Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:27:55.978Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Changes in the dynamic network structure of PTSD symptoms pre-to-post combat

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2019

Adva Segal*
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
Ilan Wald
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
Gad Lubin
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health, Medical Corps, Israel Defense Forces, Ramat Gan, Israel
Eyal Fruchter
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health, Medical Corps, Israel Defense Forces, Ramat Gan, Israel
Keren Ginat
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health, Medical Corps, Israel Defense Forces, Ramat Gan, Israel
Ariel Ben Yehuda
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health, Medical Corps, Israel Defense Forces, Ramat Gan, Israel
Daniel S. Pine
Affiliation:
National Institutes of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
Yair Bar-Haim
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
*
Author for correspondence: Adva Segal, E-mail: advasegal@tau.ac.il
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Combat exposure is associated with elevated risk for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Despite considerable research on PTSD symptom clustering, it remains unknown how symptoms of PTSD re-organize following combat. Network analysis provides a powerful tool to examine such changes.

Methods

A network analysis approach was taken to examine how symptom networks change from pre- to post-combat using longitudinal prospective data from a cohort of infantry male soldiers (Mage = 18.8 years). PTSD symptoms measured using the PTSD Checklist (PCL) were assessed after 6 months of combat training but before deployment and again after 6 months of combat (Ns = 910 and 725 at pre-deployment and post-combat, respectively)

Results

Stronger connectivity between PTSD symptoms was observed post-combat relative to pre-deployment (global strength values of the networks were 7.54 pre v. 7.92 post; S = .38, p < 0.05). Both the re-experiencing symptoms cluster (1.92 v. 2.12; S = .20, p < 0.03) and the avoidance symptoms cluster (2.61 v. 2.96; S = .35, p < 0.005) became more strongly inter-correlated post-combat. Centrality estimation analyses revealed that psychological reaction to triggers was central and linked the intrusion and avoidance sub-clusters at post-combat. The strength of associations between the arousal and reactivity symptoms cluster remained stable over time (1.85 v. 1.83; S = .02, p = .92).

Conclusions

Following combat, PTSD symptoms and particularly the re-experiencing and avoidance clusters become more strongly inter-correlated, indicating high centrality of trigger-reactivity symptoms.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) affects anywhere between 5% and 30% of combat deployed soldiers (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Wilk, Riviere, McGurk, Castro and Hoge2010; Wald et al., Reference Wald, Degnan, Gorodetsky, Charney, Fox, Fruchter, Goldman, Lubin, Pine and Bar-Haim2013), with many soldiers experiencing sub-clinical symptoms associated with considerable psychological and behavioral disability (Stein et al., Reference Stein, Walker, Hazen and Forde1997). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) has traditionally conceptualized PTSD as involving re-experiencing, avoidance, and hyperarousal clusters, with the addition of altered mood and cognition in DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Despite considerable research and debate on symptom clustering in patients (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Kilpatrick and Schnurr2016; Guina, Reference Guina2016; Hoge, Reference Hoge2016; Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, Yehuda, Castro, McFarlane, Vermetten, Jetly, Koenen, Greenberg, Shalev, Rauch and Marmar2016), work that examines changing relations among PTSD symptoms as a function of intense combat stress in otherwise healthy participants is scarce.

A network analysis approach provides a tool to examine changes in PTSD symptom clusters as these transform over time and as a function of designated events (e.g. Armour et al., Reference Armour, Fried, Deserno, Tsai and Pietrzak2017; Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, McFarlane, Silove and Hadzi-Pavlovic2017). Network analysis is designed to examine the interplay between symptoms that constitute the presumed clinical space of a disorder. The network is comprised of nodes representing the observed variables (e.g. the different symptoms of PTSD), and edges representing the associations among these symptoms. The network is graphically visualized and statistically analyzed. The relative importance of a specific symptom in a network can be inferred by its location within the network, with symptoms that are highly correlated with other symptoms located at the center and symptoms with lessor weaker associations located more peripherally (Borsboom and Cramer, Reference Borsboom and Cramer2013; Boschloo et al., Reference Boschloo, Van Borkulo, Rhemtulla, Keyes, Borsboom and Schoevers2015).The strength of a network can be estimated, and the relative connectivity of different networks can be compared (van Borkulo, Reference van Borkulo2015).

Network analysis approaches assume that a disorder is defined by a causal system of psychopathological symptoms that are mutually dependent. As these associations get stronger a clinical disorder might emerge (Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Curtiss and McNally2016). For example, combat can lead to stress reactions, which could influence other symptoms such as emotional distress, which may in turn increase arousal and trigger nightmares or other re-experiencing symptoms. As these symptoms become more strongly inter-related, a full-blown PTSD may surface. Theories of PTSD have attempted to capture the complexity of relations between symptoms emerging following traumatic exposure (Keane et al., Reference Keane, Zimering and Caddell1985; Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998; Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Keane and Resick2007). For example, Horowitz (Reference Horowitz1986) suggested that traumatic exposure involves an intrusive phase associated with arousal and re-experiencing symptoms, and a denial phase characterized by efforts to avoid such intrusive repetitions combined with numbness and amnesia. According to this theory, fluctuations between intrusion and avoidance allow processing of the trauma and as a result the intensity of symptoms decrease over time. Failure in these processes may lead to persistent post-traumatic reactions. Network analyses can contribute to the understating of complex interplays between different PTSD symptoms, shedding light on the unique roles of particular symptoms, and characterize the associations between them.

To date, network analysis studies of PTSD symptoms typically focused on patients. Specifically, two prior cross-sectional studies applied networks analysis to chronic PTSD symptoms. A study of earthquake survivors revealed high centrality for hypervigilance and foreshortened future, while also noting associations among other clusters (McNally et al., Reference McNally, Robinaugh, Wu, Wang, Deserno and Borsboom2015). Another study of network structure found clinically significant DSM-5 PTSD symptoms in veterans to involve high centrality of negative trauma-related emotions, flashbacks, detachment, and physiological cue reactivity (Armour et al., Reference Armour, Fried, Deserno, Tsai and Pietrzak2017). Only one prior study applied network analysis in a longitudinal design (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, McFarlane, Silove and Hadzi-Pavlovic2017), comparing network structure 1 week following trauma and 12 months later. Re-experiencing symptoms were central to other symptoms in the acute phase, with intrusions and physiological reactivity being the most central symptoms in the networks. Overall network connectivity was significantly stronger at 12 months than in the acute phase. In addition, the network associations among the re-experiencing symptoms strengthened at 12 months, and physiological reactivity was strongly associated with exaggerated startle response, which was also associated with hypervigilance.

Here, for the first time, we longitudinally describe how associations between PTSD symptoms evolve over time in newly recruited infantry soldiers preparing for combat deployment and then after combat. The first assessment occurred following 6 months of intensive combat training (see below for description) but before deployment to real combat. Soldiers were assessed again after 6 months of combat deployment where they were exposed to a wide array of combat experiences. Comparing the associations between PTSD symptoms at these unique time points in the deployment cycle can provide insights as to how stress symptoms develop under different levels of military-related stress and demands. Network analysis theory assumes that strength of inter-relation between symptoms can give rise to clinical psychopathology. Therefore, although network analyses are somewhat exploratory in nature, we tentatively hypothesized that network strength would be greater post-combat than at pre-deployment, given that some PTSD symptoms may be more loosely related when stress levels are moderate during pre-deployment training relative to their association post real combat exposure.

Methods

Participants

Participants were newly recruited Israel Defense Force (IDF) infantry soldiers (all male, M age = 18.8 years, s.d. = 1.0, range = 18–24). In total, 84.8% of the participants were born in Israel (5.4% in the USA, 1.8% in Russia, and 7.9% in other countries). Participants were physically and mentally healthy and eligible for mandatory military service in the IDF. Mean number of years of formal education was 12.09 (s.d. = .77, 80% with matriculation). Participants were asked to rate their levels of PTSD symptoms at two time points: (a) following a 6 months of basic and advanced combat training prior to their first combat deployment (pre-deployment, n = 910); and (b) following 6 months of combat (post-combat, n = 725). These two time points involved progressive increase in military-related stress, with milder stress at training, followed by much higher stress in deployment while performing combat missions on daily bases. In total, 604 participants had a full data set without any missing data for both time points.

Combat experiences

Self-reported combat experiences were collected using the Combat Experiences Scale (Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, Castro, Messer, McGurk, Cotting and Koffman2004; Wald et al., Reference Wald, Degnan, Gorodetsky, Charney, Fox, Fruchter, Goldman, Lubin, Pine and Bar-Haim2013). Data were collected between 2008 and 2010. The pre-deployment data refer to 6 months of intensive basic and advanced combat training. During the training period, soldiers were assigned to occasional guarding and patrol missions, typically in calm and less challenging areas, hence, some minor reporting of combat experiences may be noted for this pre-deployment assessment. However, at this pre-deployment assessment, the participants were not yet exposed to real combat. The participants were then deployed to routine security missions in intensive conflict zones. Their activity during combat deployment included close-quarter arrests and take-downs, patrol, and riot containment (i.e. thrown stones, Molotov cocktail bombs, stabbing attempts, and occasionally receiving incoming rocket, mortar, or small-arms fire). Participants provided written informed consent at each of the data collection points. The Institutional Review Boards of Tel Aviv University, the IDF, and the Israeli Ministry of Health approved the study.

Measurement of PTSD symptoms

Following Hoge et al. (Reference Hoge, Castro, Messer, McGurk, Cotting and Koffman2004), symptoms of PTSD were evaluated with the PTSD Checklist (PCL, specific stressor version; Blanchard et al., Reference Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley and Forneris1996) in relation to the combat training epoch and the combat deployment epoch for pre-deployment and post-combat, respectively. This self-report questionnaire consists of 17 items assessing the presence and severity of PTSD symptoms corresponding to DSM-IV criteria, reflecting three clusters of symptoms: intrusion, avoidance, and hyperarousal. Scores range from 17 to 85, with higher scores reflecting greater symptom severity. Cronbach's α of the PCL in the current study was 0.90 pre-deployment and 0.93 post-combat.

Data analysis

Data analyses were conducted between October 2017 and February 2018. The data were analyzed using the free software environment R, and was carried out in four steps: (1) we first estimated the pre-deployment and post-combat network structures and provide visualization of these; (2) we then computed the differences in connectivity between the pre-deployment and post-combat networks using the Network Comparison Test; (3) we evaluated the importance of nodes by computing centrality indices; and (4) we estimated the stability and accuracy of the networks.

Handling of missing data

Routines for handling of missing data in networks analysis are still underdeveloped. For overall networks connectivity estimation (n = 910 pre-deployment and n = 725 post-combat), we followed Briganti et al. (Reference Briganti, Kempenaers, Braun, Fried and Linkowski2018) and Santos et al. (Reference Santos, Kossakowski, Schwartz, Beeber and Fried2018), applying pairwise complete observations, such that for each computed partial correlation, the full available data were used. For the longitudinal comparison (NCT), we included only participants that provided complete data on PTSD symptoms at both measurement time points (n = 604).

Network estimation

We estimated the networks’ partial correlation coefficients at pre-deployment and post-combat using the Gaussian Graphical Model (EBICglasso) option in the R-package qgraph. This structural network is composed of ‘nodes’ representing PTSD symptoms and ‘edges’ representing the partial correlations between them (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Hastie and Tibshirani2008). This information is visualized to exhaust the relevant information in a simple and efficient way using the Fruchterman–Reingold's algorithm (Fruchterman and Reingold, Reference Fruchterman and Reingold1991) that places nodes close to each other or further apart according to the degree of their partial correlation. Controlling for all symptoms, the EBICglasso function employs a LASSO regularization method, which minimizes false edge detection by setting small edges to zero (Tibshirani, Reference Tibshirani2011; Van Borkulo et al., Reference van Borkulo, Borsboom, Epskamp, Blanken, Boschloo, Schoevers and Waldorp2014). The size of partial correlations between symptoms is visualized by edge thickness, which we used to explore changes between symptoms associations within a network and between the two networks. The maximum edge value was set to 0.45, the strongest value identified across networks; the minimum edge value was set to 0.03, to simplify interpretation of the networks graph.

Our primary analyses were guided by the DSM definition of the PTSD clusters (re-experiencing, avoidance, and arousal). However, to empirically identify communities of symptoms in the networks, we also applied the spinglass algorithm that is based on the notion that nodes of the same community would be connected by edges, whereas nodes of different communities would not (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Algesheimer and Tessone2016). To enhance stability, we run our data through the algorithm 1000 times and extracted the communities with the highest frequency. Online Supplementary Fig. S1 depicts communities of PCL symptoms in pre-deployment and post-combat networks.

Longitudinal comparisons between networks

Longitudinal comparisons were conducted using the Network Comparisons Tests (NCT; van Borkulo, Reference van Borkulo2015). This test requires complete data sets without missing data for both compared time points. Therefore, for these specific comparisons, 604 participants with fully complete data were used. Specifically, we longitudinally compared pre-deployment and post-combat: (1) general network strength connectivity; and (b) specific PTSD clusters (intrusion, avoidance, and hyperarousal). These comparisons used a permutation technique with 1000 iterations to index significant difference in network strength values.

Centrality estimation

Centrality estimation was used to identify the importance of specific symptoms in the networks. Three measures were computed: betweenness, reflecting the degree to which a node is located in the shortest path between any two nodes; closeness, estimating the average distance from a node to all other nodes, as reflected by the inverse of all shortest path lengths between one node and all other nodes; and strength, the sum of all weighted edges of a node with other nodes. The higher the degree of a centrality measure, the more central a given node is in a network (Opsahl et al., Reference Opsahl, Agneessens and Skvoretz2010).

Accuracy and stability estimation

A major challenge in networks analysis is estimating a network's stability and accuracy. We adopted the method developed by Epskamp et al. (Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018). First the accuracy of the edged weights of the network is estimated by drawing bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (CIs) around the edged weights. Smaller CIs represent greater accuracy in estimation. A second analysis relates to the stability of the order of the centrality measures applying a sub-setting bootstrap technique in which participants are dropped from the network analysis and a re-estimation of the network is conducted, producing acentrality stability coefficient (CS-coefficient), where values are considered sufficient when they are ⩾0.25 and preferably ⩾0.50. Finally, to complement these two analyses, we used the bootstrapped difference test that compares edge-weights and nodes’ strength and show whether they differ significantly from each other. All bootstrapping routines used 1000 iterations.

Results

Combat exposure and severity of PTSD symptom

In total, 645 participants had a full data set for both time points. Only 16% of the soldiers reported experiencing more than a single combat event at pre-deployment. In contrast, 45% of the soldiers reported serious combat exposure post-deployment. Indeed, the two time points differed markedly on the Combat Experiences Scale, t (644) = 21.5, p < 0.0001 (see Table 1 for combat experiences).

Table 1. Frequency of specific combat experiences pre-deployment and post-combat

Soldiers reported average PCL scores of 25.35 (s.d. = 9.51) and 25.30 (s.d. = 10.67) at pre-deployment and post-combat, respectively. These relatively low PCL scores reflect the generally healthy character of this cohort of young infantry soldiers. Of note, all participants had successfully passed rigorous psychological screens and received clean bills of mental health prior to the pre-deployment assessment. Nevertheless, 3.3% reported symptoms at a probable PTSD level (defined by having one intrusion symptom, three avoidance symptoms, two hyperarousal symptoms, and a total PCL score ⩾50) in the pre-deployment assessment. Probable PTSD frequency increased to 4.8% post-combat, χ2(1) = 28.73, p < 0.0001.

PTSD symptoms networks

Figure 1 depicts PCL symptom network structures pre-deployment and post-combat. Analyses also reveal changes in cross-cluster connections following combat. Overall, positive connections emerged between all symptoms within the two networks, with stronger global network connectivity post-combat (7.92) relative to pre-deployment (7.54), S = .38, p < 0.05. For parsimony, we highlight the three PTSD clusters measured by the PCL: intrusion, avoidance, and hyperarousal.

Fig. 1. Network structures of PTSD symptoms (PCL): (a) pre-deployment; and (b) post-combat. Each network consists of 17 nodes representing the 17 symptoms of PTSD per DSM-IV, and edges (lines) represent the partial correlation coefficients between these nodes (symptoms), that is, the connections between two nodes after controlling for all other connections in the network. Black edges represent positive associations and red edges represent negative associations. Edge thickness and brightness reflect the association's (edge) strength (e.g. the B4–B5 edge is thicker at post-combat relative to pre-deployment indicating a stronger partial correlation between these two symptoms post-combat). To simplify interpretation and network visualization, the locations of the nodes have been kept to identical positions between the two networks by taking the mean coordinators of nodes’ locations in the individual layout networks. Symptom numbers and their abbreviated descriptions are provided in the figure.

Within-cluster connections

Overall connectivity among the five intrusion symptoms (cluster B) of the PCL (intrusive thoughts, nightmares, flashbacks, upset by reminders, and physiological cue reactivity) became stronger from pre-to-post combat (the global strength values were 1.92 v. 2.12; S = .20, p < 0.03). Specifically, the connections between trauma-triggered symptoms – upset by reminders (B4) and physiological cue reactivity (B5) strengthened following combat. Enhanced connections between symptoms also appeared within the avoidance cluster (cluster C), with stronger connections following combat relative to pre-deployment (the global strength values were 2.96 v. 2.61; S = .35, p < 0.005). Furthermore, the architecture of the avoidance cluster changed following combat. Pre-deployment involved two distinct sub-clusters. One consisted of avoidance of thoughts (C1), avoidance of situations (C2), and trauma-related amnesia (C3), whereas the other encompassed disinterest in activities (C4), detachment (C5), emotional numbing (C6), and foreshortened future (C7). In contrast, post-combat all these symptoms align together as a single and more tightly connected cluster. Specifically, strong connection emerged between trauma-related amnesia (C3) and both disinterest in activities (C4), and foreshortened future (C7).

Finally, connections between the arousal and reactivity symptoms (cluster D) were stable over time (the global strength values were 1.85 v. 1.83; S = .02, p = .92). This specific network appears to be comprised of two separate sub-clusters: sleep disturbance (D1), irritability/anger (D2), and difficulty concentrating (D3) on the one hand; and hypervigilance (D4) and exaggerated startle response (D5) on the other hand.

Stable connections between symptoms not affected by combat exposure

The edge-weight bootstrapped difference test revealed that some PTSD symptoms appear to be strongly connected regardless of combat exposure, as illustrated in the summary plot (online Supplementary Fig. S2). Specifically, nightmares (B2) and flashbacks (B3); avoidance of thoughts (C1) and avoidance of situations (C2); detachment (C5) and emotional numbing (C6); irritability/anger (D2) and difficulty concentrating (D3); and hypervigilance (D4) and exaggerated startle response (D5).

Centrality analysis

The centrality stability coefficient of strength showed acceptable stability (0.52). Lower CS-coefficients were noted for betweenness (0.13) and closeness (0.21; online Supplementary Fig. S4). Therefore, our centrality interpretations are based on the strength index. Additional accuracy and stability analyses are available in Supplementary Materials (online Supplementary Figs S2–S5).

The centrality estimation analyses revealed that symptom B4 (upset by reminders) is central in the two networks but acts as a bridge symptom between the intrusion and avoidance sub-clusters only post-combat (Fig. 2). In addition, the centrality bootstrapped difference test indicated that across time, the nodes with the highest centrality strength values were: flashbacks (B3 = 1.31 and 1.12, respectively, for pre-deployment and post-combat) and upset by reminders (B4 = 1.27 and 1.43), which were significantly larger than almost half of the other symptoms in the network. The nodes with the lowest values were hypervigilance (D4 = −1.89 and −2.43) and disinterest in activities (C4 = −1.95 and 1.19; Fig. 2), which were significantly smaller than almost all other symptoms (online Supplementary Fig. S5).

Fig. 2. Centrality estimates: strength, betweenness, and closeness at pre-deployment and post-combat. Centrality indices show that across time the most central nodes are flashbacks (B3) and upset by reminders (B4), which have the highest values of node strength (positive scores), whereas the least central nodes are hypervigilance (D4) and disinterest in activities (C4) with the lowest strength values (negative scores). The centrality interpretations are based on the strength index that had a good stability across time. Centrality indices are shown as standardized z-scores.

Discussion

The current study examined PTSD symptom dynamics following combat exposure. A network analysis approach contrasted correlation systems to generate data on changes in PTSD symptom associations and organization. Results indicate that intrusion symptoms became more tightly connected following combat, suggesting a consolidation process of sorts. Specifically, symptoms of reactivity to triggers (i.e. upset by reminders – B4 and physiological cue reactivity – B5) became more strongly connected to each other and central in the PTSD symptoms network. These changes suggest that reaction to triggers represents a key factor in PTSD symptom evolution following potentially traumatic exposure. This finding is consistent with prior results indicating centrality of such symptoms after exposure to trauma (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, McFarlane, Silove and Hadzi-Pavlovic2017). We assessed PTSD symptoms following 6 months of combat. It would be of interest to assess whether the centrality of reaction to triggers remains dominant following a longer period. It is conceivable that in cases of prolonged and chronic PTSD (e.g. several years) other sets of symptoms (e.g. avoidance) may become centralized, or alternatively, network strength may again reduce as most soldiers may come to healthy terms with their combat experiences. Future longitudinal research is needed to unveil such developmental trajectories.

Avoidance symptoms also became more closely associated following combat. Interestingly, trauma-related amnesia (C3) linked what appeared to be two separate avoidance sub-clusters at the pre-deployment baseline. Trauma-related amnesia may in fact play a central role in symptom evolution, despite suggestions from some models of PTSD (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Berntsen and Johansen2008), which view this particular symptom as less central. The division of the avoidance cluster into two separate sub-clusters received support in previous studies showing that avoidance of thoughts and situations (C1 and C2) reflected one cluster, and the other avoidance symptoms, C3–C7, may be part of a separate numbing/dysphoric cluster (e.g. King et al., Reference King, Leskin, King and Weathers1998; Simms et al., Reference Simms, Watson and Doebbelling2002; Elhai et al., Reference Elhai, Biehn, Armour, Klopper, Frueh and Palmieri2011). Indeed, DSM-5 now defines the avoidance cluster as distinct from a cluster of negative mood and cognition. Our findings suggest that when stress level is high (i.e. combat deployment), trauma-related amnesia (C3) may form a bridge between the two avoidance sub-clusters per DSM-IV or possibly the two distinct clusters per DSM-5.

The results also find a modified structure of associations between the intrusions and avoidance clusters following combat, primarily via strengthened connections with reactivity to triggers. As in other work, this suggests that intrusion and avoidance symptoms are inherently linked (e.g. Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, McFarlane, Silove and Hadzi-Pavlovic2017). The intrusion-avoidance dynamic plays a central role in the definition of PTSD since DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). The common assumption is that the tendency to avoid trauma reminders is a response to the distress caused by the re-experiencing symptoms. Although avoidance reduces the distress in the short run through negative reinforcement, it impedes the processing of trauma information and might prevent opportunities to change negative beliefs and perceptions relating to it (Keane et al., Reference Keane, Zimering and Caddell1985; Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998; Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000). Disrupting this vicious intrusion-avoidance cycle is a prime focus of current evidence-based treatments for PTSD, such as prolonged exposure (Foa et al., Reference Foa, Hembree and Rothbaum2007). The current findings again highlight the close associations between re-experiencing and avoidance in combat deployed soldiers.

Surprisingly, the arousal and reactivity cluster structure did not change in the current study. Moreover, data found evidence of separated arousal sub-clusters, each strongly interconnected. One sub-cluster consists of sleep disturbance (D1), irritability/anger (D2), and difficulty concentrating (D3). The other sub-cluster consists of hypervigilance (D4) and exaggerated startle response (D5). These results may be unique to infantry soldiers who extensively train for combat, expect deployment, and then deploy. Thus, symptoms related to sleep, irritability/anger, and concentration (D1, D2, and D3), and their interconnectedness may be already dominant and meaningful following 6 months of pre-deployment combat training. The current results also correspond with previous network studies in PTSD (McNally et al., Reference McNally, Robinaugh, Wu, Wang, Deserno and Borsboom2015; Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, McFarlane, Silove and Hadzi-Pavlovic2017; Spiller et al., Reference Spiller, Schick, Schnyder, Bryant, Nickerson and Morina2017), where hypervigilance (D4) and exaggerated startle response (D5) were strongly associated and located peripherally in the network. It was suggested that this connection may reflect a self-reinforcing feedback loop, where hypervigilance predisposes to exaggerated startle response and it in turn may trigger and preserve hypervigilance (McNally et al., Reference McNally, Robinaugh, Wu, Wang, Deserno and Borsboom2015). The current findings are also in line with model fit studies of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms suggesting the best fit is achieved in a model that differentiates the arousal cluster to two separate factors: hypervigilance (D4) and exaggerated startle response (D5) as part of anxious arousal, and the other arousal symptoms (D1–D3) as part of externalizing behaviors and dysphoric arousal (Elhai et al., Reference Elhai, Ford, Ruggiero and Frueh2009, Reference Elhai, Biehn, Armour, Klopper, Frueh and Palmieri2011; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Wang, Cao, Wang, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Zhang, Zhao, Fan and Elhai2014; Armour et al., Reference Armour, Tsai, Durham, Charak, Biehn, Elhai and Pietrzak2015; Tsai et al., Reference Tsai, Harpaz-Rotem, Armour, Southwick, Krystal and Pietrzak2015).

The findings of stronger network connectivity in the re-experiencing and avoidance sub-clusters following combat also correspond with cross-sectional research on PTSD symptom structure (Engdahl et al., Reference Engdahl, Elhai, Richardson and Frueh2011), and with literature indicating that the dominance of these different sub-clusters and their inter-connectedness may fluctuate at different time points following traumatic exposure (e.g. Shalev et al., Reference Shalev, Freedman, Peri, Brandes, Sahar, Orr and Pitman1998; Solomon and Mikulincer, Reference Solomon and Mikulincer2006; Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, Horesh and Ein-Dor2009). Intrusion symptoms appear to play a central role in the early development of the disorder (Yehuda et al., Reference Yehuda, McFarlane and Shalev1998; Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, Horesh, Ein-Dor and Ohry2012; Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, McFarlane, Silove and Hadzi-Pavlovic2017), whereas avoidance symptoms become more dominant as time passes, presumably as afunction of a defense strategy that contains the distress generated by the intrusion symptoms (McFarlane, Reference McFarlane1992; Shalev et al., Reference Shalev, Peri, Canetti and Schreiber1996).

The current results should be considered in light of potential limitations. First, we used self-report assessments of PTSD symptoms severity. Although previous research indicates that PCL scores are correlated strongly with standardized clinician ratings [e.g. the Clinically Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS); Blanchard et al., Reference Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley and Forneris1996], future studies may wish to assess symptom network structures using clinician ratings. Second, participants in the current study had 6 months of military training before the pre-deployment measurement. This training, although highly stressful and entailing both combat simulation and some routine security missions, does not reflect a clear-cut trauma as does real combat experience. As such, the pre-deployment PCL scores relating to this pre-deployment training epoch may lack the intensity of combat-related trauma, raising a question about the validity of the reported symptoms. Importantly however, this limitation is inherent to longitudinal designs exploring changes in symptom networks structure from pre- to post-combat and therefore must rely on pre-combat reports. Third, the current study reveals PTSD symptoms structure in a highly homogeneous cohort (18-year-old male infantry soldiers) and a very specific traumatic context – combat. For better insight on generalizability of the current results, it would be important to test more divergent populations as well as different traumatic contexts. Fourth, the sample of this study is a non-selected sample of young healthy soldiers that were exposed to potential traumatic combat events. This affords a longitudinal developmental view of PTSD symptoms associations over time following traumatic exposure. However, it is still unclear whether the networks studied here be similar to those of clinical samples of soldiers with PTSD. Such longitudinal research would require a much larger sample than was available to the current study. Finally, the current study used the PCL, which relates to DSM-IV criteria for PTSD. Although many of the symptoms are the same as in the previous DSM, new symptom items were added to the DSM-5 diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Thus, while some important inferences can be drawn from the current analyses, future studies using DSM-5 symptoms are needed.

In conclusion, the present study offers a new insight into the dynamic associations between networks of PTSD symptoms as it evolves in soldiers from pre-to-post combat exposure. The longitudinal results highlight the importance of interaction between intrusive and avoidance symptom clusters, which integrate into a more tightly correlated unit of symptoms following combat. If close associations between re-experiencing symptoms and avoidance symptoms are cemented following combat exposure, a potential target for intervention may be reduction of the strength of these associations in the acute phases following trauma and before chronic pathology develops. Knowledge on the dynamics of PTSD symptoms association over time could clarify the impact of traumatic exposure on mental health and offer focused targets for therapy.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000539.

Financial support

This research was supported by the Israel Science Foundation, grant number 1811/17.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Armour, C, Tsai, J, Durham, TA, Charak, R, Biehn, TL, Elhai, JD and Pietrzak, RH (2015) Dimensional structure of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress symptoms: support for a hybrid Anhedonia and Externalizing Behaviors model. Journal of Psychiatric Research 61, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armour, C, Fried, EI, Deserno, MK, Tsai, J and Pietrzak, RH (2017) A network analysis of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and correlates in U.S. military veterans. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 45, 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, EB, Jones-Alexander, J, Buckley, TC and Forneris, CA (1996) Psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist (PCL). Behaviour Research and Therapy 34, 669673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D and Cramer, AOJ (2013) Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 9, 91121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boschloo, L, Van Borkulo, CD, Rhemtulla, M, Keyes, KM, Borsboom, D and Schoevers, RA (2015) The network structure of symptoms of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. PLoS ONE 10, e0137621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briganti, G, Kempenaers, C, Braun, S, Fried, EI and Linkowski, P (2018) Network analysis of empathy items from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index in 1973 young adults. Psychiatry Research 265, 8792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, RA, Creamer, M, O'Donnell, M, Forbes, D, McFarlane, AC, Silove, D and Hadzi-Pavlovic, D (2017) Acute and chronic posttraumatic stress symptoms in the emergence of posttraumatic stress disorder: a network analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 74, 135142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A and Clark, DM (2000) A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 38, 319345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elhai, JD, Ford, JD, Ruggiero, KJ and Frueh, BC (2009) Diagnostic alterations for post-traumatic stress disorder: examining data from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication and National Survey of Adolescents. Psychological Medicine 39, 19571966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elhai, JD, Biehn, TL, Armour, C, Klopper, JJ, Frueh, BC and Palmieri, PA (2011) Evidence for a unique PTSD construct represented by PTSD's D1–D3 symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 25, 340345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engdahl, RM, Elhai, JD, Richardson, JD and Frueh, BC (2011) Comparing posttraumatic stress disorder's symptom structure between deployed and nondeployed veterans. Psychological Assessment 23, 1.Google ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S, Borsboom, D and Fried, EI (2018) Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behavior Research Methods 50, 195212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, EB and Rothbaum, BO (1998) Treating the Trauma of Rape: Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for PTSD. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Hembree, E and Rothbaum, B (2007) Prolonged Exposure Therapy for PTSD: Emotional Processing of Traumatic Experiences. New York, NY: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Friedman, MJ, Keane, TM and Resick, PA (2007) Handbook of PTSD: Science and Practice. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, J, Hastie, T and Tibshirani, R (2008) Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics (Oxford, England) 9, 432441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, MJ, Kilpatrick, DG and Schnurr, PP (2016) Changes to the definition of posttraumatic stress disorder in the DSM-5 – reply. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 1203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fruchterman, TM and Reingold, EM (1991) Graph drawing by force‐directed placement. Software: Practice and Experience 21, 11291164.Google Scholar
Guina, J (2016) Changes to the definition of posttraumatic stress disorder in the DSM-5. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 12011202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, SG, Curtiss, J and McNally, RJ (2016) A complex network perspective on clinical science. Perspectives on Psychological Science 11, 597605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoge, CW (2016) Changes to the definition of posttraumatic stress disorder in the DSM-5 – reply. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 12021203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoge, CW, Castro, CA, Messer, SC, McGurk, D, Cotting, DI and Koffman, RL (2004) Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. New England Journal of Medicine 351, 1322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoge, CW, Yehuda, R, Castro, CA, McFarlane, AC, Vermetten, E, Jetly, R, Koenen, KC, Greenberg, N, Shalev, AY, Rauch, SA and Marmar, CR (2016) Unintended consequences of changing the definition of posttraumatic stress disorder in DSM-5 critique and call for action. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 750752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, MJ (1986) Stress-response syndromes: a review of posttraumatic and adjustment disorders. Psychiatric Services 37, 241249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keane, TM, Zimering, RT and Caddell, JM (1985) A behavioral formulation of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans. Behavior Therapist 8, 912.Google Scholar
King, DW, Leskin, GA, King, LA and Weathers, FW (1998) Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD Scale: evidence for the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Assessment 10, 90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, P, Wang, L, Cao, C, Wang, R, Zhang, J, Zhang, B, Wu, Q, Zhang, H, Zhao, Z, Fan, G and Elhai, JD (2014) The underlying dimensions of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in an epidemiological sample of Chinese earthquake survivors. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 28, 345351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFarlane, AC (1992) Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 180, 439445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, RJ, Robinaugh, DJ, Wu, GWY, Wang, L, Deserno, MK and Borsboom, D (2015) Mental disorders as causal systems: a network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychological Science 3, 836849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Opsahl, T, Agneessens, F and Skvoretz, J (2010) Node centrality in weighted networks: generalizing degree and shortest paths. Social Networks 32, 245251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, DC, Berntsen, D and Johansen, MK (2008) A memory based model of posttraumatic stress disorder: evaluating basic assumptions underlying the PTSD diagnosis. Psychological Review 115, 9851011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santos, HP Jr, Kossakowski, JJ, Schwartz, TA, Beeber, L and Fried, EI (2018). Longitudinal network structure of depression symptoms and self-efficacy in low-income mothers. PLoS ONE 13, e0191675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalev, AY, Peri, T, Canetti, L and Schreiber, S (1996) Predictors of PTSD in injured trauma survivors: a prospective study. American Journal of Psychiatry 153, 219225.Google ScholarPubMed
Shalev, AY, Freedman, S, Peri, T, Brandes, D, Sahar, T, Orr, SP and Pitman, RK (1998) Prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry 155, 630637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simms, LJ, Watson, D and Doebbelling, BN (2002). Confirmatory factor analyses of posttraumatic stress symptoms in deployed and nondeployed veterans of the Gulf War. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111, 637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, Z and Mikulincer, M (2006) Trajectories of PTSD: a 20-year longitudinal study. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 659666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, Z, Horesh, D and Ein-Dor, T (2009) The longitudinal course of posttraumatic stress disorder symptom clusters among war veterans. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 70, 837843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, Z, Horesh, D, Ein-Dor, T and Ohry, A (2012) Predictors of PTSD trajectories following captivity: a 35-year longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research 199, 188194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiller, TR, Schick, M, Schnyder, U, Bryant, RA, Nickerson, A and Morina, N (2017) Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in a clinical sample of refugees: a network analysis. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 8, 1318032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, MB, Walker, JR, Hazen, AL and Forde, DR (1997) Full and partial posttraumatic stress disorder: findings from a community survey. The American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 11141119.Google ScholarPubMed
Thomas, JL, Wilk, JE, Riviere, LA, McGurk, D, Castro, CA and Hoge, CW (2010) Prevalence of mental health problems and functional impairment among active component and national guard soldiers 3 and 12 months following combat in Iraq. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 614623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tibshirani, R (2011) Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso: a retrospective. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology) 73, 273282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J, Harpaz-Rotem, H, Armour, C, Southwick, SM, Krystal, JH and Pietrzak, RH (2015) Dimensional structure of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: results from the National Health and Resilience in Veterans Study. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 76, 546553.Google ScholarPubMed
van Borkulo, CD (2015) Network comparison test: permutation-based test of differences in strength of networks. Available at https://github.com/cvborkulo/NetworkComparisonTest (Accessed 4 August 2016).Google Scholar
van Borkulo, CD, Borsboom, D, Epskamp, S, Blanken, TF, Boschloo, L, Schoevers, RA and Waldorp, LJ (2014) A new method for constructing networks from binary data. Scientific Reports 4, 5918.Google ScholarPubMed
Wald, I, Degnan, KA, Gorodetsky, E, Charney, DS, Fox, NA, Fruchter, E, Goldman, D, Lubin, G, Pine, DS and Bar-Haim, Y (2013) Attention to threats and combat-related posttraumatic stress symptoms: prospective associations and moderation by the serotonin transporter gene. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 401408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Z, Algesheimer, R and Tessone, CJ (2016) A comparative analysis of community detection algorithms on artificial networks. Scientific Reports 6, 30750.10.1038/srep30750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R, McFarlane, A and Shalev, A (1998) Predicting the development of posttraumatic stress disorder from the acute response to a traumatic event. Biological Psychiatry 44, 13051313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Frequency of specific combat experiences pre-deployment and post-combat

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Network structures of PTSD symptoms (PCL): (a) pre-deployment; and (b) post-combat. Each network consists of 17 nodes representing the 17 symptoms of PTSD per DSM-IV, and edges (lines) represent the partial correlation coefficients between these nodes (symptoms), that is, the connections between two nodes after controlling for all other connections in the network. Black edges represent positive associations and red edges represent negative associations. Edge thickness and brightness reflect the association's (edge) strength (e.g. the B4–B5 edge is thicker at post-combat relative to pre-deployment indicating a stronger partial correlation between these two symptoms post-combat). To simplify interpretation and network visualization, the locations of the nodes have been kept to identical positions between the two networks by taking the mean coordinators of nodes’ locations in the individual layout networks. Symptom numbers and their abbreviated descriptions are provided in the figure.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Centrality estimates: strength, betweenness, and closeness at pre-deployment and post-combat. Centrality indices show that across time the most central nodes are flashbacks (B3) and upset by reminders (B4), which have the highest values of node strength (positive scores), whereas the least central nodes are hypervigilance (D4) and disinterest in activities (C4) with the lowest strength values (negative scores). The centrality interpretations are based on the strength index that had a good stability across time. Centrality indices are shown as standardized z-scores.

Supplementary material: File

Segal et al. supplementary material

Figures S1-S5

Download Segal et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.1 MB