Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T12:22:35.357Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Amygdala and ventral anterior cingulate activation predicts treatment response to cognitive behaviour therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 November 2007

R. A. Bryant*
Affiliation:
Brain Dynamics Centre, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
K. Felmingham
Affiliation:
Brain Dynamics Centre, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
A. Kemp
Affiliation:
Brain Dynamics Centre, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia
P. Das
Affiliation:
Brain Dynamics Centre, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia Neuroscience Institute of Schizophrenia and Allied Disorders (NISAD), NSW, Australia
G. Hughes
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia
A. Peduto
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia
L. Williams
Affiliation:
Brain Dynamics Centre, Westmead Hospital, Sydney, NSW, Australia
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr R. A. Bryant, School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia. (Email: r.bryant@unsw.edu.au)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) is the treatment of choice for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), approximately half of patients do not respond to CBT. No studies have investigated the capacity for neural responses during fear processing to predict treatment response in PTSD.

Method

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) responses of the brain were examined in individuals with PTSD (n=14). fMRI was examined in response to fearful and neutral facial expressions presented rapidly in a backwards masking paradigm adapted for a 1.5 T scanner. Patients then received eight sessions of CBT that comprised education, imaginal and in vivo exposure, and cognitive therapy. Treatment response was assessed 6 months after therapy completion.

Results

Seven patients were treatment responders (defined as a reduction of 50% of pretreatment scores) and seven were non-responders. Poor improvement after treatment was associated with greater bilateral amygdala and ventral anterior cingulate activation in response to masked fearful faces.

Conclusions

Excessive fear responses in response to fear-eliciting stimuli may be a key factor in limiting responses to CBT for PTSD. This excessive amygdala response to fear may reflect difficulty in managing anxiety reactions elicited during CBT, and this factor may limit optimal response to therapy.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Introduction

Cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) is the treatment of choice for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Across a broad range of acute and chronic populations, CBT has been shown to perform better than other psychological or psychopharmacological interventions (Foa & Meadows, Reference Foa and Meadows1997; Harvey et al. Reference Harvey, Bryant and Tarrier2003). Despite this relative success of CBT, half of patients still suffer PTSD after treatment (Bradley et al. Reference Bradley, Greene, Russ, Dutra and Westen2005). Most CBT interventions for PTSD involve repeated exposure to trauma reminders and/or memories, as well as cognitive restructuring techniques that aim to teach more realistic appraisals of the trauma and oneself. Accordingly, successful response to CBT involves management of anxiety elicited by trauma memories and related cognitions (Foa & Meadows, Reference Foa and Meadows1997). Consistent with this proposal, there is evidence that limited response to CBT is predicted by pretreatment PTSD severity (Blanchard et al. Reference Blanchard, Hickling, Malta, Jaccard, Devineni, Veazey and Galovski2003).

There is increasing recognition that fundamental mechanisms of psychotherapy need to be understood by determining the biological mechanisms underpinning psychotherapy (Linden, Reference Linden2006). Understanding the biological differences between patients who do and do not respond to treatment will both elucidate the biological mechanisms of available treatments and advance our capacity to match treatments to different patients (Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Pittenger, Polan and Kandel2005). The majority of studies that have used neuroimaging techniques to predict treatment response have focused on major depression or obsessive-compulsive disorder, and the majority of these have studied prediction of pharmacotherapy (for reviews, see Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Pittenger, Polan and Kandel2005; Evans et al. Reference Evans, Dougherty, Pollak and Rauch2006). These studies have measured neural activity during rest or during symptom provocation tasks. The latter approach can be a more sensitive measure because it allows assessment of neural responses to stimuli that are important in the maintenance of the disorder, and therefore may be more attuned to detecting neural activity that predicts treatment response (Evans et al. Reference Evans, Dougherty, Pollak and Rauch2006). To date, there are no reports of neural predictors of treatment response for PTSD.

Current biological models of PTSD are based on fear-conditioning models and posit that a traumatic event (unconditioned stimulus) leads to a strong fear reaction (unconditioned response) that becomes conditioned to many stimuli associated with the traumatic event. Accordingly, when people are exposed to reminders of the trauma (conditioned stimuli), they experience a strong fear reaction (conditioned response) (Charney et al. Reference Charney, Deutch, Krystal, Southwick and Davis1993). Consistent with animal models of extinction learning (Davis & Myers, Reference Davis and Myers2002), biological models posit that recovery from PTSD involves successful learning that the previously conditioned stimuli are no longer threatening (Bryant, Reference Bryant, Kato, Kawata and Pitman2006).

Preclinical research has elucidated the neural underpinnings of fear conditioning (Davis, Reference Davis1992; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux1998). There is converging evidence from a variety of experimental techniques demonstrating a crucial role of the amygdala in the acquisition and expression of conditioned fear (Davis, Reference Davis and Aggleton2000). Accordingly, biological models of PTSD have hypothesized amygdala-mediated fear conditioning to underlie the disorder (Charney et al. Reference Charney, Deutch, Krystal, Southwick and Davis1993). Specifically, it has been proposed that reduced medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) activity leads to impaired inhibition over amygdala fear-processing networks, resulting in amygdala hyper-responsivity in PTSD (Bremner et al. Reference Bremner, Staib, Kaloupek, Southwick, Soufer and Charney1999). Consistent with this model, there is strong evidence for reduced activation in the mPFC in PTSD patients during fear processing across a range of brain-imaging studies (e.g. Liberzon et al. Reference Liberzon, Taylor, Amdur, Jung, Chamberlain and Minoshima1999). In addition, there is also evidence for increased amygdala response to fear stimuli in PTSD (Liberzon et al. Reference Liberzon, Taylor, Amdur, Jung, Chamberlain and Minoshima1999; Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Macklin, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2000; Armony et al. Reference Armony, Corbo, Clément and Brunet2005).

On the premise that CBT for PTSD involves successful management of fear responses elicited during therapy, we proposed that treatment response would be predicted by neural networks that promote fear regulation. We therefore hypothesized that poor response to CBT would be associated with greater activation in the amygdala and reduced mPFC recruitment during fear processing before treatment.

Method

Fourteen treatment-seeking participants (eight females, six males) of mean age 42.2 years were survivors of interpersonal violence (n=9) or motor vehicle accidents (n=5) that occurred an average of 4.4 years before treatment. All participants were right-handed. Table 1 presents a summary of clinical characteristics of the PTSD sample. Diagnoses of PTSD were made by clinical consensus by two clinical psychologists (independent of the study), according to DSM-IV criteria (APA, 1994) using the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al. Reference Blake, Weathers, Nagy, Kaloupek, Klauminzer, Charney and Keane1990). Participants with a history of neurological disorder, psychosis, or current substance abuse were excluded. Nine participants had co-morbid major depressive disorder, and one had co-morbid panic disorder diagnosed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1997). Six participants were medicated with antidepressant medication (three in the treatment responder group and three in the treatment non-responder group), which was not altered during the course of the study. The study also included a comparison group of 14 healthy participants who had had never experienced a criterion A stressor and with no current or lifetime psychiatric disorders. Informed written consent was obtained from participants according to the institutional review board.

Table 1. Clinical details for PTSD participants (n=14)

PTSD, Post-traumatic stress disorder; NR, treatment non-responder; TR, treatment responder; F, female; M, male; MVA, motor vehicle accident; MDD, major depressive disorder.

Participants received eight once-weekly sessions of CBT. Therapy involved education, imaginal exposure, cognitive restructuring, and relapse prevention (see Bryant et al. Reference Bryant, Moulds, Guthrie, Dang and Nixon2003). Prior to treatment, participants viewed standardized grey-scale face stimuli (Gur et al. Reference Gur, Sara, Hagendoorn, Marom, Hughett, Macy, Turner, Bajcsy, Posner and Gur2002) that consisted of four female and four male individuals depicting fear and neutral expressions. Participants viewed faces under a rapidly presented viewing condition in a backwards masking paradigm (Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Macklin, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2000). We used a rapid presentation paradigm because there is convergent evidence that it activates amygdala response in PTSD individuals (Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Macklin, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2000; Armony et al. Reference Armony, Corbo, Clément and Brunet2005). Furthermore, recent commentaries have proposed that fear stimuli presented outside awareness engage neural networks that are particularly relevant to pathological anxiety states because preconscious fear processing underpins many anxiety disorders (Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Pittenger, Polan and Kandel2005). All faces were matched for overall luminosity and size, and were equally aligned on a black background template. Each sequence comprised 240 stimuli (120 fear and 120 neutral) in a pseudo-random sequence of 30 blocks (comprising eight fear or eight neutral stimuli each). Each face (fear or neutral) stimulus was presented for 16.7 ms, followed by a 163.3-ms neutral mask. These durations were based on parameters established in a psychophysics experiment, undertaken using an equivalent block design task, that demonstrated that participants could not detect the content of non-conscious presentations (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Liddell, Rathjen, Brown, Gray, Phillips, Young and Gordon2004). This timing was sufficient to saturate the retina, but precluded conscious awareness of the presence of the stimulus. Our protocol was based on signal detection (Macmillan, Reference Macmillan1986), and provides an exhaustive criterion for ensuring unconscious processing of fear. The inter-stimulus interval (ISI) was 1088 ms. The ISI was jittered by ±200 ms to ensure that stimulus onset did not coincide with a constant slice position during image acquisition. Face stimuli were presented using a projector (Sanyo ProX, Multiverse Projector) and mirror system. Participants received standardized and synchronized visual and audio (through headphones) instructions and were asked to actively attend to the face stimuli, in preparation for a post-scanning briefing about these stimuli. Participants were instructed to focus on the first face even though it may be difficult to see.

Imaging was performed on a 1.5 T Siemens Vision Plus scanner using an echo echoplanar protocol. A total of 90 functional T2*-weighted volumes (three per stimulus block) were acquired, comprising 15 non-contiguous slices parallel to the intercommissural (AC–PC) line, with 6.6 mm thickness and TR=3.3 s, TE=40 ms, flip angle=90°; FOV 24×24 cm2, matrix size 128×128. Three initial ‘dummy’ volumes were acquired to ensure blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) saturation. Preprocessing (realignment and unwarping, spatial normalization into standardized MNI space, smoothing using an 8-mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel) and statistical analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were conducted using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM2, Wellcome Department of Neurology, London, UK).

To examine change in PTSD severity independent of initial severity, residual change was calculated from a regression of pretreatment total CAPS scores on post-treatment total CAPS scores (Siegle et al. Reference Siegle, Carter and Thase2006). Whole-brain voxelwise regressions of residual PTSD severity on mean fMRI signal for fear (or neutral) facial expression were used to detect neural activity associated with treatment recovery (Type 1 error: p<0.001). To test a priori hypotheses, significant clusters in activated regions from whole-brain analysis (amygdala and anterior cingulate) were examined in correlation and planned contrast analyses. Group differences were examined in a series of independent sample t tests (treatment responder versus non-responder; controls versus treatment responder; controls versus treatment non-responder) using the same hypothesis-driven regions of interest (ROIs). ROIs were defined by the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL) masks (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. Reference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landaeau, Papthanasciou, Crivello, Etard, Delcroix, Mazoyer and Joliot2002). Neural activity was examined for the contrast of fear versus neutral with an α-level of p<0.05 (small volume corrected) and an extent threshold of >5 contiguous voxels per cluster.

Results

The average pretreatment CAPS score was 75.5, and the post-treatment score was 38.6. Seven participants were treatment responders (defined as a reduction of 50% of pretreatment scores) and seven were non-responders. There was a significant difference between the groups in change in total CAPS scores from pre- to post-treatment [F(1, 12)=10.9, p<0.01], with treatment responders displaying greater PTSD symptom reduction than non-responders. Initial PTSD severity accounted for 19.3% of variance in post-treatment CAPS severity scores. The average pretreatment score on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996) was 26.5 and the post-treatment score was 17.9. There were no significant differences between treatment responders and non-responders on change in total BDI score from pre- to post-treatment [F(1, 12)=1.36, p>0.05].

To test a priori hypotheses, correlations were conducted between post-treatment PTSD severity and amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Significant positive correlations were found between post-treatment CAPS scores and bilateral amygdala (right: 24, 0, −22; z=3.25, p=0.000; voxels=105; r=0.75, p<0.01; left: −18, −2, −22; z=3.48, p=0.000; voxels=98; r=0.8, p<0.01) (Fig. 1 a). There was a significant positive correlation between ventral ACC and post-treatment CAPS (0, 36, −4; z=2.04, p=0.021; voxels=17; r=0.55, p<0.05).

Fig. 1. (a) Positive correlations between the post-treatment Clinician-Administered Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Scale (CAPS) score (controlling for pretreatment CAPS score) and amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activity to fearful faces prior to treatment. (b) Group differences in amygdala and ACC activity between treatment non-responders (n=7) and treatment responders (n=7).

Treatment non-responders had significantly greater bilateral amygdala prior to treatment than treatment responders (right: 26, 2, −10; z=1.85, p<0.05; voxels=16; left: −18, −2, −20; z=2.13, p<0.05; voxels=23) and significantly greater activity in the right ventral anterior cingulate (0, 26, −2; z=2.23, p<0.05; voxels=96) (Fig. 1 b). Treatment responders had significantly greater bilateral dorsal ACC activity than treatment non-responders (left: −4, −2, 30; z=3.54, p=0.000; voxels=95; right: 8, 6, 28; z=3.11, p=0.010; voxels=29).

Overall, PTSD participants had markedly greater bilateral amygdala activity (right: 26, 2, −16; z=2.48, p=0.007; voxels=107; left: −26, −2, −12; z=2.33, p=0.01; voxels=35) and greater right rostral ACC activity (8, 32, 14; z=3.02, p=0.001; voxels=687) than controls. Relative to controls, the treatment non-responders had significantly greater bilateral amygdala activity to fear (right: 26, 4, −18; z=3.23, p=0.001; voxels=117; left: −16, −4, −18; z=2.42, p=0.008; voxels=77). Treatment responders also displayed greater amygdala activity than controls, but this increase was of smaller magnitude than that of the non-responders (right: 32, −8, −12; z=2.14, p=0.016; voxels=38; left: −26, −8, −12; z=2.20, p=0.014; voxels=12). In terms of ACC, treatment non-responders had greater activity in right rostral (2, 40, 16; z=2.66, p=0.004; voxels=316) and right ventral (2, 22, −8; z=2.43, p=0.008; voxels=26) ACC and reduced activity than controls in dorsal ACC (−4, −2, 30; z=2.42, p=0.008; voxels=31). Treatment responders had similar increases in bilateral (predominantly left) rostral and dorsal ACC (left: −2, 36, 30; z=3.64, p=0.001; voxels=469) than controls.

Discussion

Poor response to CBT was associated with increased bilateral amygdala recruitment during fear processing before treatment. These networks have been linked to fear processing in both healthy controls (Liddell et al. Reference Liddell, Brown, Kemp, Barton, Das, Peduto, Gordon and Williams2005; Phan et al. Reference Phan, Britton, Taylor, Fig and Liberzon2006) and PTSD patients (Liberzon et al. Reference Liberzon, Taylor, Amdur, Jung, Chamberlain and Minoshima1999; Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Macklin, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2000; Armony et al. Reference Armony, Corbo, Clément and Brunet2005). Although CBT requires activation of fear networks for CBT to be efficacious, there also needs to be adequate management of the anxiety elicited in therapy (Foa & Meadows, Reference Foa and Meadows1997). If amygdala-based fear processing is excessive, it may be more difficult to regulate the anxiety elicited during CBT. This interpretation is consistent with reports that poor response to treatment is associated with higher levels of pretreatment PTSD severity (Blanchard et al. Reference Blanchard, Hickling, Malta, Jaccard, Devineni, Veazey and Galovski2003). This finding also accords with animal studies that show that rats display impaired extinction learning when extinction is preceded by arousing fear (Maren & Chang, Reference Maren and Chang2006). This convergent evidence suggests that response to CBT is less beneficial when there is excessive anxiety prior to the therapy commencing. The current evidence provides the first evidence for the amygdala-based networks implicated in this apparent obstacle to treatment response.

Contrary to our hypothesis, we found that poor treatment response was associated with increased right ventral ACC recruitment. This finding may have occurred because of the nature of the rapid presentations of the fear stimuli. Rapid presentations of fear stimuli that use the backwards masking paradigm are thought to predominantly engage subcortical networks (Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Macklin, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2000), and may elicit ‘bottom-up’ activation through the brainstem and amygdala extending through to the ventral ACC, which may reflect automatic and rapid orienting to threat (Liddell et al. Reference Liddell, Brown, Kemp, Barton, Das, Peduto, Gordon and Williams2005). In this sense, the current paradigm may not be directly comparable with previous PTSD studies that report consciously processed fear stimuli that engage ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) regulatory networks (Lanius et al. Reference Lanius, Williamson, Densmore, Boksman, Gupta and Neufeld2001; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Wright, Cannistraro, Wedig, McMullin, Martis, Macklin, Lasko, Cavanagh, Krangek, Orr, Pitman, Whalen and Rauch2005; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Kemp, Felmingham, Barton, Olivieri, Peduto, Gordon and Bryant2006).

It is worth noting that recent commentators have suggested that the primary pathology in PTSD may be hyper-responsivity of the amygdala rather than deficient mPFC (Milad et al. Reference Milad, Rauch, Pitman and Quirk2006). Functional connectivity evidence suggests that there may be ‘bottom-up’ activation of the amygdala over the mPFC (Gilboa et al. Reference Gilboa, Shalev, Laor, Lester, Louzoun, Chisin and Bonne2004). This proposal is supported by evidence in PTSD patients (Rauch et al. Reference Rauch, Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Macklin, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2000) and healthy controls (Liddell et al. Reference Liddell, Brown, Kemp, Barton, Das, Peduto, Gordon and Williams2005) of excessive amygdala response to fear stimuli that are presented in paradigms involving backward masking so that they potentially bypass top-down regulation by the mPFC. Relevant to this finding is evidence from animal research that the amygdala can influence mPFC (Garcia et al. Reference Garcia, Voulmba, Baudry and Thompson1999). Accordingly, our finding that pretreatment amygdala hyper-responsivity to fear predicting poor treatment response is consistent with evidence of the role of the amygdala in fear reactions.

It is interesting to contrast the current finding with previous studies that have investigated the neural networks associated with treatment response to CBT in different conditions. A study of obsessive-compulsive disorder that used positron emission tomography (PET) found that pretreatment activation in the left orbitofrontal cortex predicted treatment response for both behaviour therapy and fluoxetine (Brody et al. Reference Brody, Saxena, Schwartz, Stoessel, Maidment, Phelps and Baxter1998). A study of CBT for social phobia found that social anxiety reduction was associated with reduced activation of amygdala and hippocampal regions during public speaking assessed before and after treatment (Furmark et al. Reference Furmark, Tillfors, Marteinsdottir, Fischer, Pissiota, Långstrõm and Fredrikson2002). These findings point to the distinct patterns of neural activity associated with treatment response for different disorders, and highlight the need for disorder-specific research to identify neural predictors of successful response.

In terms of depression, our finding contrasts with a recent study that found that decreased amygdala response to emotional stimuli predicted poor treatment response to CBT for depressed patients (Siegle et al. Reference Siegle, Carter and Thase2006). Depression (characterized by dampened arousal) and PTSD (characterized by elevated arousal) may require opposite levels of amygdala recruitment for successful treatment response. There is considerable evidence that rostral ACC activity prior to antidepressant treatment is predictive of good treatment response (Mayberg et al. Reference Mayberg, Brannan, Mahurin, Jerabek, Brickman, Tekell, Silva, McGinnis, Glass, Martin and Fox1997; Davidson et al. Reference Davidson, Irwin, Anderle and Kalin2003; Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Brody, Ho, Zohrabi, Maidment and Baxter2003; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Ridler, Suckling, Williams, Fu, Merlo-Pich and Bullmore2007). It has been suggested that rostral ACC may integrate affective and cognitive processes, and this network's role in cognitive control over emotional responses may result in this region being predictive of treatment response to antidepressants (Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Pittenger, Polan and Kandel2005). Comparison between these studies and the present result are difficult, however, because we used subliminal presentations of fearful faces, which may engage different networks than stimuli used in depression studies.

We recognize that we did not include a comparison condition that received an alternate treatment to CBT. This form of comparison condition is required in future research to determine the specific predictive capacity of neural responses for CBT. Future research also needs to determine the neural networks that predict successful response to CBT and pharmacotherapy for PTSD. We also note the small sample size, which limits the statistical power of the study. These limitations notwithstanding, this study represents the first demonstration of neural processes predicting treatment response in PTSD and points to the need for further research to identify neural mechanisms that may promote better response to treatment. Future treatment studies should study the mechanisms by which amygdala activation predicts poor treatment response by indexing how amygdala activation is associated with avoidance, poor emotion regulation, or other strategies that may impede treatment response. Understanding the neural processes associated with successful response to CBT may point to specific mechanisms that can be modified to enhance treatment response.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council Program Grant (300304).

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Armony, A, Corbo, V, Clément, M-H, Brunet, A (2005). Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 19611963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Steer, AJ, Brown, GK (1996). Manual for Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd edn. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Blake, DD, Weathers, FW, Nagy, LM, Kaloupek, DG, Klauminzer, G, Charney, DS, Keane, TM (1990). A clinician rating scale for assessing current and lifetime PTSD: the CAPS-1. Behavior Therapist 13, 187188.Google Scholar
Blanchard, EB, Hickling, EJ, Malta, LS, Jaccard, J, Devineni, T, Veazey, CH, Galovski, TE (2003). Prediction of response to psychological treatment among motor vehicle accident survivors with PTSD. Behavior Therapy 34, 351363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R, Greene, J, Russ, E, Dutra, L, Westen, D (2005). A multidimensional meta analysis of psychotherapy for PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 214227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, JD, Staib, LH, Kaloupek, D, Southwick, SM, Soufer, R, Charney, DS (1999). Neural correlates of exposure to traumatic pictures and sound in Vietnam combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: a positron emission tomography study. Biological Psychiatry 45, 806816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, AL, Saxena, S, Schwartz, JM, Stoessel, PW, Maidment, R, Phelps, ME, Baxter, LR (1998). FDG-PET predictors of response to behavioral therapy and pharmacotherapy in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 84, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, RA (2006). Cognitive behavior therapy: implications from advances in neuroscience. In PTSD: Brain Mechanisms and Clinical Implications (ed. Kato, N., Kawata, M. and Pitman, R.K.), pp. 255270. Springer-Verlag: Tokyo.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, RA, Moulds, ML, Guthrie, RM, Dang, ST, Nixon, RDV (2003). Imaginal exposure alone and imaginal exposure with cognitive restructuring in treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 71, 706712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charney, DS, Deutch, AY, Krystal, JH, Southwick, SM, Davis, M (1993). Psychobiologic mechanisms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 294305.Google Scholar
Chen, C-H, Ridler, K, Suckling, J, Williams, S, Fu, CHY, Merlo-Pich, E, Bullmore, E (2007). Brain imaging correlates of depressive symptom severity and predictors of symptom improvement after antidepressant treatment. Biological Psychiatry 62, 407414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, RJ, Irwin, W, Anderle, MJ, Kalin, NH (2003). The neural substrates of affective processing in depressed patients treated with venlafaxine. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 6475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience 15, 353375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M (2000). The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety. In The Amygdala (ed. Aggleton, J.P), pp. 213287. Oxford University Press: New York.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M, Myers, KM (2002). The role of glutamate and gamma-aminobutyric acid in fear extinction: clinical implications for exposure therapy. Biological Psychiatry 52, 9981007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A, Pittenger, C, Polan, HJ, Kandel, ER (2005). Toward a neurobiology of psychotherapy: basic science and clinical applications. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 17, 145158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, KC, Dougherty, DD, Pollak, MH, Rauch, SL (2006). Using neuroimaging to predict treatment response in mood and anxiety disorders. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry 18, 3342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JBW (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders – Clinician Version (SCID-CV). American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Meadows, EA (1997). Psychosocial treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder: a critical review. Annual Review of Psychology 48, 449480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furmark, T, Tillfors, M, Marteinsdottir, I, Fischer, H, Pissiota, A, Långstrõm, B, Fredrikson, M (2002). Common changes in cerebral blood flow in patients with social phobia treated with citalopram or cognitive behavior therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 425433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, R, Voulmba, RM, Baudry, M, Thompson, RF (1999). The amygdala modulates prefrontal cortex activity relative to conditioned fear. Nature 402, 294296.Google Scholar
Gilboa, A, Shalev, AY, Laor, L, Lester, H, Louzoun, Y, Chisin, R, Bonne, O (2004). Functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 55, 263272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, RC, Sara, R, Hagendoorn, M, Marom, O, Hughett, P, Macy, L, Turner, T, Bajcsy, R, Posner, A, Gur, RE (2002). A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 115, 137143.Google Scholar
Harvey, AG, Bryant, RA, Tarrier, N (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review 23, 501522.Google Scholar
Lanius, RA, Williamson, PC, Densmore, M, Boksman, K, Gupta, MA, Neufeld, R (2001). Neural correlates of traumatic memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 19201922.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J (1998). Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going? Biological Psychiatry 44, 12291238.Google Scholar
Liberzon, I, Taylor, SF, Amdur, R, Jung, TD, Chamberlain, KR, Minoshima, S (1999). Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biological Psychiatry 45, 817826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, BJ, Brown, KJ, Kemp, AH, Barton, MJ, Das, P, Peduto, A, Gordon, E, Williams, LM (2005). A direct brainstem-amygdala-cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. Neuroimage 24, 235243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linden, DEJ (2006). How psychotherapy changes the brain – the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Molecular Psychiatry 11, 528538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macmillan, N (1986). The psychophysics of nonconscious perception. Behavioral Brain Sciences 9, 3839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maren, S, Chang, C (2006). Recent fear is resistant to extinction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103, 1802018025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberg, HS, Brannan, SK, Mahurin, RK, Jerabek, PA, Brickman, JS, Tekell, JL, Silva, JA, McGinnis, S, Glass, TG, Martin, CC, Fox, PT (1997). Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport 8, 10571061.Google Scholar
Milad, MR, Rauch, SL, Pitman, RK, Quirk, GJ (2006). Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychology 73, 6171.Google Scholar
Phan, KL, Britton, JC, Taylor, SF, Fig, LM, Liberzon, I (2006). Corticolimbic blood flow during nontraumatic emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 184192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauch, SL, Whalen, PJ, Shin, LM, McInerney, SC, Macklin, ML, Lasko, NB, Orr, SP, Pitman, RK (2000). Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry 47, 769776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxena, S, Brody, AL, Ho, ML, Zohrabi, N, Maidment, KM, Baxter, LR (2003). Differential brain metabolic predictors of response to paroxetine in obsessive-compulsive disorder versus major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 522532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, LM, Wright, CI, Cannistraro, PA, Wedig, MM, McMullin, K, Martis, B, Macklin, ML, Lasko, M, Cavanagh, SR, Krangek, TS, Orr, SP, Pitman, RK, Whalen, PJ, Rauch, SL (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 273281.Google Scholar
Siegle, GJ, Carter, CS, Thase, ME (2006). Use of fMRI to predict recovery: from unipolar depression with cognitive behavior therapy. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 735738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N, Landaeau, B, Papthanasciou, D, Crivello, F, Etard, O, Delcroix, N, Mazoyer, B, Joliot, M (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, LM, Kemp, AH, Felmingham, KL, Barton, M, Olivieri, G, Peduto, AS, Gordon, E, Bryant, RA (2006). Trauma modulates amygdale and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear. Neuroimage 29, 347357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, LM, Liddell, BJ, Rathjen, J, Brown, KJ, Gray, J, Phillips, M, Young, A, Gordon, E (2004). Mapping the time course of nonconscious and conscious perception of fear: an integration of central and peripheral measures. Human Brain Mapping 21, 6474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Clinical details for PTSD participants (n=14)

Figure 1

Fig. 1. (a) Positive correlations between the post-treatment Clinician-Administered Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Scale (CAPS) score (controlling for pretreatment CAPS score) and amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activity to fearful faces prior to treatment. (b) Group differences in amygdala and ACC activity between treatment non-responders (n=7) and treatment responders (n=7).