Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T05:42:15.293Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Acheulean Handaxe Technological Persistence: A Case of Preferred Cultural Conservatism?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 March 2018

Meir Finkel*
Affiliation:
The Jacob M. Alkow Department of Archaeology and Ancient Near Eastern Cultures, Tel Aviv University, P.O.B. 39040, Ramat Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
Ran Barkai*
Affiliation:
The Jacob M. Alkow Department of Archaeology and Ancient Near Eastern Cultures, Tel Aviv University, P.O.B. 39040, Ramat Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
*
Meir Finkel (Corresponding author): finkel2010@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

One of the unsolved ‘paradoxes’ in prehistoric archaeology is that of the gap between the considerable advances in human biological and cultural evolution during the Lower Palaeolithic period, and the over one million years of ‘stagnation’ of the Acheulean handaxe. Most of the research on this topic has focused on innovation – why it was delayed or failed to take place – while overlooking the fact that innovation had occurred in many other fields during the same period. We suggest that practical, social, and adaptive mechanisms were in force in certain areas of human behaviour and led to enhanced innovation, while conservatism was preferred in handaxe technology and use. In this study we emphasise the dependency of Acheulean groups on calories obtained from large mammals, and especially megafauna, as well as the central role of handaxes in processing large carcasses. It is our contention that the handaxe’s role in Acheulean adaptation was pivotal and it thus became fixed in human society, probably through the psychological bias towards majority imitation, which subsequently became a social norm or tradition. In brief, we suggest that the technological persistence of the Acheulean handaxe played an adaptive role that was based on a preferred cultural conservatism and led to the successful survival of Lower Palaeolithic populations over hundreds of thousands of years in the Old World.

Résumé

Le hachereau acheuléen persistance technologique: Un cas de préférence du conservatisme culturel? de Meir Finkel et Ran Barkai

L’un des ‘paradoxes‘ non résolus de l’archéologie préhistorique est l’écart entre les considérables avancées dans l’évolution humaine biologique et culturelle au cours de la période du paléolithique inférieur et le million et quelques d’années de ‘stagnation‘ du hachereau herculéen. La plupart des recherches sur ce sujet se sont concentrées sur l’innovation: pourquoi a-t-elle été retardée ou a-t-elle échoué à se mettre en place, tout en négligeant le fait que des innovations avaient eu lieu dans bien d’autres domaines au cours de la même période. Nous proposons que des mécanismes pratiques, sociaux et d’adaptation étaient présents dans certaines secteurs du comportement humain et conduisaient à une innovation rehaussée tandis que le conservatisme avait la faveur en matière de technologie et d’usage du hachereau. Dans cette étude nous insistons sur la dépendance des groupes acheuléens sur les calories provenant des gros mammifères, en particulier de la mégafaune, ainsi que le rôle central des hachereaux dans la transformation des grosses carcasses. C’est notre point de vue que le rôle du hachereau dans l’adaptation acheuléenne était primordial et il s’en est trouvé fixé dans la société humaine probablement à travers un penchant psychologique vers une imitation de la majorité, ce qui est devenu par la suite une norme ou une tradition sociale. En bref, nous proposons que la persistance technologique du hachereau acheuléen a joué un rôle d’adaptation qui reposait sur une préférence pour un conservatisme culturel et a conduit au succès de leur survie les populations du paléolithique inférieur sur des centaines de milliers d’années dans l’Ancien Monde.

Zussamenfassung

Die technologische Beständigkeit des Faustkeils des Acheuléen: Ein Fall bevorzugten kulturellen Konservatismus? von Meir Finkel und Ran Barkai

Eines der ungelösten „Paradoxe“ der prähistorischen Archäologie ist das der Kluft zwischen den erheblichen Fortschritten der menschlichen biologischen und kulturellen Evolution während des Altpaläolithikums und der mehr als eine Million Jahre dauernden „Stagnation“ des Acheuléen-Faustkeils. Meist fokussierte die Forschung auf die Frage der Innovation – warum sie sich verzögerte oder gar nicht erst stattfand –, während die Tatsache übersehen wurde, dass in der gleichen Epoche in vielen anderen Bereichen Innovationen auftraten. Wir schlussfolgern deshalb, dass praktische, soziale und adaptive Mechanismen in bestimmten Bereichen menschlichen Verhaltens in Kraft waren und zu erhöhter Innovation führten, während in der Faustkeiltechnologie und seiner Nutzung ein Konservatismus bevorzugt wurde. In dieser Untersuchung stellen wir die Abhängigkeit der Menschengruppen des Acheuléen von Kalorien, die sie von Großsäugern erlangten, heraus, insbesondere von Megafauna, sowie die zentrale Rolle von Faustkeilen in der Verarbeitung großer Tierkörper. Es ist unsere Überzeugung, dass die Rolle des Faustkeils für die Adaption während des Acheuléen entscheidend war und er deshalb in der menschlichen Gesellschaft festgeschrieben wurde, möglicherweise durch eine psychologische Neigung zur Majoritäts-Imitation,woraus nach und nach eine soziale Norm oder Tradition wurde. Kurz gesagt gehen wir davon aus, dass die technologische Beständigkeit des Faustkeils des Acheuléen eine adaptive Rolle spielte, die auf einem bevorzugten kulturellen Konservatismus basierte und zum erfolgreichen Überleben altpaläolithischer Gruppen während hunderttausender Jahre in der Alten Welt führte.

Resumen

La persistencia tecnológica de los bifaces achelenses: ¿un caso de conservadurismo cultural? por Meir Finkel y Ran Barkai

Una de las paradojas sin resolver en arqueología prehistórica es el lapso temporal existente entre los considerables avances en evolución biológica y cultural durante el Paleolítico Inferior, y el ‘estancamiento’ durante más de un millón de años de los bifaces achelenses. La mayor parte de la investigación relacionada con este aspecto se ha centrado en la innovación -porqué se retrasó o no se llevó a cabo- mientras se ignora el hecho de que estas innovaciones se produjeron en muchos otros campos durante el mismo período. Nosotros sugerimos que los mecanismos prácticos, sociales y adaptativos estuvieron vigentes en ciertas áreas del comportamiento humano y condujeron a una mayor innovación, mientras que el conservadurismo se impuso en la tecnología y uso de los bifaces. En este estudio destacamos la dependencia de los grupos achelenses de las calorías obtenidas de los grandes mamíferos, especialmente la megafauna, así como el papel central de los bifaces en el procesado de las grandes carcasas. Siguiendo esta línea argumental, el papel del bifaz achelense en los procesos de adaptación fue fundamental y, por tanto, quedó fijado en la sociedad, probablemente en el ámbito psicológico a través de la imitación, llegando posteriormente a convertirse en una norma o tradición social. En resumen, sugerimos que la persistencia tecnológica de las hachas achelenses jugó un rol adaptativo basado en un conservadurismo cultural y condujo a la supervivencia de las poblaciones del Paleolítico Inferior durante cientos de miles de años en el Viejo Mundo.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© The Prehistoric Society 2018 

In modern culture, innovation is conceived as a key feature in the human struggle to survive, develop, and prosper. Innovation is associated with modernism and progress and is usually perceived as the antithesis of religious, social, or organisational conservatism. The layman sees innovation as integral to adaptation and survival and might consider that evolution prefers innovation over conservatism. In reality, however, since innovation is a conscious action, whereas natural mutation and selection are random, innovation does not necessarily promote survival. In practical terms, when measuring evolutionary success in the survival time of species, the most successful species are the conservative ‘living fossils’ such as the horseshoe crab or Cycas plant, which have not changed morphologically for over 100 Mya (Mayr Reference Mayr2001, 195, 287). On the other hand, mutations that supposedly represent innovation often do not survive the test of natural selection. The catchphrase of the advocates of evolutionary punctuated equilibrium theory, Eldredge and Gould, is ‘stasis is data’ (Reference Eldredge and Gould1972), meaning that the ‘usual fate of most species is stasis’ (Mayr Reference Mayr1992). We suggest that this basic notion may be applicable in the case of Acheulean handaxe technology: ie, a preferred stasis with no evolutionary drive for innovation.

The research in Palaeolithic archaeology suffers from a similar conceptual bias towards innovation. One of the unsolved phenomena is that of the gap between the considerable transformations in human biology and culture during the Lower Palaeolithic, and the impression of stagnation of the Acheulean handaxe (1.8/1.6–0.4/0.25 Mya – depending on regional differences). Much of the literature deals with the causes of this apparent stagnation, focusing on the seeming lack of creativity and innovation in Lower Palaeolithic times (eg, Elias Reference Elias2012; Mithen Reference Mithen1998; Renfrew & Morley Reference Renfrew and Morley2009). In general, such studies fail to address the dissonance between the handaxe’s apparent stagnation and the advances in other areas (including the use of fire, expansion into new territories, the development of predetermined blank production and the Levallois method, big game hunting, and a protein and fat-based diet). We may ask why the suggested mechanisms behind this seeming stagnation did not similarly apply to hinder the concomitant transformations that did take place. One possible explanation is that the handaxe ‘stagnation’ had an adaptive advantage – a direct functional edge and/or social tradition/norm that evolved around its manufacture and use. Our study develops the notion that cultural conservatism (Gould 1977; Sharon et al. Reference Sharon, Alperson-Afil and Goren-Inbar2011) may have been a preferred adaptive strategy and a key reason for the persistence observed in Lower Palaeolithic handaxe use in the Old World.

In general, Acheulean lithic technology is characterised by the production and use of flakes and flakes shaped as tools. However, its hallmark is considered to be the Acheulean handaxe (see Gowlett Reference Gowlett2013; Pope et al. Reference Pope, Russel and Watson2006; Machin Reference Machin2009; Sharon Reference Sharon2010; but also Barkai Reference Barkai2009) (Fig. 1). Handaxes are, in most cases, relatively large items (most commonly 10 cm or longer and a few cm thick; however smaller items also feature), shaped by extensive bifacial knapping that reflects manual dexterity, symmetry, and, in some cases, prolonged life cycles (Lycett & Gowlett Reference Lycett and Gowlett2008; Machin Reference Machin2009; Sharon Reference Sharon2010). Such bifaces are usually used as a fossil directeur for the Acheulean cultural complex, regardless of the fact that modern research strongly indicates the variability and complexity of Acheulean lithic technologies (eg, Agam et al. Reference Agam, Marder and Barkai2015; Bar Yosef Reference Bar Yosef2006; Dennell Reference Dennell2009; Hosfield Reference Hosfield2008; Gowlett Reference Gowlett2011; Lycett & Gowlett Reference Lycett and Gowlett2008).

Fig.1 Examples of Acheulean flint bifaces from the Revadim site, Israel

The present study begins with a survey of views regarding the handaxe technological ‘stagnation’, the consequent debate, and advances in other fields. Theories are then presented regarding the causes of this so-called stagnation or slow development. The last section presents our hypothesis that preferred cultural conservatism, based on the functional role of the handaxe in processing carcasses of large game, was the major cause of ‘stagnation’ and persistence. This preferred conservatism was reinforced by knowledge transmission mechanisms that enabled handaxe production and use to become a social norm and a key feature in the survival and prosperity of humans during the Lower Palaeolithic.

THE ACHEULEAN HANDAXE – A CASE OF TECHNOLOGICAL PERSISTENCE?

The handaxe is recognised as the hallmark of the Acheulean cultural complex for three main reasons: its wide geographic distribution; its continuous presence throughout the Acheulean (1.8/1.6–0.25 Mya in Africa and the Levant); and its persistent morphology and production technology. These characteristics have earned the handaxe the sobriquet ‘the Swiss Army knife of the Palaeolithic’. Various views concerning the level of technological stagnation or of change are often derived from the research terminology: stasis, stagnation, conservatism, slow change, monotony, and so forth. Each of these terms has its distinct nuance and thus represents a different level of ‘freedom’ in interpretation. While this article focuses on the continued persistence of the handaxe in the Acheulean cultural complex, some noticeable lithic innovations, such as prepared core technologies (eg, Adler et al. Reference Adler, Wilkinson, Blockley, Mark, Pinhasi, Schmidt-Magee and Nahapetyan2014; Nowell & White Reference Nowell and White2010) and the production of small flakes by means of recycling (eg, Agam et al. Reference Agam, Marder and Barkai2015; Shimelmitz Reference Shimelmitz2015) also took place alongside the tool’s apparent stagnation. These innovations, however, were not as geographically and chronologically widespread as the persistence of the handaxe. The following views relate to handaxe persistence and its bearing on Acheulean technology and culture.

The differing views on the issue can be divided into two schools: those that perceive stasis and those that emphasise a slow change. The terminology used to define the Acheulean technology by members of the first group include: ‘conservative’ (Scarre Reference Scarre2009, 95); ‘behavioral conservatism’ (Klein Reference Klein1999, 337); ‘essentially unchanged’ (Gamble Reference Gamble2007, 221); ‘rigid conformity’ and ‘conservatism’ (Bar-Yosef Reference Bar Yosef2006, 483); ‘essential ideas were handed over from tool-maker to tool-maker over an exceptionally long period’ (Lycett & Gowlett Reference Lycett and Gowlett2008, 309). Pope et al. (Reference Pope, Russel and Watson2006, 54–5) reached a similar conclusion regarding the symmetry and aesthetic form of the handaxe. Other scholars present data from specific sites, employing similar terminology such as: ‘definite conservatism’ (Gesher Benot Ya’akov, Israel: Sharon et al. Reference Sharon, Alperson-Afil and Goren-Inbar2011, 395); ‘remarkably strong intra-assemblage uniformity … strong rules or conventions in technological operations’ (Awash region, Ethiopia: Schick & Clark Reference Schick and Clark2003, 26); ‘uniformity … is consistent with a pattern of strong conventions held across Acheulean society’ (Hunsgi and Baichbal Valleys and Isampur Quarry, India: Petraglia et al. Reference Petraglia, Shipton and Paddayya2005, 208).

The second school of thought, which acknowledges the existence of variability, includes Nowell and White, who contend that although the convention concerning the ‘… technological conservatism of unparalleled magnitude, a million years of stasis usually attributed to the limited cognitive and linguistic abilities of the hominins involved’ (2010, 70), this misidentified stagnation is the result of analytical shortcomings. These researchers contend that handaxe variation can be detected at the global, regional (eg, in UK – see also a recent study by Bridgland and White (Reference Bridgland and White2015) suggesting chorological changes reflected in biface assemblages) and site levels (such as Boxgrove). They also state that the innovation and adoption of the handaxe itself was substantial (and flexible) enough to support transformations in the life history of Acheulean hominins. Inventiveness is suggested to have produced variations on a theme in the Acheulean, as in the case of the ‘prepared cores’ at Gesher Benot Ya’akov, Cagny La Garenne, France, and other sites, but these were relatively local (Nowell & White Reference Nowell and White2010, 73). It is generally assumed that a threshold – demographic, cognitive, physiological, social, or a combination thereof – had to be passed in order to make the next local ‘invention’ a global innovation (see also Hopkinson et al. Reference Hopkinson, Nowell and White2013).

Hosfield (Reference Hosfield2008, 15) suggests that the gap between the ‘sheer longevity and overall morphological stability of the Acheulean handaxe’ and behavioural variability and flexibility found in other aspects of hominin life is due to differences in research resolution. He suggests that the morphological stability of handaxes is not a reflection of behavioural stability, but is due to problems with the identification of handaxe variability. Other researchers who seek to explain the Acheulean industry variance still begin from the assumption of stasis (Machin Reference Machin2009, 36). Based on the same assumption, Hopkinson and White (Reference Hopkinson and White2005, 23–5) engage with the role of the individual in creating the variance/stagnation in the Acheulean handaxe industry. Somel et al. (Reference Somel, Liu and Khaitobich2013, 113) present an account of human brain evolution and employ the phrase ‘limited progress’ to describe the Acheulean technology.

McNabb et al. (Reference McNabb, Binyon and Hazelwood2004) argue that the reason for diversity among handaxes should be attributed to the stylistic choices of the individuals who made them. McNabb and Cole (Reference McNabb and Cole2015) recently employed orders of intentionality as a measure of cognitive evolution in order to suggest that what we observe today is basically variability, appearing within long periods of apparent stasis.

In conclusion, some perceive the Acheulean technological stagnation to be represented by a single tool type – the handaxe – that consistently appears both chronologically and geographically. Within this ‘stagnation’, variations exist in its manufacture and use at different sites and in different periods. For our case, including all its variations, the handaxe was sustained as the same ‘tool category’ (similar, in a way, to the modern hammer, axe, knife, or chopstick, which retain the same basic shape and function, albeit with many variations) for a prolonged time span, and we refer here to this continuity as ‘stagnation’.

TRANSFORMATION IN OTHER AREAS OF HUMAN BEHAVIOUR

Unlike the suggested ‘stagnation’ in handaxe technology, the Acheulean cultural complex features a variety of transformations in other fields. Before presenting some of these in brief, it is worth noting that at least two different human groups were producing and using handaxes: Homo ergaster/erectus and Homo heidelbergensis, and that Neanderthals continued to make bifaces in late Middle Palaeolithic Europe (however, we should keep in mind that the jury is out regarding the variability within the genus Homo; see for instance Schwartz & Tattersall Reference Schwartz and Tattersall2015). Machin (Reference Machin2009) surveyed several changes in Hominin characteristics between 1.8 Mya and 500 Kya, such as the increase in body size, brain size, and encephalisation; the development of communication and social complexity; changes in life history (longer periods of childhood and adolescence); social organisation; and strategies for procuring raw materials and food provisioning. Regarding food, Machin emphasises the rising place of meat in the diet and the transition from scavenging to hunting; a suggestion that has been recently strongly reinforced (Domínguez-Rodrigo & Pickering Reference Domínguez-Rodrigo and Pickering2017). The need for lengthy parental supervision might have led to monogamy, the division of labour between man and woman, intra-group changes, and the promotion of social learning (Machin Reference Machin2009, 38–40; Nowell & White Reference Nowell and White2010, 74–6). Regarding human cognitive abilities as reflected in the findings at Gesher Benot Ya’akov, Goren-Inbar (Reference Goren-Inbar2011) argues that inventions undoubtedly occurred in the Lower Palaeolithic: eg, the use of the soft hammer for shaping handaxes and the early appearance of the Levallois method (or prepared core technologies) that flourished later in the Middle Palaeolithic. Regarding the dissonance between these developments and handaxe stagnation’, Goren-Inbar further contends that ‘These innovations, together with the creativity expressed in the full control of fire and its uses, clearly negate the notion of stagnation and/or stasis’ (2011, 1045).

Dennell (Reference Dennell2009, 335) emphasises the transition to big game hunting in the Lower Palaeolithic and Klein (Reference Klein1999, 343–60) reviews transformations in human behaviour during the Lower Palaeolithic and the changes in various areas such as the use of fire, non-lithic tools, and the types of animals hunted. Nowell and White (Reference Nowell and White2010) argue that significant changes in human life history occurred in the Middle Pleistocene, such as increased geographic dispersion, and meat eating. The migration from Africa to Asia and Europe required abilities that enabled adaptation to diverse environmental and climatic conditions. Finally, the basic question remains: How can we explain the gap between the range of biological, social, and technological transformations and the ‘stagnation’ in handaxe technology?

POSSIBLE CAUSES FOR ACHEULEAN HANDAXE TECHNOLOGICAL ‘STAGNATION’ & THE LACK OF INNOVATION

Scholars combine various fields of research (human cognition, knowledge and information transmission, social learning, and demographic variables such as group size, composition, and distribution) to explain the apparent technological ‘stagnation’ in question. We contend here that the fundamental flaw in these theories is that if they hold true for the handaxe, then they are assumed to hold true for other transformations as well.

Recently, Corbey et al. (Reference Corbey, Jagich, Vaesen and Collard2016) suggested that handaxe persistence in time and space is better explained not only by cultural and social reasons but partly by genetic inheritance. Moncel et al. (Reference Moncel, Arzarellob, Boëdac, Bonilauri, Chevrier, Gaillard, Forestier, Yinghua, Sémah and Zeitoun2016) somewhat follow this line of thinking, suggesting that the explanation for the combination of wide geographic distribution and the diversity of the handaxe lies in convergent evolution. In our view, such explanations need to be better supported and argued, and for the time being we therefore focus on functional–behavioural–cultural propositions. The persistence in handaxe shape and technology is, according to Mithen (Reference Mithen1996), the result of technological information transmission, mainly via imitation mechanisms. Given the complexities of handaxe manufacture, a more advanced learning mechanism known as ‘guided learning’ may have also been involved. Shipton and Neilsen (Reference Shipton and Nielsen2015) argue that the ‘unparalleled homogeneity of the Acheulean’ is due to over-imitation and shared intentionality. Recent studies have identified the presence of inexperienced knappers in specific ‘knowledge transmission grounds’ at Palaeolithic sites. These learning grounds may reflect the presence of knappers in the process of learning as well as learning transmission mechanisms at such sites (Assaf et al. Reference Assaf, Barkai and Gopher2015 and references within; Tehrani & Riede Reference Tehrani and Riede2008).

Lycett and Gowlett (Reference Lycett and Gowlett2008) contend that four models of cultural transmission can be taken into account: parent to offspring; teacher/leader to a number of individuals; numerous individuals to one individual; and peer learning. Each of these supports a different rate of innovation, dissemination, and consolidation. They further suggest that the low rate of innovation during the Lower Palaeolithic seems to be related to the parent-to-offspring model, although this method predicts inter-group variance that is not clearly observed in lithic findings of the period. A model of knowledge transmission from many individuals to one predicts a relatively low rate of innovation and greater variation between groups. Lycett and Gowlett (Reference Lycett and Gowlett2008) further suggest this model as suited to the Acheulean evidence. Stout (Reference Stout2011) argues that another factor in the innovation rate equation is that of the rise of technological complexity. As technological complexity increases, it creates the likelihood for more combinations to arise based on the existing variance, some of which will become the next innovations. He contends that sufficient complexity can be detected already in the Lower Palaeolithic, supporting the innovation of ‘pre-Levallois’ cores and knapping with a soft hammer to produce refined handaxes at 0.5 Mya (Stout Reference Stout2011). It should be noted that yet another mechanism for conveying information in social learning is that of conservatism transmission, which researchers of the Acheulean tend to ignore as a possible explanation for the handaxe paradox. Recent studies from the field of primatology also emphasise the explanatory potential of an approach focused on conservatism and functional fixedness (Gruber Reference Gruber2016; Wrangham et al. Reference Wrangham, Koops, Machanda, Worthington, Bernard, Brazeau and Muller2016). In the following we discuss the ‘conservatism transmission’ concept in greater detail.

Several approaches have been presented for reconstructing learning mechanisms in the Lower Palaeolithic. Their common theme is that relative technological stagnation is due to the low rate of innovation for reasons associated with learning ability and group size. None of the approaches suggests the possibility that the persistence could be due to a conscious and adaptive preference to produce the same tool type for generations.

Another view of preferred conservatism comes from studies of human life history. Nowell and White (Reference Nowell and White2010) argue that significant changes in human life history took place during the Middle Pleistocene. The rising place of meat and fat in the human diet is most likely related to the increased dependence on hunting, the adoption and use of fire, the reduction in gut size, the 20–60% expansion of brain size, and the increase in body size. All of these transformations are interrelated, though the sequence of cause and effect is far from being understood. The increased brain size led to the birth of helpless infants (relative to other mammals), which in turn led to extended infancy and longer puberty, as well as extended life expectancy long after menopause. To explain the relative technological ‘stagnation’, Nowell and White (Reference Nowell and White2010) distinguish between innovation – such as the adoption of the handaxe that marks the transition to the Acheulean – and inventiveness – a lesser degree of a novelty, such as variations in handaxe manufacture. How can we explain these processes according to the changes in human life history? Human life history is divided into five phases: infancy, childhood, juvenile, adolescence, and adulthood. Of these, childhood and adolescence are unique to the human species. Childhood has two important advantages: first, it allows the mother to stop breastfeeding and reduces the interval between births; and second – and of greater importance in our case – childhood allows 4 years of learning social behaviour. Adolescence provides the additional years of development that are essential for technological learning, experimentation, social integration, language acquisition, and other cultural transformations. The researchers suggest that Homo erectus childhood was shorter than that of Modern humans, which would have resulted in an imprinting of the handaxe ‘idea’ during the child’s early learning process from his ‘ultra conservative’ parents (Nowell & White Reference Nowell and White2010, 76). Nowell and White, however, do not indicate why the parents were so conservative. Instead, they ask, is it possible that a short childhood in a relatively small group, with a limited number of associates with whom one could learn and experiment, could have harmed the option to create real innovation?

Somel et al. (Reference Somel, Liu and Khaitobich2013) support the importance of long childhood. Referring to data on the rate of production of proteins in the brain due to different genes, they contend that the process of creating synapses (synaptogenesis) in the frontal cortex, the quantity of which has a major effect on the brain’s plasticity, takes place primarily between the ages of 3½ and 10. This period is the ‘window of opportunity’ in which many human cognitive abilities are developed. In chimpanzees and macaque monkeys this period is limited to a few months only. From the suggested Early Palaeolithic short childhood, the effect we can deduce regarding human cognitive ability is that of its possible bearing on a limited scope of innovation (Somel et al. Reference Somel, Liu and Khaitobich2013, 124). The biological perspective sheds light on the relative ‘stagnation’ in handaxe technology. On the attempt to ascribe the slow change in the form of tools to conformity caused by the early stage of cortical organisation, Bar-Yosef (Reference Bar Yosef2006, 483) writes that the explanation based on ‘predetermined solutions embedded in the technical “mind” of [the] individual who conformed to the social context of society’ is more applicable than the one based on brain development.

Demographic studies indicate that a clear link can be identified between the dynamics of culture – the stability of traditions and the development of variance within them – and demographic processes such as population size, composition, and distribution (Steele & Shennan Reference Steele and Shennan2009), although others, such as Vaesen et al. (Reference Vaesen, Collard, Cosgrove and Roebroeks2016) argue that population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity. Richerson et al. (Reference Richerson, Boyd and Bettinger2009) maintain that if we assume that the development of practical novelties is a rare event, then the rate of innovations should be relatively limited in small populations. In such populations, practical innovations will disappear randomly due to the problem of information transmission. In relatively large populations there are more potential inventors, and the chance of incidental loss of innovation is relatively small. Premo and Kuhn (Reference Premo and Kuhn2010) assert that the stability of Lower Palaeolithic technology is not related to human limited cognitive ability, as other scholars claim, but is derived, rather, from the high extinction rate of small social groups that characterised this period. Nevertheless, the question remains: Why did extinctions limit transformations only in lithic technology and not in the other described technologies and life-history characteristics?

In summary, we have reviewed a variety of explanations, some mutually supportive, others mutually contradictory, for the handaxe ‘stagnation’ phenomenon. The basic problem with this variety of explanations is that they present theories that are considered to be valid in principle in other fields in which transformation and innovation occurred, such as the emergence of the Levallois method or the use of fire. Why, then, should the handaxe phenomenon be considered to differ from other fields of human behaviour? What was so unique to it or its use that led to its persistence for more than a million years?

PREFERRED CONSERVATISM AS A MAJOR DRIVE TOWARDS TECHNOLOGICAL PERSISTENCE

Here we engage with conservatism on two levels: the practical function of the handaxe; and the cognitive-social mechanism that entrenched the production and use of handaxes as a prolonged tradition. The term conservatism does not imply that humans resisted change; rather, it suggests that in this specific realm change was not perceived as an essential adaptive element. Conservatism, both social and biological, proves itself when the environment is reasonably stable and change is unnecessary. Under these circumstances the drive for innovation is relatively low and the modes of action that have proven themselves have an advantage over those that have not yet stood the test of time. Conservatism does not have to be expressed in all aspects of life. Many examples of conservatism concurrent with innovation are known (Lucas Reference Lucas2005, 83), and this may well be the case here. Such a state of affairs may be due to several reasons:

  • Technological development in one field and not in another. For example, information technology compared to the relative ‘stagnation’ of internal combustion engine technology.

  • The drive for adaptation in a particular field compared to relatively constant conditions in other fields. For example, the development of sophisticated weapons compared to the relative stagnation of rifle and hand grenade technologies.

  • Fields in which conservatism is based on deep foundations compared to those in which it is not. For example, the conservatism of social or religious norms and codes compared to the innovative use of technology. This phenomenon is particularly prevalent in traditional and religious societies.

The Acheulean handaxe might fall under one of these categories. However, the specific question remains as to what were those areas of life in which the handaxe had an advantage? The solution may lie in one or a combination of two alternatives:

  • The handaxe itself was functionally important, so its production and use gave a direct evolutionary/adaptive advantage over a very long period of time.

  • For various reasons the manufacture and use of handaxes became a social norm (probably related to the first alternative), and the tendency to imitate the majority and/or to punish deviators preserved the norm for millennia.

Lycett (Reference Lycett2015) categorises selective biases that may act on artefact variation as ‘content biases’ (functional bias and aesthetic bias), ‘contextual biases’ (model- based biases based on prestige, success, similarity, etc), and frequency-based biases (conformity, saturation, and rarity). Here we suggest that conservatism was adopted and preferred due to a combination of biases that slowly shifted from functional (and aesthetic) preferences to conformity, as will be elaborated upon below.

THE FUNCTIONAL BASIS FOR CULTURAL CONSERVATISM – THE ROLE OF THE HANDAXE IN MEGAFAUNA PROCESSING

Most scholars would agree, we believe, that the available functional, technological, and experimental evidence suggests that the primary use of Lower Palaeolithic handaxes, as well as Middle Palaeolithic bifaces, lay in processing animal carcasses (eg, Claud et al. Reference Claud, Brenet, Maury and Mourre2009; Claud Reference Claud2008; Reference Claud2012; Jones Reference Jones1980; Keeley Reference Keeley1980,160–70; Machin et al. Reference Machin, Robert, Hosfield and Mithen2007; Mitchell Reference Mitchell1996; Solodenko et al. Reference Solodenko, Zupancich, Nunziante Cesaro, Marder, Lemorini, Barkai and Petraglia2015). It is true that in some cases handaxes were used in other tasks than in solely assisting the extraction of calories from different game taxa (eg, Domínguez-Rodrigo et al. Reference Domínguez-Rodrigo, Serrallonga, Juan-Tresserras, Alcala and Luque2001) and, thus, some see the handaxe as a multipurpose tool. However, the bulk of the available data indicates not only the repeated archaeological association of handaxes and processed animal parts, but also the efficiency and suitability of handaxes in skinning, cutting, defleshing, and dismembering carcasses, and in particular carcasses of large game taxa (eg, Jones Reference Jones1980;Reference Jones1981;Reference Jones1994; Key & Lycett Reference Key and Lycett2015; Reference Key and Lycett2016). It is also true that while handaxes are present at many Lower Palaeolithic sites associated with elephant and other large taxa remains, some multi- as well as single elephant carcass sites lack handaxes altogether (see Slodenko et al. Reference Solodenko, Zupancich, Nunziante Cesaro, Marder, Lemorini, Barkai and Petraglia2015 for details).

We do not discuss here a possible explanation for handaxes not having been found at those sites, and we do not claim that the only possible way of processing a large animal carcass was by using a handaxe. We do, however, offer an hypothesis regarding the repeated association of handaxes and very large game at Lower Palaeolithic sites in the Old World, coupled with the dependency of Palaeolithic humans on animal meat and fat (eg, Ben-Dor et al. Reference Ben-Dor, Gopher, Hershkovitz and Barkai2011; Reference Ben-Dor, Gopher and Barkai2016; Domínguez-Rodrigo & Pickering Reference Domínguez-Rodrigo and Pickering2017; Zink & Lieberman Reference Zink and Lieberman2016), and the intriguing production of handaxes made from elephant limb bones (Zutuvski & Barkai Reference Zutovski and Barkai2016 for details). While it is generally accepted that meat fueled the physiological developments in Homo erectus populations (eg, Aiello & Wheeler Reference Aiello and Wheeler1995; Domínguez-Rodrigo et al. Reference Domínguez-Rodrigo, Bunn, Mabulla, Baquedano, Uribelarrea, Pérez-González, Gidna, Yravedra, Diez-Martin, Egeland, Barba, Arriaza, Organista and Ansón2014; Pontzer et al. Reference Pontzer, Brown, Raichlen, Dunsworth, Hare, Walker and Luke2016), we suggest that greater emphasis should be placed on the role of fat and the significance of animals capable of supplying large quantities of fat year round (Ben-Dor et al. Reference Ben-Dor, Gopher, Hershkovitz and Barkai2011; Reference Ben-Dor, Gopher and Barkai2016). Elephants and mammoths are such animals, and thus we argue that the dependency of early humans on the caloric contribution of fat and meat, in addition to plant-based calories (eg, Melamed et al. Reference Melamed, Kislev, Geffen, Lev-Yadun and Goren-Inbar2016), led to the preference for procuring calories from elephants and mammoths, when available. Other large prey (rhinoceros, hippopotamus, wild cattle, and wild horse) are also important contributors of fat and meat, and these taxa were of course regularly exploited by early humans as well. However, no animal matched the extraordinary package of fat and meat as that to be found in the elephant and mammoth. Smaller taxa such as deer and gazelles, among the ungulates, as well as tortoises, rabbits, and birds, were also a source of calories whenever possible, but again none of these matches the ideal food package represented by the proboscidea.

Skinning, cutting, defleshing, and dismembering elephants and mammoths is a tedious and demanding task (eg, Gingerich & Stanford Reference Gingerich and Stanford2016). The presence of proboscidea remains bearing cut marks at Palaeolithic sites (see Slodenko et al. Reference Solodenko, Zupancich, Nunziante Cesaro, Marder, Lemorini, Barkai and Petraglia2015 for details) as well as the butchered elephant skull from the site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, associated with many handaxes (Goren-Inbar et al. Reference Goren-Inbar, Lister, Werker and Chech1994), supports our contention regarding the link between Lower Palaeolithic humans, elephants, and handaxes. The same holds true for a biface bearing fat residue from the Acheulean site of Revadim, Israel (Slodenko et al. Reference Solodenko, Zupancich, Nunziante Cesaro, Marder, Lemorini, Barkai and Petraglia2015). The presence of butchered elephant/mammoth remains at many Palaeolithic sites worldwide (eg, Agam and Barkai Reference Agam and Barkai2016; Blasco et al. Reference Blasco, Jordi, Fernández-Peris, Arsuaga, Bermúdez de Castro and Carbonell2013; Germonpré et al. Reference Germonpré, Sablin, Khlopachev and Grigorieva2008; Iakovleva et al. Reference Iakovleva, Djindjian, Maschenko, Konik and Moigne2012; Kufel-Diakowska et al. Reference Kufel-Diakowska, Wilczyński, Wojtal and Sobczyk2016; Rabinovich et al. Reference Rabinovich, Ackermann, Aladjem, Barkai, Biton, Milevski, Solodenko and Marder2012; Smith Reference Smith2015) suggests that elephants played a major role in the early human diet and adaptation (but see Lupo & Schmitt Reference Lupo and Schmitt2016 and Smith Reference Smith2015). Direct evidence of proboscidean consumption is also provided by isotopic studies, indicating a significant dependence upon mammoths by early humans in Europe (e.g. Bocherens Reference Bocherens2011; Bocherens et al. Reference Bocherens, Drucker, Germonpre, Lázničková-Galetová, Naito, Wissing, Brůžek and Oliva2015; Naito et al. Reference Naito, Chikaraishi, Drucker, Ohkouchi, Semal, Wißing and Bocherens2016).

The importance of proboscideans in the Palaeolithic diet is further stressed through cases in which selected elephant body parts were carried into Palaeolithic caves (eg, Blasco et al. Reference Blasco, Jordi, Fernández-Peris, Arsuaga, Bermúdez de Castro and Carbonell2013; Germonpré et al. Reference Germonpré, Udrescu and Fiers2014; Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Stiner, Dennell, Wang, Zhang and Gao2010), implying their high nutritional value (especially regarding elephant heads, see Agam and Barkai Reference Agam and Barkai2016), along with the social and symbolic roles that certainly could have accompanied such an activity. We do not delve here into the question of whether proboscideans were procured by hunting, scavenging, or both, but suggest in brief that, in our opinion (Agam and Barkai submitted), relevant archaeological, ethnographic, and ethno-historic evidence points in favor of hunting as a relevant elephant/mammoth procurement strategy. The scope of this article does not allow us also to engage with the ways in which Palaeolithic groups dealt with this enormous ‘food package’, but we hope to be able to develop this line of inquiry elsewhere.

We suggest that handaxes were efficient and effective tools in processing large carcasses, enabling the removal of large quantities of fat and meat and the separation of body parts in order to manipulate and transport them. The handaxe allows the application of considerable force and leverage during cutting and dismembering, and its continuous and mostly curved and sharp working edge is ideal for massive and intensive meat and fat processing tasks (eg, Key & Lycett Reference Key and Lycett2015; Reference Key and Lycett2016) (Fig. 2). Moreover, handaxes could be re-honed in order to prolong the use of the tool for continuous operations, such as the processing of very large game (eg, Claud Reference Claud2012). We thus see the handaxe as the primary tool that assisted butchery during Lower Palaeolithic times, and in particular the processing of large game such as the elephant. The intriguing production of handaxes made of elephant bones might serve as another clue, emphasising the possible role of these components within the cosmology of the groups that used stone handaxes in their preparation for the consumption of elephants (see Zutuvski & Barkai Reference Zutovski and Barkai2016). Last but not least is the chronological and geographical connection between elephants/mammoths and handaxes. We suggest that these two go hand in hand, and that when megafauna no longer comprised part of the diet, handaxes/bifaces ceased to be produced and used. It is our contention that the central role of handaxes and large game in Lower Palaeolithic adaptation is of pivotal significance, and that it is the role of handaxes in processing these large ‘food packages’ that ensured their long service during Lower Palaeolithic times.

Fig. 2 An experiment in using flint handaxes in butchering operations (courtesy of Ruth Blasco and Jordi Rosell)

Within the framework of our hypothesis, it is interesting to recall Bar-Yosef’s suggestion that, considering the variety of difficulties encountered by humans as they spread from Africa to Asia and Europe, it is not surprising that the handaxe – as a useful and effective tool specifically employed for cutting raw meat – persisted over such a long period: ‘It is against this background that we should view the conservation of knapping techniques and the retention of tool morphotypes like the bifaces that served, like Swiss Army Knives, as multi-purpose tools, and were undoubtedly essential in chopping meat into small pieces consumed (and digested) raw’ (Bar Yosef Reference Bar Yosef2006, 490).

HANDAXE VARIABILITY: ‘INSIDE THE BOX’ MODIFICATIONS

‘Variability’ and ‘variation’ are the major terms used by those who subscribe to the view of a dynamic and variable process over that of ‘stagnation’ in handaxe persistence during Palaeolithic times. A recent publication of several handaxe assemblages from a Lower Palaeolithic site in Syria offers a good example of the scale of variability in handaxe production during different Acheulean human occupations (Jagher Reference Jagher2016). Consistent with our proposal of functional-based conservatism, we refer to the recent understanding that function is not substantially affected by morphological variation.

McNabb and Cole (Reference McNabb and Cole2015) argue that handaxe variability reflects situational/local symmetry and refinement and that no gradual slope of advancement can be seen in Acheulean material culture. They propose the term ‘variable equilibrium’ to describe the phenomenon of variability within long periods of apparent stasis.

Key and Lycett (Reference Key and Lycett2016) conducted a large-scale experiment in handaxe cutting efficiency (n=500 handaxes) and stated that variability in size and shape does not necessarily have a strong impact on the cutting effectiveness.

In a recent experiment focusing on the role of copying variance in handaxe form, Schillinger et al. (Reference Schillinger, Mesoudi and Lycett2016) argue that what seems to be cultural-based artefact variability may not result from an ‘intent’ or ‘mental template’ but from behavioural differences among individual artisans that over time developed into ‘visibly distinct traditions’.

In another recent study on the role of micro-evolutionary processes in handaxe variation, while taking into account factors of raw material, copying errors, and their relationship to mechanisms of social learning, Lycett et al. (Reference Lycett, Schillinger, Eren, von Cramon-Taubadel and Mesoudi2015) contend that handaxe variation was probably both generated and constrained by those opposing factors. We concur with these ideas, with the addition of the social factors.

FUNCTION LEADS THE WAY TO A NEW SOCIAL NORM

In this section we review a list of possible reasons that might have led to the transformation of the original functional role of the handaxe (including its variation) into a dual role, combining the practical with the social.

Sharon (Reference Sharon2008) studied the impact of raw material on Acheulean large flake production and suggested that the phenomenon that we define as ‘stasis’ – a similar form regardless of the original shape, size, and type of raw material in fact reflects inventiveness and resourcefulness. The conclusion to be drawn from these data is that raw material availability, type, shape, and technological constraints were not the primary factors dictating the characteristics of handaxes. This leads to the possibility that the main consideration guiding the Acheulean knapper was a functional and/or cultural preference. Experimental knapping also supports the notion that early humans had a preconception of the desired end product of the reduction sequence (Eren et al. Reference Eren, Roos, Story, von Cramon-Taubadel and Lycett2014).

Another functional realm of the handaxe was suggested by Kohn and Mithen (Reference Kohn and Mithen1999), who contended that handaxes might have played a role in Lower Palaeolithic mating systems by demonstrating the overall cognitive, physical, and mental competency of males in order to be selected by females for gene exchange. Such a mechanism would have led to preferred cultural conservatism. We may be dealing here with the conservatism of a social code that developed in human society through many generations of mate selection based on handaxe production and use (following the Handicap Principle), and eventually manifested in the framework of technological conservatism. It is of interest to note that Lower Palaeolithic quarries and workshops in Isampur, India: ‘… presents tentative evidence for shared intentions and cooperation in biface manufacture’ (Shipton Reference Shipton2013, 63 and references within). Ethnographic studies too reveal that large-scale quarrying activity might be characterised by cooperation (eg, Burton Reference Burton1984; Hampton Reference Hampton1999, 226–32, 256–62).

We suggest that handaxe production might have been a form of preferred social activity, replaced by later stone tool making methods and technologies (Levallois, blades, etc), storytelling around the campfire, rituals, and other mechanisms. The mechanism of handaxe production might explain its enduring persistence. Hunting big game, aside from its major contribution to diet and survival, is also viewed as a preferred male social activity (eg, Speth Reference Speth2010 and references therein). Handaxe production may be another example of male ‘display’ and technological ‘gadgetry’ based on the functional necessity of this item in the extraction of calories from big game (see Hawkes et al. Reference Hawkes, O’Connell and Blurton Jones2014 and references within).

Machin touches upon the basic human cognitive tendency for aesthetics. Aesthetic objects generate emotional and intellectual pleasure (Machin Reference Machin2009, 48), and symmetrical handaxes may have had a combined aesthetic and functional advantage (Machin et al. Reference Machin, Robert, Hosfield and Mithen2007). Gowlett (Reference Gowlett2011) argues that a constant proportion in Acheulean handaxes, in many cases the 0.61/1 ratio or the ‘golden ratio’ (the proportion preferred in the natural world, including an aesthetic measure by humans), testifies to the deep roots of this human trait. He also suggests that the difficult to achieve elongation that characterises 5–10% of the handaxes from 1.75 Mya, is the outcome of both functionality and symbolic value (Gowlett Reference Gowlett2013). This argument can be added to the preferred mate selection and preferred social activity hypotheses, with particular shape/size perhaps constituting personal and/or group markers.

In some specific and interesting cases ‘imitations’ of the typical stone handaxes were made from elephant limb bones (see Zutovski & Barkai Reference Zutovski and Barkai2016 for an updated account). This remarkable Acheulean trait combined two of the most fundamental elements found at many Lower Palaeolithic sites: namely, large game and bifaces – and may express a cosmological connection between Acheulean subsistence and lifeways, closing an ontological circle by producing bone handaxes from the remains of elephants that might have been processed using stone handaxes.

Assuming that the handaxe had one or more of the above-mentioned functions, in addition to its practical role in processing animal carcasses, we need to explore the learning/knowledge transmission mechanism that cemented its central role in human adaptation during the Lower Palaeolithic.

Studies in cognitive psychology on the type of social learning called ‘conservatism transmission’ – an adaptive mechanism aimed at acquiring knowledge by observing the behaviour of others and the mental state that underlies their behaviour – offer a possible explanation. Henrich and Boyd (Reference Henrich and Boyd1998) suggested that the psychological bias to ‘acquire’ behaviour, which is common to most members of a group (‘following the herd’), has an advantage over social solitary learning in a wide range of situations. One possible consequence of this learning/transmission mechanism is the creation of long-term boundaries between groups. This type of learning/transmission mechanism can explain the persistence of certain human behaviours in a particular group, including the production and use of a specific technology. On the other hand, the possible creation of boundaries between groups might impede the wider inter-group transmission of knowledge. In the case of the handaxe, the combination of long-term persistence and wide geographic distribution suggests that this mechanism does not provide an appropriate explanation. However, one cannot rule out the possibility that the emphasis in research on exclusively identifying the Acheulean culture with handaxes has led to less attention being paid to Lower Palaeolithic sites devoid of handaxes (eg, Bizat Ruhama in Israel, Grand Dolina in Atapuerca, and the Clactonian in Britain, see Zaidner et al. Reference Zaidner, Yeshurun and Mallol2010; García-Medrano et al. Reference García-Medrano, Ollé, Mosquera, Cáceres and Carbonell2015 and Mithen Reference Mithen1996, respectively).Thus future research may prove that the pattern of greater Acheulean variability or ‘sub-cultures’ actually fits the model presented above.

Tehrani and Riede (Reference Tehrani and Riede2008), who claim that stone knapping was too complex to be transferred through imitation, emphasise the role played by active teaching. Based on ethnographically documented traditions, including weaving, stone-knapping, and foraging, they state that teaching has been an important mechanism in material culture transmission since at least the Lower Palaeolithic. They also argue that, through teaching, highly complex forms can be expected to survive for very long periods since both the donors and the recipients actively collaborate in knowledge transfer. We strongly believe that such a scenario is applicable for the Acheulean handaxes, as most scholars agree its production could not be transmitted by imitation only.

The following human traits enhance conformity and conservatism and should be taken into account in this discussion:

  1. 1. Strong reciprocity: ‘A strong reciprocator is predisposed to cooperate with others and punish non-cooperators, even when this behavior cannot be justified in terms of self-interest, extended kinship, or reciprocal altruism’ (Gintis Reference Gintis2000);

  2. 2. Homophily: The tendency for individuals to associate and bond with others like themselves (Flynn et al. Reference Flynn, Reagans and Guillory2010);

  3. 3. The need for closure (NFC), which reflects an aversion to ambiguity and uncertainty as well as a preference for firm, definitive answers to questions (Kruglanski & Webster Reference Kruglanski and Webster1996). Persons with high NFC are more motivated to reach agreement with their peers and are more disturbed by violations of social norms (Flynn et al. Reference Flynn, Reagans and Guillory2010);

  4. 4. The chameleon effect: the unconscious mimicry of the postures, mannerisms, facial expressions, and other behaviours of one’s interaction partners, such that one’s behaviour passively and unintentionally transforms to match that of others in one’s social environment. The chameleon effect might have played a role in human evolution by allowing individuals to maintain harmonious relationships with fellow group members (Chartrand & Bargh Reference Chartrand and Bargh1999; see also Lakin et al. Reference Lakin, Jefferis, Cheng and Chartrand2003).

An examination of the conservativeness/flexibility of 47 cultural traits in 277 African societies revealed that the traits affecting family structure and kinship had demonstrated a high degree of conservation over generations. These traits, transmitted by family members, indicate that cultural transmission in the family is the most conservative mechanism (Guglielmino et al. Reference Guglielmino, Viganotti, Hewlett and Cavalli-Sforza1995). The dominance of vertical transmission was also observed in North American Native Peoples, where learning from group members was a more important process than learning from members of other groups (Mathew & Perreault Reference Mathew and Perreault2015). Traits that require the expertise of a specialised mode of subsistence were also found to be vertically transmitted in Austronesian cultures (Mace & Jordan Reference Mace and Jordan2011). In sum, these data reflect the substantial support for preferred cultural conservatism.

A direct correlation exists between the bias towards conservatism and evolving ‘traditions’ in a variety of animals, such as birds, rodents, and Japanese Macaque monkeys (for a review see Jablonka & Lamb Reference Jablonka and Lamb2005, 155–91), and thus the transmission of human culture is much more elaborated than those of chimpanzees. As for human material culture, Eerkens and Lipo (Reference Eerkens and Lipo2007) argue that employing the conservatism transmission approach explains many phenomena in the archeological record. Claidière and Whiten (Reference Claidiere and Whiten2012) link sociological research to the study of animal behaviour. They divided the issue of conformity into two sub-issues: information/knowledge conformity (for acquiring non-social information in order to gain an adaptive edge in new environments); and normative conformity (for acquiring social information in order to facilitate cooperative relationships with other members of the group). Conformity and punishment evolved concomitantly in large groups and enabled improved inter-group cooperation because conformity reduces variance, which is the basis of disagreement, and at the same time it supports punishment. Punishment, in turn, protects the group from deviations and gives conformists an advantage over ‘independent’ imitators (Boyd & Richardson Reference Boyd and Richardson1992; Guzman et al. Reference Guzman, Rodriguez-Sickert and Rowthorn2007). It should be noted that a willingness of societies to risk costly punishment has been observed around the globe (Henrich et al. Reference Henrich, McElreath, Ensminger, Barr, Barrett, Bolyanatz, Cardenas, Gurven, Gwako, Henrich, Lesorogol, Marlowe, Tracer and Ziker2006). It is generally accepted that norms provide an evolutionary advantage to individuals (Kameda et al. Reference Kameda, Takezawa and Hastie2005). A study of norm enforcement in Ju/‘hoansi hunter-gatherers revealed that norms are obeyed, and the imposition of penalties on violators occurs even when such costly behaviour cannot be explained by immediate preferences. Wiessner (Reference Wiessner2005) suggests that one of the motives for this is the need to maintain ‘industrial peace’ in the social group. Punishment of norm violators is a mechanism for maintaining social cohesion and is central in hunter-gatherer societies. Regarding Acheulean handaxe persistence, we suggest that the bias to imitate majority behaviour that then evolved into social norms may provide a plausible explanation.

THE HANDAXE & THE CHOPSTICK – A TEST-CASE FOR PREFERRED CULTURAL CONSERVATISM BASED ON FUNCTION, LEADING TO THE CREATION OF A SOCIAL NORM

In his book Chopsticks: A cultural and culinary history, Wang (Reference Wang2015) describes in depth a phenomenon not so very different to what we have just described concerning the handaxe. Forty-two bone sticks found in Neolithic Longqiuzhuang ruins in present-day Gaoyou, Jiangssu, dated to 6600–5500 BCE, are believed by some scholars to be the earliest recorded chopsticks, although others suggest that they were used as hairpins (Wang Reference Wang2015, 16–17). It was only in the Bronze Age, or during the Shang dynasty (c. 1600–1046 BCE), that the Chinese began using chopsticks to prepare and eat food. Bronze pieces found at two sites in southern China (Anhui) and south-west China (Yunnan) may have been originally employed in cooking – stirring and mixing, and only later on in feeding. The earliest ivory chopsticks were found in Changyang, Hubei, dated to the Zhou period (1045–256 BCE). The spoon was the primary eating implement in ancient China, but due to the broad appeal and increasing variety of floured-wheat foods, it lost its primacy across China to the chopsticks. Later on, the spreading influence of the Tang culture in Asia extended the use of chopsticks to the north (Mongolian pastureland), north-east, and east (Korean Peninsula and the Japanese islands) and south (the Indochina Peninsula). ‘A chopsticks cultural sphere thus began to take shape, albeit with discernible variation in time and space’ (Wang Reference Wang2015, 66). Was the ‘Handaxe cultural sphere’ the first of many later similar phenomena?

Wang goes on to describe how chopsticks were (and still are) used as popular wedding gifts and in wedding ceremonies, and in celebrating happy occasions like birthdays and sad ones like funerals. The Japanese call the utensil ‘the sticks for one’s life’, for chopsticks are, for the Japanese as well as for other Asians, a symbol of life. As part of acquiring cultural meaning, chopsticks, usually made of bamboo or cheap wood, are sometimes also made of rare and expensive materials (depending on geography) such as gold, silver, copper, rhinoceros horn, deer antler, ivory, ebony, mahogany, and the most precious one – jade (Wang Reference Wang2015, 120–43). Chopstick design also varies in length (18–33 cm in Tang China), the shape of the top (rounded or four-sided), ‘two-ended’ vs ‘one-ended’, and more (Wang Reference Wang2015, 77–84). Altogether, we suggest that the phenomenon described in the case of chopsticks has some parallels with that of the handaxe – a tool that was originally developed to best fit an essential function but which over time gained social and cultural meanings, resulting in a basic shape that persisted over long periods, combined with multiple variations in size, material, and level of finish.

CONCLUSIONS

The unresolved issue of the technological persistence of the Acheulean handaxe has been intensively discussed in the literature for many years, employing a variety of research methodologies. The vast majority of studies on this topic focus on innovation – the reasons for its absence or delay – while ignoring the fact that the suggested mechanisms behind this seeming ‘stagnation’ should have similarly hindered the concomitant transformations that did take place. Here, we explored a different possible explanation for this seeming paradox, and suggest that different trajectories of social, and behavioural transformations characterise Palaeolithic societies for over a million years. In some fields of human behaviour, where advantage was gained, innovations evolved and spread, while in other fields, such as handaxe technology, conservatism was preferred. We suggest that the handaxe played a dual function in the Lower Palaeolithic period. On the one hand, it served as an essential working tool mainly for butchery and dismemberment of large animal carcasses, and occasionally also for other tasks such as woodworking. On the other hand, producing handaxes acquired a social meaning that was based as some kind of norm. Taken together, the handaxe was the flagship of Acheulean adaptation and way of life. In the changing world of Acheulean hunter-gatherers, it became the anchor of successful adaptation, the key to maintaining the Acheulean mode of existence. By focusing on preferred cultural conservatism and following a safe, familiar, path of survival, Lower Palaeolithic hominins stuck to the handaxe for over one million years through a particular style of knowledge transmission and strict behavioural and practical norms. We argue that this handaxe conservatism enabled change and transformation to be initiated, assimilated, and adopted in other trajectories of behaviour and lithic technology. Had the handaxe not maintained equilibrium with the Acheulean pace of change and prevented the mode of adaptation from ‘running too fast’, real stagnation might have occurred. Based on elements borrowed from the fields of conservatism, transmission, and social learning, we argue that the persistence of the Acheulean handaxe, far from being evidence of stagnation, instead reflects a slow and safe method of adaptation that accepted innovations while ensuring group existence. Perceived thus, we suggest that the handaxe became a fixed feature in human society in the Lower Palaeolithic, probably due to the psychological bias for majority imitation that subsequently became a social norm. It is important that modern cognitive biases that ‘sanctify’ innovation do not impede our ability to analyse our predecessors’ considerations that have led to the successful survival of the human race.

References

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Adler, D.S., Wilkinson, K.N., Blockley, S., Mark, D.F., Pinhasi, R., Schmidt-Magee, B.A., Nahapetyan, S. et al. 2014. Early Levallois technology and the Lower to Middle Paleolithic transition in the Southern Caucasus. Science 345(6204), 16091613 Google Scholar
Agam, A. & Barkai, R. 2016. Not the brain alone: the nutritional potential of elephant heads in Paleolithic sites. Quaternary International 406, 218226 Google Scholar
Agam, A., Marder, O. & Barkai, R. 2015. Small flake production and lithic recycling at late Acheulean Revadim, Israel. Quaternary International 361, 4660 Google Scholar
Aiello, L.C. & Wheeler, P. 1995. The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current anthropology 36(2), 199221 Google Scholar
Assaf, E., Barkai, R. & Gopher, A. 2015. Knowledge transmission and apprentice flint-knappers in the Acheulo-Yabrudian: A case study from Qesem Cave, Israel. Quaternary International, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.02.028 Google Scholar
Barkai, R. 2009. Comment on Sharon G Acheulean giant core technology: a worldwide perspective. Current Anthropology 503, 356357 Google Scholar
Bar Yosef, O. 2006. The known and the unknown about the Acheulean. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (eds), Axe Age: Acheulean tool-making from quarry to discard, 479494. London: Equinox Google Scholar
Ben-Dor, M., Gopher, A. & Barkai, R. 2016. Neandertals’ large lower thorax may represent adaptation to high protein diet. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 160(3), 367378 Google Scholar
Ben-Dor, M., Gopher, A., Hershkovitz, I. & Barkai, R. 2011. Man the fat hunter: the demise of Homo erectus and the emergence of a new hominin lineage in the Middle Pleistocene (ca. 400 kyr) Levant. PLoS One 6(12), p. e28689 Google Scholar
Blasco, R., Jordi, R., Fernández-Peris, J., Arsuaga, J.-L., Bermúdez de Castro, J. M. & Carbonell, E. 2013. Environmental availability, behavioural diversity and diet: a zooarchaeological approach from the TD10-1 sublevel of Gran Dolina (Sierra de Atapuerca, Burgos, Spain) and Bolomor Cave (Valencia, Spain). Quaternary Science Review 70, 124144 Google Scholar
Bocherens, H. 2011. Diet and ecology of Neanderthals: implications from C and N Isotopes, insights from bone and tooth biogeochemistry. In N.J. Conard & J. Richter (eds), Neanderthal Lifeways, Subsistence and Technology: One hundred fifty years of Neanderthal study, 7385. Dordrecht: Springer Google Scholar
Bocherens, H., Drucker, D.G., Germonpre, M., Lázničková-Galetová, M., Naito, Y.I., Wissing, C., Brůžek, J. & Oliva, M. 2015. Reconstruction of the Gravettian food-web at Predmostí I using multi-isotopic tracking (13C, 15N, 34S) of bone collagen. Quaternary International 359–60, 211228 Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richardson, P.J. 1992. Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology 13(3), 171195 Google Scholar
Bridgland, D.R. & White, M.J. 2015. Chronological variations in handaxes: patterns detected from fluvial archives in north-west Europe. Journal of Quaternary Science DOI:10.1002/jqs.2805 Google Scholar
Burton, J. 1984. Quarrying in tribal societies. World Archaeology 16(2), 234247 Google Scholar
Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. 1999. The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76(6), 893910 Google Scholar
Claidiere, N. & Whiten, A. 2012. Integrating the study of conformity and culture in humans and nonhuman animals. Psychological Bulletin 1381, 126145 Google Scholar
Claud, E. 2008. Le statut fonctionnel des bifaces au Paleolithique moyen recent dans le Sud-Ouest de la France. Etude traceologique integree des outillages des sites de La Graulet, La Conne de Bergerac, Combe Brune 2, fonseigner et Chez-Pinaud/Jonzac, Unpublished PhD, thesis University of BordeauxGoogle Scholar
Claud, E. 2012. Les bifaces: des outils polyfonctionnels? Etude traceologique integree de bifaces du Paleolithique moyen recent du Sud-ouest de la France. Bulletin de la Societe Prehistorique Francaise 109(3), 413439 Google Scholar
Claud, E., Brenet, M., Maury, S. & Mourre, V. 2009. Etude experimentale des macro-traces d’ utilisatio sur les tranchants des bifaces: caraterisatio et potential diagnostique. Les Nouvelles de l’Archeologie 118, 5560 Google Scholar
Corbey, R., Jagich, A., Vaesen, K. & Collard, M. 2016. The Acheulean handaxe: More like a bird’s song than a Beatles’ tune? Evolutionary Anthropology 25, 619 Google Scholar
Dennell, R. 2009. The Palaeolithic Settlement of Asia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. & Pickering, T.R. 2017. The meat of the matter: an evolutionary perspective on human carnivory. Azania: Archaeological Research in Africa 52(1), 432 Google Scholar
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Serrallonga, J., Juan-Tresserras, J., Alcala, L. & Luque, L. 2001. Woodworking activities by early humans: A plant residue analysis on Acheulian stone tools from Peninj (Tanzania). Journal of Human Evolution 40, 289299 Google Scholar
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Bunn, H.T., Mabulla, A.Z.P., Baquedano, E., Uribelarrea, D., Pérez-González, A., Gidna, A., Yravedra, J., Diez-Martin, F., Egeland, C.P., Barba, R., Arriaza, M.C., Organista, E. & Ansón, M. 2014. On meat eating and human evolution: a taphonomic analysis of BK4b (Upper Bed II, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania), and its bearing on hominin megafaunal consumption. Quaternary International 322, 129152 Google Scholar
Eerkens, J.W. & Lipo, C.P. 2007. Cultural transmission theory and the archaeological record: Providing context to understanding variation and temporal changes in material culture. Journal of Archaeological Research 15, 239274 Google Scholar
Eldredge, N. & Gould, S.J. 1972. Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism. In T.J.M. Schopf (ed.), Models in Paleobiology, 82115. San Francisco: Freeman Cooper Google Scholar
Elias, S. (ed.) 2012. Origins of Human Innovation and Creativity. Developments in Quaternary Science 16. Amsterdam: Elsevier Google Scholar
Eren, M.I., Roos, C.I., Story, B.A., von Cramon-Taubadel, N. & Lycett, S.J. 2014. The role of raw material differences in stone tool shape variation: an experimental assessment. Journal of Archaeological Science 49, 472487 Google Scholar
Flynn, F.J., Reagans, R.E. & Guillory, L. 2010. Do you two know each other? Transitivity, homophily, and the need for (network) closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99(5), 855869 Google Scholar
Gamble, C. 2007. Origins and Revolutions: Human identity in earliest prehistory. New York: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
García-Medrano, P., Ollé, A., Mosquera, M., Cáceres, I. & Carbonell, E. 2015. The nature of technological changes: The Middle Pleistocene stone tool assemblages from Galería and Gran Dolina-subunit TD10. 1 Atapuerca, Spain. Quaternary International doi.org/ 10.1016/ j.quaint.2015.03.006 Google Scholar
Germonpré, M., Udrescu, M. & Fiers, E. 2014. Possible evidence of mammoth hunting at the Neanderthal site of Spy (Belgium). Quaternary International 337, 2842 Google Scholar
Germonpré, M., Sablin, M., Khlopachev, G.A. & Grigorieva, G.V. 2008. Possible evidence of mammoth hunting during the Epigravettian at Yudinovo, Russian Plain. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 27(4), 475492 Google Scholar
Gingerich, J.A.M. & Stanford, D.J. 2016. Lessons from Ginsberg: An analysis of elephant butchery tools. Quaternary International doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2016.03.025 Google Scholar
Gintis, H. 2000. Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of theoretical Biology 206, 169179. doi:10.1006/ jtbi.2000.2111 Google Scholar
Goren-Inbar, N. 2011. Culture and cognition in the Acheulian industry: a case study from Gesher Benot Yaqov. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366(1567), 10381049 Google Scholar
Goren-Inbar, N., Lister, A., Werker, E. & Chech, M. 1994. A butchered elephant skull and associated artifacts from the Acheulian site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Paléorient 20(1), 99112 Google Scholar
Gould, R.A. 1977. Puntutjarpa Rockshelter and the Australian Desert Culture. New York: Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History 54(1)Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J. 2011. The vital sense of proportion: Transformation, golden section, and 1:2 preference in Acheulean bifaces. PaleoAnthropology, 174187 Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J. 2013. Elongation as a factor in artefacts of humans and other animals: an Acheulean example in comparative context. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society B 368(1630), 20130114. doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0114 Google Scholar
Gruber, T. 2016. Great apes do not learn novel tool use easily: Conservatism, functional fixedness, or cultural influence? International Journal of Primatology 37(2), 296316 Google Scholar
Guglielmino, C.R., Viganotti, C., Hewlett, B. & Cavalli-Sforza, L.L. 1995. Cultural variation in Africa: Role of mechanisms of transmission and adaptation. Proccedings of the National Academy of Science USA 92, 75857589 Google Scholar
Guzman, R.A., Rodriguez-Sickert, C. & Rowthorn, R. 2007. When in Rome, do as the Romans do: the coevolution of altruistic punishment, conformist learning, and cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior 282, 112117 Google Scholar
Hampton, O. W. 1999. Culture of Stone: Sacred and Profane Uses of Stone among the Dani. College Station, TX: Texas A&M University Press Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J.F. & Blurton Jones, N.G. 2014. More lessons from the Hadza about men’s work. Human Nature 254, 596619 Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & Boyd, R. 1998. The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior 194, 215241 Google Scholar
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Ensminger, J., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J.C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D. & Ziker, J. 2006. Costly Punishment Across Human Societies. Science 312(5781), 17671770 Google Scholar
Hopkinson, T. & White, M.J. 2005. The Acheulean and the handaxe Structure and agency in the Palaeolithic. In C. Gamble & M, Porr (eds), The Hominid Individual in Context: Archaeological investigations of Lower and Middle Palaeolithic landscapes, locales and artefacts, 1328. London: Routledge Google Scholar
Hopkinson, T., Nowell, A. & White, M.J. 2013. Life histories, metapopulation ecology, and innovation in the Acheulean. PaleoAnthropology 6176. doi:10.4207/PA.2013.ART80 Google Scholar
Hosfield, R. 2008. Stability of flexibility? Handaxes and hominins in the Lower Palaeolithic. In D. Papagianni, R. Layton & H. Maschner (eds), Time and Change: Archaeological perspectives on the long-term in hunter-gatherer societies, 1536. Oxford: Oxbow books Google Scholar
Iakovleva, L., Djindjian, F., Maschenko, E.N., Konik, S. & Moigne, A. M. 2012. The late Upper Paleolithic site of Gontsy (Ukraine): A reference for the reconstruction of the hunter–gatherer system based on a mammoth economy. Quaternary International 255, 8693 Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M.J. 2005. Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Jagher, R. 2016. Nadaouiyeh Aïn Askar, an example of Upper Acheulean variability in the Levant. Quaternary International doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2016.02.006 Google Scholar
Jones, P.R. 1980. Experimental butchery with modern stone tools and its relevance for Palaeolithic archaeology. World Archaeology 12, 153165 Google Scholar
Jones, P.R. 1981. Experimental implement manufacture and use; a case study from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 292(1057), 189195 Google Scholar
Jones, P.R. 1994. Results of experimental work in relation to the stone industries of Olduvai Gorge. In M. Leakey & D. Roe (eds), Olduvai Gorge: Volume 5, Excavations in Beds III, IV and the Masek Beds, 1968–1971. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Kameda, T., Takezawa, M. & Hastie, R. 2005. Where do social norms come from?: the example of communal sharing. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14, 331334 Google Scholar
Keeley, L.H. 1980. Experimental Determination of Stone Tool Uses: A microwear analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Key, A.J.M. & Lycett, S.J. 2015. Edge angle as a variably influential factor in flake cutting efficiency: an experimental investigation of its relationship with tool size and loading. Archaeometry 57, 911927 Google Scholar
Key, A.J.M. & Lycett, S. 2016. Influence of handaxe size and shape on cutting efficiency: a large-scale experiment and morphometric analysis. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory Doi.10.1007/s10816-016-9276-0 Google Scholar
Klein, R. 1999. The Human Career: Human biological and cultural origins. Chicago: University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Kohn, M. & Mithen, S. 1999. Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity 73, 518526 Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A.W. & Webster, D. 1996. Motivated closing of the mind: Seizing and freezing. Psychological Review 103, 263283 Google Scholar
Kufel-Diakowska, B., Wilczyński, J., Wojtal, P. & Sobczyk, K. 2016. Mammoth hunting – impact traces on backed implements from a mammoth bone accumulation at Kraków Spadzista (southern Poland). Journal of Archaeological Science 65, 122133 Google Scholar
Lakin, J.L., Jefferis, V.E., Cheng, C.M. & Chartrand, T.L. 2003. The Chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27(3), 145162 Google Scholar
Lucas, G. 2005. The Archaeology of Time. New York: Routledge Google Scholar
Lupo, K.D. & Schmitt, D.N. 2016. When bigger is not better: the economics of hunting megafauna and its implications for Plio-Pleistocene hunter-gatherers. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 44, 185197 Google Scholar
Lycett, S.J. 2015. Cultural evolutionary approaches to artifact variation over time and space: basis, progress, and prospects. Journal of Archaeological Science 56, 2131 Google Scholar
Lycett, S.J. & Gowlett, J.A.J. 2008. On questions surrounding the Acheulean ‘tradition’. World Archaeology 403, 295315 Google Scholar
Lycett, S.J., Schillinger, K., Eren, M.I., von Cramon-Taubadel, N. & Mesoudi, A. 2015. Factors affecting Acheulean handaxe variation: Experimental insights, microevolutionary processes, and macroevolutionary outcomes. Quaternary International doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.08.021 Google Scholar
Mace, R. & Jordan, F.M. 2011. Macro-evolutionary studies of cultural diversity: a review of empirical studies of cultural transmission and cultural adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366, 402411. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0238 Google Scholar
Machin, A. 2009. The role of the individual agent in Acheulean biface variability – a multi-factorial model. Journal of Social Archaeology 91, 3558 Google Scholar
Machin, A., Robert, J., Hosfield, T. & Mithen, S.J. 2007. Why are some handaxes symmetrical? Testing the influence of handaxe morphology on butchery effectiveness. Journal of Archaeological Science 34(6), 883893 Google Scholar
Mathew, S. & Perreault, C. 2015. Behavioural variation in 172 small-scale societies indicates that social learning is the main mode of human adaptation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 282, 20150061. Doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0061 Google Scholar
Mayr, E. 1992. Speciational evolution or punctuated equilibria. In A. Somit & S.A. Peterson (eds), The Dynamics of Evolution, 2153. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press Google Scholar
Mayr, E. 2001. What Evolution Is? New York: Basic Books Google Scholar
Melamed, Y., Kislev, M.E., Geffen, E., Lev-Yadun, S. & Goren-Inbar, N. 2016. The plant component of an Acheulian diet at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA doi:10.1073/pnas.1607872113 Google Scholar
McNabb, J. & Cole, J. 2015. The mirror cracked: Symmetry and refinement in the Acheulean handaxe. Journal of Archaeological Science Reports 3, 100111 Google Scholar
McNabb, J., Binyon, F. & Hazelwood, L. 2004. The large cutting tools from the South African Acheulean and the question of social traditions. Current Anthropology 455, 653677 Google Scholar
Mitchell, J.C. 1996. Studying biface utilisation at Boxgrove: Roe deer butchery with replica handaxes. Lithics 16, 6469 Google Scholar
Mithen, S. 1996. Social learning and cultural tradition: interpreting Early Palaeolithic technology. In J. Steele & S. Shennan (eds), The Archaeology of Human Ancestry: Power, sex and tradition, 207229. London: Routledge Google Scholar
Mithen, S 1998. Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory. London: Routledge Google Scholar
Moncel, M-H., Arzarellob, M., Boëdac, E., Bonilauri, S., Chevrier, B., Gaillard, C., Forestier, H., Yinghua, L., Sémah, F. & Zeitoun, V. 2016. Assemblages with bifacial tools in Eurasia (third part). Considerations on the bifacial phenomenon throughout Eurasia. Comptes Rendus Palevolsous doi.org/10.1016/j.crpv.2015.11.007 Google Scholar
Naito, Y.I., Chikaraishi, Y., Drucker, D.G., Ohkouchi, N., Semal, P., Wißing, C. & Bocherens, H. 2016. Ecological niche of Neanderthals from Spy Cave revealed by nitrogen isotopes of individual amino acids in collagen. Journal of Human Evolution 93, 8290 Google Scholar
Nowell, A. & White, M. J. 2010. Growing up in the Middle Pleistocene: life history strategies and their relationship to Acheulean industries. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (eds), Stone Tools and the Evolution of Human Cognition, 6782. Boulder, CO: University of Colorado Press Google Scholar
Petraglia, M.D., Shipton, C. & Paddayya, K. 2005. Life and mind in the Acheulean: A case study from India. In C. Gamble & M. Porr (eds), The Hominid Individual in Context: Archaeological investigations of Lower and Middle Palaeolithic landscapes, locales and artefacts, 197219. London: Routledge Google Scholar
Pontzer, H, Brown, M.H., Raichlen, D.A., Dunsworth, H., Hare, B., Walker, K., Luke, A. et al. 2016. Metabolic acceleration and the evolution of human brain size and life history. Nature 533, 390392 Google Scholar
Pope, M., Russel, K. & Watson, K. 2006. Biface form and structured behavior in the Acheulean. Lithics 27, 4757 Google Scholar
Premo, L.S. & Kuhn, S.L. 2010. Modeling effects of local extinctions on culture change and diversity in the Paleolithic. PloS ONE 512, e15582 Google Scholar
Rabinovich, R., Ackermann, O., Aladjem, E., Barkai, R., Biton, R., Milevski, I., Solodenko, N. & Marder, O. 2012. Elephants at the middle Pleistocene Acheulian open-air site of Revadim Quarry, Israel. Quaternary International 276, 183197 Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. & Morley, I. 2009. Becoming Human: Innovation in prehistoric material and spiritual culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Richerson, P.J., Boyd, R. & Bettinger, R.L. 2009. Cultural innovations and demographic change. Human Biology 81(2–3), 211235 Google Scholar
Scarre, C. (ed.) 2009. The Human Past: World prehistory and the development of human societies. London: Thames and Hudson Google Scholar
Schick, K. & Clark, D.J. 2003. Biface technological development and variability in the Acheulean industrial complex in the Middle Awash region of the Afar rift, Ethiopia. In M. Soressi & H.L. Dibble (eds), Multiple Approaches to the Study of Bifacial Technologies, 130. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology Google Scholar
Sharon, G. 2008. The impact of raw material on Acheulian large flake production. Journal of Archaeological Science 35, 13291344 Google Scholar
Sharon, G. 2010. Large flake Acheulean. Quaternary International 223–4, 226233 Google Scholar
Sharon, G., Alperson-Afil, N. & Goren-Inbar, N. 2011. Cultural conservatism and variability in the Acheulean sequence of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov. Journal of Human Evolution 60, 387397 Google Scholar
Schillinger, K., Mesoudi, A. & Lycett, S. J. 2016. Differences in manufacturing traditions and assemblage-level patterns: the origins of cultural differences in archaeological data. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory Doi 10.1007/s10816-016-9280-4 Google Scholar
Shimelmitz, R. 2015. The recycling of flint throughout the Lower and Middle Paleolithic sequence of Tabun Cave, Israel. Quaternary International 361, 3445 Google Scholar
Shipton, C. 2013. A Million Years of Hominin Sociality and Cognition: Acheulean bifaces in the Hunsgi-Baichbal Valley, India. British Archaeological Report S2468. Oxford: ArchaeopressGoogle Scholar
Shipton, C. & Nielsen, M. 2015. Before cumulative culture: The evolutionary origins of overimitation and shared intentionality. Human Nature 26, 331345 Google Scholar
Smith, G.M. 2015. Neanderthal megafaunal exploitation in Western Europe and its dietary implications: a contextual reassessment of La Cotte de St Brelade (Jersey). Journal of Human Evolution 78, 181201 Google Scholar
Solodenko, N., Zupancich, A., Nunziante Cesaro, S., Marder, O., Lemorini, C., Barkai, R. & Petraglia, M.D. 2015. fat residue and use-wear found on Acheulean biface and scraper associated with butchered elephant remains at the site of Revadim, Israel. PLoS ONE 103, e0118572, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0118572 Google Scholar
Somel, M., Liu, X. & Khaitobich, P. 2013. Human brain evolution: transcripts, metabolites and their regulators. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, 112127 Google Scholar
Speth, J.D. 2010. Paleoanthropology and Archaeology of Big-game Hunting. New York: Springer Google Scholar
Steele, J. & Shennan, S. 2009. Introduction demography and cultural macroevolution. Human Biology 81(2–3), 105119 Google Scholar
Stout, D. 2011. Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366, 10501059 Google Scholar
Schwartz, J.H. & Tattersall, I. 2015. Defining the genus Homo . Science 349, 932932 Google Scholar
Tehrani, J.J. & Riede, F. 2008. Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeology 40(3), 316331 Google Scholar
Vaesen, K., Collard, N., Cosgrove, R. & Roebroeks, W. 2016. Population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the USA doi/10.1073/pnas.1520288113 Google Scholar
Wang, Q.E. 2015. Chopsticks: A cultural and culinary history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. 2005. Norm enforcement among the Ju/‘hoansi Bushmen a case of strong reciprocity? Human Nature 16(2), 115145 Google Scholar
Wrangham, R.W., Koops, K., Machanda, Z.P., Worthington, S., Bernard, A.B., Brazeau, N.F. & Muller, M.N. 2016. Distribution of a Chimpanzee social custom is explained by matrilineal relationship rather than conformity. Current Biology 26(22), 30333037 Google Scholar
Zaidner, Y., Yeshurun, R. & Mallol, C. 2010. Early Pleistocene hominins outside of Africa: recent excavations at Bizat Ruhama, Israel. PaleoAnthropology 2010, 162195 Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Stiner, M.C., Dennell, R., Wang, C., Zhang, S. & Gao, X. 2010. Zooarchaeological perspectives on the Chinese Early and Late Palaeolithic from the Ma’anshan site (Guizhou, south China). Journal of Archaeological Science 37(8), 20662077 Google Scholar
Zink, K.D. & Lieberman, D. E. 2016. Impact of meat and Lower Palaeolithic food processing techniques on chewing in humans. Nature 531, 500503 Google Scholar
Zutovski, K. & Barkai, R. 2016. The use of elephant bones for making Acheulean handaxes: A fresh look at old bones. Quaternary International doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.01.033 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig.1 Examples of Acheulean flint bifaces from the Revadim site, Israel

Figure 1

Fig. 2 An experiment in using flint handaxes in butchering operations (courtesy of Ruth Blasco and Jordi Rosell)