Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:25:58.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Changes in visual memory in mild cognitive impairment: a longitudinal study with CANTAB

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2020

María Campos-Magdaleno*
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
David Leiva
Affiliation:
Department of Methodology of Behavioural Sciences, University of Barcelona, Catalunya, Spain
Arturo X. Pereiro
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
Cristina Lojo-Seoane
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
Sabela C. Mallo
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
David Facal
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
Onésimo Juncos-Rabadán
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
*
Author for correspondence: María Campos-Magdaleno, E-mail: maria.campos@usc.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), as a stage in the cognitive continuum between normal ageing and dementia, is mainly characterized by memory impairment. The aims of this study were to examine CANTAB measures of temporal changes of visual memory in MCI and to evaluate the usefulness of the baseline scores for predicting changes in cognitive status.

Methods

The study included 201 participants aged over 50 years with subjective cognitive complaints. Visual memory was assessed with four CANTAB tests [paired associates learning (PAL), delayed matching to sample (DMS), pattern recognition memory (PRM) and spatial span (SSP)] administered at baseline and on two further occasions, with a follow-up interval of 18–24 months. Participants were divided into three groups according to the change in their cognitive status: participants with subjective cognitive complaints who remained stable, MCI participants who remained stable (MCI-Stable) and MCI participants whose cognitive deterioration continued (MCI-Worsened). Linear mixed models were used to model longitudinal changes, with evaluation time as a fixed variable, and multinomial regression models were used to predict changes in cognitive status.

Results

Isolated significant effects were obtained for age and group with all CANTAB tests used. Interactions between evaluation time and group were identified in the PAL and DMS tests, indicating different temporal patterns depending on the changes in cognitive status. Regression models also indicated that CANTAB scores were good predictors of changes in cognitive status.

Conclusions

Decline in visual memory measured by PAL and DMS tests can successfully distinguish different types of MCI, and considered together PAL, DMS, PRM and SSP can predict changes in cognitive status.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Cognitive decline in the elderly can be considered a continuum ranging from a cognitively unimpaired state (CU) to the presence of subjective cognitive complaints (SCC) without objective cognitive impairment, also called subjective cognitive decline (SCD) (Jessen et al. Reference Jessen, Amariglio, van Boxtel, Breteler, Ceccaldi and Chételat2014; Molinuevo et al. Reference Nathan, Lim, Abbott, Galluzzi, Marizzoni and Babiloni2017), followed by mild cognitive impairment (MCI), characterized by the presence of cognitive complaints, objective cognitive deterioration and preservation or minimal impairment of instrumental activities of daily living (Petersen, Reference Petersen, Lopez, Armstrong, Getchius, Ganguli, Gloss and Sager2004; Petersen et al. Reference Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy and Sarkar2018), and finally, dementia, which is characterized by cognitive and behavioural symptoms that impair normal functioning in daily life (APA, 2013). The single and multiple domain subtypes of amnestic and non-amnestic MCI that involve deterioration in only one or in more than one cognitive domain may also represent different levels of cognitive decline, with the multiple domain subtype being the most extreme clinical state (Brambati et al. Reference Brambati, Belleville, Kergoat, Chayer, Gauthier and Joubert2009; Han et al. Reference Han, Kim, Lee, Park, Lee and Kim2012). Progression along the continuum is a complex process characterized by cognitive changes, transitions and diagnostic instability at SCD and MCI stages, conversion to dementia and recovery to CU (Facal, Guàrdia-Olmos, & Juncos-Rabadán, Reference Facal, Guàrdia-Olmos and Juncos-Rabadán2015; Petersen et al. Reference Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy and Sarkar2018). However, taking the instability into account, MCI and the subtypes characterized by only memory impairments (amnestic single-domain) or by impairments in memory and in other cognitive domains (amnestic multi-domain) are considered high-risk states for progression to dementia, mainly Alzheimer's Disease (AD). Early detection of the different stages of cognitive decline and the progress of decline is a pressing research challenge in the prevention and treatment of dementia (Albert et al. Reference Albert, DeKosky, Dickson, Dubois, Feldman, Fox and Phelps2011; Petersen et al. Reference Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy and Sarkar2018).

Previous studies have shown that visual memory impairment can differentiate MCI patients from CU controls (Alescio-Lautier et al. Reference Alescio-Lautier, Michel, Herrera, Elahmadi, Chambon, Touzet and Paban2007; Barbeau et al. Reference Barbeau, Ranjeva, Didic, Confort-Gouny, Felician, Soulier and Poncet2008; Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, Pereiro, & Lojo-Seoane, Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, Pereiro and Lojo-Seoane2014a; Westerberg et al. Reference Westerberg, Mayes, Florczak, Chen, Creery, Parrish, Weintraub and Paller2013). Other studies have successfully predicted the progression from MCI to AD (De Anna et al. Reference De Anna, Felician, Barbeau, Mancini, Didic and Ceccaldi2014; Defrancesco et al. Reference Defrancesco, Marksteiner, Deisenhammer, Kemmler, Djurdjevic and Schocke2013; Didic et al. Reference Didic, Felician, Barbeau, Mancini, Latger-Florence, Tramoni and Ceccaldi2013; Oltra-Cucarella et al. Reference Owen, Beksinska, Jamnes, Leigh, Summers, Marsden and Robbins2018; Reijs et al. Reference Sahakian, Morris, Evenden, Heald, Levy, Philpot and Robbins2017; Saxton et al. Reference Summers and Saunders2004) and even complete neurodegenerative progress from the cognitively impaired state to MCI and AD (Mistridis, Krumm, Monsch, Berres, & Taylor, Reference Mistridis, Krumm, Monsch, Berres and Taylor2015). These findings indicate the importance of including reliable visual memory tests for diagnosing MCI and for studying the course of decline in different aspects of visual memory in progression to AD.

Computerized assessment of visual memory using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; Cambridge Cognition Ltd., 2012; Sahakian et al. Reference Saunders and Summers1988) has been used to differentiate controls, MCI and AD participants in cross-sectional studies (Alladi, Arnold, Mitchell, Nestor, & Hodges, Reference Alladi, Arnold, Mitchell, Nestor and Hodges2006; De Rover et al. Reference De Rover, Pironti, McCabe, Acosta-Carbonero, Arana and Morein-Zamir2011; Juncos-Rabadán et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, Pereiro and Lojo-Seoane2014a; Junkkila, Oja, Laine, & Karrasch, Reference Junkkila, Oja, Laine and Karrasch2012; Swainson et al. Reference Sweeney, Kmiec and Kupfer2001). CANTAB includes tests that assess visual episodic memory (EM) and visual working memory (WM). Both types of memory have been shown to be impaired early on in AD (Belleville, Sylvain-Roy, de Boysson, & Ménard, Reference Belleville, Sylvain-Roy, de Boysson and Ménard2008; Economou, Papageorgiou, & Karageorgiou, Reference Economou, Papageorgiou and Karageorgiou2006; Van Geldrop et al. Reference Weisberg2015). Deterioration in EM has been found to be a particularly strong predictor of progression to AD (Belleville et al. Reference Belleville, Sylvain-Roy, de Boysson and Ménard2008; Landau et al. Reference Landau, Harvey, Madison, Reiman, Foster, Aisen and Jagust2010).

Longitudinal evidence from research using the CANTAB visual memory tests remains scarce (Cacciamani et al. Reference Cacciamani, Salvadori, Eusebi, Lisetti, Luchetti, Calabresi and Parnetti2018; Juncos-Rabadan et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadan, Pereiro, Facal, Lojo-Seoane, Mallo and Campos-Magdaleno2016; Mitchell, Arnold, Dawson, Nestor & Hodges, Reference Molinuevo, Rabin, Amariglio, Buckley, Dubois and Ellis2009; Summers & Saunders, Reference Swainson, Hodges, Galton, Semple, Michael, Dunn and Sahakian2012). Summers & Saunders (Reference Swainson, Hodges, Galton, Semple, Michael, Dunn and Sahakian2012) found that the decline in visual memory performance assessed with CANTAB measures [paired associates learning (PAL), spatial span (SSP), spatial WM)] in combination with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test identified 100% of cases of MCI patients who progressed to AD after 20 months. However, Cacciamani et al. (Reference Cacciamani, Salvadori, Eusebi, Lisetti, Luchetti, Calabresi and Parnetti2018) reported improvements in spatial WM, spatial recognition memory and PAL after a follow-up period of 12 months in a small sample of MCI patients. Further investigation including larger sample sizes and longer intervals between assessments must be carried out to analyze the discriminant value and evolution of these memory measures.

The main purpose of the present study was to determine longitudinal patterns of performance of visual memory CANTAB tests in patients diagnosed at baseline with MCI and assessed twice with a follow-up interval of around 18 months to measure stability or deterioration of the condition. A secondary aim was to assess the usefulness of baseline CANTAB measures for predicting changes in cognitive status at the final follow-up stage.

Methodology

Participants

Participants were selected from the Compostela Aging Study (CompAS), an ongoing longitudinal project involving the detection and follow-up of MCI in patients with subjective cognitive complaints and no prior diagnostic of dementia, psychiatric or neurological disorders attending primary care centres in Galicia, an autonomous region in northwest Spain (Juncos-Rabadán et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Pereiro, Facal, Rodríguez, Lojo, Caamaño and Eiroa2012). We selected 201 patients aged over 50 years who had completed three visits (at baseline, Time 1 and Time 2) with a between-test interval of around 18 months. The mean interval was 18.49 months (3.64 standard deviation, s.d.) between baseline and Time 1, 17.72 months (3.81 s.d.) between Time 1 and Time 2, and 36.83 months (5.17 s.d.) between baseline and Time 2. None of the participants had previously been diagnosed with MCI or dementia, clinical stroke, traumatic brain injury, motor-sensory defects, alcohol or drug abuse/dependence, or any neurological or psychiatric disease. At baseline, participants were classified as single-domain amnestic MCI (sda-MCI), multiple-domain amnestic MCI (mda-MCI), single-domain non-amnestic MCI (sdna-MCI) or multiple-domain non-amnestic MCI (mdna-MCI), according to standard criteria (Albert et al. Reference Albert, DeKosky, Dickson, Dubois, Feldman, Fox and Phelps2011; Dubois et al. Reference Dubois, Feldman, Jacova, Dekosky, Barberger-Gateau, Cummings and Scheltens2007; Petersen, Reference Petersen, Lopez, Armstrong, Getchius, Ganguli, Gloss and Sager2004). The criteria for diagnosis of MCI included the following: (a) self-reported, informant-corroborated concerns about cognition, assessed by a short version of the subjective memory complaints questionnaire (SMCQ; Benedet & Seisdedos, Reference Benedet and Seisdedos1996); (b) performance of 1.5 standard deviations (s.d.) below age and education norms in one or more cognitive domains, assessed by the subscales of the Spanish version of the Cambridge cognitive examination, CAMCOG-R (Huppert et al. Reference Huppert, Jorm, Brayne, Girling, Barkely, Beardsall and Paykel1996; Spanish version: López-Pousa, Reference López-Pousa2003; Pereiro, Ramos-Lema, Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, & Lojo-Seoane, Reference Petersen2015), except for memory, assessed by the short and long delay free recall from the Spanish version of the California verbal learning test (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober1987; Spanish version: Benedet & Alejandre, Reference Benedet and Alejandre1998); (c) no significant or minimal impact on activities of daily living, assessed by instrumental activities of daily living scale (Lawton & Brody, Reference Lawton and Brody1969); and (d) the absence of dementia as established by the DSM-IV and NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Participants performing as cognitively normal adults in general functioning and specific domain tests, according to norms by age and years of education, and presenting SCC, were included in the SCC group. This group met the following criteria: (a) attending primary care health centres with self-reported cognitive concerns; and (b) confirmation of these concerns by the short Spanish version of the questionnaire for subjective memory complaints (Benedet & Seisdedos, Reference Benedet and Seisdedos1996) administered to participants and a family member. The SCC group was considered a control group. All diagnoses were reached by consensus at a special meeting of the research team.

In each successive follow-up assessment, participants were reclassified as SCC, sda-MCI, mda-MCI, sdna-MCI, mdna-MCI and probable dementia (DSM-IV and NINCDS-ADRDA) by applying the same criteria as at baseline. At the third evaluation, participants were classified into three groups according to the changes in their cognitive status: participants with SCC at baseline who remained stable at Time 2 (SCC-stable group, n = 148, 71.49%); participants diagnosed with MCI at baseline who remained stable at Time 2 (MCI-stable group, n = 31, 15.45%); and participants diagnosed as sda-MCI or sdna-MCI at baseline who progressed to mda-MCI, mdna-MCI or dementia at Time 1 or Time 2 (MCI-worsened group, n = 22, 13.04%). Probable AD or other types of dementia were diagnosed according to the delayed matching to sample (DMS)-IV and NINCDS-ADRDA criteria, and progression to dementia was confirmed by consultation of the medical history and recording the date of neurological diagnosis. We assumed, in accordance with Brambati et al. (Reference Brambati, Belleville, Kergoat, Chayer, Gauthier and Joubert2009) and Campos-Magdaleno, Díaz-Bóveda, Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, & Pereiro (Reference Campos-Magdaleno, Díaz-Bóveda, Juncos-Rabadán, Facal and Pereiro2016), that the change from single-domain to multiple-domain corresponds to cognitive worsening, in which multi-domain MCI represents the most severely impaired of the MCI subtypes.

All participants gave their written informed consent prior to participation in the study. The research project was approved by the Galician Ethics Committee for Clinical Research (Xunta de Galicia, Spain), and the study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards established in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and revised in Seoul 2008.

Materials and procedure

Four CANTAB visual memory tests were administered: PAL, pattern recognition memory (PRM), DMS and SSP. The PAL test assesses visuospatial EM and learning (Sahakian et al. Reference Saunders and Summers1988). One or more boxes containing a pattern are displayed on the screen and are opened in random order. The patterns shown in the boxes are then displayed in the middle of the screen, one at a time, and participants are asked to touch the box in which the pattern was originally located. If the participant makes an error, the patterns are shown again as a reminder of the locations. The level of difficulty (2, 4, 6 and 8 patterns) was increased throughout the tests. The outcome variable was the total number of errors adjusted to level 6, which represents a high level of difficulty and has been used by several researchers to study MCI and AD (Alladi et al. Reference Alladi, Arnold, Mitchell, Nestor and Hodges2006; Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Nathan, Hammond, Robbins, Hodges and Sahakian2011; Lenehan, Summers, Saunders, Summers, & Vickers, Reference Lenehan, Summers, Saunders, Summers and Vickers2016; Mitchell et al. Reference Molinuevo, Rabin, Amariglio, Buckley, Dubois and Ellis2009; Polcher et al. 2017). The PRM test assesses visual PRM in a two-choice forced discrimination paradigm (Swainson et al. Reference Sweeney, Kmiec and Kupfer2001). The participants were presented with two blocks of 12 visual patterns, each displayed separately. In the recognition phase, subjects are required to choose between a pattern they have already seen and a novel pattern. The outcome measure was the percentage of correct responses, considered in some previous studies as a specific EM outcome (De Jager, Milwain, & Budge, Reference De Jager, Milwain and Budge2002; Juncos-Rabadán, Pereiro, Facal, Reboredo, & Lojo-Seoane, Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Pereiro, Facal, Reboredo and Lojo-Seoane2014b; Nathan et al. Reference O'Connell, Coen, Kidd, Warsi, Chin and Lawlor2017). DMS assesses both simultaneous and short-term visual memory (Owen et al. Reference Pereiro, Ramos-Lema, Juncos-Rabadán, Facal and Lojo-Seoane1993; Sahakian et al. Reference Saunders and Summers1988). Participants must select the pattern that exactly matches the sample from four abstract choices that include distractors. In some trials, the sample and the choice patterns are shown simultaneously, while in others there is a delay of 0, 4000 or 12 000 ms. The outcome measure was the percentage of correct responses, also considered an EM measure task (Juncos-Rabadán et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Pereiro, Facal, Reboredo and Lojo-Seoane2014b, Reference Juncos-Rabadan, Pereiro, Facal, Lojo-Seoane, Mallo and Campos-Magdaleno2016; Sweeney, Kmiec, & Kupfer, Reference Van Geldrop, Heringa, van den Berg, Olde Rikkert, Biessels and Kessels2000). SSP is a computerized version of the Corsi blocks task that assesses visual working memory capacity (Owen et al. Reference Pereiro, Ramos-Lema, Juncos-Rabadán, Facal and Lojo-Seoane1993). A pattern of white squares is shown on the screen. Some of the squares change colour, one at a time, in a variable sequence. At the end of the presentation of each sequence, a tone indicates that the participant should touch each of the boxes in the same order that they were originally presented. The number of boxes in the sequence is increased from a level of two at the start of the test until a final level of nine, with three sequences at each level. The outcome variable, the span length, was calculated for the longest sequence successfully recalled and was used as an index for the SSP task (Saunders & Summers, Reference Saxton, Lopez, Ratcliff, Dulberg, Fried, Carlson and Kuller2010).

The four CANTAB tests were administered in a more extensive counterbalanced assessment carried out by trained psychologists. To control the effect of visual acuity on the performance of the CANTAB, we measured the visual acuity of both eyes with the Lighthouse near visual acuity test.

Statistical analysis

Cross-sectional analyses were carried out at baseline for socio-demographic and principal neuropsychological measures, which were modelled using non-parametric tests (e.g. Kruskal–Wallis and Mann–Whitney tests) to determine differences between groups, given the skewed empirical distributions and the small sample size in some cases. In order to model longitudinal changes in the CANTAB measures, we initially used (generalized) linear mixed models -(G)LMM- with random intercepts and random slopes. We considered that the intercepts might differ according to the memory trajectories of the participants and that different slopes would represent various temporal patterns of change in the memory performance. We finally discarded random slopes in the estimated models due to convergence issues. The statistical models included the following independent variables or predictors as fixed effects: evaluation time (baseline, Time 1 and Time 2), group (SCC-stable, MCI-stable and MCI-worsened), and their interaction (evaluation time × group). By specifying group and evaluation time as fixed factors we can test pairwise comparisons of the estimated marginal means for the dependent variables for each group and at each evaluation time. As all models included random effects for intercepts and heteroskedasticity due to the group, the covariate age at baseline was standardized to enable interpretation of the intercept. Separate models were constructed for each dependent variable: PAL total errors adjusted for 6 shapes, PRM total per cent correct, DMS total per cent correct and SSP length. The SCC-stable group was considered the reference group, and baseline was considered the reference evaluation time. (G)LMMs assuming Gaussian response were used to model changes in percentages. (G)LMMs assuming Poisson response were used to model count data related to errors and SSP length. When (G)LMM assumptions were not fulfilled (e.g. overdispersion of the data), a negative binomial distribution was used to model count data. A general procedure was used to model the relationship between responses and predictors: first, a null model including only the intercept was estimated (model 1); group and time predictors and their interaction were then gradually added in two subsequent models (2 and 3). Several goodnesses of fit indexes were used (e.g. Akaike's Information Criterion) to choose the best (G)LMMs for each response. In addition, we also modelled longitudinal changes in other cognitive outcomes, such as MiniMental State Examination (MMSE) and CAMCOG-R scores, which clearly represent general cognitive performance, following the same procedures as with the CANTAB scores (see online Supplementary Material S2 for more details).

GLMs were used to predict changes in cognitive status at the final follow-up stage by using the baseline CANTAB scores. Specifically, multinomial logistic regression models were used to assess the extent to which cognitive evolution groups at the final follow-up stage could be predicted by visual memory scores at baseline. Four multinomial logistic regression models were constructed with each of the CANTAB measures as predictors as well as a multiple regression model combining these measures as predictors. The age of participants was added as a covariate in all the abovementioned GLMs. Information criteria indices, such as AIC and BIC, were used to select the best candidate subset of predictors, as proposed by other authors (Fox, Reference Fox2016; Weisberg, Reference Westerberg, Mayes, Florczak, Chen, Creery, Parrish, Weintraub and Paller2014); given that these indices are unbounded, the best fits are indicated by lower values. Thus, models with the lowest Akaike Information Criterion/Bayesian Information Criterion (AIC/BIC) values were considered to provide the best fit to the data. The general criterion applied was the selection of the model that showed, within the set of fit indicators, at least some positive evidence. For instance, a minimum difference in BIC of 2 units, which is equivalent to a minimum Bayes Factor of 3 (see Table 22.1 in Fox, Reference Fox2016, for further details), is considered supporting evidence for a specific model. As with the GLMs, AIC was used to assess the goodness of fit of the different models. Finally, the area under the curve (AUC) index was estimated for all models in order to evaluate the predictive capacity of each.

Cross-sectional statistical analysis was performed with SPSS for Windows, version 21.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). The (G)LMMs were constructed in R environment (version 3.6.2; R Core Team, Reference Reijs, Ramakers, Köhler, Teunissen, Koel-Simmelink, Nathan and Vandenberghe2019) with the nlme (version 3.1-143; Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, & Sarkar, Reference Polcher, Frommann, Koppara, Wolfsgruber, Jessen and Wagner2018) and lme4 packages (version 1.1-21; Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015).

Results

Socio-demographic and neuropsychological profiles of the groups at baseline are summarized in Table 1. Comparisons revealed no differences between groups in years of education and the Charlson Comorbidity Index (CCI). For the cognitive variables (except for the MMSE scores, which were similar in both MCI groups), the SCC-stable group performed best, followed by the MCI-stable group and MCI-worsened group. The MCI-worsened was the oldest group. Finally, MCI-stable had the highest scores in subjective cognitive complaints. No significant differences were found between groups in visual acuity. Results obtained with the (G)LMMs showed that cognitive decline was significantly more pronounced in the MCI groups (see Section S2 in online Supplementary Material: (G)LMMs were estimated for MMSE and CAMCOG-R scores). Specifically, a significant interaction between Time and Group predictors was found in those models in which MMSE [χ2(2) = 21.50; p < 0.001] and CAMCOG-R [χ2(2) = 19.99; p < 0.001] scores were included as responses. The interaction can be summarized by the greater decrease in the general cognitive performance of the individuals included in MCI-worsened group than in the individuals included in the other two groups.

Table 1. Mean and standard deviations (in parentheses) of the demographic and neuropsychological measures at baseline for the three groups: subjective cognitive complaints (SCC) that remain stable (SCC-stable); mild cognitive impairment that remains stable (MCI-stable); mild cognitive impairment that worsened (MCI-worsened)

MMSE, MiniMental State Examination; CCI, Charlson Comorbidity Index; SCC, Subjective Cognitive Complaints (patient); CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination (total score); CVLT SDFR, California Verbal Learning Test, Short Delay Free Recall; CVLT LDFR, California Verbal Learning Test, Long Delay Free Recall. Visual Acuity = Lighthouse test.

* = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01.

PAL total errors adjusted 6 shapes

We used (G)LMMs assuming a response according to a negative binomial distribution because of the presence of overdispersion (i.e. the spread parameter is significantly greater than the location parameter). Model 3, which included evaluation time, group, interaction evaluation time × group, and the random effects for the intercepts, yielded the best fit (see Table 2). The results of Model 3 showed significant effects of the covariate Age [χ2(1) = 54.02; p < 0.001], the variables evaluation time [χ2(1) = 14.72; p < 0.001] and group [χ2(2) = 75.81; p < 0.001] and the evaluation time × group interaction [χ2(2) = 108.83; p < 0.001], indicating different temporal patterns in the two MCI and the SCC stable groups over time. Estimated means from the aforementioned model indicated that the scores of the SCC-stable group scarcely changed over time (e. g. mean difference between baseline and T2 = 1.5; p = 0.02) whereas the errors in the MCI-Stable and MCI-Worsened groups increased (baseline-T2 means differences equal 12.07 and 36.99, respectively; p < 0.001). Figure 1 shows the estimated longitudinal trends for PAL total adjusted errors 6 shapes in the three groups across the three evaluation times.

Fig. 1. Estimated marginal means and errors bars from Model 1 for PAL, PRM, DMS and SSP in the three groups across the three evaluation times. SE, standard error; BL, baseline assessment; T1, Time 1 assessment; T2, Time 2 assessment.

Table 2. Summary of models compared for PAL total errors adjusted-6 shapes. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Model 1 is the null mixed model (i.e. random intercepts and age covariate only); Model 2 is the mixed model with main effects; Model 3 is the mixed model with main effects and interactions. Coefficients and standard errors (in parentheses) are shown on a log scale of number of errors (i.e. natural log of the response).

*** p < 0.01.

PAL total errors adjusted 6 shapes at baseline also proved to be a good predictor of changes in cognitive status at the end of the follow-up [χ2(2) = 68.44; p < 0.001]. In this regard, the relative risk of being in the MCI-worsened group when PAL errors increased by one unit, relative to the reference SCC-stable group (see Section S1 in online Supplementary Material), was 1.035. The model including this variable as the only predictor displayed a good predictive capacity (AUC = 0.78).

PRM total per cent correct

GLMMs using normal response (Gaussian) for percentages showed that Model 2 (represented in Table 3) yielded a better fit than the other models. Model 2 included only random effects for the intercepts and fixed effect for Age at baseline [χ2(1) = 32.99; p < 0.001], Evaluation Time [χ2(1) = 0.32; p = 0.57] and Group [χ2(2) = 89.89; p < 0.001]. According to this model, Age at baseline and Group had significant effects, but the Time predictor did not have a significant effect. The latter predictor was retained in the model in order to estimate and show marginal means across time. Mean distributions indicated that the percentage of hits in PRM did not change over time, indicating that the initial differences between groups were maintained throughout evaluation times (SCC-stable> MCI-stable = MCI-worsened). Figure 1 represents the longitudinal trends for PRM total per cent correct in the three groups across the three evaluation times.

Table 3. Summary of model comparison for PRM total per cent correct. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Model 1 is the null mixed model (i.e. random intercepts and age covariate only); Model 2 is the mixed model with main effects; Model 3 is the mixed model with main effects and interactions. Coefficients and standard errors (in parentheses).

*** p < 0.01.

PRM total per cent correct at the baseline was found to be a useful predictor of changes in cognitive status at the end of the study period [χ2(2) = 75.49; p < 0.001]. Specifically, estimated multinomial logistic model (see Section S1 in online Supplementary Material) showed that by increasing the scores of this CANTAB test by one unit, the expected relative risk of being classified in the MCI-worsened group is 0.874 relative to the reference group, which was SCC-stable. The simple multinomial logistic model appeared to have a good predictive capacity (AUC = 0.79).

DMS total per cent correct

Model 3 yielded the best fit for percentages of correct responses in DMS obtained by means of GLMMs with Gaussian response (see Table 4), which included random effects for the intercepts and fixed effect for age at baseline [χ2(1) = 66.63; p < 0.001], evaluation time [χ2(1) = 0.32; p = 0.57], group [χ2(1) = 51.76; p < 0.001] and the time × group interaction [χ2(1) = 22.96; p < 0.001]. According to this model, age at baseline had a significant effect and, given the significant interaction, group effect depends on time and vice versa. In this regard, the distribution of the estimated means indicated a significant decline in the DMS per cent correct in the MCI-worsened group over time (baseline-T2 means difference = 13.89; p < 0.001). By contrast, neither the SCC-stable group nor the MCI-stable group yielded significant differences when measurement times were compared (baseline-T2 mean difference = −1.13; p = 0.38) (baseline-T2 mean difference = −2.49; p = 0.43) (see Fig. 1).

Table 4. Summary of compared models for DMS total per cent correct. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Model 1 is the null mixed model (i.e. random intercepts and age covariate only); Model 2 is the mixed model with main effects; Model 3 is the mixed model with main effects and interactions. Coefficients and standard errors (in parentheses).

*** p < 0.01.

The multinomial logistic regression model using DMS total per cent correct at baseline as the only predictor showed that this measure was useful for predicting the classification of individuals according to the change in cognitive status criteria [χ2(2) = 38.32; p < 0.001]. The relative risk ratio for being classified as MCI-worsened when the baseline DMS scores increased by one unit was 0.92 (see Section S1 in online Supplementary Material). The predictive capacity of the model can be regarded as good (AUC = 0.71).

SSP length

GLMMs using a Poisson response (i.e. the assumption of equidispersion was met) for SSP length showed that Model 2 produced a better fit than the other alternatives. This model (see Table 5) included only random effects for the intercepts and a fixed effect for age at baseline [χ2(1) = 6.50; p = 0.011], evaluation time [χ2(1) = 0.07; p = 0.80] and group [χ2(2) = 10.41; p = 0.006]. The evaluation time predictor was retained in the model in order to estimate and show marginal means across time. Considering the estimated marginal means of SSP length on three measurement occasions (see Fig. 1), significant differences were found between SCC-stable and MCI-worsened groups (mean differences in the three contrasts equal approximately 1.11; p < 0.01) but not between MCI-worsened and MCI-stable groups (three means differences close to −0.60; p > 0.05) or between the SCC-stable and MCI-stable groups (mean differences in the pairwise contrasts around 0.51; p > 0.05).

Table 5. Summary of models compared for SSP length. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Model 1 is the null mixed model (random intercepts and age covariate only); Model 2 is the mixed model with main effects; Model 3 is the mixed model with main effects and interactions. Coefficients and standard errors (in parentheses) are shown on the log scale of number of correct responses (i.e. natural log of the response).

**p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Inclusion of SSP length in a multinomial logistic model to predict membership in the cognitive evolution groups led to the observation of a significant effect [see section S1 of online Supplementary Material; χ2(2) = 51.50; p < 0.001]. An increase of one unit in the baseline SSP length score indicates that inclusion in the MCI-worsened group at the end of the study is less likely than being classified as SCC-stable (relative risk ratio is equal to approximately 0.12). The AUC (0.71) also indicates a good predictive capacity.

Combined CANTAB measures

Finally, we tested the predictive value of a set of predictors comprising the four CANTAB measures (i.e. PAL total errors adjusted 6 shapes, PRM total per cent correct, DMS total per cent correct and SSP length) after controlling for age. The corresponding multinomial logistic model showed a significant effect of all CANTAB scores on the membership in cognitive evolution groups (see Section S1 in online Supplementary Material; Wald's tests for all estimated coefficients associated with CANTAB scores yielded p < 0.05) and the estimates were consistent with those included in the previous models including only one CANTAB score. The predictive capacity of the multinomial logistic model combining all CANTAB scores was very good (AUC = 0.86).

Discussion

This study aimed to analyze the longitudinal patterns of performance of three visual EM CANTAB tests and one visual WM test in three diagnostic groups classified according to the changes in their cognitive status, and also to show the usefulness of the measures for predicting changes in the cognitive status of individuals at the end of the study. Overall, the results showed the existence of different patterns of longitudinal performance depending on the changes in the diagnosis of the participants. Some CANTAB outcomes differentiated participants who showed no cognitive impairment (SCC-Stable) and participants with MCI, and even between MCI participants who remained stable or worsened. The results indicate that assessing visual memory with CANTAB measures may be useful for differentiating between different stages of MCI in the cognitive continuum of dementia. Estimated simple and multiple multinomial logistic models were used to assess the utility of CANTAB scores at the initial stage to predict cognitive evolution at the end of the study proved to have a good to very good predictive capacity (AUCs between 0.71 and 0.86; see Section S1 of online Supplementary Material for further information regarding the model estimates and performance). In summary, the models showed that the higher the visual memory score the lower the risk of being classified in the group with the worst cognitive outlook.

The age of participants at baseline significantly influenced the performance of all tests over time. Older participants scored lower on all measures, regardless of the diagnostic group (SCC-stable, MCI-stable, MCI-worsened). The influence of age on the performance in the CANTAB visual memory tests of old adults with MCI and without cognitive impairment has been documented in cross-sectional studies (Juncos-Rabadán et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, Pereiro and Lojo-Seoane2014a). The current findings add new evidence from a longitudinal design.

The study findings also show a main effect of Group, with the MCI-worsened group obtaining the worst scores in all CANTAB measures used at the three evaluation times. This group comprised participants with greater cognitive impairment, who were found to have progressed to multiple-domain MCI or dementia at either of the follow-up evaluations. The profile with the worst performance in visual memory tests of multiple-domain MCI has already been shown in previous studies (Juncos-Rabadán et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Pereiro, Facal, Reboredo and Lojo-Seoane2014b). Our results support the capacity of the CANTAB visual memory tests to show different performance profiles and discriminate between groups in the cognitive continuum from normal ageing to dementia, and suggest the use of these tests for early diagnosis of cognitive impairment. The findings obtained with CANTAB scores are consistent with some additional analyses done to verify that cognitive decline is significantly more pronounced in MCI groups. The findings showed that the changes differed significantly in the three study groups and that the individuals included in the MCI-worsened group showed the most negative changes in the general cognitive performance.

Regarding the main effect of the variable Evaluation Time, the PAL test was the only measure that indicated significant differences at the three evaluation moments in all participants. This significant main effect adds new evidence to previous studies on the utility of the PAL to assess visual memory and learning in old adults with and without cognitive impairment (Fowler, Saling, Conway, Semple, & & Louis, Reference Fowler, Saling, Conway, Semple and & Louis2002; Junkkila et al. Reference Junkkila, Oja, Laine and Karrasch2012; O'Connell et al. Reference Oltra-Cucarella, Sánchez-Sansegundo, Lipnicki, Crawford, Lipton and Katz2004; Polcher et al. 2017). Moreover, our results indicate that the PAL measure can detect changes in longitudinal performance related to evolution along a continuum of cognitive decline. Taking into account that longitudinal research is scarce, this finding is an important contribution and adds evidence to the pioneering work by Blackwell et al. (Reference Blackwell, Sahakian, Vesey, Semple, Robbins and Hodges2004), who observed that the same CANTAB measure was significantly correlated with the degree of subsequent cognitive deterioration in the early stages of AD.

The most interesting findings of the present study are the significant interactions between evaluation time × group in the PAL and DMS measures. Regarding the PAL total errors adjusted-6 shapes, the interaction was significant for the MCI-stable and the MCI-worsened groups, indicating the existence of specific longitudinal patterns of performance for each. The marginal means indicate a small increase in errors in the SCC-stable group between the baseline and the follow-up evaluations, while in both MCI groups the errors increased significantly in the same periods. The increase was more important for the MCI-worsened group. The differences in PAL temporal patterns indicate a decline in the performance over time for all groups; however, they also enable discrimination between the least cognitively impaired group (SCC-stable) and the MCI groups, as well as between the MCI group that remain stable (MCI-stable) and the MCI groups in which further deterioration occurs (MCI-worsened). Our findings add a new perspective to those reported by Cacciamani et al. (Reference Cacciamani, Salvadori, Eusebi, Lisetti, Luchetti, Calabresi and Parnetti2018), who observed a marked improvement in PAL when comparing the baseline performance with the 6-month follow-up, but no difference in performance between 6- and 12-month follow-ups. This improvement may be the result of a practice effect due to the short follow-up period; however, the practice effect may disappear when longer follow-up intervals between PAL tests are used in longitudinal assessments.

Regarding the DMS, the evaluation time × group interaction was only significant in the MCI-worsened group, in which the test performance declined over time. The performance of the other two groups, SSC-stable and MCI-stable, did not vary significantly. The evaluation time × group interaction was not significant for either the PRM total per cent correct or SSP length. However, the estimated marginal means showed significant differences between SCC-stable and MCI-worsened groups, indicating a clear decline in the latter group over time.

The measures in which a significant evaluation time × group interaction was observed correspond to the two CANTAB tests (PAL and DMS) most closely related to EM (De Jager et al. Reference De Jager, Milwain and Budge2002; Juncos-Rabadán et al. Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Facal, Pereiro and Lojo-Seoane2014a, Reference Juncos-Rabadán, Pereiro, Facal, Reboredo and Lojo-Seoane2014b, Reference Juncos-Rabadan, Pereiro, Facal, Lojo-Seoane, Mallo and Campos-Magdaleno2016; Nathan et al. Reference O'Connell, Coen, Kidd, Warsi, Chin and Lawlor2017; Sweeney et al. Reference Van Geldrop, Heringa, van den Berg, Olde Rikkert, Biessels and Kessels2000). PAL involves visuospatial EM and learning, and DMS involves short-term memory of complex visual patterns. Decline in EM has been described as one of the most potent predictors of progression to Alzheimer's disease (Belleville et al. Reference Belleville, Sylvain-Roy, de Boysson and Ménard2008; Landau et al. Reference Landau, Harvey, Madison, Reiman, Foster, Aisen and Jagust2010), and our results show that the PAL total errors adjusted-6 shapes and the DMS total per cent correct enable detection of longitudinal changes that may be indicative of progression in the continuum of cognitive deterioration.

However, the measures the PRM total per cent correct and the SSP length that differed significantly between groups (group main effect) did not indicate differences between groups over time (evaluation time × group interaction). PRM involves memory and subsequent recognition of sequences of visual patterns, which may be related to the attentional span capacity, which is associated with WM. In previous studies, contradictory findings regarding span length as a measure of WM that differentiates participants according to diagnosis and progression have been reported. While a large number of studies support the existence of impairment in span length prior to the diagnosis of dementia (Belleville, Fouquet, Hudon, Zomahoun, & Croteau, Reference Belleville, Fouquet, Hudon, Zomahoun and Croteau2017; Economou et al. Reference Economou, Papageorgiou and Karageorgiou2006; Gagnon & Belleville, Reference Gagnon and Belleville2011; Saunders & Summers, Reference Saxton, Lopez, Ratcliff, Dulberg, Fried, Carlson and Kuller2010; Van Geldrop et al. Reference Weisberg2015), other studies obtained contradictory or non-meaningful results (Griffith et al. Reference Griffith, Netson, Harrell, Zamrini, Brockington and Marson2006; Guarch, Marcos, Salamero, Gastó, & Blesa, Reference Guarch, Marcos, Salamero, Gastó and Blesa2008; Kessels, Overbeek, & Bouman, Reference Kessels, Overbeek and Bouman2015), questioning the value of the measure for early detection of cognitive impairment. Our findings indicate that the PRM measure and the SSP cannot differentiate longitudinal patterns between groups.

We conclude that visual EM declines in people with MCI over time and that this decline may be a cognitive indicator of the progression in the continuum ranging from the stage characterized by the presence of cognitive complaints without objective cognitive impairment to dementia, through the different levels of severity of MCI. PAL total errors adjusted-6 shapes outcome, and DMS per cent correct total measures differentiate the changes in participants in the continuum of cognitive deterioration: people with and without objective deterioration, and people who worsen or remain stable over time. In addition, the between-evaluation intervals used in longitudinal studies should be wide enough to prevent practice effects.

Membership of groups characterized by a change in cognitive status developed at the second follow-up stage (T 2) has proven to be accurate in the light of different types of evidence. First, a different pattern of change was observed in CANTAB measurements according to this classification. Secondly, different patterns of change were also observed in other cognitive scores such as MMSE and CAMCOG-R when comparing the groups included in this study. Finally, comparison of membership in groups obtained by the procedure described in this study with a classification obtained by means of non-parametric clustering of multivariate trajectories (i.e. individual trajectories in the 4 CANTAB scores) revealed a similarity index of 0.74, which indicates a good level of agreement. In summary, we demonstrated that the visual CANTAB scores (a) are useful for predicting cognitive evolution in the time-period included in this study, (b) differ over time depending on the change in the cognitive status of individuals, and (c) allow researchers to classify individuals consistently in comparison with other cognitive outcomes (i.e. clinical assessment at the second follow-up).

The limitations of the present study include the fact that only one group of patients with MCI that worsened over time was considered. By not having a larger number of participants in whom deterioration tended to worsen, it was not possible to differentiate people who progress to multiple-domain MCI from those who progress to dementia, and both were included within the same group. This hinders interpretation of the results, as although the participants progress in the same direction of the continuum of cognitive deterioration, they show important differences regarding the degree of cognitive impairment and functional capacity. Differences between both types of participants in their CANTAB longitudinal profiles should be considered in future studies. On the other hand, the interval of 36 months between baseline and the final evaluation may not be long enough for a full assessment of the progress. We hope in the future to be able to collect longitudinal data over a longer period of time, as the current longitudinal research is still ongoing. We expect to conduct a third follow-up evaluation to assess changes that have occurred in a period of approximately 54 months (4.5 years) after baseline.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720001142.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported through FEDER founds by the Spanish Directorate General of Scientific and Technical Research (Project Ref. PSI2014-55316-C3-1-R), the National Research Agency (Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities) (Project Ref. PSI2017-89389-C2-1-R) and by the Galician Government (Consellería de Cultura, Educación e Ordenación Universitaria; axudas para a consolidación e estruturación de unidades de investigación competitivas do Sistema Universitario de Galicia; GI-1807-USC: Ref. ED431-2017/27).

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in Seoul 2008.

References

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., & Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association work groups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's and Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association, 7(3), 270279. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alescio-Lautier, B., Michel, B. F., Herrera, C., Elahmadi, A., Chambon, C., Touzet, C., & Paban, V. (2007). Visual and visuospatial short-term memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: Role of attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 19481960. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alladi, S., Arnold, R., Mitchell, J., Nestor, P. J., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment: Applicability of research criteria in a memory clinic and characterization of cognitive profile. Psychological Medicine, 36(4), 507515. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Barbeau, E. J., Ranjeva, J. P., Didic, M., Confort-Gouny, S., Felician, O., Soulier, E., & Poncet, M. (2008). Profile of memory impairment and gray matter loss in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 46(4), 10091019. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belleville, S., Fouquet, C., Hudon, C., Zomahoun, H. T. V., & Croteau, J., & Consortium for the Early Identification of Alzheimer's disease-Quevec. (2017). Neuropsychological measures that predict progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's type dementia in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 27(4), 328353. doi:10.1007/s11065-017-9361-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belleville, S., Sylvain-Roy, S., de Boysson, C., & Ménard, M. C. (2008). Characterizing the memory changes in persons with mild cognitive impairment. Progress in Brain Research, 169, 365375. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00023-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedet, M. J., & Alejandre, M. A. (1998). TAVEC: Test de Aprendizaje Verbal de España-Complutense. Madrid: TEA ediciones.Google Scholar
Benedet, M. J., & Seisdedos, N. (1996). Evaluación Clínica de las Quejas de Memoria en la Vida Cotidiana. Madrid: Editorial Médica Panamericana.Google Scholar
Blackwell, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Vesey, R., Semple, J. M., Robbins, T. W., & Hodges, J. R. (2004). Detecting dementia: Novel neuropsychological markers of preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 17(1-2), 4248. doi:10.1159/000074081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brambati, S. M., Belleville, S., Kergoat, M. J., Chayer, C., Gauthier, S., & Joubert, S. (2009). Single- and multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Two sides of the same coin? Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 28(6), 541549. doi:10.1159/000255240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacciamani, F., Salvadori, N., Eusebi, P., Lisetti, V., Luchetti, E., Calabresi, P., & Parnetti, L. (2018). Evidence of practice effect in CANTAB spatial working memory test in a cohort of patients with mild cognitive impairment. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 25(3), 237248. doi:10.1080/23279095.2017.1286346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos-Magdaleno, M., Díaz-Bóveda, R., Juncos-Rabadán, O., Facal, D., & Pereiro, A. X. (2016). Learning and serial effects on verbal memory in mild cognitive impairment. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 23(4), 237250. doi:10.1080/23279095.2015.1053887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CANTAB® [Cognitive assessment software]. Cambridge Cognition. (2012). All rights reserved. www.cantab.com.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, S. R., Blackwell, A. D., Nathan, P. J., Hammond, G., Robbins, T. W., Hodges, J. R., & Sahakian, B. J. (2011). Differential cognitive deterioration in dementia: A two year longitudinal study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 24(1), 125136. doi:10.3233/JAD-2010-100450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Anna, F., Felician, O., Barbeau, E., Mancini, J., Didic, M., & Ceccaldi, M. (2014). Cognitive changes in mild cognitive impairment patients with impaired visual recognition memory. Neuropsychology, 28(1), 98105. doi:10.1037/neu0000032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Defrancesco, M., Marksteiner, J., Deisenhammer, E., Kemmler, G., Djurdjevic, T., & Schocke, M. (2013). Impact of white matter lesions and cognitive deficits on conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 34(3), 665672. doi:10.3233/JAD-122095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Jager, C. A., Milwain, E., & Budge, M. (2002). Early detection of isolated memory deficits in the elderly: The need for more sensitive neuropsychological tests. Psychological Medicine, 32(3), 483491. doi:10.1017/S003329170200524X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. (1987). California verbal learning test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
De Rover, M., Pironti, V. A., McCabe, J. A., Acosta-Carbonero, J., Arana, F. S., Morein-Zamir, S., et al. (2011). Hippocampal dysfunction in patients with mild cognitive impairment: A functional neuroimaging study of a visuospatial paired associates learning task. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 20602070. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Didic, M., Felician, O., Barbeau, E. J., Mancini, J., Latger-Florence, C., Tramoni, E., & Ceccaldi, M. (2013). Impaired visual recognition memory predicts Alzheimer's disease in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 35(5-6), 291299. doi:10.1159/000347203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., Dekosky, S. T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., & Scheltens, P. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurology, 6(8), 734746. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Economou, A., Papageorgiou, S., & Karageorgiou, C. (2006). Working-delayed memory difference detects mild cognitive impairment without being affected by age and education. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(4), 528535. doi:10.1080/13803390590949340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Facal, D., Guàrdia-Olmos, J., & Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2015). Diagnostic transitions in mild cognitive impairment by use of simple Markov models. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 30(7), 669676. doi:10.1002/gps.4197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, K, Saling, S., Conway, M. M., Semple, E. L., & & Louis, J. M., W. (2002). Paired associate performance in the early detection of DAT. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(1), 5871. doi:10.1017/S1355617702811067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. (2016). Applied regression analysis and generalized linear models (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.Google Scholar
Gagnon, L. G., & Belleville, S. (2011). Working memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: Contribution of forgetting and predictive value of complex span tasks. Neuropsychology, 25(2), 226236. doi:10.1037/a0020919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, R. H., Netson, K. L., Harrell, L. E., Zamrini, E. Y., Brockington, J. C., & Marson, D. C. (2006). Amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Diagnostic outcomes and clinical prediction over a two-year time period. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(2), 166175. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guarch, J., Marcos, T., Salamero, M., Gastó, C., & Blesa, R. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment: A risk indicator of later dementia, or a preclinical phase of the disease? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23(3), 257265. doi:10.1002/gps.1871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, J. W., Kim, T. H., Lee, S. B., Park, J. H., Lee, J. J., & Kim, K. W. (2012). Predictive validity and diagnostic stability of mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 8, 553559. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.08.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huppert, F., Jorm, A. F., Brayne, C., Girling, D. M., Barkely, C., Beardsall, L., & Paykel, E. S. (1996). Psychometric properties of the CAMCOG. Ageing, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 3, 14. doi:10.1080/13825589608256624.Google Scholar
Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., & Chételat, G., … Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's and Dementia, 10(6), 844852. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juncos-Rabadán, O., Facal, D., Pereiro, A. X., & Lojo-Seoane, C. (2014a). Visual memory profiling with CANTAB in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) subtypes. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29(10), 10401049. doi:10.1002/gps.4095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juncos-Rabadan, O., Pereiro, A. X., Facal, D., Lojo-Seoane, C., Mallo, S. C., & Campos-Magdaleno, M. (2016). Longitudinal changes in visual memory in mild cognitive impairment versus normal aging in people with subjective cognitive complaint. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 12(7), 754P755. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2016.06.1438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juncos-Rabadán, O., Pereiro, A. X., Facal, D., Reboredo, A., & Lojo-Seoane, C. (2014b). Do the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery episodic memory measures discriminate amnestic mild cognitive impairment? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29(6), 602609. doi:10.1002/gps.4042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juncos-Rabadán, O., Pereiro, A. X., Facal, D., Rodríguez, N., Lojo, C., Caamaño, J. A., & Eiroa, P. (2012). Prevalence and correlates of cognitive impairment in adults with subjective cognitive complaints in primary care centres. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 33(4), 226232. doi:10.1159/000338607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Junkkila, J., Oja, S., Laine, M., & Karrasch, M. (2012). Applicability of the CANTAB-PAL computerized memory test in identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 34(2), 8389. doi:10.1159/000342116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessels, R. P. C., Overbeek, A., & Bouman, Z. (2015). Assessment of verbal and visuospatial working memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's dementia. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 9(3), 301305. doi:10.1590/1980-57642015DN93000014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landau, S. M., Harvey, D., Madison, C. M., Reiman, E. M., Foster, N. I., Aisen, P. S., & Jagust, W. J. (2010). Comparing predictors of conversion and decline in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 75(3), 230238. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e8e8b8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist, 9(3), 179186.10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenehan, M. E., Summers, M. J., Saunders, N. L., Summers, J. J., & Vickers, J. C. (2016). Does the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) distinguish between cognitive domains in healthy older adults?. Assessment, 23(2), 163172. doi:10.1177/1073191115581474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Pousa, S. (2003). CAMDEX-R: prueba de exploración Cambridge revisada para la valoración de los trastornos mentales en la vejez. TEA Ediciones: Adaptación española. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Mistridis, P., Krumm, S., Monsch, A. U., Berres, M., & Taylor, K. I. (2015). The 12 years preceding mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: The temporal emergence of cognitive decline. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 48(4), 10951107. doi:10.3233/JAD-150137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molinuevo, J. L., Rabin, L. A., Amariglio, R., Buckley, R., Dubois, B., & Ellis, K. A. (2017). Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 13(3), 296311. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2016.09.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nathan, P. J., Lim, Y. Y., Abbott, R., Galluzzi, S., Marizzoni, M., & Babiloni, C., … PharmaCog Consortium. (2017). Association between CSF biomarkers, hippocampal volume and cognitive function in patients with amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). Neurology of Aging, 53, 110. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.01.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connell, H., Coen, R., Kidd, N., Warsi, M., Chin, A. V., & Lawlor, B. A. (2004). Early detection of Alzheimer's Disease (AD) using the CANTAB paired Associates Learning Test. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(12), 12071208. doi:10.1002/gps.1180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oltra-Cucarella, J., Sánchez-Sansegundo, M., Lipnicki, D. M., Crawford, J. D., Lipton, R. B., & Katz, M. J., … Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium (COSMIC). (2018). Visual memory tests enhance the identification of amnestic MCI cases at greater risk of Alzheimer's disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 25, 110. doi:10.1017/S104161021800145X.Google Scholar
Owen, A. M., Beksinska, M., Jamnes, M., Leigh, P. N., Summers, B. A., Marsden, C. D., & Robbins, T. W. (1993). Visuospatial memory deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 31(7), 627644. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90135-M.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereiro, A. X., Ramos-Lema, S., Juncos-Rabadán, O., Facal, D., & Lojo-Seoane, C. (2015). Normative scores of the Cambridge Cognitive Examination-Revised in healthy Spanish population. Psicothema, 27(1), 3239. doi:10.7334/psicothema2014.169.Google ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 183194. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C., Lopez, O., Armstrong, M. J., Getchius, T. S., Ganguli, M., Gloss, D., & Sager, M. (2018). Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment: Report of the guideline development, dissemination, and implementation subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology, 90(3), 126135. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000004826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., & Sarkar, D., & R Core Team (2018). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme.Google Scholar
Polcher, A., Frommann, I., Koppara, A., Wolfsgruber, S., Jessen, F., & Wagner, M. (2017). Face-name associative recognition deficits in subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 56(3), 11851196. doi:10.3233/JAD-160637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Reijs, B. L., Ramakers, I. H., Köhler, S., Teunissen, C. E., Koel-Simmelink, M., Nathan, P. J., & Vandenberghe, R. (2017). Memory correlates of Alzheimer's disease cerebrospinal fluid markers: A longitudinal cohort study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 60(3), 11191128. doi:10.3233/JAD-160766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahakian, B. J., Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L., Heald, A., Levy, R., Philpot, M., & Robbins, T. W. (1988). A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain, 111, 695718. doi:10.1093/brain/111.3.695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, N. L. J., & Summers, M. J. (2010). Attention and working memory deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(4), 350357. doi:10.1080/13803390903042379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxton, J., Lopez, O. L., Ratcliff, G., Dulberg, C., Fried, L. P., Carlson, M. C., & Kuller, L. (2004). Preclinical Alzheimer disease: Neuropsychological test performance 1.5 to 8 years prior to onset. Neurology, 63(12), 23412347. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000147470.58328.50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Summers, M. J., & Saunders, N. L. J. (2012). Neuropsychological measures predict decline to Alzheimer's dementia from mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 26(4), 498508. doi:10.1037/a0028576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swainson, R., Hodges, J. R., Galton, C. J., Semple, J., Michael, A., Dunn, B. D., & Sahakian, B. J. (2001). Early detection and differential diagnosis of Alzheimer's Disease and depression with neuropsychological tasks. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 12(4), 265280. doi:10.1159/000051269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweeney, J. A., Kmiec, J. A., & Kupfer, D. J. (2000). Neuropsychologic impairments in bipolar and unipolar mood disorders on the CANTAB neurocognitive battery. Biological Psychiatry, 48(7), 674684. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00910-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Geldrop, B., Heringa, S. M., van den Berg, E., Olde Rikkert, M. G., Biessels, G. J., & Kessels, R. P. (2015). Working memory binding and episodic memory formation in aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37(5), 538548. doi:10.1080/13803395.2015.1037722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, S. (2014). Applied linear regression (4th ed.). Hoboken, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Westerberg, C., Mayes, A., Florczak, S. M., Chen, Y., Creery, J., Parrish, T., Weintraub, S., & Paller, K. A. (2013). Distinct medial temporal contributions to different forms of recognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 51(12), 24502461. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean and standard deviations (in parentheses) of the demographic and neuropsychological measures at baseline for the three groups: subjective cognitive complaints (SCC) that remain stable (SCC-stable); mild cognitive impairment that remains stable (MCI-stable); mild cognitive impairment that worsened (MCI-worsened)

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Estimated marginal means and errors bars from Model 1 for PAL, PRM, DMS and SSP in the three groups across the three evaluation times. SE, standard error; BL, baseline assessment; T1, Time 1 assessment; T2, Time 2 assessment.

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of models compared for PAL total errors adjusted-6 shapes. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Figure 3

Table 3. Summary of model comparison for PRM total per cent correct. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Figure 4

Table 4. Summary of compared models for DMS total per cent correct. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Figure 5

Table 5. Summary of models compared for SSP length. All models include random effects for intercepts and age at baseline as a covariate

Supplementary material: File

Campos-Magdaleno et al. supplementary material

Campos-Magdaleno et al. supplementary material

Download Campos-Magdaleno et al. supplementary material(File)
File 229.6 KB