Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T10:26:37.384Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contrasting patterns of deficits in visuospatial memory and executive function in patients with major depression with and without ECT referral

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 August 2010

E. Tsaltas*
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece
S. Kalogerakou
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece
V.-M. Papakosta
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece
D. Kontis
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece 1st Department of Psychiatry, Psychiatric Hospital of Attica, Athens, Greece
E. Theochari
Affiliation:
1st Department of Psychiatry, Psychiatric Hospital of Attica, Athens, Greece
M. Koutroumpi
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece
E. Anyfandi
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece
I. Michopoulos
Affiliation:
2nd Department of Psychiatry, Atticon Hospital, Athens University Medical School, Athens, Greece
C. Poulopoulou
Affiliation:
Experimental Neurophysiology Laboratory, Department of Neurology, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece
G. Papadimitriou
Affiliation:
1st Department of Psychiatry, Psychiatric Hospital of Attica, Athens, Greece
P. Oulis
Affiliation:
1st Department of Psychiatry, Psychiatric Hospital of Attica, Athens, Greece
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr E. Tsaltas, D.Phil., Experimental Psychologist, Assistant Professor in Psychiatry, Experimental Psychology Laboratory, 1st Department of Psychiatry, Athens University Medical School, Eginition Hospital, 74, Vas. Sofias Ave., 115 28 Athens, Greece. (Email: tsaltasl@med.uoa.gr)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The pretreatment neuropsychological profile of drug-resistant patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) referred for electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) may differ from that of their drug-respondent MDD counterparts. Such differences could help in identifying distinct MDD subtypes, thus offering insights into the neuropathology underlying differential treatment responses.

Method

Depressed patients with ECT referral (ECTs), depressed patients with no ECT referral (NECTs) and non-psychiatric Controls (matched groups, n=15) were assessed with memory and executive function tests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB).

Results

ECTs scored significantly lower than NECTs in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; p=0.01). NECTs performed worse than Controls in the Paired Associates Learning (PAL) task (p<0.03; Control/NECT p<0.01) and the Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM) task (p<0.05; Controls/NECTs p<0.05); ECTs performed between Controls and NECTs, not differing from either. In the Intra/Extradimensional (IED) set-shifting task, ECTs performed worse that Controls and NECTS (IED: p<0.01; Controls/ECTs p<0.01), particularly in the shift phases, which suggests reduced attentional flexibility. In Stockings of Cambridge (SOC), ECTs abandoned the test early more often than Controls and NECTs (H=11, p<0.01) but ECTs who completed SOC performed comparably to the other two groups.

Conclusions

A double dissociation emerged from the comparison of cognitive profiles of ECT and NECT patients. ECTs showed executive deficits, particularly in attentional flexibility, but mild deficits in tests of visuospatial memory. NECTs presented the opposite pattern. This suggests predominantly frontostriatal involvement in ECT versus temporal involvement in NECT depressives.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Introduction

Despite significant advances in therapeutics, almost half of patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) fail to show a satisfactory response to treatment (Rush et al. Reference Rush, Trivedi, Wisniewski, Nierenberg, Stewart, Warden, Niederehe, Thase, Lavori, Lebowitz, McGrath, Rosenbaum, Sackeim, Kupfer, Luther and Fava2006), and the prediction of treatment outcome remains unsatisfactory. Neurobiological predictors based on functional neuroimaging hold promise, but have not yet produced useful treatment algorithms for individual patients (Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003; Konarski et al. Reference Konarski, Kennedy, Segal, Lau, Bieling, McIntyre and Mayberg2009; Li et al. Reference Li, Lin, Chou, Chen, Hsieh, Wu, Lin and Su2010). Prediction based on clinical features also remains problematic (Joyce & Paykel, Reference Joyce and Paykel1989). However, literature reviews suggest that a careful account of MDD phenotypes encompassing neuropsychological profiling may hold promise (Porter et al. Reference Porter, Bourke and Gallagher2007; Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009).

In line with this view, the purpose of the current study was to investigate the neuropsychological underpinnings of treatment-resistant/refractory depression. However, the conceptual and operational definitions of pharmacoresistant depression remain divergent to date (Berlim & Turecki, Reference Berlim and Turecki2007). We therefore added the criterion of referral for electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) to the conventional criteria of pharmacoresistance used in the majority of controlled trials (Berlim & Turecki, Reference Berlim and Turecki2007). ECT is an effective treatment of MDD (APA, 2001), leading to a reduction in depression scores of >60% (Lerer et al. Reference Lerer, Shapira, Calev, Tubi, Drexler, Kindler, Lidsky and Schwartz1995; Sackeim et al. Reference Sackeim, Prudic, Devanand, Nobler, Lisanby, Peyser, Fitzsimons, Moody and Clark2000; McCall et al. Reference McCall, Dunn and Rosenquist2004; Falconer et al. Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010). Because reservations regarding the side-effects of ECT have restricted its use to pharmacoresistant MDD, it is reasonable to consider ECT referral as a stringent criterion of pharmacoresistance.

A second reason for focusing on MDD patients with ECT referral was precisely the reservations regarding ECT use. These arise from reported adverse effects on cognition (Rami-Gonzajez et al. Reference Rami-Gonzajez, Bernardo, Boget, Gil-Verona, Salamero and Junque2001; UK ECT Review Group, 2003; Robertson & Pryor, Reference Robertson and Pryor2006), although controlled trial evidence on ECT-related, lasting cognitive deficits is very limited (Halliday et al. Reference Halliday, Davidson, Browne and Kreeger1968; Ng et al. Reference Ng, Schweitzer, Alexopoulos, Celi, Wong, Tuckwell, Sergejew and Tiller2000; Sackeim et al. Reference Sackeim, Prudic, Fuller, Keilp, Lavori and Olfson2007). Accordingly, recent reviews have concluded that the degree of cognitive impairment residual to ECT cannot be estimated reliably at present (UK ECT Review Group, 2003; Robertson & Pryor, Reference Robertson and Pryor2006; Falconer et al. Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010). The definitive evaluation of cognitive deficits caused by ECT therefore calls for further research, which must take account of two methodological issues.

The first issue is the use of inadequate neuropsychological instruments in previous studies. Global neuropsychological tests such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al. Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) lack the sensitivity to circumscribe memory deficits. More specialized tests such as the Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (AVLT; Rey, Reference Rey1964) and various forms of paired associates with short retention intervals lack the flexibility to detect the nature and level of ECT-induced cognitive impairments (Goldstein et al. Reference Goldstein, Filskov and Weaver1977; Robertson & Pryor, Reference Robertson and Pryor2006; Falconer et al. Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010). It has therefore been recommended that future ECT evaluation should be based on neuropsychological batteries offering the resolution needed for testing patients with a suspected history of brain injury or disease (Robertson & Pryor, Reference Robertson and Pryor2006). A recent study (Falconer et al. Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010) contributed to the resolution of this issue by introducing the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) in the evaluation of ECT. CANTAB is highly appropriate as it detects and differentiates frontal from temporal and amygdalo-hippocampal dysfunction (Sahakian et al. Reference Sahakian, Downes, Eagger, Evenden, Levy, Philpot, Roberts and Robbins1990; Sahgal et al. Reference Sahgal, Sahakian, Robbins, Wray, Lloyd, Cook, McKeith, Disley, Eagger, Boddington and Edwardson1991; Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, James, Owen, Sahakian, McInnes and Rabbitt1994; Lange et al. Reference Lange, Sahakian, Quinn, Marsden and Robbins1995; Owen et al. Reference Owen, Sahakian, Semple, Polkey and Robbins1995, Reference Owen, Morris, Sahakian, Polkey and Robbins1996, Reference Owen, Iddon, Hodges, Summers and Robbins1997; Fowler et al. Reference Fowler, Saling, Conway, Semple and Louis1997; Rahman et al. Reference Rahman, Robbins and Sahakian1999; Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009). It is also sensitive to deficits associated with depression (Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Sahakian, McKay, Herrod, Robbins and Paykel1996; Porter et al. Reference Porter, Gallagher, Thompson and Young2003; Barnett et al. Reference Barnett, Sahakian, Werners, Hill, Brazil, Gallagher, Bullmore and Jones2005; Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009). Using CANTAB, Falconer et al. (Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010) indeed noted a spatial memory impairment 1 month post-ECT. As the study is one of few to detect anterograde memory loss more than 2 weeks post-ECT, the finding suggests that CANTAB is a sensitive instrument for evaluating the cognitive effects of ECT on aspects of memory.

The second methodological issue hindering the assessment of ECT-related cognitive deficits is that those can only be assessed against a ‘baseline’ of depression, which itself produces such deficits (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009). Some of these deficits reportedly persist after recovery with treatment other than ECT (Reischies & Neu, Reference Reischies and Neu2000; Neu et al. Reference Neu, Kiesslinger, Schlattmann and Reischies2001; Steffens et al. Reference Steffens, Welsh-Bohmer, Burke, Plassman, Beyer, Gersing and Potter2004; Biringer et al. Reference Biringer, Lundervold, Stordal, Mykletun, Egeland, Bottlender and Lund2005; Clark et al. Reference Clark, Sarna and Goodwin2005; Paelecke-Habermann et al. Reference Paelecke-Habermann, Pohl and Leplow2005; Nakano et al. Reference Nakano, Baba, Maeshima, Kitajima, Sakai, Baba, Suzuki, Mimura and Arai2008). Cognitive deficits residual to depression may be subsumed in deficits attributed to ECT, unless proper control procedures are used. Instruments developed to distinguish between depression- and ECT-related deficits, such as the Squire Memory Questionnaire (SMQ; Squire et al. Reference Squire, Wetzel and Slater1979), are of debatable usefulness (Robertson & Pryor, Reference Robertson and Pryor2006). To establish the sensitivity of a neuropsychological procedure such as CANTAB to ECT-related cognitive deficits, care must be taken to ensure that the CANTAB-based, pretreatment profile of ECT candidates does not differ from the corresponding neuropsychological profile of MDD patients of comparable severity who are drug respondent and therefore do not attract ECT referral. The study by Falconer et al. (Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010) did not address this issue.

The current study used CANTAB to compare the neuropsychological profile of MDD patients with ECT referral to the profiles of (a) matched MDD patients who did not attract ECT referral and (b) demographically matched non-psychiatric controls. Additionally, given that the cognitive deficits associated with both MDD and ECT are not restricted to memory but also include executive dysfunction, the study used a battery of CANTAB tests addressing visuospatial learning and memory (as Falconer et al. Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010) but also cognitive flexibility, spatial working memory and planning. These added tests access frontal function more directly and are particularly relevant given that the spatial memory deficit detected 1 month post-ECT by Falconer et al. (Reference Falconer, Cleland, Fielding and Reid2010) suggests frontal rather than temporal lobe impairment (Owen et al. Reference Owen, Sahakian, Semple, Polkey and Robbins1995).

The study hypothesis was that MDD pharmacoresistant ECT candidates, prior to treatment, may present a CANTAB-based neuropsychological profile different from that of matched, drug-respondent MDD patients and non-psychiatric controls. This may help to access the neuropsychological underpinnings of pharmacoresistant MDD, and possibly bear predictive potential in terms of MDD treatment outcome. If, however, no neuropsychological differences were detected, this would support the claim that CANTAB is an appropriate and sensitive instrument for the definitive evaluation of ECT-related cognitive deficits.

Method

Participants

All patients signed informed consent forms. The patients were female, as the study was carried out in the Women's Mental Health Unit of Eginition Hospital. All were diagnosed as having an episode in the context of MDD, according to DSM-IV-TR criteria (APA, 2000). Exclusion criteria were (a) ECT within the past 5 years, (b) co-morbid bipolar disorder, schizophrenia or schizo-affective disorder, (c) central nervous system disorders (dementia, Parkinson's, epilepsy, brain tumours, organic brain syndrome), (d) major medical illness (cerebrovascular disorders, serious endocrine disorders, malignancies), and (e) alcohol or other substance dependence. All patients were on psychotropic medication on admission (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants

MDD, Major depressive disorder; ECT, electroconvulsive treatment; MDD-NECT, MDD patients without ECT referral; MDD-ECT, MDD patients with ECT referral; SSRI, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor; SNRI, serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor; HAMD-24, 24-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination.

Values are given as mean±standard deviation or percentage.

Controls (n=15)

These were female volunteers matched for age and educational level with ECT candidates. Exclusion criteria were as above, plus a psychiatric history.

ECTs (n=15)

These were MDD patients with more than two episodes, referred for ECT. Three of them presented with mood-congruent psychotic features. As part of the standard pre-ECT evaluation procedure, upon admission, ECT referral was re-examined by two attending psychiatrists not participating in the study. Diagnosis was confirmed by the Standard Clinical Diagnostic Interview (SCID-I/P; First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams2002) and it was documented that ECTs had failed to respond to at least two courses of adequate dosages of antidepressant medications of different classes, for at least 6 weeks (the most frequently used definition of pharmacoresistant depression; Berlim & Turecki, Reference Berlim and Turecki2007). Severity of depression was assessed by the 24-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD-24; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960). As part of the procedure, ECT candidates underwent complete blood cell count, serum chemistry analysis, thyroid function tests, plasma cholinesterase measurement, chest radiography, electrocardiography, electro-encephalography (EEG), brain computed tomography, cardiovascular and neurological evaluations.

NECTs (n=15)

These were MDD patients with more than two episodes, assessed with SCID-I/P and HAMD-24. All were being evaluated for admission to the Eginition Women's Mental Health Unit. None had received ECT referral. They were matched with ECT candidates for age, education and age of illness onset.

Instruments

Psychometric scales

Severity of depression of ECTs and NECTs was assessed by the HAMD-24. Global cognitive functioning was assessed by the MMSE.

Neuropsychological tests

A five-test CANTAB battery was used (60–70 min duration). Testing began with a motor screening test (MOT) introducing subjects to the touch screen, while assessing difficulties in vision, movement or comprehension. Two visuospatial memory tasks [Paired Associates Learning (PAL) and Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM)] and two executive function tasks [Intra/Extradimensional (IED) set shifting and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC)] followed, in the same order for all participants. Test summaries are given below (see also Owen et al. Reference Owen, Sahakian, Semple, Polkey and Robbins1995).

PAL assesses the ability to form associations between shapes and their locations on the screen (visual learning–memory). Subjects must indicate the square in which each shape was previously presented, starting with one shape in one of six squares, and ending with eight shapes in eight squares. PAL is sensitive to changes in medial temporal lobe functioning and discriminates between early Alzheimer's disease and depression (Swainson et al. Reference Swainson, Hodges, Galton, Semple, Michael, Dunn, Iddon, Robbins and Sahakian2001).

SRM tests recognition memory as the ability to recognize the correct spatial location of a square presented on five occasions sequentially, followed by a paired series of novel and previously shown squares. SRM is primarily sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction and relatively insensitive to temporal lobe damage (Owen et al. Reference Owen, Sahakian, Semple, Polkey and Robbins1995).

IED examines attentional flexibility. It includes visual discrimination acquisition and reversal, attentional set formation, maintenance and shifting. Its intra- and extradimensional shift phases are primarily sensitive to changes in frontostriatal function.

SOC tests spatial working memory and planning. It assesses the ability to rearrange a set of balls according to a sample in the minimum number of moves. SOC gives a measure of frontal lobe function.

Procedure

Psychometric and neuropsychological assessments were carried out by trained psychologists, blind as to the MDD patients' future treatment plan. CANTAB tests were administered according to CANTAB manual protocols, on a touch-sensitive screen. The CANTAB measures used are shown in Table 2. Testing took place between 09:00 and 15:00 hours. Controls were only subjected to neuropsychological assessment.

Table 2. Summary of results from individual CANTAB tests

CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery; ECT, electroconvulsive therapy; Controls, non-psychiatric control group; NECTs, depressed patients with no ECT referral; ECTs, depressed patients with ECT referral.

Values are given as mean (standard error).

Significant main effect

p<0.05; †† p<0.01; significant contrast with Controls

* p<0.05, ** p<0.01; [†], [*]: approaching significance: p<0.06–0.08.

Statistical analysis

The 1999 Statistica for Windows package, version 5.5 (Statsoft Inc., USA), was used.

Demographic and clinical data

Age and years of education were compared for the three groups by one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs). ECTs and NECTs were further compared with respect to illness history indices through one-way ANOVAs. Familiality and drugs received at intake were compared by χ2 tests. ECTs and NECTs were compared for HAMD-24 and MMSE scores by one-way ANOVAs.

Neuropsychological data

CANTAB datasets were examined for normality (the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). If the criterion was not met, data were transformed appropriately [log10 for latencies, √(x+0.5) for counts, arcsine (√x) for rates] and retested. Failing the criterion again, the dataset was analysed by Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA. On the other measures ECTs, NECTs and Controls were compared through one-way ANOVAs. Significant group effects were examined by contrast testing. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen's d (X-ECTs−X-NECTs)/[(SD-ECTs+SD-NECTs)/2].

Results

Sample characteristics

There were no significant group differences in demographics (Table 1 a). NECTs reported (non-significantly) more episodes of illness than ECTs (Table 1 b). ECTs had significantly more hospitalizations than NECTs (1.73±1.16 and 3.27±1.67; Mann–Whitney U test: n=15, z adjusted=2.72, p=0.007). A difference in pharmacotherapy approaching significance was noted for antipsychotics [χ2(df=1)=3.59, p=0.06] (Table 1 c).

Performance in psychometric scales (Table 1d)

ECTs had higher HAMD-24 scores than NECTs, a difference approaching significance [F(1, 28)=3.83, p<0.060]; it is noteworthy that HAMD-24 scores of ECTs were almost identical (31.93±6.45 v. 31.3±6.9) to those reported in one of the largest ECT trials (Sackeim et al. Reference Sackeim, Prudic, Fuller, Keilp, Lavori and Olfson2007). MMSE scores for ECTs were significantly compromised compared to those for NECTs [F(1, 28)=7.67, p<0.01].

Performance in neuropsychological tests

MOT

The three groups did not differ in MOT measures.

PAL (Table 2a)

The measure of adjusted total errors produced a significant group effect [F(2, 42)=3.84, p=0.03]. Contrast testing revealed a deficit of NECTs against Controls [Control/NECT: F(1, 42)=7.6, p<0.01]. ECTs did not differ from either group. When stage errors were examined (Fig. 1 a), a significant group effect was noted only at the eight-shape stage [F(2, 42)=4.52, p=0.02], reflecting a NECT deficit [Controls/NECTs: F(1, 42)=7.65, p<0.01]. The first trial memory score also yielded a significant group effect [F(2, 42)=4.06, p=0.03], reflecting a significant NECT deficit [Control/NECTs: F(1, 42)=7.65, p<0.01]. In both cases, ECTs performed intermediately, their difference from Controls verging on significance [Controls/ECTs respectively: F(1, 42)=3.80; F(1, 42)=3.90, p<0.06]. The total trials measure was significant [F(2, 42)=4.11, p=0.02; Fig. 1 b]. ECTs and NECTs were both inferior to Controls [F(1, 42): Controls/NECTs=7.59, p<0.01; Controls/ECTs=4.27, p<0.05].

Fig. 1. (a) In Paired Associates Learning (PAL), a significant group effect in total errors [F(2, 42)=3.84, p<0.05] was due to a deficit in depressed patients with no electroconvulsive treatment referral (NECTs) compared to Controls. Analysis of PAL stage errors indicated that this deficit was specific to the most demanding eight-shape PAL stage [F(2, 42)=4.52, p<0.05; Controls v. NECTs: F(1, 42)=8.73, p<0.005]. Depressed patients with ECT referral (ECTs) performed at an intermediate level, differing from neither Controls nor NECTs at any stage. (b) In the measure of adjusted total trials alone [F(2, 42)=4.11, p<0.05], both NECTs and ECTs performed significantly worse than Controls [F(1, 42): Controls v. NECTs=7.59, p<0.001; Controls v. ECTs=4.27, p<0.05]. Data shown are means and 0.95 confidence intervals; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01.

SRM (Table 2b)

The measure of the percentage correct responses yielded a group effect [F(2, 42)=3.32, p<0.05] due to a significant deficit of NECTs versus Controls [Control/NECT: F(1, 42)=6.10, p=0.02]. ECTs performed between Control and NECT levels.

IED (Table 2c)

The adjusted measures of total errors (Fig. 2 a) and total trials [respectively, F(2, 42)=4.91, p=0.01; F(2, 42)=5.01, p=0.01] yielded significant group effects. NECTs did not differ significantly from Controls whereas ECTs' performance was significantly compromised on both measures [Controls/ECTs respectively: F(1, 42)=8.40 and 8.11, p<0.01; NECTs/ECTs respectively: F(1, 42)=6.15, p<0.05; F(1, 42)=6.88, p=0.01]. The measure of Stages Completed gave the same pattern [Kruskal–Wallis: H (2, n=45)=8.34, p=0.02]. To determine whether the emerging ECT deficit was specific to discrete IED tasks (discrimination: stages 1+3+4; reversal: stages 2+5+7; set shifting: stages 6+8), separate one-way ANOVAS were carried out. The three groups performed comparably in discrimination and reversal learning. The group effect in the IED shift phases approached significance [F(2, 42)=2.84, p=0.07], again due to increased ECTs' errors (Fig. 2 b).

Fig. 2. (a) In the Intra/Extradimensional (IED) shift task, the adjusted measure of total errors yielded a significant group effect [F(2, 42): 4.91, p<0.01]. This was due to the compromised performance of depressive patients with electroconvulsive treatment referral (ECTs) compared to either Controls [Controls v. ECTs: F(1, 42)=8.40, p<0.01] or to depressive patients without ECT referral [NECTs: NECTs v. ECTs, F(1, 42)=6.15, p<0.05]. NECTs did not differ from Controls. (b) Analysis of errors in discrete IED tasks revealed normal performance of both ECTs and NECTs (p>0.15) in the acquisition of a simple discrimination (IED stages 1+3+4) and in discrimination reversal (IED stages 2+5+7). A deficit approaching significance was noted in IED [stages 6+8: F(2, 42)=2.84, p<0.07] due to increased errors in the ECT group. Data shown are means and 0.95 confidence intervals; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01.

SOC (Table 2d)

ECTs had a high drop-out rate in the early SOC stages (six drop-outs, four at the two-move, two at the three-move stage: patients refused to continue and SOC was manually interrupted), whereas all Controls and NECTs completed the SOC (Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA: H (2, n=45)=11.00, p=0.004). Given that once a subject dropped out she received a score of zero in subsequent stages, the high ECT drop-out rates affected the data so that subsequent analyses had to be restricted to measures of problems solved in minimum moves, global and stage specific (two-, three-, four- or five-move stages). A significant group effect emerged for total problems solved [F(2, 42)=3.55, p=0.04, Fig. 3]. Contrast testing showed a significant deficit in ECTs [Controls/ECTs: F(1, 42)=7.03, p<0.01]. NECTs performed at an intermediate level, differing from neither group. In the stage analyses (Fig. 3), significant effects were noted at the two-move [Kruskal–Wallis: H (2, n=45)=6.799, p=0.03] and three-move stages [F(2, 42)=9.63, p=0.001], where ECTs performed significantly worse than Controls and NECTs [F(1, 42), Controls/ECTs=16.46, NECTs/ECTs=12.10, p=0.001]. The three groups performed comparably at the more difficult four- and five-move stages, even though early ECT drop-outs were represented in the data by a score of zero.

Fig. 3. In Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) depressive patients with electroconvulsive treatment referral (ECTs) solved significantly fewer problems using minimum moves compared to Controls and depressive patients with no ECT referral (NECTs) [F(2, 42)=3.55, p<0.05; Controls v. ECTs: F(1, 42)=7.03, p<0.01]. The ECT deficit was restricted to the easier SOC stages of problems soluble in two [Kruskal–Wallis: H (2, n=45)=6.799, p=0.05] and three moves [F(2, 42)=9.63, p<0.001: Controls v. ECTs, F(1, 42)=16.46; NECTs v. ECTs F(1, 42)=12.10, p<0.001]. NECTS did not differ from Controls. The three groups did not differ in the more difficult stages of problems soluble in four and five moves. The early stages ECT deficit was probably due to significantly more early abandons of the test by ECTs compared to Controls and NECTs [Kruskal–Wallis: H (2, n=45)=11.00, p<0.001). Data shown are means and 0.95 confidence intervals; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01; Kruskal–Wallis: +p<0.05.

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to investigate whether the factor of ECT referral on grounds of pharmacoresistance would yield quantitative or qualitative neuropsychological differences in MDD patient groups. This could contribute to the neuropsychological phenotyping of MDD, which we suggest holds promise for prediction of treatment outcome. A second aim was to anchor the neuropsychological profile of MDD-ECT candidates to those of matched MDD-NECT patients and of non-psychiatric controls, using CANTAB. Thus, a conceptual baseline would be established, facilitating subsequent assessment of the therapeutic and/or adverse cognitive effects of ECT (Robertson & Pryor, Reference Robertson and Pryor2006).

Comparison of clinical profiles

ECT candidates provided the basis for selection of NECT and Control participants: our three groups were matched for age, education and general health status. The mean age was relatively low (48.56±11.25 years), and therefore effects of depression on neuropsychological functioning would not be expected to be as severe as those encountered in elderly depressives (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Griffiths, Mackinnon and Jacomb1997; Porter et al. Reference Porter, Bourke and Gallagher2007), obscuring group differences through floor effects. A shortcoming of our study in terms of generalizability of the results is its small sample size and its limitation to females. We opted for females for reasons of availability, but also to reduce variance, because evidence indicates gender differences in cognitive and affective functioning (Wager et al. Reference Wager, Phan, Liberzon and Taylor2003; Postma et al. Reference Postma, Jager, Kessels, Koppeschaar and van Honk2004). To minimize the impact of the small sample size we have provided effect sizes (Table 2) in addition to conventional statistics.

We were partly successful in matching our clinical groups for illness history (Table 1). Both groups consisted of patients undergoing episodes severe enough to warrant hospitalization. They were comparable in reported illness age of onset and duration, but differed in number of previous episodes and hospitalizations. NECTs reported (non-significantly) more episodes, but the fewer episodes of ECTs had led to significantly more hospitalizations. Hospitalizations, current and past, constitute a factor associated with increased severity of depression and of impairment in CANTAB tests, including IED (Purcell et al. Reference Purcell, Maruff, Kyrios and Pantelis1997), delayed matching to sample, spatial span and response to failure (Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Sahakian, McKay, Herrod, Robbins and Paykel1996).

All patients were receiving pharmacotherapy at the time of assessment (Table 1). NECTs were more frequently on ‘first-line’ pharmacotherapy with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and benzodiazepines, although several took serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) and/or atypical antipsychotics. ECTs were more likely to receive tricyclics or SNRIs, supplemented by atypical antipsychotics. This pattern, though not yielding significant differences in our groups, reflects the pharmacological resistance contributing to ECT referral; it is common to switch patients not showing improvement following an antidepressant course (usually an SSRI) to a different class (e.g. SNRI) with encouraging results (Papakostas et al. Reference Papakostas, Fava and Thase2008).

The possibility that differences in SNRIs or atypical antipsychotics may contribute to the neuropsychological differentiation of our MDD groups must be considered. Antidepressants differentially affect cognition depending on their receptor-binding profiles (Furlan et al. Reference Furlan, Kallan, Ten Have, Pollock, Katz and Lucki2001; Cassano et al. Reference Cassano, Puca, Scapicchio and Trabucchi2002; Riedel et al. Reference Riedel, Eikmans, Heldens and Schmitt2005; Gualtieri & Johnson, Reference Gualtieri and Johnson2007). However, SSRIs and SNRIs seem to have comparable, beneficial effects on mental processing speed and motor performance, with superior SNRI benefit in episodic and working memory (Herrera-Guzmán et al. Reference Herrera-Guzmán, Gudayol-Ferré, Herrera-Guzmán, Guàrdia-Olmos, Hinojosa-Calvo and Herrera-Abarca2009).

With respect to antipsychotics, there is evidence that some atypical ones simulate cognitive deficits in healthy individuals (Mehta et al. Reference Mehta, Sahakian, McKenna and Robbins1999). However, in schizophrenia, data strongly suggest that atypical ones in particular either have no effect or improve executive functioning (Meyer-Lindberg et al. Reference Meyer-Lindberg, Gruppe, Bauer, Lis, Krieger and Galhoffer1997; Burke et al. Reference Burke, Patel, Morris and Reveley1998; Cuesta et al. Reference Cuesta, Peralta and Zarzuela2001; Velligan et al. Reference Velligan, Newcomer, Pultz, Csernansky, Hoff, Mahurin and Miller2002; McGurk et al. Reference McGurk, Lee, Jayathilake and Meltzer2004; Mishara & Goldberg, Reference Mishara and Goldberg2004; Tyson et al. Reference Tyson, Laws, Roberts and Mortimer2004; Woodwart et al. Reference Woodward, Purdon, Meltzer and Zald2005; O'Grada & Dinan, Reference O'Grada and Dinan2007). In depression, data on the cognitive effects of antipsychotics are sparse but in the same direction (Olver et al. Reference Olver, Ignatiadis, Maruff, Burrows and Norman2008; Frasch et al. Reference Frasch, Bullacher, Jäger, Kilian, Rink, Wittek, Becker and Neumann2009).

Thus, the fact that MDD-ECT patients were receiving SNRIs and atypical antipsychotics in greater proportion than their MDD-NECT counterparts and still demonstrated an executive deficit strengthens rather than weakens our finding: the confounding effects of this difference would be expected to lead to an underestimate of executive deficits exhibited by MDD-ECT patients. However, the superior performance of ECTs versus NECTs in visuospatial memory tasks may be attributable to the factor of atypical antipsychotics, to the extent that these also seem to ameliorate performance in these tasks (Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Young, Rock, Purdon, Gold and Breier2006). It seems unlikely, however, that antipsychotics would concurrently ameliorate visuospatial memory and compromise executive deficits.

In summary, our MDD groups suffered recurrent depressive episodes for the same length of time in comparable life periods, but ECTs had a more serious illness course, including more hospitalizations and a broader range of pharmacological treatments. This suggests more severe, resistant depression in ECTs and was corroborated by higher HAMD-24 scores. Severity of depression influences certain aspects of neuropsychological performance (Porter et al. Reference Porter, Bourke and Gallagher2007). Therefore, a down-the-board CANTAB deficit of ECTs versus NECTs in our study would have been difficult to interpret, all the more so because MMSE global cognitive functioning was significantly compromised in ECTs. We did not, however, observe such uniform deficits.

Comparison of neuropsychological profiles

Given the greater severity of depression, the broader range of psychotropic medication and MMSE deficit in our ECT group, their CANTAB performance would be expected to be inferior to that of Controls and possibly of NECTs. Instead, a double dissociation emerged in the profiles of the two MDD groups. In the two memory tests ECTs showed non-significant deficits against Controls on all but one measure of PAL and SRM. By contrast, NECTs were significantly inferior to Controls in all but one measure of both tests (Table 2, Fig. 1). As ECTs outperformed NECTs in PAL, which is a good detector of early Alzheimer's (Swainson et al. Reference Swainson, Hodges, Galton, Semple, Michael, Dunn, Iddon, Robbins and Sahakian2001), the possibility that the MMSE deficit of ECTs reflects a higher incidence of preclinical dementia can be excluded.

The opposite pattern emerged in the tests examining executive function (Table 2, Figs 2 and 3). NECTs performed like Controls on all global measures of IED, whereas ECTs were significantly inferior to both groups. This deficit was not generalized to all IED components: ECTs were unimpaired in discrimination acquisition and reversal, but showed an increase approaching significance in errors during the IED shift phases (p<0.07; Fig. 2). This suggests an attentional flexibility deficit. The SOC performance of ECTs was also significantly inferior to that of the other two groups. This deficit seemed to be limited to increased rates of early drop-outs, resulting in score reductions in the initial, easiest, SOC stages: the three groups did not differ in subsequent, more difficult, SOC stages. ECT candidates who completed SOC (nine out of 15) in fact sustained their group's performance to levels comparable to Controls, although drop-outs were represented in the analysis with zero scores. Hence, the SOC deficit noted seems attributable not to executive dysfunction per se, but possibly to increased sensitivity to negative feedback in the ECT group (Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Sahakian, McKay, Herrod, Robbins and Paykel1996; Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009).

As an estimate of the sensitivity of our four CANTAB tests to the factor of ECT referral, we calculated effect sizes on the basis of means and standard deviations of our ECT and NECT groups (Cohen's d: Table 2). PAL measures yielded low effect sizes (0.50 >d>0.06). The percentage correct measure of SRM was a better differentiator, yielding a moderate d value of 0.63. The global IED measures of total errors, total trials and stages completed were good differentiators between ECT and NECT performance, yielding robust d values (0.86, 0.87 and 0.85 respectively). Discrimination and reversal errors yielded low effect sizes (d=0.50 and 0.27). The measure of IED shift errors was as good a differentiator of our clinical groups as IED global measures (d=0.76). This supports our conclusion that the IED deficit in ECTs reflects mainly compromised attentional flexibility. Finally, the only SOC measures to yield robust effect sizes were the easiest categories of problems (two to three moves: d=0.98 and 1.49), while the more difficult categories of four- and five-move problems produced very low effect sizes (d=0.32 and 0.06). This supports our argument that the SOC deficit in ECTs is an artefact of early drop-out rates, probably reflecting increased sensitivity to negative feedback.

Implications for differential neuroanatomical involvement

Inferences on the relationship of the cognitive performance of our patient groups with brain functioning are, of course, subject to confirmation by neuroimaging techniques. However, it is noteworthy that this is not the first instance where the PAL/IED combination has been effective in differentiating cognitive profiles within a clinical population previously considered uniform. Barnett et al. (Reference Barnett, Sahakian, Werners, Hill, Brazil, Gallagher, Bullmore and Jones2005) showed that visuospatial learning and executive function (PAL and IED respectively) were independently impaired in first-episode psychosis, the pattern of deficit reflecting differential clinical response. As neuropsychological tasks in general, PAL and IED are not entirely domain specific (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009). However, Barnett et al. (Reference Barnett, Sahakian, Werners, Hill, Brazil, Gallagher, Bullmore and Jones2005) argue for a broad dissociation between the cognitive systems engaged by them, with a greater burden on temporo-hippocampal processing in PAL and a greater frontostriatal load in the extradimensional shift stage of IED (Smith & Milner, Reference Smith and Milner1981; Owen et al. Reference Owen, Roberts, Polkey, Sahakian and Robbins1991, Reference Owen, Roberts, Hodges, Summers, Polkey and Robbins1993; Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Sahakian, Hodges, Rosser, Lange and Robbins1996, Reference Lawrence, Hodges, Rosser, Kershaw, Ffrench-Constant, Rubinsztein, Robbins and Sahakian1998; Miyashita et al. Reference Miyashita, Kameyama, Hasegawa and Fukushima1998; Rogers et al. Reference Rogers, Andrews, Grasby, Brooks and Robbins2000; Wood et al. Reference Wood, Proffitt, Mahony, Smith, Buchanan, Brewer, Stuart, Velakoulis, McGorry and Pantelis2002). Therefore, the PAL/IED combination is well suited for examining disorders potentially involving either or both frontostriatal and medial temporal lobe dysfunctions. This is all the more important because different relative loads of the two impairments may signal discrete subtypes of the disorder and, perhaps, differential response to treatment. In that light, it is important to establish whether memory and executive deficits necessarily coexist in MDD, or may present independently. Indeed, in our study the PAL/IED combination highlighted a double dissociation in the cognitive profiles of MDD patients with and without ECT referral.

An additional observation was that our ECTs were more likely than NECTs to abandon SOC early, though unimpaired in the more difficult stages of SOC. MDD patients demonstrate exaggerated responses to negative feedback during neuropsychological testing (Brittlebank et al. Reference Brittlebank, Scott, Williams and Ferrier1993; Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Sahakian, Rubinsztein, Michael, Rogers, Robbins and Paykel1999; Lembke & Ketter, Reference Lembke and Ketter2002), including an increased probability of failing SOC trials subsequent to incorrect responding (Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Sahakian, Herrod, Robbins and Paykel1997). It has been suggested (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009) that depressive states may influence the impact of negative feedback by affecting the connectivity of the amygdala with prefrontal cortical regions (Johnstone et al. Reference Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007; Siegle et al. Reference Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer and Thase2007; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Suckling, Ooi, Fu, Williams, Walsh, Mitterschiffthaler, Pich and Bullmore2008) and thus interfering with top-down control of emotional behaviour (Johnstone et al. Reference Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007). Our data suggest that MDD-ECT candidates may have a deficit in this axis.

In summary, our data suggest that MDD patients receiving ECT referral present a cognitive profile different to that of drug-respondent MDD patients. This neuropsychological profile suggests greater frontostriatal and milder temporo-hippocampal involvement in these patients than in the general MDD population. It also suggests that these patients may be characterized by dysfunctional amygdala–prefrontal cortex connectivity. The fact that the ECTs/NECTs differentiation seems qualitative rather than quantitative suggests that it is not attributable simply to the increased illness severity associated with ECT referral. Moreover, the differentiation cannot be attributed to compromised global cognitive functioning in ECT candidates, as reflected by their low MMSE scores in the present study. Follow-up data of these ECT candidates 2 months post-ECT show that remission of depression is accompanied by complete recovery of MMSE scores and improvement of memory scores, while the executive deficit characterizing ECT candidates in the current study persists in magnitude (Kalogerakou et al., unpublished observations).

In conclusion, these findings are salient to research efforts towards the neuropsychological subtyping of MDD, in addition to the evaluation of the therapeutic and adverse cognitive effects of ECT treatment.

Acknowledgements

This work was aided by Grant 70/4/9100 (2008–2009) from the Special Account for Research Grants, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, to Dr E. Tsaltas.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

APA (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn, text revision. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
APA (2001). The Practice of Electroconvulsive Therapy: Recommendations for Treatment, Training, and Privileging. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Barnett, JH, Sahakian, BJ, Werners, U, Hill, KE, Brazil, R, Gallagher, O, Bullmore, ET, Jones, PB (2005). Visuospatial learning and executive function are independently impaired in first-episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine 35, 10311041.Google Scholar
Berlim, MT, Turecki, G (2007). What is the meaning of treatment resistant/refractory major depression (TRD)? A systematic review of current randomized trials. European Neuropsychopharmacology 17, 696707.Google Scholar
Biringer, E, Lundervold, A, Stordal, K, Mykletun, A, Egeland, J, Bottlender, R, Lund, A (2005). Executive function improvement upon remission of recurrent unipolar depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 255, 373380.Google Scholar
Brittlebank, AD, Scott, J, Williams, JM, Ferrier, IN (1993). Autobiographical memory in depression: state or trait marker? British Journal of Psychiatry 162, 118121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, JG, Patel, JKM, Morris, PK, Reveley, MA (1998). Risperidone improves antisaccade error rates in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 29, 115.Google Scholar
Cassano, GB, Puca, F, Scapicchio, PL, Trabucchi, M (2002). Paroxetine and fluoxetine effects on mood and cognitive functions in depressed nondemented elderly patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63, 396402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, CH, Suckling, J, Ooi, C, Fu, CH, Williams, SC, Walsh, ND, Mitterschiffthaler, MT, Pich, EM, Bullmore, E (2008). Functional coupling of the amygdala in depressed patients treated with antidepressant medication. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 19091918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, H, Griffiths, K, Mackinnon, A, Jacomb, P (1997). A quantitative review of cognitive deficits in depression and Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 3, 631651.Google Scholar
Clark, L, Chamberlain, SR, Sahakian, BJ (2009). Neurocognitive mechanisms in depression: implications for treatment. Annual Review of Neuroscience 32, 5774.Google Scholar
Clark, L, Sarna, A, Goodwin, GM (2005). Impairment of executive function but not memory in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar I disorder and in euthymic patients with unipolar depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 19801982.Google Scholar
Cuesta, MJ, Peralta, V, Zarzuela, A (2001). Effects of olanzapine and other antipsychotics on cognitive function in chronic schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Research 48, 1728.Google Scholar
Elliott, R, Sahakian, BJ, Herrod, JJ, Robbins, TW, Paykel, ES (1997). Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 63, 7482.Google Scholar
Elliott, R, Sahakian, BJ, McKay, AP, Herrod, JJ, Robbins, TW, Paykel, ES (1996). Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychological Medicine 26, 975989.Google Scholar
Falconer, DW, Cleland, J, Fielding, S, Reid, IC (2010). Using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) to assess the cognitive impact of electroconvulsive therapy on visual and visuospatial memory. Psychological Medicine 40, 10171025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M, Spitzer, R, Gibbon, M, Williams, J (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE, McHugh, PR (1975). ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12, 189198.Google Scholar
Fowler, KS, Saling, MM, Conway, EL, Semple, JM, Louis, WJ (1997). Computerised neuropsychological tests in the early detection of dementia: prospective findings. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 3, 139146.Google Scholar
Frasch, K, Bullacher, C, Jäger, M, Kilian, R, Rink, M, Wittek, R, Becker, T, Neumann, NU (2009). Effects of symptom reduction and psychotropic medication on cognitive impairment in depression. Psychopathology 42, 5966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furlan, PM, Kallan, MJ, Ten Have, T, Pollock, BG, Katz, I, Lucki, I (2001). Cognitive and psychomotor effects of paroxetine and sertraline on healthy elderly volunteers. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 9, 429438.Google Scholar
Goldstein, SG, Filskov, SB, Weaver, LA (1977). Neuropsychological effects of electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 33, 798806.Google ScholarPubMed
Gualtieri, CT, Johnson, LG (2007). Bupropion normalizes cognitive performance in patients with depression. Medscape General Medicine 9, 22.Google ScholarPubMed
Halliday, AM, Davidson, K, Browne, MW, Kreeger, LC (1968). A comparison of the effects on depression and memory of bilateral ECT and unilateral ECT to the dominant and non-dominant hemispheres. British Journal of Psychiatry 114, 9971012.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 23, 5662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrera-Guzmán, I, Gudayol-Ferré, E, Herrera-Guzmán, D, Guàrdia-Olmos, J, Hinojosa-Calvo, E, Herrera-Abarca, JE (2009). Effects of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic–noradrenergic reuptake treatments on memory and mental processing speed in patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 43, 855863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnstone, T, van Reekum, CM, Urry, HL, Kalin, NH, Davidson, RJ (2007). Failure to regulate: counterproductive recruitment of top-down prefrontal-subcortical circuitry in major depression. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 88778884.Google Scholar
Joyce, PR, Paykel, ES (1989). Predictors of drug response in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 8999.Google Scholar
Keefe, RSE, Young, CA, Rock, SL, Purdon, SE, Gold, JM, Breier, A (2006). One-year double-blind study of the neurocognitive efficacy of olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 81, 115.Google Scholar
Konarski, JZ, Kennedy, SH, Segal, ZV, Lau, MA, Bieling, PJ, McIntyre, RS, Mayberg, HS (2009). Predictors of nonresponse to cognitive behavioural therapy or venlafaxine using glucose metabolism in major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 34, 175180.Google Scholar
Lange, KW, Sahakian, BJ, Quinn, NP, Marsden, CD, Robbins, TW (1995). Comparison of executive and visuospatial memory function in Huntington's disease and dementia of Alzheimer's type matched for degree of dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 58, 598606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, AD, Hodges, JR, Rosser, AE, Kershaw, A, Ffrench-Constant, C, Rubinsztein, DC, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (1998). Evidence for specific cognitive deficits in preclinical Huntington's disease. Brain 121, 13291341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, AD, Sahakian, BJ, Hodges, JR, Rosser, AE, Lange, KW, Robbins, TW (1996). Executive and mnemonic functions in early Huntington's disease. Brain 119, 16331645.Google Scholar
Lembke, A, Ketter, TA (2002). Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 302304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerer, B, Shapira, B, Calev, A, Tubi, N, Drexler, H, Kindler, S, Lidsky, D, Schwartz, JE (1995). Antidepressant and cognitive effects of twice vs. three times weekly ECT. American Journal of Psychiatry 152, 564570.Google Scholar
Li, CT, Lin, CP, Chou, KH, Chen, IY, Hsieh, JC, Wu, CL, Lin, WC, Su, TP (2010). Structural and cognitive deficits in remitting and non-remitting recurrent depression: a voxel-based morphometric study. NeuroImage 50, 347356.Google Scholar
Mayberg, HS (2003). Modulating dysfunctional limbic-cortical circuits in depression: towards development of brain-based algorithms for diagnosis and optimised treatment. British Medical Bulletin 65, 193207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, WV, Dunn, A, Rosenquist, PB (2004). Quality of life and function after ECT. British Journal of Psychiatry 185, 405409.Google Scholar
McGurk, SR, Lee, MA, Jayathilake, K, Meltzer, HY (2004). Cognitive effects of olanzapine treatment in schizophrenia. Medscape General Medicine 6, 27.Google Scholar
Mehta, MA, Sahakian, BJ, McKenna, PJ, Robbins, TW (1999). Systemic sulpiride in young adult volunteers simulates the profile of cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 146, 162174.Google Scholar
Meyer-Lindberg, A, Gruppe, H, Bauer, U, Lis, S, Krieger, S, Galhoffer, B (1997). Improvement of cognitive function in schizophrenic patients receiving clozapine or zotepine: results from a double-blind study. Pharmacopsychiatry 30, 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishara, AL, Goldberg, TE (2004). A meta-analysis and critical review of the effects of conventional neuroleptic treatment on cognition in schizophrenia: opening a closed book. Biological Psychiatry 55, 10131022.Google Scholar
Miyashita, Y, Kameyama, M, Hasegawa, I, Fukushima, T (1998). Consolidation of visual associative long-term memory in the temporal cortex of primates. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 70, 197211.Google Scholar
Murphy, FC, Sahakian, BJ, Rubinsztein, JS, Michael, A, Rogers, RD, Robbins, TW, Paykel, ES (1999). Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychological Medicine 29, 13071321.Google Scholar
Nakano, Y, Baba, H, Maeshima, H, Kitajima, A, Sakai, Y, Baba, K, Suzuki, T, Mimura, M, Arai, H (2008). Executive dysfunction in medicated, remitted state of major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 111, 4651.Google Scholar
Neu, P, Kiesslinger, U, Schlattmann, P, Reischies, FM (2001). Time-related cognitive deficiency in four different types of depression. Psychiatry Research 103, 237247.Google Scholar
Ng, C, Schweitzer, I, Alexopoulos, P, Celi, E, Wong, L, Tuckwell, V, Sergejew, A, Tiller, J (2000). Efficacy and cognitive effects of right unilateral electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of ECT 16, 370379.Google Scholar
O'Grada, C, Dinan, T (2007). Executive function in schizophrenia: what impact do antipsychotics have? Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental 22, 397406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olver, JS, Ignatiadis, S, Maruff, P, Burrows, GD, Norman, TR (2008). Quetiapine augmentation in depressed patients with partial response to antidepressants. Human Psychopharmacology 23, 653660.Google Scholar
Owen, AM, Iddon, JL, Hodges, JR, Summers, BA, Robbins, TW (1997). Spatial and non-spatial working memory at different stages of Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 35, 519532.Google Scholar
Owen, AM, Morris, RG, Sahakian, BJ, Polkey, CE, Robbins, TW (1996). Double dissociations of memory and executive functions in working memory task following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions, or amygdalohippocampectomy in man. Brain 119, 15971615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, AM, Roberts, AC, Hodges, JR, Summers, BA, Polkey, CE, Robbins, TW (1993). Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. Brain 116, 11591175.Google Scholar
Owen, AM, Roberts, AC, Polkey, CE, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (1991). Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimensional set shifting performance following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia 29, 9931006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, AM, Sahakian, BJ, Semple, J, Polkey, CE, Robbins, TW (1995). Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excision, frontal lobe excision or amygdalo-hyppocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia 33, 124.Google Scholar
Paelecke-Habermann, Y, Pohl, J, Leplow, B (2005). Attention and executive functions in remitted major depression patients. Journal of Affective Disorders 89, 125135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papakostas, GI, Fava, M, Thase, ME (2008). Treatment of SSRI-resistant depression: a meta-analysis comparing within- versus across-class switches. Biological Psychiatry 63, 699704.Google Scholar
Porter, RJ, Bourke, C, Gallagher, P (2007). Neuropsychological impairment in major depression: its nature, origin and clinical significance Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 41, 115128.Google Scholar
Porter, RJ, Gallagher, P, Thompson, JM, Young, AH (2003). Neurocognitive impairment in drug-free patients with major depressive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 182, 214220.Google Scholar
Postma, A, Jager, G, Kessels, RP, Koppeschaar, HP, van Honk, J (2004). Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain and Cognition 54, 2434.Google Scholar
Purcell, R, Maruff, P, Kyrios, M, Pantelis, C (1997). Neuropsychological function in young patients with unipolar major depression. Psychological Medicine 27, 12771285.Google Scholar
Rahman, S, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (1999). Comparative cognitive neuropsychological studies of frontal lobe function: implications for therapeutic strategies in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 10, 1528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rami-Gonzajez, L, Bernardo, M, Boget, T, Gil-Verona, JA, Salamero, M, Junque, C (2001). Subtypes of memory dysfunction associated with ECT: characteristics and neurobiological bases. Journal of ECT 17, 129135.Google Scholar
Reischies, FM, Neu, P (2000). Comorbidity of mild cognitive disorder and depression – a neuropsychological analysis. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 250, 186193.Google Scholar
Rey, A (1964). L'examen clinique en psychologie. Presses Universitaires de France: Paris.Google Scholar
Riedel, WJ, Eikmans, K, Heldens, A, Schmitt, JA (2005). Specific serotonergic reuptake inhibition impairs vigilance performance acutely and after subchronic treatment. Journal of Psychopharmacology 19, 1220.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, James, M, Owen, AM, Sahakian, BJ, McInnes, L, Rabbitt, P (1994). Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): a factor analytic study in a large number of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia 5, 266281.Google Scholar
Robertson, H, Pryor, R (2006). Memory and cognitive effects of ECT: informing and assessing patients. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment 12, 228238.Google Scholar
Rogers, RD, Andrews, TC, Grasby, PM, Brooks, DJ, Robbins, TW (2000). Contrasting cortical and subcortical activations produced by attentional-set shifting and reversal learning in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, 142162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rush, AJ, Trivedi, MH, Wisniewski, SR, Nierenberg, AA, Stewart, JW, Warden, D, Niederehe, G, Thase, ME, Lavori, PW, Lebowitz, BD, McGrath, PJ, Rosenbaum, JF, Sackeim, HA, Kupfer, DJ, Luther, J, Fava, M (2006). Acute and longer term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: a STAR*D report. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 19051917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackeim, HA, Prudic, J, Devanand, DP, Nobler, MS, Lisanby, SH, Peyser, S, Fitzsimons, L, Moody, BJ, Clark, J (2000). A prospective, randomised, double-blind comparison of bilateral and right unilateral electroconvulsive therapy at different stimulus intensities. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 425433.Google Scholar
Sackeim, HA, Prudic, J, Fuller, R, Keilp, J, Lavori, PW, Olfson, M (2007). The cognitive effects of electroconvulsive therapy in community settings. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 244254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahakian, BJ, Downes, JJ, Eagger, S, Evenden, JL, Levy, R, Philpot, MP, Roberts, AC, Robbins, TW (1990). Sparing of attentional relative to mnemonic function in a subgroup of patients with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Neuropsychologia 28, 11971213.Google Scholar
Sahgal, A, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW, Wray, CJ, Lloyd, S, Cook, JH, McKeith, IG, Disley, JCA, Eagger, S, Boddington, S, Edwardson, JA (1991). Detection of visual memory and learning deficits in Alzheimer's disease using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. Dementia 2, 150158.Google Scholar
Siegle, GJ, Thompson, W, Carter, CS, Steinhauer, SR, Thase, ME (2007). Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features. Biological Psychiatry 61, 198209.Google Scholar
Smith, ML, Milner, B (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia 19, 781793.Google Scholar
Squire, LR, Wetzel, CD, Slater, PC (1979). Memory complaints after electroconvulsive therapy: assessment with a new self-rating instrument. Biological Psychiatry 14, 791801.Google Scholar
Steffens, DC, Welsh-Bohmer, KA, Burke, JR, Plassman, BL, Beyer, JL, Gersing, KR, Potter, GG (2004). Methodology and preliminary results from the Neurocognitive Outcomes of Depression in the Elderly study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 17, 202211.Google Scholar
Swainson, R, Hodges, JR, Galton, CJ, Semple, J, Michael, A, Dunn, BD, Iddon, JL, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (2001). Early detection and differential diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease and depression with neuropsychological tasks. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 12, 265280.Google Scholar
Tyson, PJ, Laws, KR, Roberts, KH, Mortimer, AM (2004). Stability of set-shifting and planning abilities in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 129, 229239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UK ECT Review Group (2003). Efficacy and safety of electroconvulsive therapy in depressive disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet 361, 799808.Google Scholar
Velligan, DI, Newcomer, J, Pultz, J, Csernansky, J, Hoff, AL, Mahurin, R, Miller, AL (2002). Does cognitive function improve with quetiapine in comparison to haloperidol? Schizophrenia Research 15, 239248.Google Scholar
Wager, TD, Phan, KL, Liberzon, I, Taylor, SF (2003). Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: a meta-analysis of findings from neuroimaging. NeuroImage 19, 513531.Google Scholar
Wood, SJ, Proffitt, T, Mahony, K, Smith, DJ, Buchanan, JA, Brewer, W, Stuart, GW, Velakoulis, D, McGorry, PD, Pantelis, C (2002). Visuospatial memory and learning in first episode schizophreniform psychosis and established schizophrenia: a functional correlate of hippocampal pathology? Psychological Medicine 32, 429438.Google Scholar
Woodward, ND, Purdon, SE, Meltzer, HY, Zald, DH (2005). A meta-analysis of neuropsychological change to clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, and risperidone in schizophrenia. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 8, 457472.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of results from individual CANTAB tests

Figure 2

Fig. 1. (a) In Paired Associates Learning (PAL), a significant group effect in total errors [F(2, 42)=3.84, p<0.05] was due to a deficit in depressed patients with no electroconvulsive treatment referral (NECTs) compared to Controls. Analysis of PAL stage errors indicated that this deficit was specific to the most demanding eight-shape PAL stage [F(2, 42)=4.52, p<0.05; Controls v. NECTs: F(1, 42)=8.73, p<0.005]. Depressed patients with ECT referral (ECTs) performed at an intermediate level, differing from neither Controls nor NECTs at any stage. (b) In the measure of adjusted total trials alone [F(2, 42)=4.11, p<0.05], both NECTs and ECTs performed significantly worse than Controls [F(1, 42): Controls v. NECTs=7.59, p<0.001; Controls v. ECTs=4.27, p<0.05]. Data shown are means and 0.95 confidence intervals; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. (a) In the Intra/Extradimensional (IED) shift task, the adjusted measure of total errors yielded a significant group effect [F(2, 42): 4.91, p<0.01]. This was due to the compromised performance of depressive patients with electroconvulsive treatment referral (ECTs) compared to either Controls [Controls v. ECTs: F(1, 42)=8.40, p<0.01] or to depressive patients without ECT referral [NECTs: NECTs v. ECTs, F(1, 42)=6.15, p<0.05]. NECTs did not differ from Controls. (b) Analysis of errors in discrete IED tasks revealed normal performance of both ECTs and NECTs (p>0.15) in the acquisition of a simple discrimination (IED stages 1+3+4) and in discrimination reversal (IED stages 2+5+7). A deficit approaching significance was noted in IED [stages 6+8: F(2, 42)=2.84, p<0.07] due to increased errors in the ECT group. Data shown are means and 0.95 confidence intervals; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. In Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) depressive patients with electroconvulsive treatment referral (ECTs) solved significantly fewer problems using minimum moves compared to Controls and depressive patients with no ECT referral (NECTs) [F(2, 42)=3.55, p<0.05; Controls v. ECTs: F(1, 42)=7.03, p<0.01]. The ECT deficit was restricted to the easier SOC stages of problems soluble in two [Kruskal–Wallis: H (2, n=45)=6.799, p=0.05] and three moves [F(2, 42)=9.63, p<0.001: Controls v. ECTs, F(1, 42)=16.46; NECTs v. ECTs F(1, 42)=12.10, p<0.001]. NECTS did not differ from Controls. The three groups did not differ in the more difficult stages of problems soluble in four and five moves. The early stages ECT deficit was probably due to significantly more early abandons of the test by ECTs compared to Controls and NECTs [Kruskal–Wallis: H (2, n=45)=11.00, p<0.001). Data shown are means and 0.95 confidence intervals; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01; Kruskal–Wallis: +p<0.05.