16p11.2 and autism. New England Journal of Medicine 358, 667675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed