Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T09:48:33.692Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Abnormalities of the late positive potential during emotional processing in individuals with psychopathic traits: a meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2019

William Vallet*
Affiliation:
CH Le Vinatier, Lyon, Bron, France Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/Joliot, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris Saclay, NeuroSpin center, 91191Gif-sur-Yvette, France
Antoine Hone-Blanchet
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Jerome Brunelin
Affiliation:
CH Le Vinatier, Lyon, Bron, France INSERM-U1028, CNRS-UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, PSYR2 Team, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
*
Author for correspondence: William Vallet, E-mail: William.vallet@inserm.fr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Individuals with psychopathic traits display deficits in emotional processing. A key event-related potential component involved in emotional processing is the late positive potential (LPP). In healthy controls, LPP amplitude is greater in response to negative stimuli than to positive or neutral stimuli. In the current study, we aimed to compare LPP amplitudes between individuals with psychopathic traits and control subjects when presented with negative, positive or neutral stimuli. We hypothesized that LPP amplitude evoked by emotional stimuli would be reduced in individuals with psychopathic traits compared to healthy controls.

Methods

After a systematic review of the literature, we conducted a meta-analysis to compare LPP amplitude elicited by emotional stimuli in individuals with psychopathic traits and healthy controls.

Results

Individuals with psychopathic traits showed significantly reduced LPP amplitude evoked by negative stimuli (mean effect size = −0.47; 95% CI −0.60 to −0.33; p < 0.005) compared to healthy controls. No significant differences between groups were observed for the processing of positive (mean effect size = −0.15; 95% CI −0.42 to 0.12; p = 0.28) and neutral stimuli (mean effect size = −0.12; 95% CI 0.31 to 0.07; p = 0.21).

Conclusions

Measured by LPP amplitude, individuals with psychopathic traits displayed abnormalities in the processing of emotional stimuli with negative valence whereas processing of stimuli with positive and neutral valence was unchanged as compared with healthy controls.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Psychopathy is characterized by a set of affective, relational, and behavioral symptoms including egocentricity, impulsivity, irresponsibility, shallow emotions, pathological lying, manipulation, persistent violation of social norms and expectations, and lack of empathy, guilt and remorse (Hare, Reference Hare1996). Across the international classifications, disturbances in emotional processing are heralded as the core features of psychopathy.

An objective way to investigate emotional processing in vivo is to measure the amplitude of evoked response potentials (ERP) relative to an emotional stimulus using electroencephalography (EEG). Among ERPs of interest elicited through emotional stimuli, the latency and amplitude of the late positive potential (LPP) evoked by visual emotional stimuli have been investigated in numerous studies (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, MacNamara and Olvet2010). The LPP is maximal at centro-parietal midline sites (Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Cuthbert, Bradley, Cacioppo, Ito and Lang2000; Keil et al., Reference Keil, Bradley, Hauk, Rockstroh, Elbert and Lang2002; Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Dunning and Foti2007; Foti and Hajcak, Reference Foti and Hajcak2008), with an approximate onset of greater amplitude at 200 ms (Cuthbert et al., Reference Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer and Lang2000; Codispoti et al., Reference Codispoti, Bradley and Lang2001; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Junghöfer, Weike and Hamm2004a; Foti et al., Reference Foti, Hajcak and Dien2009) with broader latency than the P300 (Gao and Raine, Reference Gao and Raine2009), outlasting stimulus onset up to 1800 ms (e.g. Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, MacNamara and Olvet2010). In healthy individuals, studies reported greater LPP amplitude for visual emotional stimuli of either negative or positive valence, compared to neutral stimuli (e.g. Cuthbert et al., Reference Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer and Lang2000; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Cuthbert, Bradley, Cacioppo, Ito and Lang2000; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Junghöfer, Weike and Hamm2004a; Hajcak and Nieuwenhuis, Reference Hajcak and Nieuwenhuis2006; Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Dunning and Foti2007; Foti and Hajcak, Reference Foti and Hajcak2008; Hajcak and Olvert, Reference Hajcak and Olvet2008; Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Dunning and Foti2009), and for negative stimuli compared to positive and neutral stimuli (e.g. Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Öhman, Junghöfer, Weike, Stockburger and Hamm2004b; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, He, Qi, Wang, Song, Zhan, Yi, Luo and Luo2015). Furthermore, LPP amplitude appears to be modulated by arousal and attentional processing. For instance, LPP amplitude was greater when subjects attended the arousing as compared to the neutral parts of unpleasant stimuli (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Dunning and Foti2009). Also, higher arousing stimuli elicited greater LPP amplitude than lower arousing stimuli of the same valence and neutral stimuli (Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Junghöfer, Weike and Hamm2004a).

Subjects with psychopathic traits, compared to controls, display impaired emotional processing, especially in the processing of negative stimuli as revealed by deficits in recognition of negative emotion (Dawel et al., Reference Dawel, O'Kearney, McKone and Palermo2012; Schönenberg et al., Reference Schönenberg, Mayer, Christian, Louis and Jusyte2016; Jusyte and Schönenberg, Reference Jusyte and Schönenberg2017) and reduced autonomic responses following presentation of negative stimuli (Levenston et al., Reference Levenston, Patrick, Bradley and Lang2000; Flor et al., Reference Flor, Birbaumer, Herman, Ziegler and Patrick2002; Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Stobbe, van Goozen, Calder and Goodyer2010; Vaidyanathan et al., Reference Vaidyanathan, Hall, Patrick and Bernat2011; Rothemund et al., Reference Rothemund, Ziegler, Hermann, Gruesser, Foell, Patrick and Flor2012; López et al., Reference López, Poy, Patrick and Moltó2013). Despite these observed behavioral deficits, recent studies on LPP amplitude evoked by visual emotional stimuli in subjects with psychopathic traits reported conflicting results. For instance, unpleasant stimuli evoked smaller LPP amplitude than neutral stimuli in healthy individuals with higher psychopathic traits compared to those with lower psychopathic traits. However, both groups displayed similar LPP amplitude in response to pleasant and neutral stimuli (Medina et al., Reference Medina, Kirilko and Grose–Fifer2016). In other studies, healthy individuals with high psychopathic traits showed no differences between emotional and neutral stimuli (Carolan et al., Reference Carolan, Jaspers-Fayer, Asmaro, Douglas and Liotti2014), but subjects with low psychopathic traits displayed greater LPP amplitude for emotional than for neutral stimuli (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, MacNamara and Olvet2010; Carolan et al., Reference Carolan, Jaspers-Fayer, Asmaro, Douglas and Liotti2014). Finally, some studies revealed no differences between groups with high and low psychopathic traits in LPP amplitudes evoked by emotional stimuli (e.g. Eisenbarth et al., Reference Eisenbarth, Angrilli, Calogero, Harper, Olson and Bernat2013).

In sum, the influence of emotional valence on LPP amplitude appears to be significant in healthy subjects but remains unclear in subjects with psychopathic traits. The goal of this work was to evaluate the influence of emotion on LPP amplitude elicited by visual emotional stimuli in subjects with and without psychopathic traits.

After a systematic search of the current literature, we conducted a meta-analysis to compare the influence of emotional valence on LPP amplitude in subjects with and without psychopathic traits. We hypothesized that individuals with psychopathic traits would display smaller LPP amplitude than controls when presented with emotional stimuli, especially when they are presented with negative stimuli.

Methods

Search strategy

We conducted a systematic review following the recommendations of the Cochrane collaboration (Chandler et al., Reference Chandler, Churchill, Higgins, Lasserson and Tovey2012) and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff and Altman2009).

Identification

We conducted a systematic search in the PubMed and Web of Science databases for full-length original articles (see details in online supplementary SM1).

Screening and eligibility

Two investigators (WV, JB) independently screened the results according to the eligibility criteria, first on titles and abstracts and then on full-text articles. Eligibility criteria are available in online supplementary material SM1.

Subjects characteristics

Regarding methods used for clinical diagnosis, subjects were assessed for psychopathy using the Psychopathy Checklist – Revised (PCL-R; Hare and Neumann, Reference Hare, Neumann and Patrick2006), the Psychopathy Checklist – Screening Version (PCL-SV; Hart et al., Reference Hart, Cox and Hare1995), or the Psychopathy Checklist – Youth Version (PCL-YV; Forth et al., Reference Forth, Kosson and Hare2003). We also included healthy subjects with psychopathic traits in the clinical group depending on the inclusion criteria from the respective studies. Such inclusion criteria were defined by scores in the upper tercile on the Psychopathy Personality Inventory-Revised (PPI-R; Lilienfeld and Widows, Reference Lilienfeld and Widows2005), or in the upper quartile on the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (LRSP; Levenson et al., Reference Levenson, Kiehl and Fitzpatrick1995) or on the Self-Report of Youth Behavior (SRYB; Olweus, Reference Olweus and Klein1989) or between 75 and 50th percentile on the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure (TriPM; Patrick, Reference Patrick2010).

Data collection process

We have extracted data related to LPP amplitude within each group from each study (see online supplementary SM1). Then, we have computed effect sizes from the data for LPP amplitude. We also compiled the number of subjects and clinical characteristics of subjects (age, sex, clinical diagnoses, and incarceration status), as well as information on experimental designs. When studies reported several time windows for LPP (Pincham et al., Reference Pincham, Bryce and Pasco - Fearon2015; Medina et al., Reference Medina, Kirilko and Grose–Fifer2016), we selected the time windows closely corresponding with the range of other studies. When data were missing or not fully reported, we contacted the corresponding author for further information.

Data extraction and methods of meta-analysis

Our primary outcome was LPP amplitude evoked by visual stimuli. It was calculated as the mean positive signal amplitude compared to the mean amplitude during a baseline interval for each stimulus category (pooled emotional (aggregate positive and negative stimuli), negative, positive and neutral valence) and each group (subjects with psychopathic traits, control subjects). An effect size for each study was calculated based on the extracted LPP amplitude from electrodes tested in the included studies. When original studies reported continuous psychopathy scores, we calculated the effect size based on correlation coefficient (r) by converting them in Cohen's d and collecting effect sizes and variance (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988; Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal, Cooper and Hedges1994; Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). Details on effect size calculation for included studies are given in the online supplementary material SM2.

Categorical moderator for emotional valence: meta-regression analysis

We assessed the impact of one categorical moderator in the meta-analysis to investigate whether LPP amplitude was different between individuals with psychopathic traits and controls regarding the emotional valence of stimuli. The datasets from the selected studies were divided into 4 categories according to the stimulus valence (pooled emotional, positive, negative, and neutral). The datasets were entered in separate categories in the meta-regression analysis model.

Results

Selection of studies

The primary search yielded 153 results. The flowchart diagram of the search is provided in Fig. 1. Among the 153 abstracts assessed for screening, 11 duplicates were removed, and 123 abstracts were excluded according to the eligibility criteria. The remaining 19 studies + 3 references were then assessed for eligibility based on full-length articles. Seven articles were excluded because they did not use clinical scales to assess psychopathy and 2 because they did not use visual emotional stimuli. Thirteen articles were then included in the meta-analysis.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow chart of the search process. Emo, emotional, Pos, positive, Neg, negative, and Neu, neutral. The supplementary references (Rothemund et al., Reference Rothemund, Ziegler, Hermann, Gruesser, Foell, Patrick and Flor2012; Brislin et al., Reference Brislin, Yancey, Perkins, Palumbo, Drislane, Salekin and Patrick2018 and Brennan et al., Reference Brennan, Crowley, Wu, Mayes and Baskin-Sommers2018), were not added due to characteristics of clinical measurement (externalizing scale) and absence of visual emotional stimuli.

Characteristics of selected studies

The 13 selected articles reported 23 datasets which were divided according to stimulus valence. Three datasets of emotional stimuli, 10 datasets of negative stimuli, 5 datasets of positive stimuli, and 5 datasets of neutral stimuli were included in the meta-analysis. Experimental paradigms used to measure LPP amplitude included picture-viewing paradigms, emotional Stroop task, and oddball target detection task. Stimuli were selected among Karolinska directed emotional faces (Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Flykt and Öhman1998), emotional IAPS pictures (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Ohman and Vaitl1988), NimStim pictures (Decety et al., Reference Decety, Michalska, Akitsuki and Lahey2009) of negative, positive or neutral valence and Amazon's Mechanical Turk (https://www.mturk.com/). Details on characteristics of included studies are provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of the 13 studies included in the meta-analysis

M, mean; N, sample size; d’, Cohen's d; v, variance of d’; s.d., standard deviation.

Electrode sites are given according to the 10/20 EEG system.

Scales: PCL-R: Psychopathy Checklist – Revised; PCL-SV: Psychopathy Checklist – Screening Version; PCP-YV: Psychopathy Checklist – Youth Version; PPI-R: Psychopathy Personality Inventory-Revised; LRSP: Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale; SRYB: Self-Report of Youth Behaviour; TriPM: Triarchic Psychopathy Measure. Please note that for Van Dongen et al. (Reference Van Dongen, Brazil, van der Veen and Franken2018); Ellis et al. (Reference Ellis, Schroder, Patrick and Moser2017); Sadeh and Verona, (Reference Sadeh and Verona2012) and Howard and McCullagh (Reference Howard and McCullagh2007), the effect was led and reported according to meanness, boldness and PCL-R F1, respectively.

The meta-analysis included 474 clinical subjects (75% male n = 356; 25% female n = 95). The clinical group consisted of 229 adults and 37 juveniles diagnosed with psychopathy (aged 15 to 17 years old) and 208 adults with high psychopathic traits. The control group consisted of 76 adults [56% males (n = 43); 44% females (n = 34)] and 24 males juveniles. Among the 13 studies, 9 studies reported information on medication and comorbidity as exclusion criteria (Howard and McCullagh, Reference Howard and McCullagh2007; Anderson and Stanford, Reference Anderson and Stanford2012; Carolan et al., Reference Carolan, Jaspers-Fayer, Asmaro, Douglas and Liotti2014; Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Hung and Decety2012; Sadeh and Verona, Reference Sadeh and Verona2012; Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Curtin and Newman2013; Decety et al., Reference Decety, Lewis and Cowell2015; Venable et al., Reference Venables, Hall, Yancey and Patrick2015; van Dongen et al., Reference Van Dongen, Brazil, van der Veen and Franken2018). Two studies included subjects with substance abuse (Howard and McCullagh, Reference Howard and McCullagh2007; Eisenbarth et al., Reference Eisenbarth, Angrilli, Calogero, Harper, Olson and Bernat2013) and 2 studies reported the cut-off for intelligence score under 70 IQ (Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Curtin and Newman2013; Eisenbarth et al., Reference Eisenbarth, Angrilli, Calogero, Harper, Olson and Bernat2013).

Meta-regression model: impact of emotional valence on LPP modulation in clinical sample

Regression model test for residual heterogeneity indicated that the categories of moderator were equally homogeneous [QE (df = 19) = 16.04, p = 0.65] and the omnibus test indicated a significant effect of the moderator [QM (df = 2) = 28.63, p < 0.005]. Tests for funnel plot asymmetry indicated no potential publication bias (t = 0.45, df = 18, p = 0.65; see Fig. 2). The effect of categorical moderator for LPP amplitude suggested the implication of negative emotional processing in reduction of LPP amplitude compared to processing of neutral and positive stimuli (β = −0.35, s.e. = 0.11, zval = −3.09, p < 0.005; see Fig. 3). Pooled emotional category was also significantly greater compared to neutral and positive categories (β = −0.64, s.e. = 0.13, zval = −4.89, p < 0.005).

Fig. 2. Funnel plot for meta-analysis. Points represent the observed effect sizes with standard error. In the current meta-analysis, all points falling on the pseudo confidence interval region and Eggers test for funnel plot asymmetry reported no publication bias.

Fig. 3. Forest plot for meta-analysis with categories depicting the results (sample size for clinical and control group, effect size and relative risk) of individual studies grouped according to emotional valence. For each category, a summary polygon shows the result of the random effect model according to the studies in each category.

Random effect model (RE): categories analysis

The analysis of LPP amplitude evoked by negative stimuli included 10 datasets. In line with the regression model, the clinical group displayed significant smaller LPP amplitude evoked by negative stimuli than the control group (mean effect size = −0.47; 95% CI −0.60 to −0.33; p < 0.005). The heterogeneity test suggested that the studies were homogeneous (Q = 11.46; p = 0.24). There was no significant effect at the Egger's test suggesting a symmetrical forest plot and no significant potential publication bias [(t) = 0.45; p = 0.65]. Pooled emotional category also provided significant effect between clinical and control groups with reduced LPP amplitude (mean effect size = −0.76; 95% CI −0.93 to −0.59; p < 0.005), homogeneity between studies (Q = 0.57; p = 0.74) and no publication bias [(t) = −0.86; p = 0.54].

The analysis of LPP amplitude evoked by positive stimuli included 5 datasets. The analysis reported no difference between clinical and control groups on LPP amplitude evoked by positive stimuli (mean effect size = −0.15; 95% CI −0.42 to 0.12; p = 0.28), and the heterogeneity test indicated that the studies were homogeneous (Q = 1.52; p = 0.82). There was no significant effect at the Egger's test suggesting a symmetrical forest plot and no significant potential publication bias [(t) = −0.50; p = 0.65]. The analysis on neutral stimuli reported no significant difference between clinical and control groups (mean effect size = −0.12; 95% CI 0.31 to 0.07; p = 0.21) and homogeneity was conserved (Q = 2.47; p = 0.65). Egger's test reported no publication bias [(t) = −0.22; p = 0.83].

Finally, we analyzed LPP amplitude when subjects were presented emotional stimuli only (positive, negative and pooled emotional). In line with previous findings, individuals with psychopathic traits displayed reduced LPP amplitude compared with controls (mean effect size = −0.48; 95% CI −0.61 to −0.34; p < 0.005) but homogeneity between studies was not found (Q = 28.46; p = 0.03).

Discussion

The goal of this meta-analysis was to examine the effect of emotion on LPP amplitude evoked by visual emotional stimuli between subjects with psychopathic traits and controls. Main results indicated that compared to the control group, the clinical group displayed smaller LPP amplitudes when presented with negative stimuli but not with positive or neutral stimuli.

Late positive potential and psychopathy

First, we observed smaller LPP amplitude in clinical as compared to control subjects when presented with emotional pooled stimuli or negative stimuli only. This can be interpreted in various ways. Aspects of pleasantness, arousing and affectivity of stimuli should be considered. Interestingly, LPP amplitudes elicited by low-arousal unpleasant, low-arousal pleasant and neutral stimuli were not different from each other, but LPP amplitude was greater for high-arousal unpleasant stimuli than high-arousal pleasant and neutral stimuli (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Goodman and Inzlicht2012). Furthermore, attention on non-arousing parts of unpleasant stimuli reduced LPP amplitude (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Moser and Simons2006) and latency (Dunning and Hajcak, Reference Dunning and Hajcak2009). Also, LPP amplitude was reduced when subjects made non-affective rather than affective judgments on emotional stimuli (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Moser and Simons2006). Thus, it is possible that the clinical group perceived emotional or negative stimuli as less arousing and affective than the control subjects. Future work should include self-reported pleasantness, arousal, and affectivity ratings when investigating LPP amplitude. Second, it is possible that this smaller LPP in the clinical group reflects such inter-individual ratings. Furthermore, it would be interesting to explore negative stimuli processing by including the processing of fearful stimuli. Indeed, a previous meta-analysis has reported that individuals with antisocial behaviors showed deficits (non-recognition) in processing fearful faces (Marsh and Blair, Reference Marsh and Blair2008). Results from the present work support the clinical relevance of LPP in psychopathy. Future work should also test for correlations between LPP amplitude and symptoms of psychopathy to determine whether LPP may carry such clinical relevance (Dennis and Hajcak, Reference Dennis and Hajcak2009).

LPP as a neuromarker for psychopathy?

The identification of a neuromarker for psychopathy, such as a specific modulation of an electrophysiological component, remains of major interest in clinical research for differential diagnosis and treatment optimization. First of all, EEG neuromarkers remain a potential and valuable clinical tool. For instance, the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has validated EEG neuromarkers for the diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (FDA, 2013). Neuromarkers like LPP modulation could also contribute to the ethological exploration of psychopathy. In this perspective, the current analysis of the modulation of LPP seems to be mainly driven by affective traits of psychopathy. Second of all, available treatment for patients with psychopathic traits are often not enough to respond to patients needs and the disorder is difficult to treat (Salekin, Reference Salekin2002). Therefore, it is essential to lay a robust foundation on electrophysiological functioning and improve etiological knowledge from which new therapeutic approaches will be able to build upon. Fundamental approaches of the electrophysiological process underlying cognitive functioning in psychiatric disorders provide a supplementary framework for traditional classification based primarily on symptoms/signs used to diagnose the mental disorder. Indeed, the National Institute of Mental Health's Research Domain Criteria provides a framework that emphasizes the integration of basic behavioral and neuroscience research to deepen the understanding of mental disorder (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010).

LPP amplitude has been shown to exhibit abnormal patterns across several psychiatric conditions as compared with controls. For example, conversely to those with psychopathy, individuals with risk for schizophrenia exhibited an increased LPP amplitude when presented with negative visual stimuli, suggesting an increase in affective reactivity to emotional stimuli (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Karcher, Bartholow, Siegle and Kerns2017). Abnormalities in LPP evoked by emotional stimuli were also observed in patients with anxiety and major depressive disorder, but with differential effects. Patients with anxiety disorders displayed an enhanced LPP amplitude evoked by negative stimuli (Kujawa et al., Reference Kujawa, MacNamara, Fitzgerald, Monk and Phan2015) whereas patients with major depressive disorder exhibited a reduced LPP for both positive and negative stimuli (Proudfit et al., Reference Proudfit, Bress, Foti, Kujawa and Klein2015; MacNamara et al., Reference MacNamara, Kotov and Hajcak2016). Our results suggest that a (1) reduction of LPP evoked by negative stimuli and a (2) normal LPP response to positive and neutral stimuli would be specific to individuals with psychopathy and psychopathy traits. Considering these studies, it should be further investigated if a reduction of LPP limited to negative stimuli could discriminate psychopathy from other clinical conditions. Thus, it could be possible to consider LPP as a potential neuromarker to characterize psychopathy.

Psychopathy: neurobiological correlates of disorder

Regarding the neural substrates that may underpin impairments in emotional processing, previous works have reported the implication of the occipital cortex, the amygdala, the temporal areas, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in the generation of LPP (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Huang, McGinnis-Deweese, Keil and Ding2012). Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), studies have revealed several abnormalities in individuals with psychopathy across brain structures and connectivity implicated in the generation of LPP. Among them, a reduction of hemodynamic activity was observed in the OFC and the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), the amygdala, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the ventro-medial PFC and the superior temporal gyrus (STS) (Kiehl, Smith and Hare, Reference Kiehl, Smith and Hare2001; Blair, Reference Blair2008; Dolan and Fullam, Reference Dolan and Fullam2009; Ermer et al., Reference Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2013; Lockwood et al., Reference Lockwood, Sebastian, McCrory, Hyde, Gu, De Brito and Viding2013; Cope et al., Reference Cope, Ermer, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2014). A reduced activity in the basolateral amygdala, especially during negative stimuli presentation has also been reported (Larson et al., Reference Larson, Baskin-Sommers, Stout, Balderston, Curtin, Schultz, Kiehl and Newman2013). Finally, a meta-analysis of brain imaging studies in individuals with psychopathy (Yang and Raine, Reference Yang and Raine2009) has reported functional and structural abnormalities in the right OFC, the left DLPFC and the ACC. Additionally, abnormalities in brain networks involved in attentional processes towards emotional stimuli, such as the amygdala-PFC network (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Mitchell and Blair2005; Contreras-Rodriguez et al., Reference Contreras-Rodríguez, Pujol, Batalla, Harrison, Soriano-Mas, Deus, López-Solà, Macià, Pera, Hernández-Ribas, Pifarré, Menchón and Cardoner2015), have been described in individuals with psychopathy. Moreover, in individuals with psychopathy, a reduced connectivity between the PFC and the amygdala has been previously observed (Blair, Reference Blair2008; Motzkin et al., Reference Motzkin, Newman, Kiehl and Koenigs2011). Such impairments in structural and functional brain connectivity may underpin the inability to correctly process negative emotional stimuli in individuals with psychopathic traits.

Discussion on the psychometric scales and psychopathy constructs

The majority of the scales for the assessment of psychopathic traits use a modern conception of psychopathy which suggests a pathological personality construct comprising factor conceptualization rather than a unitary construct. The PCL-R is a standard to assess psychopathy according to the factor conceptualization (Hare et al., Reference Hare, Harpur, Hakstian, Forth, Hart and Newman1990; Benning et al., Reference Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen and Krueger2003). The PCL-R and other versions [as PCL-YV is similar to PCL-R in terms of factor structure (Forth et al., Reference Forth, Kosson and Hare2003)] comprise interpersonal-affective Factor-1 (PCL-R F1), impulsive-antisocial Factor-2 (PCL-R F2) and 4 facets related to interpersonal, affective, lifestyle and antisocial traits (Hare and Neumann, Reference Hare, Neumann and Patrick2006). The PCL-R is a semi-structured interview scale dedicated to identifying personality traits and behavior related to psychopathy. However, the PCL-R is relatively limited regarding standardized administration, items tailored to individuals with criminal history and the need to access file information pertaining to official criminal records and institutional behavior (Benning et al., Reference Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen and Krueger2003). Based on factor conceptualization, several self-reported measures were developed from the PCL-R, as the LRSP, or separately, as the PPI-R. The LRSP, as the PCL-R, assesses psychopathy using a two-factor conceptualization. The primary factor of LSRP is related partly to interpersonal-affective factors of the PCL-R whereas the secondary factor is related to its impulsive-antisocial factors (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Gaughan and Pryor2008). The PPI-R conception is based on eight subscales which can be organized into two higher factors: fearless dominance (FD) and self-centered impulsivity (SCI). Regarding the validation studies for PPI-R, the FD factor is mainly associated to low emotional reactivity of the PCL-R F1, and the SCI factor is associated with anti-social behavior of the PCL-R F2 (Uzieblo et al., Reference Uzieblo, Verschuere, Van den Bussche and Crombez2010).

The severity of affective or antisocial traits in individuals with psychopathy related to these scales could be a confounding factor in the current analysis (Hare and Neumann, Reference Hare, Neumann and Patrick2006; Verona, Reference Verona2016). Indeed, previous studies have reported that deficits in attentional processing during emotional perception are linked to the severity of interpersonal affective traits (Sadeh and Verona, Reference Sadeh and Verona2008; Newman et al., Reference Newman, Curtin, Bertsch and Baskin–Sommers2010). Regarding the PPI-R, the factor results are clearly important because the FD and SCI factors are largely uncorrelated (Benning et al., Reference Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen and Krueger2003; Lilienfeld and Widows, Reference Lilienfeld and Widows2005). Among the studies included in the current article, Medina et al. (Reference Medina, Kirilko and Grose–Fifer2016) reporting results for both FD and SCI factors of the PPI-R showed that the emotional blunting to unpleasant images in the late LPP was associated with FD factor rather than SCI factor scores. In the same way, the correlation between factors of PCL-R is relatively weak (Hare, Reference Hare1991). The only study included in the current meta-analysis that has investigated this particular point reported a specific association between LPP amplitude and constructs of psychopathy of PCL-R F1 and PCL-R F2. The results showed negative correlation on PCL-R F1 and positive correlation on PCL-R F2 (Sadeh and Verona, Reference Sadeh and Verona2012). Regarding these results, the reduction of LPP toward negative emotional stimuli seems to be mainly led by affective traits of psychopathy. Importantly, several studies have previously described, relative to factors 1 and 2, the etiological heterogeneity of psychopathy construct for emotional processing (Venables et al., Reference Venables, Hall, Yancey and Patrick2015; Hicks and Patrick, Reference Hicks and Patrick2006; Schienle et al., Reference Schienle, Wabnegger, Leitner and Leutgeb2017), attentional processing (Verona et al., Reference Verona, Sprague and Sadeh2012) and conditioning (Veit et al., Reference Veit, Konicar, Klinzing, Barth, Yilmaz and Birbaumer2013).

A second model was used for assessing psychopathy and refers to the dimensional constructs of the triarchic model (Patrick et al., Reference Patrick, Fowles and Krueger2009). The main principle of this model is that psychopathy can be described with three distinct phenotypic constructs: disinhibition, boldness and meanness. The PCL-R F1 is associated with boldness and meanness phenotypic constructs but not with the disinhibition construct. The boldness subscale is also related to the FD construct indexed by the scores on the PPI-R. Meanness and disinhibition constructs, as PCL-R F2, are related to externalization features of psychopathy (Patrick et al., Reference Patrick, Hicks, Krueger and Lang2005). Previous works indicate that meanness and disinhibition have different etiological substrates (Frick and Marsee, Reference Frick, Marsee and Patrick2006). For the triarchic model, inclusion was coherent with the bi factorial conceptualization and the studies included in the current meta-analysis were based on boldness and meanness directly related to PPI-R FD and PCLR-F1.

Limitations

Limitations of this work should be acknowledged. First of all, the method of assessment can constitute a limitation between different clinical interviews (as for PCL-R and self-report as PPI-R). Whenever possible, in studies using clinical interviews, the analysis of moderating effect of mode assessment should be considered. In the current analysis, the heterogeneity of clinical scales and methods of assessment suggests that the results of this meta-analysis could not be caused by a screening effect. Regarding this limitation, the Triarchic Psychopathy measure (Patrick et al., Reference Patrick, Fowles and Krueger2009) was developed as an integrative framework to help integrate findings across research studies and reconcile differing conceptions of psychopathy (Drislane et al., Reference Drislane, Patrick and Arsal2014). Thus, the systematic use of the TriPM scale in combination with others scale from studies exploring psychopathy and electrophysiological signature will be particularly relevant to future studies. The TriPM was developed from established inventories and it allows to study psychopathy in existing datasets. Moreover, it provides a basis for establishing a latent variable operationalization of the triarchic model and could be used as an empirical referent in future meta-analytic investigations (Drislane and Patrick, Reference Drislane and Patrick2017).

The absence of systematic analyses and reports of factors and facets of psychopathy across clinical tools in the included studies is also a limitation of our work. It will be essential in future studies to systematically compare the differential effects of psychopathy dimensions.

In the current analysis, sex could not be included as moderator in analysis. Regarding the clinical sample used for this meta-analysis, the sex ratio [75% male (n = 356); 25% female] was consistent with epidemiological studies on psychopathy which predicts less prevalence of psychopathy in women. In general, analyses of sex ratio in psychopathy were very difficult to establish due to the heterogeneity in sample and study procedures, which prevented the use of statistical analyses. The last meta-analysis to date (Beryl et al., Reference Beryl, Chou and Völlm2014) concludes on sex ratio prevalence rates ranged from 1.05% to 31% in the female sample (using the PCL-R with cut off criterion of 30; or 0–16% when using the PCL: SV with a cut off criterion of 18). This is lower than the prevalence rates reported in male samples (15–30%). This difference in sex ratio may have slightly impacted the LPP modulation in a psychopathic clinical sample. Sex difference in the manifestation of psychopathy has been previously reported (for review see Cale and Lilienfeld, Reference Cale and Lilienfeld2002). At last, there seems to be a difference in LPP modulation between male and female samples during emotional regulation when presenting emotional stimuli (Gardener et al., Reference Gardener, Carr, MacGregor and Felmingham2013).

Regarding medication, it has been reported that antidepressant medication can enhance the performance of subjects in emotion recognition tasks (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, Dawson, Dourish, Favaron, Parsons, Fiore and Goodwin2013). Benzodiazepine and psychotropic medications are known to affect EEG activity (Aiyer et al., Reference Aiyer, Novakovic and Barkin2016; Jobert and Wilson, Reference Jobert and Wilson2015). Medication was not systematically reported in the included studies. Thus, this potentially may have influenced performances at emotion recognition task and modulated EEG activity.

The relative heterogeneity of cognitive tasks and stimuli used to elicit LPP in the different included studies could also constitute a limitation. However, this heterogeneity reflects the variety of stimuli that subjects must process in ecological conditions and supports the transferability of the results in real-life situations.

Finally, a relative heterogeneity across sample tests should be addressed. In the current meta-analysis, we included incarcerated and non-incarcerated subjects. As the prevalence of psychopathy reaches approximately 1% in the general population and 15–20% in the inmate population (Ogloff, Reference Ogloff2006; Coid et al., Reference Coid, Yang, Ullrich, Roberts and Hare2009; Sullivan and Kosson, Reference Sullivan, Kosson and Patrick2009), the inclusion of non-incarcerated individuals with psychopathy traits could be considered as an ecological aspect, which added value to the current analysis.

Conclusion

The current meta-analysis highlights that individuals with psychopathy displayed abnormal processing of negative stimuli but not of neutral and positive ones. The current study constitutes a first step toward the identification of a neuromarker of abnormal emotional processing in individual with psychopathy. The development of LPP as a neuromarker for psychopathy will require further investigation in order to define its possible implication in diagnostic and how it would actually be measured and controlled in practice. The consideration of new models like the Triarchic model construct and the TriPM scale could be efficient tools that allow future meta-analysis to explore the implication of phenotypic constructs in electrophysiological modulation and symptomatology in individuals with psychopathy.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002216

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Chantal Dufour from the ‘Centre de Documentation du CIUSSS de la Capitale’ and Dr Jean Leblond from the CIUSSS for their help in the literature search and statistical methodology, respectively.

Financial support

No financial support has been received for this work.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

Aiyer, R, Novakovic, V and Barkin, RL (2016) A systematic review on the impact of psychotropic drugs on electroencephalogram waveforms in psychiatry. Postgraduate Medicine 128, 656664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, NE and Stanford, MS (2012) Demonstrating emotional processing differences in psychopathy using affective ERP modulation. Psychophysiology 49, 792806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baskin-Sommers, AR, Curtin, JJ and Newman, JP (2013) Emotion-modulated startle in psychopathy: clarifying familiar effects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benning, SD, Patrick, CJ, Hicks, BM, Blonigen, DM and Krueger, RF (2003) Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical assessment. Psychological Assessment 15, 340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beryl, R, Chou, S and Völlm, B (2014) A systematic review of psychopathy in women within secure settings. Personality and Individual Differences 71, 185195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, RJR (2008) The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex: functional contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 363, 25572565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, J, Mitchell, D and Blair, K (2005) The Psychopath – Emotion and The Brain. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing, pp. 0211.Google Scholar
Borenstein, M, Hedges, LV, Higgins, J and Rothstein, HR (2009) Introduction to Meta – Analysis. New York, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 409414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, GM, Crowley, MJ, Wu, J, Mayes, LC and Baskin-Sommers, AR (2018) Neural processing of social exclusion in individuals with psychopathic traits: links to anger and aggression. Psychiatry Research 268, 263271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brislin, SJ, Yancey, JR, Perkins, ER, Palumbo, IM, Drislane, LE, Salekin, RT and Patrick, CJ (2018) Callousness and affective face processing in adults: behavioral and brain-potential indicators. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 9, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, KW, Goodman, RJ and Inzlicht, M (2012) Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8, 9399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cale, EM and Lilienfeld, SO (2002) Sex differences in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder: A review and integration. Clinical psychology review 22, 11791207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carolan, PL, Jaspers-Fayer, F, Asmaro, DT, Douglas, KS and Liotti, M (2014) Electrophysiology of blunted emotional bias in psychopathic personality. Psychophysiology 51, 3641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandler, J, Churchill, R, Higgins, J, Lasserson, T and Tovey, D (2012) Methodological standards for the conduct of new cochrane intervention reviews. Version 2.2. December 17, 2012.Google Scholar
Cheng, Y, Hung, AY and Decety, J (2012) Dissociation between affective sharing and emotion understanding in juvenile psychopaths. Development and Psychopathology 24, 623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Codispoti, M, Bradley, MM and Lang, PJ (2001) Affective reactions to briefly presented pictures. Psychophysiology 38, 474478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, p. 2.Google Scholar
Coid, J, Yang, M, Ullrich, S, Roberts, A and Hare, RD (2009) Prevalence and correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 32, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Contreras-Rodríguez, O, Pujol, J, Batalla, I, Harrison, BJ, Soriano-Mas, C, Deus, J, López-Solà, M, Macià, D, Pera, V, Hernández-Ribas, R, Pifarré, J, Menchón, JM and Cardoner, N (2015) Functional connectivity bias in the prefrontal cortex of psychopaths. Biological Psychiatry 78, 647655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cope, LM, Ermer, E, Nyalakanti, P, Calhoun, VD and Kiehl, KA (2014) Paralimbic gray matter reductions in incarcerated adolescent females with psychopathic traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 42, 659668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, BN, Schupp, HT, Bradley, MM, Birbaumer, N and Lang, PJ (2000) Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology 52, 95111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawel, A, O'Kearney, R, McKone, E and Palermo, R (2012) Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 22882304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J, Michalska, KJ, Akitsuki, Y and Lahey, BB (2009) Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Biological Psychology 80, 203211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J, Lewis, KL and Cowell, JM (2015) Specific electrophysiological components disentangle affective sharing and empathic concern in psychopathy. Journal of Neurophysiology 114, 493504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, TA and Hajcak, G (2009) The late positive potential: a neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50, 13731383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolan, MC and Fullam, RS (2009) Psychopathy and functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent responses to emotional faces in violent patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 66, 570577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drislane, LE and Patrick, CJ (2017) Integrating alternative conceptions of psychopathic personality: a latent variable model of triarchic psychopathy constructs. Journal of Personality Disorders 31, 110132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drislane, LE, Patrick, CJ and Arsal, G (2014) Clarifying the content coverage of differing psychopathy inventories through reference to the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure. Psychological Assessment 26, 350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, JP and Hajcak, G (2009) See no evil: directing visual attention within unpleasant images modulates the electrocortical response. Psychophysiology 46, 2833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenbarth, H, Angrilli, A, Calogero, A, Harper, J, Olson, LA and Bernat, E (2013) Reduced negative affect response in female psychopaths. Biological Psychology 94, 310318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, JD, Schroder, HS, Patrick, CJ and Moser, JS (2017) Emotional reactivity and regulation in individuals with psychopathic traits: evidence for a disconnect between neurophysiology and self-report. Psychophysiology 54, 15741585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ermer, E, Cope, LM, Nyalakanti, PK, Calhoun, VD and Kiehl, KA (2013) Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in incarcerated male adolescents with psychopathic traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 52, 94103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, G, Stobbe, Y, van Goozen, SH, Calder, A and Goodyer, IM (2010) Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biological Psychiatry 68, 272279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FDA (2013) De novo classification request for neuropsychiatric EEG-based assessment aid for ADHD (NEBA) system. (https://www.accessdata.fda.gov/cdrh_docs/reviews/K112711.pdf).Google Scholar
Flor, H, Birbaumer, N, Herman, C, Ziegler, S and Patrick, CJ (2002) Aversive Pavlovian conditioning in psychopaths: Peripheral and central correlates. Psychophysiology 39, 505518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forth, AE, Kosson, DS and Hare, RD (2003) Hare psychopathy checklist: Youth version. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Incorporated.Google Scholar
Foti, D and Hajcak, G (2008) Deconstructing reappraisal: descriptions preceding arousing pictures modulate the subsequent neural response. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, 977988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D, Hajcak, G and Dien, J (2009) Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. Psychophysiology 46, 521530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, PJ and Marsee, M (2006) Psychopathy and developmental pathways to antisocial behavior in youth. In Patrick, CJ (ed), Handbook of psychopathy. New-York, USA: The Guilford Press, pp. 353374.Google Scholar
Gao, Y and Raine, A (2009) P3 event-related potential impairments in antisocial and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Biological Psychology 82, 199210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardener, EK, Carr, AR, MacGregor, A and Felmingham, KL (2013) Sex differences and emotion regulation: an event-related potential study. PLoS One 8, e73475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G and Nieuwenhuis, S (2006) Reappraisal modulates the electrocortical response to unpleasant pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 6, 291297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G and Olvet, DM (2008) The persistence of attention to emotion: brain potentials during and after picture presentation. Emotion 8, 250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G, Moser, JS and Simons, RF (2006) Attending to affect: appraisal strategies modulate the electrocortical response to arousing pictures. Emotion 6, 517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G, Dunning, JP and Foti, D (2007) Neural response to emotional pictures is unaffected by concurrent task difficulty: an event-related potential study. Behavioral Neuroscience 121, 1156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajcak, G, Dunning, JP and Foti, D (2009) Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: time–course of the late positive potential. Clinical Neurophysiology 120, 505510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G, MacNamara, A and Olvet, DM (2010) Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: an integrative review. Developmental Neuropsychology 35, 129155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, RD (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised: Manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Incorporated.Google Scholar
Hare, RD (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised: Manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Incorporated.Google Scholar
Hare, RD and Neumann, CS (2006) The PCL-R Assessment of Psychopathy: Development, Structural Properties, and New Directions. In Patrick, CJ (ed), Handbook of psychopathy. New York, USA: The Guilford Press, pp. 5888.Google Scholar
Hare, RD, Harpur, TJ, Hakstian, AR, Forth, AE, Hart, SD and Newman, JP (1990) The revised Psychopathy Checklist: Reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2, 338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmer, CJ, Dawson, GR, Dourish, CT, Favaron, E, Parsons, E, Fiore, M and Goodwin, GM (2013) Combined NK (1) antagonism and serotonin reuptake inhibition: effects on emotional processing in humans. Journal of Psychopharmacology 27, 435443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, SD, Cox, DN and Hare, RD (1995) Manual for the psychopathy checklist: Screening version (PCL: SV). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Hicks, BM and Patrick, CJ (2006) Psychopathy and negative emotionality: analyses of suppressor effects reveal distinct relations with emotional distress, fearfulness, and anger-hostility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 276287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, R and McCullagh, P (2007) Neuroaffective processing in criminal psychopaths: brain event–related potentials reveal task–specific anomalies. Journal of Personality Disorders 21, 322339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K and Wang, P (2010) Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification frame-work for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 748–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jobert, M and Wilson, FJ (2015) Advanced analysis of pharmaco – EEG data in humans. Neuropsychobiology 72, 165177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusyte, A and Schönenberg, M (2017) Impaired social cognition in violent offenders: perceptual deficit or cognitive bias? European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 267, 257266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keil, A, Bradley, MM, Hauk, O, Rockstroh, B, Elbert, T and Lang, PJ (2002) Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology 39, 641649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiehl, K, Smith, A and Hare, R (2001) Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry 50, 677684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kujawa, A, MacNamara, A, Fitzgerald, KD, Monk, CS and Phan, KL (2015) Enhanced neural reactivity to threatening faces in anxious youth: evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 43, 14931501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, PJ, Ohman, A and Vaitl, D (1988) The International Affective Picture System [Photographic Slides]. Gainesville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
Larson, CL, Baskin-Sommers, AR, Stout, DM, Balderston, NL, Curtin, JJ, Schultz, DH, Kiehl, KA and Newman, JP (2013) The interplay of attention and emotion: top-down attention modulates amygdala activation in psychopathy. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 13, 757770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, MR, Kiehl, KA and Fitzpatrick, CM (1995) Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68, 151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenston, GK, Patrick, CK, Bradley, MM and Lang, PJ (2000) The psychopath as observer: emotion and attention in picture processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lilienfeld, SO and Widows, MR (2005) PPI – R: Psychopathic Personality Inventory – Revised. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Liu, Y, Huang, H, McGinnis-Deweese, M, Keil, A and Ding, M (2012) Neural substrate of the late positive potential in emotional processing. Journal of Neuroscience 32, 1456314572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockwood, PL, Sebastian, CL, McCrory, EJ, Hyde, ZH, Gu, X, De Brito, SA and Viding, E (2013) Association of callous traits with reduced neural response to others’ pain in children with conduct problems. Current Biology 23, 901905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López, R, Poy, R, Patrick, CJ and Moltó, J (2013) Deficient fear conditioning and self-reported psychopathy: the role of fearless dominance. Psychophysiology 50, 210218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundqvist, D, Flykt, A and Öhman, A (1998) The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces–KDEF. CD–ROM From Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology Section. Stockholm, Sweden: Karolinska Institutet. ISBN 91–630–7164–9.Google Scholar
MacNamara, A, Kotov, R and Hajcak, G (2016) Diagnostic and symptom–based predictors of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder: an event–related potential study. Cognitive Therapy and Research 40, 275289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, AA and Blair, RJR (2008) Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta–analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 454465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, EA, Karcher, NR, Bartholow, BD, Siegle, GJ and Kerns, JG (2017) An electrophysiological investigation of emotional abnormalities in groups at risk for schizophrenia–spectrum personality disorders. Biological Psychology 124, 119132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medina, AL, Kirilko, E and Grose–Fifer, J (2016) Emotional processing and psychopathic traits in male college students: an event–related potential study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 106, 3949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, JD, Gaughan, ET and Pryor, LR (2008) The Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale: an examination of the personality traits and disorders associated with the LSRP factors. Assessment 15, 450463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, Altman, DG and Prisma Group (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta–analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine 6, e1000097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motzkin, JC, Newman, JP, Kiehl, KA and Koenigs, M (2011) Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 1734817357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, JP, Curtin, JJ, Bertsch, JD and Baskin–Sommers, AR (2010) Attention moderates the fearlessness of psychopathic offenders. Biological Psychiatry 67, 6670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogloff, JR (2006) Psychopathy/antisocial personality disorder conundrum. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 40, 519528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olweus, D (1989) Prevalence and incidence in the study of antisocial behavior: definitions and measurements. Cross-national research. In Klein, MW (ed), self-reported crime and delinquency. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer–Nijhoff, pp. 187201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, CJ (2010) Operationalizing the triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Preliminary description of brief scales for assessment of boldness, meanness, and disinhibition. Unpublished test manual, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL.Google Scholar
Patrick, CJ, Hicks, BM, Krueger, RF and Lang, AR (2005) Relations between psychopathy facets and externalizing in a criminal offender sample. Journal of Personality Disorders 19, 339356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, CJ, Fowles, DC and Krueger, RF (2009) Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and Psychopathology 21, 913938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pincham, HL, Bryce, D and Pasco - Fearon, RM (2015) The neural correlates of emotion processing in juvenile offenders. Developmental Science 18, 9941005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Proudfit, GH, Bress, JN, Foti, D, Kujawa, A and Klein, DN (2015) Depression and event-related potentials: emotional disengagement and reward insensitivity. Current Opinion in Psychology 4, 110113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R (1994) Parametric measures of effect size. In Cooper, H and Hedges, LV (eds), The Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, pp. 231244.Google Scholar
Rothemund, Y, Ziegler, S, Hermann, C, Gruesser, SM, Foell, J, Patrick, CJ and Flor, H (2012) Fear conditioning in psychopaths: event–related potentials and peripheral measures. Biological Psychology 90, 5059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, N and Verona, E (2008) Psychopathic personality traits associated with abnormal selective attention and impaired cognitive control. Neuropsychology 22, 669680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, N and Verona, E (2012) Visual complexity attenuates emotional processing in psychopathy: implications for fear–potentiated startle deficits. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 12, 346360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salekin, RT (2002) Psychopathy and therapeutic pessimism: clinical lore or clinical reality? Clinical Psychology Review 22, 79112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schienle, A, Wabnegger, A, Leitner, M and Leutgeb, V (2017) Neuronal correlates of personal space intrusion in violent offenders. Brain Imaging Behavior 11, 454460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönenberg, M, Mayer, SV, Christian, S, Louis, K and Jusyte, A (2016) Facial affect recognition in violent and nonviolent antisocial behavior subtypes. Journal of Personality Disorders 30, 708719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schupp, HT, Cuthbert, BN, Bradley, MM, Cacioppo, JT, Ito, T and Lang, PJ (2000) Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology 37, 257261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schupp, HT, Junghöfer, M, Weike, AI and Hamm, AO (2004 a) The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: an ERP analysis. Psychophysiology 41, 441449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schupp, HT, Öhman, A, Junghöfer, M, Weike, AI, Stockburger, J and Hamm, AO (2004 b) The facilitated processing of threatening faces: an ERP analysis. Emotion 4, 189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, EA and Kosson, DS (2006) Ethnic and cultural variations in psychopathy. In Patrick, CJ (ed), Handbook of Psychopathy. New-York, USA: The Guilford Press, pp. 437458.Google Scholar
Uzieblo, K, Verschuere, B, Van den Bussche, E and Crombez, G (2010) The validity of the psychopathic personality inventory – revised in a community sample. Assessment 17, 334346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaidyanathan, U, Hall, JR, Patrick, CJ and Bernat, EM (2011) Clarifying the role of defensive reactivity deficits in psychopathy and antisocial personality using startle reflex methodology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120, 253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dongen, JDM, Brazil, IA, van der Veen, FM and Franken, IHA (2018) Electrophysiological correlates of empathic processing in individuals with psychopathic meanness traits. Neuropsychology 32, 9961006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veit, R, Konicar, L, Klinzing, JG, Barth, B, Yilmaz, Ö and Birbaumer, N (2013) Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy as a function of interpersonal and affective disturbances. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venables, NC, Hall, JR, Yancey, JR and Patrick, CJ (2015) Factors of psychopathy and electrocortical response to emotional pictures: further evidence for a two–process theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 124, 319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verona, E (2016) Interactions cognition–emotion et personnalite psychopathique: des trajectoires distinctes vers les comportements antisociaux et violents. Santé Mentale au Québec 41, 6583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verona, E, Sprague, J and Sadeh, N (2012) Inhibitory control and negative emotional processing in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121, 498510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y and Raine, A (2009) Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 174, 8188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, C, He, W, Qi, Z, Wang, L, Song, D, Zhan, L, Yi, S, Luo, Y and Luo, W (2015) The time course of emotional picture processing: an event–related potential study using a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow chart of the search process. Emo, emotional, Pos, positive, Neg, negative, and Neu, neutral. The supplementary references (Rothemund et al., 2012; Brislin et al., 2018 and Brennan et al., 2018), were not added due to characteristics of clinical measurement (externalizing scale) and absence of visual emotional stimuli.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of the 13 studies included in the meta-analysis

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Funnel plot for meta-analysis. Points represent the observed effect sizes with standard error. In the current meta-analysis, all points falling on the pseudo confidence interval region and Eggers test for funnel plot asymmetry reported no publication bias.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Forest plot for meta-analysis with categories depicting the results (sample size for clinical and control group, effect size and relative risk) of individual studies grouped according to emotional valence. For each category, a summary polygon shows the result of the random effect model according to the studies in each category.

Supplementary material: File

Vallet et al. supplementary material

Vallet et al. supplementary material 1

Download Vallet et al. supplementary material(File)
File 26.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Vallet et al. supplementary material

Vallet et al. supplementary material 2

Download Vallet et al. supplementary material(File)
File 16 KB