Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T08:21:54.432Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altered interoceptive activation before, during, and after aversive breathing load in women remitted from anorexia nervosa

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2017

L. A. Berner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
A. N. Simmons
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA
C. E. Wierenga
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA
A. Bischoff-Grethe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
M. P. Paulus
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Oklahoma, USA
U. F. Bailer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Division of General Psychiatry, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
A. V. Ely
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
W. H. Kaye*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: W. H. Kaye, M.D., UCSD Department of Psychiatry, UCSD Eating Disorder Research and Treatment Program , Chancellor Park, 4510 Executive Dr., Suite 315, San Diego, CA 92121-3021, USA. (Email: wkaye@ucsd.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The neural mechanisms of anorexia nervosa (AN), a severe and chronic psychiatric illness, are still poorly understood. Altered body state processing, or interoception, has been documented in AN, and disturbances in aversive interoception may contribute to distorted body perception, extreme dietary restriction, and anxiety. As prior data implicate a potential mismatch between interoceptive expectation and experience in AN, we examined whether AN is associated with altered brain activation before, during, and after an unpleasant interoceptive state change.

Methods

Adult women remitted from AN (RAN; n = 17) and healthy control women (CW; n = 25) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging during an inspiratory breathing load paradigm.

Results

During stimulus anticipation, the RAN group, relative to CW, showed reduced activation in right mid-insula. In contrast, during the aversive breathing load, the RAN group showed increased activation compared with CW in striatum and cingulate and prefrontal cortices (PFC). The RAN group also showed increased activation in PFC, bilateral insula, striatum, and amygdala after stimulus offset. Time course analyses indicated that RAN responses in interoceptive processing regions during breathing load increased more steeply than those of CW. Exploratory analyses revealed that hyperactivation after breathing load was associated with markers of past AN severity.

Conclusions

Anticipatory deactivation with a subsequent exaggerated brain response during and after an aversive body state may contribute to difficulty predicting and adapting to internal state fluctuation. Because eating changes our interoceptive state, restriction may be one method of avoiding aversive, unpredictable internal change in AN.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a serious, often chronic eating disorder of unknown etiology. Aberrant interoception, or the perception and integration of signals relating to body homeostasis (e.g. hunger, heartbeat, respiration), has been postulated to contribute to AN symptoms including body image distortion, extreme restriction despite starvation, and alexithymia (Pollatos et al. Reference Pollatos, Kurz, Albrecht, Schreder, Kleemann, Schopf, Kopietz, Weismann and Schandry2008; Nunn et al. Reference Nunn, Framptom, Fuglset, Torzsok-Sonnevend and Lask2011; Zucker et al. Reference Zucker, Merwin, Bulik, Moskovich, Wildes and Groh2013; Kerr et al. Reference Kerr, Moseman, Avery, Bodurka, Zucker and Simmons2016). In support of this notion, reported sensory over-responsiveness (Brand-Gothelf et al. Reference Brand-Gothelf, Parush, Eitan, Admoni, Gur and Stein2016) and altered neural responses to taste stimuli have been documented in individuals ill with and remitted from AN (RAN) (Cowdrey et al. Reference Cowdrey, Park, Harmer and McCabe2011; Oberndorfer et al. Reference Oberndorfer, Frank, Simmons, Wagner, McCurdy, Fudge, Yang, Paulus and Kaye2013b ).

Signaling of both predicted and actual interoceptive experience may be particularly relevant for understanding AN. Learning depends on associations between anticipated and actual outcomes (Schultz et al. Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997), and differences between the expected and observed interoceptive state, or prediction errors, may promote avoidance learning and behavior (Paulus & Stein, Reference Paulus and Stein2006). Women with AN have difficulty distinguishing actual from anticipated sensations (Khalsa et al. Reference Khalsa, Craske, Li, Vangala, Strober and Feusner2015), and RAN women show altered neural activation during anticipation and receipt of sucrose tastes (Oberndorfer et al. Reference Oberndorfer, Frank, Simmons, Wagner, McCurdy, Fudge, Yang, Paulus and Kaye2013b ; Frank et al. Reference Frank, Collier, Shott and O'Reilly2016). Return to homeostasis after state changes may also be impaired in AN: Interoceptive accuracy in AN decreases after eating (Khalsa et al. Reference Khalsa, Craske, Li, Vangala, Strober and Feusner2015), and habituation to fullness is protracted (Zucker et al. Reference Zucker, Merwin, Bulik, Moskovich, Wildes and Groh2013). Altered AN brain response before, during, and after interoceptive events could contribute to problematic learning from experience about body-related changes, thereby perpetuating maladaptive avoidance.

Many AN behaviors involve avoidance of unpleasant physical sensations (e.g. feelings of fullness; Courty et al. Reference Courty, Godart, Lalanne and Berthoz2015), suggesting that brain responses before and during aversive interoceptive experiences may be closely associated with AN symptoms. In individuals with AN, unlike healthy individuals, eating and anticipating eating stimulate dysphoric mood (Steinglass et al. Reference Steinglass, Sysko, Mayer, Berner, Schebendach, Wang, Chen, Albano, Simpson and Walsh2010) and aversive sensations (e.g. dyspnea; Khalsa et al. Reference Khalsa, Craske, Li, Vangala, Strober and Feusner2015). ‘Noisy’ interoceptive signals may interfere with accurate prediction of the impact of future aversive stimuli, contributing to anxiety (Paulus & Stein, Reference Paulus and Stein2006, Reference Paulus and Stein2010; Barrett & Simmons, Reference Barrett and Simmons2015). RAN women show altered insular and prefrontal activation in response to aversive tastes (Cowdrey et al. Reference Cowdrey, Park, Harmer and McCabe2011) and before and during brief pain (Strigo et al. Reference Strigo, Matthews, Simmons, Oberndorfer, Klabunde, Reinhardt and Kaye2013). However, brief stimulation may not adequately capture the altered responsivity to prolonged aversive homeostatic state changes that could contribute to AN. Eating, for example, involves non-painful hunger-to-satiety state change. No study to date has examined anticipation of and adaptation to prolonged, non-painful, aversive state changes in AN.

Non-painful aversive interoception recruits circuitry that includes the insula, cingulate, lateral prefrontal cortices (PFC), striatum, and amygdala. Future events are predicted in the anterior insula (Craig, Reference Craig2002, Reference Craig2003), and direct sensation processing involves posterior insula. The difference between anticipated and experienced sensation is computed in the mid- to posterior insula, with afferents and efferents to anterior and posterior insula (Barrett & Simmons, Reference Barrett and Simmons2015). The amygdala and striatum are involved in evaluation of the rewarding or aversive aspects of the interoceptive experience (Craig, Reference Craig2002; Khalsa et al. Reference Khalsa, Rudrauf, Feinstein and Tranel2009; Hayes & Northoff, Reference Hayes and Northoff2012; Simmons et al. Reference Simmons, Avery, Barcalow, Bodurka, Drevets and Bellgowan2013). Projections from anterior and mid-cingulate and orbitofrontal and lateral PFC to anterior insula may modulate cognitive reactions during the stimulus (Craig, Reference Craig2002; Critchley et al. Reference Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman and Dolan2004; Khalsa et al. Reference Khalsa, Rudrauf, Feinstein and Tranel2009; Hayes & Northoff, Reference Hayes and Northoff2012; Simmons et al. Reference Simmons, Avery, Barcalow, Bodurka, Drevets and Bellgowan2013). Finally, return to homeostasis after aversive interoceptive experience is associated with activation in rostral anterior cingulate, lateral PFC, and anterior insula (Peiffer, Reference Peiffer2009; Leknes et al. Reference Leknes, Lee, Berna, Andersson and Tracey2011).

In this study, we used an inspiratory breathing load paradigm and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine whether AN is associated with altered brain response before, during, and after aversive interoceptive state changes. We studied remitted participants to avoid confounding effects of malnutrition on neural function. The well-validated task has been shown to activate regions of the insula, PFC, striatum, and anterior cingulate (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Flagan, Simmons, Gillis, Kotturi, Thom, Johnson, Van Orden, Davenport and Swain2012; Galli et al. Reference Galli, Shukla, Simmons, Davenport and Paulus2013; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, May, Poppa, Davenport, Tapert and Paulus2014; Berk et al. Reference Berk, Stewart, May, Wiers, Davenport, Paulus and Tapert2015). The majority of prior data from this paradigm suggests that less healthy or less resilient individuals tend to show reduced activation during anticipation and increased activation during breathing load (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Flagan, Simmons, Gillis, Kotturi, Thom, Johnson, Van Orden, Davenport and Swain2012; Haase et al. Reference Haase, Thom, Shukla, Davenport, Simmons, Paulus and Johnson2014, Reference Haase, May, Falahpour, Isakovic, Simmons, Hickman, Liu and Paulus2015, Reference Haase, Stewart, Youssef, May, Isakovic, Simmons, Johnson, Potterat and Paulus2016; Berk et al. Reference Berk, Stewart, May, Wiers, Davenport, Paulus and Tapert2015; see online Supplementary Table S1 for summary). Unlike brief pain, taste, or food picture tasks previously used to study RAN (Cowdrey et al. Reference Cowdrey, Park, Harmer and McCabe2011; Oberndorfer et al. Reference Oberndorfer, Simmons, McCurdy, Strigo, Matthews, Yang, Irvine and Kaye2013a ; Strigo et al. Reference Strigo, Matthews, Simmons, Oberndorfer, Klabunde, Reinhardt and Kaye2013), the breathing load paradigm uses prolonged, aversive stimulation to induce a negative interoceptive state while importantly avoiding the confounds of symptom-specific responses to food stimuli. We therefore preliminarily predicted that RAN, similar to less healthy or less resilient individuals in prior breathing load studies, would show increased activation in insular and prefrontal cortices during prolonged aversive stimulation and decreased activation in the anterior and mid-insula during anticipation relative to controls. We also predicted that the time course of the RAN response would differ from that of controls. Exploratory analyses examined associations between task-related activation and clinical symptoms.

Materials and methods

Participants

Twenty RAN women and 26 healthy control women (CW) participated in this study. CW participants were recruited from San Diego, and RAN participants were recruited nationally. Masters-level or higher research staff administered standard structured interviews to confirm past eating disorder diagnosis and exclude current eating disorder diagnoses (see online Supplement for further detail). AN remission was defined as in prior studies (Bailer et al. Reference Bailer, Price, Meltzer, Mathis, Frank, Weissfeld, McConaha, Henry, Brooks-Achenbach, Barbarich and Kaye2004, Reference Bailer, Frank, Henry, Price, Meltzer, Weissfeld, Mathis, Drevets, Wagner, Hoge, Ziolko, McConaha and Kaye2005; Frank et al. Reference Frank, Bailer, Henry, Drevets, Meltzer, Price, Mathis, Wagner, Hoge, Ziolko, Barbarich-Marsteller, Weissfeld and Kaye2005; Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Aizenstein, Venkatraman, Fudge, May, Mazurkewicz, Frank, Bailer, Fischer, Nguyen, Carter, Putnam and Kaye2007; Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Aizenstein, Mazurkewicz, Fudge, Frank, Putnam, Bailer, Fischer and Kaye2008; Strigo et al. Reference Strigo, Matthews, Simmons, Oberndorfer, Klabunde, Reinhardt and Kaye2013; Wierenga et al. Reference Wierenga, Bischoff-Grethe, Melrose, Irvine, Torres, Bailer, Simmons, Fudge, McClure and Ely2015). Women from both groups were excluded if they met diagnostic criteria for a current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) Axis I diagnosis, and CW participants with a history of any eating disorders were also excluded. Please see online Supplementary Information for further eligibility criteria. Participants provided written, informed consent to participate in the study. The research was approved by the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) Human Research Protections Program.

Procedure

Participants completed fMRI scans during the first 10 days (the early follicular phase) of their menstrual cycles.

Aversive inspiratory breathing load paradigm

The inspiratory breathing load paradigm has been used to study aversive interoception in healthy and psychiatric populations (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Flagan, Simmons, Gillis, Kotturi, Thom, Johnson, Van Orden, Davenport and Swain2012; Galli et al. Reference Galli, Shukla, Simmons, Davenport and Paulus2013; Haase et al. Reference Haase, Thom, Shukla, Davenport, Simmons, Paulus and Johnson2014; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, May, Poppa, Davenport, Tapert and Paulus2014; Berk et al. Reference Berk, Stewart, May, Wiers, Davenport, Paulus and Tapert2015; Haase et al. Reference Haase, May, Falahpour, Isakovic, Simmons, Hickman, Liu and Paulus2015). During fMRI scanning, participants wore a nose clip and were instructed to breathe through a hose that intermittently restricted breathing for 40-s periods via 40 cm H20/L/s inspiratory loads (Fig. 1). Colored rectangles signaled the likelihood of an upcoming breathing load period (yellow = 25% chance of subsequent breathing load, blue = 0% chance). Participants were made aware of these stimulus-outcome associations via oral instructions: ‘A blue box indicates that there will be no restriction placed on your breathing. However, a yellow box means there will be a 25% chance that your breathing will become restricted for 40 s. So, blue means no restriction and yellow means there is a 1-in-4 chance of breathing restriction.’ Overlaid on each rectangle, an arrow pointing left or right cued participants to press a left button or a right button, respectively. Accuracy and reaction time (RT) on this continuous performance task measured attention. During the task, cardiac and respiratory waveforms were recorded (see online Supplementary Information for further detail).

Fig. 1. Breathing load task design. Participants wore a nose clip and breathed through a hose that intermittently restricted their breathing via 40 cm H20/L/s loads. During scanning, participants completed a continuous performance task. Participants were instructed to press a left or right button in response to left or right pointing arrows, respectively. During ‘anticipation’ conditions, colored rectangles behind these arrows (mean duration 6 s) signaled the likelihood of an upcoming breathing load period (duration 40 s). A blue rectangle signaled no subsequent breathing load, and a yellow rectangle signaled a 25% chance of subsequent breathing load. A post-load period (mean duration 2 s) followed all breathing load conditions. The task was presented in an event-related design in two runs (total time = 17 min and 4 s; TR = 2 s). Across both runs, 34 baseline conditions and 32 anticipation conditions were jittered. Eight anticipation conditions were followed by breathing load. All conditions were jittered to optimize resolution of the hemodynamic response function.

Clinical symptoms and state measures

Participants completed the Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (Garner, Reference Garner1991) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996). Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) Harm Avoidance subscale scores (TCI-HA; Cloninger et al. Reference Cloninger, Przybeck and Svrakic1994) and Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory scores (STAI; Spielberger et al. Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene1970), which measure anxious temperament and trait anxiety, respectively, were available for a subset of participants and therefore were included only in secondary exploratory analyses (see online Supplement). After experiencing breathing load for the first time pre-scan and again immediately after scanning, participants rated the pleasantness, unpleasantness, and intensity of the experience from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely’ using 10 cm visual analogue scales (VAS). Additional post-scan VAS ratings of feelings of faintness, choking, abdominal distress, chest pain, and heart palpitations were also collected.

Image acquisition

T2*-weighted echo-planar images were acquired on a 3T General Electric Discovery MR 750 (Milwaukee, WI). High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired to permit activation localization and spatial normalization. EPI-based field maps corrected susceptibility-induced geometric distortions. See online Supplementary Information for additional image acquisition details.

Statistical analysis

Behavioral task performance and VAS ratings

To examine whether behavioral performance on the continuous performance task differed between groups, which could indicate differential effects of the aversive breathing load, a Group (RAN, CW) × Condition (Anticipation, Breathing Load, Post-Breathing Load) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on behavioral task RT and accuracy measures. Mean VAS intensity, pleasantness, and unpleasantness scores were submitted to a Group (RAN, CW) × Dimension (pleasantness, unpleasantness, and intensity) repeated measures ANOVA. Due to significant violations of the normality assumption, independent samples Mann–Whitney U tests compared groups on post-scan VAS ratings of other sensations.

MRI statistical analyses

Group-level statistical analyses were conducted using AFNI, FSL, and R statistical packages (see online Supplement for additional detail).

Primary analyses examined whether groups differed in mean peak activation during breathing load anticipation, breathing load receipt, and after breathing load termination. We conducted between-group t tests (using AFNI's 3dttest ++) within each condition. Effect sizes (Cohen's d) were calculated for these within-condition, between-group contrasts. We also conducted an exploratory Group (CW, RAN) × Condition (anticipation, breathing load, post-breathing load) whole-brain linear mixed effects analysis (LME) analysis (see online Supplement).

Secondary analyses examined potential group differences in the time course of blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) responses across all stages of interoception (from anticipation to breathing load receipt to post-breathing load). Time course data were modeled using AFNI's 3dDeconvolve TENT function across all three conditions (see online Supplement). A Group (RAN, CW) × Time LME using AFNI's 3dLME was used to compare time-course data from the RAN and CW groups in our a priori search regions of interest (ROIs).

To limit multiple comparisons, we restricted our fMRI analyses a priori to six bilateral regions involved in non-painful aversive interoception (Hayes & Northoff, Reference Hayes and Northoff2012) and implicated in aversive interoceptive prediction error (Barrett & Simmons, Reference Barrett and Simmons2015): (1) the insula in its entirety; (2) an anterior/mid-cingulate (ACC) region, including subgenual, rostral, and dorsal components of the anterior cingulate gyrus traditionally associated with affective and cognitive processing, respectively; (3) the posterior cingulate (PCC); (4) a lateral PFC region, including ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC; (5) a striatal region, including the caudate, putamen, and ventral striatum; and (6) the amygdalae (see online Supplementary Fig. S1). Region selection was also informed by healthy control activation documented in the breathing load task (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Flagan, Simmons, Gillis, Kotturi, Thom, Johnson, Van Orden, Davenport and Swain2012; Galli et al. Reference Galli, Shukla, Simmons, Davenport and Paulus2013; Haase et al. Reference Haase, Thom, Shukla, Davenport, Simmons, Paulus and Johnson2014; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, May, Poppa, Davenport, Tapert and Paulus2014; Berk et al. Reference Berk, Stewart, May, Wiers, Davenport, Paulus and Tapert2015) and locations of altered anticipatory and processing-related activation in women RAN during aversive taste and pain tasks (Cowdrey et al. Reference Cowdrey, Park, Harmer and McCabe2011; Strigo et al. Reference Strigo, Matthews, Simmons, Oberndorfer, Klabunde, Reinhardt and Kaye2013). Regions were derived from the Harvard–Oxford atlas (Desikan et al. Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire, Hyman, Albert and Killiany2006).

Small-volume correction was applied as follows: Intrinsic smoothness was estimated using the spatial autocorrelation function (ACF) option in AFNI's 3dFWHMx. Results in each region were then corrected for multiple comparisons using Monte Carlo simulations via AFNI's 3dClustSim, including ACF estimates. This robust approach employs non-Gaussian models and spatial ACFs (Eklund et al. Reference Eklund, Nichols and Knutsson2016). First-nearest neighbor clustering and a per-voxel threshold of p < 0.01 (two-tailed) was set for each of the six search regions with a cluster-wise two-tailed alpha threshold of 0.05.

Exploratory associations with clinical variables and potential confounds

Mean percent signal change was extracted from significant clusters resulting from within-condition t tests. Huber robust regressions (Huber, Reference Huber1964), were conducted in R to examine associations of percent signal change with illness duration, lowest post-pubertal body mass index (BMI), months since last AN symptoms, VAS ratings, and psychological measures. To control for family-wise error, these exploratory analyses were Bonferroni-corrected for the number of significant clusters within each breathing task condition and the number of clinical assessments tested (corrected alphas = 0.0033, 0.0002, and 0.00013 for anticipation, breathing load, and post-breathing load analyses, respectively). Additional exploratory analyses examined the potentially confounding impact of past comorbidities and AN subtype.

Results

Participant characteristics

One RAN and one CW did not complete the second run of the task, and two RAN participants were excluded because of excessive head movement, yielding a final sample of 17 RAN (11 restricting subtype, six binge-eating/purging subtype) and 25 CW who completed two full runs of the task and were included in analyses. The RAN and CW groups did not differ in age (RAN mean = 26.5; CW mean = 28.3; p = 0.449), BMI (RAN mean = 21.6; CW mean = 22.0; p = 0.568), or years of education (RAN mean = 15.9; CW mean = 15.3; p = 0.294; see online Supplementary Table S1). Consistent with prior findings (e.g. Wierenga et al. Reference Wierenga, Bischoff-Grethe, Melrose, Irvine, Torres, Bailer, Simmons, Fudge, McClure and Ely2015), past mood and anxiety disorders were more common in the RAN group, and available data suggested that compared with controls, the RAN group reported higher, but non-clinically significant, levels of depressive symptoms, trait anxiety, and harm avoidance (see online Supplementary Table S2).

Task performance, VAS ratings, and physiology

There was no statistically significant Group × Condition interaction on accuracy or RT on the task, and there were no main effects of group on task performance (see online Supplementary Table S2). RAN participants reported stronger post-scan choking sensations relative to CW, but groups did not differ on ratings of pleasantness, unpleasantness, or intensity of the breathing load experience, nor did they differ on reported feelings of faintness, heart palpitations, chest pain, or abdominal distress after the scan (see online Supplementary Table S3).

Groups did not differ in average (RAN: 0.59 ± 0.39 mm; CW: 0.67 ± 0.44 mm; p = 0.501) or maximum (RAN: 1.80 ± 0.84 mm; CW: 1.74 ± 0.86 mm; p = 0.832) motion during the task. Because of hardware malfunction, physiological data could be analyzed for only a subset of participants; however, analyses of available data provide some indication that groups did not differ in heart rate, breathing rate, or breathing rate variability (see online Supplement).

Group differences in peak activation within conditions

Results of between-group t tests within each task condition are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 2. Effect size maps of group effects are shown in online Supplementary Fig. S2. Results of exploratory Group × Condition whole-brain voxel-wise analyses are in online Supplementary Table S4.

Fig. 2. Within-condition group differences in peak activation. (a) During breathing load anticipation, the RAN group showed reduced activation in the right mid-insula compared with controls. RAN participants deactivated right mid-insula during aversive interoceptive stimulus anticipation, whereas healthy controls activated this region. (b) During breathing load, the RAN group showed increased activation compared with controls in bilateral PCC, right dACC, bilateral IFG and left MFG, left putamen, and right caudate. (c) After breathing load, the RAN group showed increased activation compared with controls in bilateral anterior insula, mid- and posterior insula (graph shown for right mid-insula), bilateral caudate and putamen (graph shown for right putamen), left amygdala, bilateral PCC, bilateral ACC, bilateral IFG and left MFG, bilateral putamen and caudate, and bilateral amygdala. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. When multiple clusters are shown, the graphs correspond to circled clusters. RAN, women remitted from anorexia nervosa; CW, healthy control women; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; amy, amygdala.

Table 1. Between-group differences in activation during breathing load anticipation, breathing load, and post-breathing load

Coordinates are presented in LPI format. The percentage overlap between clusters from the Group × Time analysis (see Table 2) and between-group, within-condition t-tests is noted in the last column. Hem, hemisphere; L, left; R, right; BA, Brodmann area; CThr, cluster threshold [p < 0.05, small-volume corrected (voxel-wise p < 0.01) with Monte Carlo simulations (via Analysis of Functional NeuroImages 3dClustSim) to guard against false-positive results].

Breathing load anticipation

The RAN group showed reduced activation compared with CW in right mid-insula during breathing load anticipation (Fig. 2a ).

Breathing load

During breathing load, the RAN group compared with CW showed increased activation in right dorsal ACC (dACC), bilateral PCC, bilateral inferior frontal and left middle frontal gyri (IFG and MFG), right caudate, and left putamen (Fig. 2b ).

Post-breathing load

After breathing load, the RAN group showed increased activation compared with CW in clusters within all ROIs: bilateral anterior, mid-, and posterior insula, bilateral dACC, right PCC, bilateral IFG and MFG, bilateral caudate and putamen, and bilateral amygdala (Fig. 2c ).

Group differences in the time course of activation

Statistically significant Group × Time interactions were detected in clusters in mid and posterior insula, subcallosal and dACC, PCC, MFG, IFG, and left amygdala, indicating that the time course of the BOLD response, from aversive stimulus anticipation to termination, differed in RAN and CW in all of these regions (Table 2; Fig. 3; and see online Supplementary Fig. S3).

Fig. 3. Group differences in activation time course. Group × Time interaction results suggest that the time course of activation in RAN participants statistically significantly differed from those of controls in left mid-insula and left IFG. Additional clusters in which time course differed between groups are shown in online Supplementary Fig. S3. Time course graphs for anticipation, breathing load, and post-breathing load conditions are shown in in light gray and with gray shading the background. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. RAN, women remitted from anorexia nervosa; CW, healthy control women; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus.

Table 2. Group × time interactions: mixed effects time series analysis

Coordinates are presented in LPI format. Hem, hemisphere; L, left; R, right; BA, Brodmann area; CThr, cluster threshold [voxel-wise p < 0.01, cluster-wise p < 0.05, small-volume corrected with Monte Carlo simulations (via Analysis of Functional NeuroImages 3dClustSim) to guard against false-positive results].

In general, CW activation these areas plateaued or gradually increased over the course of the task. In contrast, RAN participants showed steadily increasing activation during the breathing load that peaked after the offset of the aversive stimulus (Fig. 3 and see online Supplementary Fig. S3).

Exploratory associations with clinical variables

Exploratory Huber robust regression analyses revealed that RAN participants with the longest durations of illness showed the greatest hyperactivation of right MFG (p < 0.0001) and left caudate (p < 0.0001) after breathing load termination (see online Supplementary Fig. S4a). In addition, RAN participants with the greatest hyperactivation of right posterior insula after breathing load termination reported the most pronounced feelings of faintness after the scan (p < 0.0001; see online Supplementary Fig. S4b). RAN participants with the longest illness durations also tended to show the greatest hyperactivation of right IFG during breathing load (p = 0.0004), and RAN participants with the lowest lifetime post-pubertal BMIs trended toward more pronounced hyperactivation of right IFG after breathing load termination (p = 0.001), but these findings did not survive correction for multiple comparisons and should not be interpreted without replication. Results of additional regression analyses are reported in the online Supplement.

AN subtype and history of major depression or an anxiety disorder did not have an appreciable effect on RAN activation (see online Supplement).

Discussion

This investigation tested the hypothesis that AN is associated with altered processing of non-painful aversive interoceptive afferents. Several results support this hypothesis. First, although RAN and CW rated the experience similarly, RAN showed altered activation in aversive interoception circuitry before, during, and after breathing load. Results of within-condition analyses revealed reduced RAN anticipatory activation in right posterior mid-insula, a region implicated in the computation of the difference between anticipated and experienced stimuli. In contrast, RAN showed increased activation during and after aversive stimulus processing in frontal, striatal, and limbic regions involved in the evaluation of stimulus salience and modulation of cognitive reactions to stimuli. These between-group differences were consistently associated with large effect sizes. Second, RAN women showed elevated and steeply increasing activation during the receipt of an aversive stimulus, whereas in CW, activation remained fairly steady or more gradually increased during the sustained aversive experience. Taken together, these findings are consistent with altered anticipatory signaling and interoceptive network hyper-responsivity in RAN. Consequently, RAN may be more sensitive to and have more difficulty adjusting to changes in homeostatic state. Our exploratory results preliminarily indicate that altered processing of interoceptive state changes in RAN may contribute to or result from increased sensitivity to interoceptive sensations, prolonged periods of starvation, and ultimately more severe AN.

Anticipation phase: interoceptive prediction signaling

In contrast to prior findings of increased RAN activation in the insula during the anticipation of acute pain (Strigo et al. Reference Strigo, Matthews, Simmons, Oberndorfer, Klabunde, Reinhardt and Kaye2013) and food image exposure (Oberndorfer et al. Reference Oberndorfer, Simmons, McCurdy, Strigo, Matthews, Yang, Irvine and Kaye2013a ), anticipation of breathing load was associated with decreased insular activation in RAN compared with CW. This was one of the largest between-group differences observed (d = 1.03). Whereas in these prior pain and food tasks, interoceptive stimuli are rapidly presented (1–16 s), aversive breathing load in the current study is chronically applied for 40-s intervals. Thus, unlike anticipation periods in rapid tasks, the breathing task's anticipation period precedes a prolonged interoceptive state change. RAN deactivation of the mid-insula during anticipation of an aversive experience may therefore constitute a faulty prediction or preparatory signal for aversive state change. Unlike RAN, highly resilient elite athletes show an increased mid-insula anticipatory response and decreased receipt response to breathing load (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Flagan, Simmons, Gillis, Kotturi, Thom, Johnson, Van Orden, Davenport and Swain2012). Thus, a decreased RAN response during anticipation may ultimately interfere with adaptive processing of the protracted aversive experience. Our results add to a growing literature suggesting anticipatory neural signals in RAN are altered.

Receipt phase: interoceptive processing

The integration and processing of interoceptive stimuli also involves mid- and posterior insula (Craig, Reference Craig2002, Reference Craig2003), and mid-insula may be essential to online monitoring of interoceptive state change (Klein et al. Reference Klein, Ullsperger and Danielmeier2013). Again, in contrast to brief pain paradigm results, we did not find reduced insula activation in RAN during stimulus receipt. Breathing load tasks have not been used to study individuals with eating, anxiety, mood, or obsessive compulsive-related disorders, but findings in participants with substance use disorders and individuals high and low in resilience provide interesting comparisons with our RAN results. The time course of the mid-insula and posterior insula BOLD response in RAN increased more steeply during breathing load than that of CW. The pattern of reduced activation during anticipation and steeply increasing insular activation during breathing load is similar to prior findings of decreased anticipatory insular activation and increased activation during breathing load in adolescents with substance use disorders compared with controls (Berk et al. Reference Berk, Stewart, May, Wiers, Davenport, Paulus and Tapert2015). The response to the aversive stimulus observed in RAN is also consistent with that seen in individuals low in resilience (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Flagan, Simmons, Gillis, Kotturi, Thom, Johnson, Van Orden, Davenport and Swain2012). Thus, the observed RAN pattern of escalating mid-insular activation during aversive stimulation may represent a transdiagnostic neural signature of difficulty adapting to interoceptive change.

The mid-insular location of altered RAN response time course and reduced activation during anticipation is very near clusters of aberrant activation previously observed in RAN women during stomach sensation processing (Kerr et al. Reference Kerr, Moseman, Avery, Bodurka, Zucker and Simmons2016), food picture viewing (Oberndorfer et al. Reference Oberndorfer, Simmons, McCurdy, Strigo, Matthews, Yang, Irvine and Kaye2013a ) and unexpected sucrose taste omission (Frank et al. Reference Frank, Collier, Shott and O'Reilly2016). Our findings are also consistent with insular anatomical abnormalities documented in both ill adult and adolescent AN and remitted adults (Frank, Reference Frank2015; Shott et al. Reference Shott, Pryor, Yang and Frank2015).

RAN women also showed aberrant neural responses during breathing load within the larger aversive interoceptive circuit, including the striatum, lateral PFC, and PCC. Similarly increased activation in PFC has been observed in RAN during receipt of pain (Strigo et al. Reference Strigo, Matthews, Simmons, Oberndorfer, Klabunde, Reinhardt and Kaye2013) and during breathing load in adolescents with active substance use disorders (Berk et al. Reference Berk, Stewart, May, Wiers, Davenport, Paulus and Tapert2015). Increased RAN activation in prefrontal top-down modulatory regions may represent an effort to downregulate ‘noisy interoceptive afferents’ (Paulus & Stein, Reference Paulus and Stein2010). Increased activation in RAN during breathing load in caudate, putamen and MFG and IFG is consistent with findings of increased RAN response to aversive taste in these visceral and sensory information processing and integration regions (Cowdrey et al. Reference Cowdrey, Park, Harmer and McCabe2011), and further suggests a generalized AN hypersensitivity to aversive interoceptive events.

Increased and steeply rising prefrontal, insular, and cingulate activation during breathing load in RAN may also represent an error signal or an effort to adjust to a faulty prediction signal. In healthy individuals, the processing of unexpected compared with expected pain engages greater activation in similar regions to those hyperactivated or more steeply increasing in activation in RAN compared with CW during breathing load – bilateral PFC and insula (Seidel et al. Reference Seidel, Pfabigan, Hahn, Sladky, Grahl, Paul, Kraus, Kublbock, Kranz, Hummer, Lanzenberger, Windischberger and Lamm2015). Similarly, receipt of an unexpected sucrose taste is associated with increased activation in right cingulate and right medial frontal cortex in ill AN compared with CW (Frank et al. Reference Frank, Reynolds, Shott, Jappe, Yang, Tregellas and O'Reilly2012). Overall, interoceptive circuit-wide RAN hyper-responsivity may suggest difficulty adapting to and regulating responses to aversive interoceptive change.

Return to homeostasis phase: post-aversive interoceptive experience

RAN women also showed pronounced increases in activation and, in the case of right posterior insula, delayed decreases in activation after breathing load termination. One hypothetical explanation is a delayed ability to return to baseline in RAN. Prolonged hyperactivation in RAN could add ‘noise’ to aversive interoceptive processing signals and disrupt learning from experience. This could interfere with response to eating exposures during treatment and contribute to high relapse rates. Despite this potentially clinically-relevant and very preliminary hypothesis, increased post-load activation in RAN may instead reflect carry-over of breathing-load-related (rather than post-load) hyperactivation. Further investigation is needed to understand the neural response to interoceptive negative reinforcement in RAN.

Limitations and directions for future study

Several study limitations are important to acknowledge. First, although group differences in carbon dioxide levels have not been documented in prior breathing load task studies, and analysis of available data suggests our groups did not differ in cardiac or respiratory rate, we did not collect carbon dioxide levels from participants. Future studies would benefit from systematically examining the relationship between breathing rate, expired carbon dioxide, and neural activation in AN. Second, differences in fitness levels between groups could explain our findings. Although RAN and CW groups did not differ on hours of exercise per week (p = 0.147), the relationship between aversive interoceptive state change processing and fitness should be examined in future studies of AN. Third, we were unable to use computational modeling to quantify prediction error as this approach requires a rapid, event-related design that would not have been appropriate for the breathing paradigm. Fourth, our sample size was modest, and physiological and self-report data were missing from some participants. Despite statistical investigation of the effects of past comorbidities in the RAN group, their potential impact cannot be ruled out completely. Future studies with larger sample sizes are necessary to thoroughly examine the potential influence of diagnostic subtype, anxiety symptoms, and past comorbidities on altered interoceptive processing associated with AN. Inclusion of clinical control groups (e.g. women with a history of panic disorder or panic attacks) may help to further characterize these influences. Further, since we included only remitted AN, our results should be compared with those from an ill sample. Additional research is needed to better understand why altered interoceptive network activation in RAN was not associated with subjective intensity or unpleasantness of the breathing load.

Clinical implications

Our results have important implications for understanding AN pathogenesis and improving AN treatment. Neuropsychological task data suggest impairments or inefficiencies in cognitive flexibility in AN, some of which persist after recovery (Tchanturia et al. Reference Tchanturia, Davies, Roberts, Harrison, Nakazato, Schmidt, Treasure and Morris2012), but no study to date has focused on the ability to adapt to a change in interoceptive state. Our data suggest a brain-based difficulty predicting and adapting to internal milieu state shifts that may contribute to the severity and persistence of AN. Theories linking interoceptive prediction error to anxiety (Paulus & Stein, Reference Paulus and Stein2010; Barrett & Simmons, Reference Barrett and Simmons2015), associations between perceived sensory sensitivity and emotion dysregulation in AN (Merwin et al. Reference Merwin, Moskovich, Wagner, Ritschel, Craighead and Zucker2013), and our observed relationships among markers of AN severity and prefrontal and striatal hyperactivation after aversive interoception all support this notion. In addition, more pronounced insular hyperactivation after aversive interoception in RAN participants was associated with increased feelings of faintness after scanning. In the context of unreliable and unhelpful interoceptive signals and neural hypersensitivity to non-painful aversive interoceptive stimuli, individuals with AN may rely on external feedback to guide their behavior and pursue a state of interceptive ‘sameness.’ Because eating changes our homeostatic state, starvation may be one method of avoiding aversive, unpredictable internal change in AN.

No empirically-supported psychotherapies exist for the treatment of adults with AN, and our results preliminarily suggest that this may be because existing interventions rely on new learning from experience. Integration of stimulus anticipation and receipt is critical for learning about and responding to salient stimuli (Schultz et al. Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997), and our findings add to a growing body of evidence that this integration is altered in AN (Frank et al. Reference Frank, Reynolds, Shott, Jappe, Yang, Tregellas and O'Reilly2012). Determining next whether these alterations are plastic may help direct future treatment efforts for AN. Future studies should incorporate measures of cognitive flexibility, examine the rate and magnitude of habituation, and apply computational modeling to aversive interoceptive tasks to clarify how aversive learning may contribute to AN treatment resistance. Additionally, future research should further investigate the role of anticipation, processing, and recovery from interoceptive state changes in AN as they directly relate to eating behavior.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001635.

Acknowledgements

This research and preparation of this manuscript were supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health to WHK (R01 MH042984), WHK and UFB (R01 MH092793), and LAB (F32 MH108311), the Price Foundation, and the Hilda and Preston Davis Foundation. The authors declare no conflict of interest. The authors thank Miki Ogasawara for assistance with fMRI data collection and pre-processing. We would also like to thank Daria Orlowska, Grace Rasmusson, Laura Greathouse, Raesana Williams, and Jocelyn Danielle Bulante for assistance with participant screening and data collection. Finally, we thank the women who participated in this study for their time.

References

Bailer, UF, Frank, GK, Henry, SE, Price, JC, Meltzer, CC, Weissfeld, L, Mathis, CA, Drevets, WC, Wagner, A, Hoge, J, Ziolko, SK, McConaha, CW, Kaye, WH (2005). Altered brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [carbonyl11C]WAY-100635. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 10321041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailer, UF, Price, JC, Meltzer, CC, Mathis, CA, Frank, GK, Weissfeld, L, McConaha, CW, Henry, SE, Brooks-Achenbach, S, Barbarich, NC, Kaye, WH (2004). Altered 5-HT(2A) receptor binding after recovery from bulimia-type anorexia nervosa: relationships to harm avoidance and drive for thinness. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 11431155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, LF, Simmons, WK (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16, 419429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Brown, GK (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Berk, L, Stewart, JL, May, AC, Wiers, RW, Davenport, PW, Paulus, MP, Tapert, SF (2015). Under pressure: adolescent substance users show exaggerated neural processing of aversive interoceptive stimuli. Addiction 110, 20252036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brand-Gothelf, A, Parush, S, Eitan, Y, Admoni, S, Gur, E, Stein, D (2016). Sensory modulation disorder symptoms in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a pilot study. International Journal of Eating Disorders 49, 5968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloninger, CR, Przybeck, TR, Svrakic, DM (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): a Guide to its Development and Use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University: St. Louis, MO.Google Scholar
Courty, A, Godart, N, Lalanne, C, Berthoz, S (2015). Alexithymia, a compounding factor for eating and social avoidance symptoms in anorexia nervosa. Comprehensive Psychiatry 56, 217228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowdrey, FA, Park, RJ, Harmer, CJ, McCabe, C (2011). Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry 70, 736743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, AD (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 655666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, AD (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13, 500505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, HD, Wiens, S, Rotshtein, P, Ohman, A, Dolan, RJ (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience 7, 189195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, RS, Ségonne, F, Fischl, B, Quinn, BT, Dickerson, BC, Blacker, D, Buckner, RL, Dale, AM, Maguire, RP, Hyman, BT, Albert, MS, Killiany, RJ (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eklund, A, Nichols, T, Knutsson, H (2016). Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113, 79007905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, GK, Bailer, UF, Henry, SE, Drevets, W, Meltzer, CC, Price, JC, Mathis, CA, Wagner, A, Hoge, J, Ziolko, S, Barbarich-Marsteller, N, Weissfeld, L, Kaye, WH (2005). Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [11c]raclopride. Biological Psychiatry 58, 908912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, GK, Collier, S, Shott, ME, O'Reilly, RC (2016). Prediction error and somatosensory insula activation in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 41, 150103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, GKW (2015). Advances from neuroimaging studies in eating disorders. CNS Spectrums 20, 391400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, GKW, Reynolds, JR, Shott, ME, Jappe, L, Yang, TT, Tregellas, JR, O'Reilly, RC (2012). Anorexia nervosa and obesity are associated with opposite brain reward response. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 20312046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galli, G, Shukla, A, Simmons, AN, Davenport, PW, Paulus, MP (2013). Sex differences in the neural processing of aversive interoceptive events: the benefit of relief. PLoS ONE 8, e84044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garner, DM (1991). Eating Disorder Inventory-2 Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL.Google Scholar
Haase, L, May, AC, Falahpour, M, Isakovic, S, Simmons, AN, Hickman, SD, Liu, TT, Paulus, MP (2015). A pilot study investigating changes in neural processing after mindfulness training in elite athletes. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9, 229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haase, L, Stewart, JL, Youssef, B, May, AC, Isakovic, S, Simmons, AN, Johnson, DC, Potterat, EG, Paulus, MP (2016). When the brain does not adequately feel the body: links between low resilience and interoception. Biological Psychology 113, 3745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haase, L, Thom, NJ, Shukla, A, Davenport, PW, Simmons, AN, Paulus, MP, Johnson, DC (2014). Mindfulness-based training attenuates insula response to an aversive interoceptive challenge. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11, 182190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, DJ, Northoff, G (2012). Common brain activations for painful and non-painful aversive stimuli. BMC Neuroscience 13, 60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, P (1964). Robust estimation of location parameter. Annals of Mathematical Statistics 35, 73101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, KL, Moseman, SE, Avery, JA, Bodurka, J, Zucker, NL, Simmons, WK (2016). Altered insula activity during visceral interoception in weight-restored patients with anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 521528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalsa, SS, Craske, MG, Li, W, Vangala, S, Strober, M, Feusner, JD (2015). Altered interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa: effects of meal anticipation, consumption and bodily arousal. International Journal of Eating Disorders 48, 889897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalsa, SS, Rudrauf, D, Feinstein, JS, Tranel, D (2009). The pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience 12, 14941496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, TA, Ullsperger, M, Danielmeier, C (2013). Error awareness and the insula: links to neurological and psychiatric diseases. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leknes, S, Lee, M, Berna, C, Andersson, J, Tracey, I (2011). Relief as a reward: hedonic and neural responses to safety from pain. PLoS ONE 6, e17870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merwin, RM, Moskovich, AA, Wagner, HR, Ritschel, LA, Craighead, LW, Zucker, NL (2013). Emotion regulation difficulties in anorexia nervosa: relationship to self-perceived sensory sensitivity. Cognition and Emotion 27, 441452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunn, K, Framptom, I, Fuglset, T, Torzsok-Sonnevend, M, Lask, B (2011). Anorexia nervosa and the insula. Medical Hypotheses 76, 353357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberndorfer, T, Simmons, A, McCurdy, D, Strigo, I, Matthews, S, Yang, T, Irvine, Z, Kaye, W (2013 a). Greater anterior insula activation during anticipation of food images in women recovered from anorexia nervosa versus controls. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 214, 132141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberndorfer, TA, Frank, GK, Simmons, AN, Wagner, A, McCurdy, D, Fudge, JL, Yang, TT, Paulus, MP, Kaye, WH (2013 b). Altered insula response to sweet taste processing after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. The American Journal of Psychiatry 170, 11431151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, MP, Flagan, T, Simmons, AN, Gillis, K, Kotturi, S, Thom, N, Johnson, DC, Van Orden, KF, Davenport, PW, Swain, JL (2012). Subjecting elite athletes to inspiratory breathing load reveals behavioral and neural signatures of optimal performers in extreme environments. PLoS ONE 7, e29394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, MP, Stein, MB (2006). An insular view of anxiety. Biological Psychiatry 60, 383387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, MP, Stein, MB (2010). Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Structure and Function 214, 451463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peiffer, C (2009). Dyspnea relief: more than just the perception of a decrease in dyspnea. Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology 167, 6171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollatos, O, Kurz, A-L, Albrecht, J, Schreder, T, Kleemann, A, Schopf, V, Kopietz, R, Weismann, M, Schandry, R (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors 9, 381388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W, Dayan, P, Montague, PR (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 15931599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidel, E, Pfabigan, DM, Hahn, A, Sladky, R, Grahl, A, Paul, K, Kraus, C, Kublbock, M, Kranz, GS, Hummer, A, Lanzenberger, R, Windischberger, C, Lamm, C (2015). Uncertainty during pain anticipation: the adaptive value of preparatory processes. Human Brain Mapping 36, 744755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shott, ME, Pryor, TL, Yang, TT, Frank, GKW (2015). Greater insula white matter fiber connectivity in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 498507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simmons, WK, Avery, JA, Barcalow, JC, Bodurka, J, Drevets, WC, Bellgowan, P (2013). Keeping the body in mind: insula functional organization and functional connectivity integrate interoceptive, exteroceptive, and emotional awareness. Human Brain Mapping 34, 29442958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, RD (1970). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Manual. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Steinglass, JE, Sysko, R, Mayer, L, Berner, LA, Schebendach, J, Wang, Y, Chen, H, Albano, AM, Simpson, HB, Walsh, BT (2010). Pre-meal anxiety and food intake in anorexia nervosa. Appetite 55, 214218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, JL, May, AC, Poppa, T, Davenport, PW, Tapert, SF, Paulus, MP (2014). You are the danger: attenuated insula response in methamphetamine users during aversive interoceptive decision-making. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 142, 110119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strigo, IA, Matthews, SC, Simmons, AN, Oberndorfer, T, Klabunde, M, Reinhardt, LE, Kaye, WH (2013). Altered insula activation during pain anticipation in individuals recovered from anorexia nervosa: evidence of interoceptive dysregulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders 46, 2333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tchanturia, K, Davies, H, Roberts, M, Harrison, A, Nakazato, M, Schmidt, U, Treasure, J, Morris, R (2012). Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: examining the evidence using the Wisconsin card sorting task. PLoS ONE 7, e28331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A, Aizenstein, H, Mazurkewicz, L, Fudge, J, Frank, GK, Putnam, K, Bailer, UF, Fischer, L, Kaye, WH (2008). Altered insula response to taste stimuli in individuals recovered from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 513523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A, Aizenstein, H, Venkatraman, VK, Fudge, J, May, JC, Mazurkewicz, L, Frank, GK, Bailer, UF, Fischer, L, Nguyen, V, Carter, C, Putnam, K, Kaye, WH (2007). Altered reward processing in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 18421849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wierenga, CE, Bischoff-Grethe, A, Melrose, AJ, Irvine, Z, Torres, L, Bailer, UF, Simmons, A, Fudge, JL, McClure, SM, Ely, A (2015). Hunger does not motivate reward in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry 77, 642652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zucker, NL, Merwin, RM, Bulik, CM, Moskovich, A, Wildes, JE, Groh, J (2013). Subjective experience of sensation in anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy 51, 256265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Breathing load task design. Participants wore a nose clip and breathed through a hose that intermittently restricted their breathing via 40 cm H20/L/s loads. During scanning, participants completed a continuous performance task. Participants were instructed to press a left or right button in response to left or right pointing arrows, respectively. During ‘anticipation’ conditions, colored rectangles behind these arrows (mean duration 6 s) signaled the likelihood of an upcoming breathing load period (duration 40 s). A blue rectangle signaled no subsequent breathing load, and a yellow rectangle signaled a 25% chance of subsequent breathing load. A post-load period (mean duration 2 s) followed all breathing load conditions. The task was presented in an event-related design in two runs (total time = 17 min and 4 s; TR = 2 s). Across both runs, 34 baseline conditions and 32 anticipation conditions were jittered. Eight anticipation conditions were followed by breathing load. All conditions were jittered to optimize resolution of the hemodynamic response function.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Within-condition group differences in peak activation. (a) During breathing load anticipation, the RAN group showed reduced activation in the right mid-insula compared with controls. RAN participants deactivated right mid-insula during aversive interoceptive stimulus anticipation, whereas healthy controls activated this region. (b) During breathing load, the RAN group showed increased activation compared with controls in bilateral PCC, right dACC, bilateral IFG and left MFG, left putamen, and right caudate. (c) After breathing load, the RAN group showed increased activation compared with controls in bilateral anterior insula, mid- and posterior insula (graph shown for right mid-insula), bilateral caudate and putamen (graph shown for right putamen), left amygdala, bilateral PCC, bilateral ACC, bilateral IFG and left MFG, bilateral putamen and caudate, and bilateral amygdala. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. When multiple clusters are shown, the graphs correspond to circled clusters. RAN, women remitted from anorexia nervosa; CW, healthy control women; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; amy, amygdala.

Figure 2

Table 1. Between-group differences in activation during breathing load anticipation, breathing load, and post-breathing load

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Group differences in activation time course. Group × Time interaction results suggest that the time course of activation in RAN participants statistically significantly differed from those of controls in left mid-insula and left IFG. Additional clusters in which time course differed between groups are shown in online Supplementary Fig. S3. Time course graphs for anticipation, breathing load, and post-breathing load conditions are shown in in light gray and with gray shading the background. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. RAN, women remitted from anorexia nervosa; CW, healthy control women; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus.

Figure 4

Table 2. Group × time interactions: mixed effects time series analysis

Supplementary material: File

Berner supplementary material

Berner supplementary material

Download Berner supplementary material(File)
File 2.9 MB