Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T13:37:13.095Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does it Matter if the President Isn't Pious? White Evangelicals and Elite Religiosity in the Trump Era

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2022

Jack Thompson*
Affiliation:
University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
*
Corresponding author: Jack Thompson, email: j.thompson4@exeter.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Trump's unwavering support among white evangelicals seems a contradiction considering his seeming irreligiosity and well publicized moral transgressions. This contradiction raises an interesting question concerning whether Trump represents something of a unique case when it comes to white evangelical evaluations of elite religiosity, or whether his support among the group indicative of a new era of evangelical support for candidates that does not assess religiosity as it used to. Drawing on contemporary debates from the religious psychology and Christian nationalism scholarship, I use data from Wave 61 of the American Trends Panel (ATP) to test whether white evangelicals who encounter threats to their religious identities are more likely to view that God played some role in Trump's election. Overall, I find that white evangelicals consider themselves minorities as a result of their beliefs. Most importantly, these beliefs condition white evangelical beliefs about God's role in Trump's election. Specifically, I find that increases in threat perceptions lead to an increase in the probability of a white evangelical believing that Trump's election was part of God's plan. These findings provide a new vantage point for understanding why so many white evangelicals view Trump's election as a divine outcome despite the fact they are indifferent to his irreligiosity.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Religion and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

Ever since Trump's victory in the 2016 election, scholars and commentators alike note his robust and unwavering levels of support among white evangelicals. Eighty-one percent of white evangelicals reported voting for Trump in 2016 (Martinez and Smith Reference Martinez and Smith2016), a number that had declined by just 3 points in the 2020 election (Smith Reference Smith2020).Footnote 1 These trends are noteworthy considering that a group which was so mobilized in opposition to Bill Clinton by the Lewinsky scandal (Green et al. Reference Green, Rozell and Wilcox2000) would exhibit such high levels of support for a presidential candidate who had an alleged extramarital affair with an adult film actress (Feuer Reference Feuer2018), and was also unable to name a single bible passage when asked which was his favorite (Merritt 2016). These developments have spurned debate over why so many white evangelicals are willing to support Trump given this apparent irreligiosity and well-publicized moral transgressions (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018; Wehner Reference Wehner2019; Ayris Reference Ayris2020; Margolis Reference Margolis2020; Whitehead and Perry Reference Whitehead and Perry2020).

This contradiction raises a number of interesting questions. For instance, does Trump represent something of a unique case when it comes to white evangelical evaluations of elite religiosity? Or is his support among the group indicative of a new era of evangelical support for candidates that do not assess religiosity as it used to? In considering these questions, it is useful to consider patterns of white evangelical voter behavior in recent presidential election cycles. The 2012 election provided an initial indication that white evangelicals were willing to support non-traditional Republican candidates. Indeed, while there was a great deal of speculation as to whether white evangelicals would support Mitt Romney on account of his Mormon faith, the group voted as heavily for Romney as they had done for McCain in 2008 and Bush in 2004 (Pew Research Center 2012). When faced with another non-traditional candidate in the 2016 election, white evangelical voters backed Trump at remarkably similar levels they had Romney in 2012. Taken together, the results of the 2012 and 2016 elections indicate that white evangelicals are likely to support the Republican nominee for president, perhaps somewhat irrespective of candidate religiosity.Footnote 2

If white evangelicals no longer view religiosity as a particularly important heuristic when deciding whether to support Republican candidates, what then might explain their support for Trump? Contemporary work at the intersection of religion and political science proposes that negative partisanship—or the tendency of voters to form their political opinions in opposition to parties they dislike—helps to explain white evangelical support for Trump (Margolis Reference Margolis2020). However, such studies underplay the role that perceived threats to religious identities play in shaping candidate support. Past scholarship highlights how perceived threats to religious identity mobilize support for Republican candidates (e.g., Campbell and Monson Reference Campbell and Monson2008). 2016Scholars have expanded on the threat literature by demonstrating how perceived threats to Christian identity not only shape support for Trump but, crucially, also operate independent of the effects of religiosity (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018).

In this paper, I build on these works by exploring why so many Evangelicals are willing to support Trump despite his apparent irreligiosity and well-documented personal transgressions. To do this, I begin by reviewing academic debates about religion as a social identity. I then provide an overview of the scholarship on the impact of religious identity on evaluations of the religiosity of elites, focusing on the effects of evangelical identity on support for Republican candidates who use religious cues to mobilize support. With these streams of literature in mind, I draw attention to how Trump's apparent irreligiosity and moral transgressions give us reason to doubt whether or not Trump's religiosity matters at all to white evangelicals.

Next, I build a theoretical framework for thinking about why so many white evangelicals not only support Trump, but, interestingly, think that God played a role in his election—an interesting and seemingly contradictory finding given his aforenoted irreligiosity and well-documented personal behavior that has yet to be fully explored within the extant scholarship. Drawing on contemporary debates in the Christian nationalism and religious psychology scholarship, I posit that white evangelicals who perceive threats to their religious identity are likely to believe that God played some role in Trump's election in order to arrest the specific decline of Christian America. I hypothesize that it is these religious identity threats that condition white evangelicals' beliefs in whether Trump's election was a divine outcome. If white evangelicals do believe that God played some role in Trump's election to address the perceived marginalization of their beliefs, then this finding may go some way in explaining why some 81% of them support Trump despite his seeming irreligiosity.

Overall, I find that white evangelicals have a high likelihood of considering themselves minorities as a result of their religious beliefs. Moreover, these beliefs condition white evangelicals' views about whether Trump's election was a divine outcome. Specifically, I find that increases in the salience of perceived identity threat led to an increase in the likelihood of white evangelical respondents believing that Trump's election was a part of God's plan. Notably, these increases coincide with a decrease in the likelihood of white evangelical respondents believing that God does not get involved in elections. The findings contribute to the scholarship in two important ways. First, they add to the extant scholarship on the role that perceived threats to religious identity play shaping attitudes toward Republican candidates (Campbell and Monson Reference Campbell and Monson2008). Second, they provide evidence of how threats to religious identity operate independently of the effects of religiosity on white evangelicals' views toward Trump. These findings lend weight to the argument that assessments of candidate religiosity increasingly matter less when it comes to understanding why religious voters are willing to support non-traditional candidates such as Trump (Margolis Reference Margolis2020).

White evangelicals and social identity theory

Scholarship within the field of social psychology outlines how the groups to which people belong provide individuals with a social identity (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979). Individuals who identify strongly with their religious group not only share a set of common beliefs, but also perceive their group memberships as central to their idea of self, thereby gaining a sense of self-esteem from group membership and a robust link to fellow group members (Ysseldyk et al. Reference Ysseldyk, Matheson and Anisman2010). The unique characteristics of religion include compelling affective experiences and a sense of moral authority, which imbue religious social identities with significant meaning for many individuals (Wellman and Tokuno Reference Wellman and Tokuno2004). Another important component to identity development concerns the ingroup–outgroup distinction (Tajfel Reference Tajfel1978; Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979). In the particular case of white evangelicals, much of the outgroup hostility expressed by the group is driven toward racial and religious minorities (Hetherington et al. Reference Hetherington, Weiler and Weiler2009).

Religious identities have a long history of being important explanatory measures in the political science scholarship (Layman Reference Layman1997; Smith Reference Smith2000; Wald and Calhoun-Brown Reference Wald and Calhoun-Brown2014), and their application to the study of political attitudes and behaviors has been no less pervasive in recent years (Williams Reference Williams2012; Margolis Reference Margolis2018). Many of these recent analyses have come in the wake of the 2016 election, which revealed an electorate highly polarized along the lines of religion (Martí Reference Martí2019; Margolis Reference Margolis2020). Consequently, it is unsurprising to find an emergent scholarship pointing to the importance of religious group identities conditioning political behavior in the Trump era.Footnote 3 In addition to these works, scholars play close attention to how religion affects the political behavior of white Americans in particular, finding a consistentFootnote 4 link to white evangelicalism and support for Trump (Martí Reference Martí2019; Margolis Reference Margolis2020).

While religious affiliation can be a social identity, it is important to qualify that I do not provide a direct test of the impact of white evangelical identity on my outcome measures. An objective basis for group membership—that is, coding ATP respondents who reported being “white,” “Protestant,” and “evangelical” as white evangelical as I do here—does not provide a sufficient basis for justifying a social identity, per se, since individuals experience attachment to their social group to varying degrees. Given my inability to account for white evangelicals' varying levels of attachment to their social identities, my argument is that social identity theory simply offers a useful frame for understanding how white evangelicals may be conceptualized as a group with a set of common beliefs about God's role in Trump's election.

Elite religiosity and the impact of religious identity

While many have questioned Trump's relative popularity among white evangelicals given his seeming religiously and well-documented personal transgressions (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018; Wehner Reference Wehner2019; Ayris Reference Ayris2020; Whitehead and Perry Reference Whitehead and Perry2020), we have not yet seen an analysis that directly addresses the question of Trump's religiosity with empirical data. The closest that an empirical analysis that has come to addressing this question is Margolis (Reference Margolis2020). Margolis (Reference Margolis2020) uses support for Trump among highly devout evangelicals as a proximate measure of elite religious evaluations, finding that those with higher levels of religiosity were more reticent to support Trump in the initial stages of the 2016 Republican Presidential primaries. While these findings provide evidence that non-traditional candidates such as Trump were less likely to enjoy the support of devout evangelicals in 2016, they do not offer direct evidence of their specific evaluations of Trump's religiosity.

Trump's apparent irreligiosity and personal transgressions bring into question the relative impact of elite religiosity on elite evaluations and voter behavior. Elite religiosity is defined as an emphasis on the importance of religious descriptive representation among individuals who are highly religious themselves (e.g., Castle et al. Reference Castle, Layman, Campbell and Green2017; Smidt and Miles Reference Smidt and Miles2017). While the descriptive representation scholarship has focused on the impact readily-visible identities such as race (Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2017), it is likely that identities such as religion, which can easily be hidden from public view, function in a similar manner to those which cannot be easily hidden (Haider-Markel Reference Haider-Markel2007). Consistent with this observation, studies find religious identity to have a substantive impact on evaluations of elites (Castle et al. Reference Castle, Layman, Campbell and Green2017; Smidt and Miles Reference Smidt and Miles2017).

To illustrate the impact of white evangelical identity on preferences for elite religiosity, I draw on data from Wave 61 of the American Trends Panel (ATP). Wave 61 of the ATP contains a number of items asking respondents whether it is important to have a president who exhibits a number of qualities. Table 1 displays the percentage of ATP respondents who thought it was important to have a president who (i) shares their religious beliefs, (ii) has strong religious beliefs themselves, and (iii) stands up for their religious beliefs. As evidenced by Table 1, a strong majority of white evangelical respondents report that it is important to have a president who exhibits all three qualities. Further, these figures are consistently higher than respondents with other religious identities, lending weight to the argument that white evangelicals exhibit strong preferences for elite religiosity.

Table 1. Preferences for elite religiosity, by religious identity

Notes: Table entries represent the percentage of respondents who thought that it was either “somewhat” or “very” important to have a president who exhibited each quality.

Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

The American public know notoriously little about elite policy positions or voting records, and instead rely on “informational shortcuts” when making evaluations about elites (Popkin Reference Popkin2020). In this way, elite identity cues help the public evaluate their ideological orientations, likely positions, and personality traits. Identity markers such as religious affiliation and religiosity therefore function as heuristics that allow the public to make evaluations about which elites will best represent them (McDermott Reference McDermott2009). Consistent with this theorizing, scholars find religious cues to have a substantive effect on elite approval, elite evaluations, and voter behavior (Olson and Warber Reference Olson and Warber2008; Calfano and Djupe Reference Calfano and Djupe2009; Weber and Thornton Reference Weber and Thornton2012; Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Layman, Green and Sumaktoyo2018).

Elites who frame themselves as being religious are more likely to attract support from highly religious voters whilst repelling less-devout individuals (McLaughlin and Wise Reference McLaughlin and Wise2014). Republican elites often deploy covert cues to mobilize religious support (Albertson Reference Albertson2015). Covert cues do not directly reference God or their Christian faith (Calfano and Djupe Reference Calfano and Djupe2009), instead relying on specific references to biblical passages that are detected by religious groups such as evangelical Protestants who are typically loyal to the Republican Party (Schwadel Reference Schwadel2017).Footnote 5 A salient and practical example of covert cues includes Ted Cruz directly quoting scripture after victory in the 2016 Iowa Caucuses (Blair Reference Blair2016).

This observation is interesting considering the specific case of Trump. Trump's attempt to use covert cues to mobilize evangelical support is noteworthy because of his apparent lack of awareness of specific bible passages (Merritt 2016). If the attempted use of covert cues by Trump has not proved effective in convincing white evangelicals of his religiosity, then we have reason to suspect that they will not perceive him as being particularly religious.Footnote 6 Another reason to doubt whether white evangelicals perceive Trump as being religious are his well-publicized personal transgressions, including his alleged extramarital affair with the adult-film actress Stormy Daniels. Given the Christian Right's reaction to the Lewinsky Scandal (Green et al. Reference Green, Rozell and Wilcox2000), it is not out of the realm of possibility that some reacted the same way to Trump.Footnote 7

To illustrate that white evangelicals do not evaluate Trump as being particularly religious, I once again draw on Wave 61 of the ATP. Wave 61 of the ATP contains several items that ask respondents how religious they perceive a number of candidates in the 2020 election to be. Table 2 displays the percentage of respondents who thought that each candidate was either “somewhat” or “very” religious. As evidenced by the first column in Table 2, just 37.19% of white evangelicals perceive Trump as being “somewhat” or “very” religious. The remaining columns in Table 2 indicate that white evangelicals also perceive a slew of Republican/Democratic candidates as being either religious or irreligious. For instance, 87.44% of white evangelicals think that Mike Pence is either “somewhat” or “very” religious, while none of the Democratic candidates are perceived as being particularly religious white evangelicals. In sum, the data illustrate that Trump is something of a unique case when it comes to white Evangelicals' evaluations of elite religiosity; they perceive a slew of Democratic elites as irreligious,Footnote 8 whilst concurrently demonstrating that they are able to evaluate non-Trump Republicans (Pence) as being highly religious.

Table 2. Perceptions of elite religiosity, by religious identity

Notes: Table entries represent the percentage of respondents who thought that each presidential candidate was either “somewhat” or “very” religious.

Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Declining influence, marginalized beliefs, and elections as divine outcomes

While religious cleavages were once shaped by religious tradition, social changes beginning in the 50s led to “religious restructuring” whereby religious commitment played an increasingly vital role in structuring individual differences (Wuthnow Reference Wuthnow1988). Resultingly, the religiously committed had a greater propensity to incorporate religion into their social identifies, were exposed to culturally traditional religious doctrine, and became more embedded in social networks that communicated distinctive political values (Castle Reference Castle2019). The sum of these changes was an emergent “culture war” (Hunter Reference Hunter1992) over issues including public education (Zimmerman Reference Zimmerman2009), abortion (Mouw and Sobel Reference Mouw and Sobel2001), and later same sex marriage and transgender rights (Castle Reference Castle2019).

Because culture politics usually involves disagreement about what society should or should not prescribe as the appropriate way of life, any political controversies on conflicts over cultural norms qualify as cultural politics. Since cultural politics are perceived as intrinsically good or evil, issues over culture are often framed as “explosive” or “nonbargainable” (Leege et al. Reference Leege, Wald, Krueger and Mueller2009). In this way, cultural politics deals with what certain subcultural groups (for instance, the religiously affiliated) perceive as being right or wrong for society. Subcultural groups argue it is crucially important that they are able to practice and propound their own values within a pluralist society (Leege et al. Reference Leege, Wald, Krueger and Mueller2009). However, if certain cultural subgroups come to feel that they are not able to do either of these things, individuals with a salient attachment to their group membership may come to feel threatened. The perceived implications of an identity threat may elicit varying degrees of distress depending on the group membership that is targeted (Branscombe et al. Reference Branscombe, Ellemers, Spears, Doosje, N, R and B1999). Threats to some identities, such as religious identities, may be perceived as having greater implications than threats to identities associated with other groups. For instance, a threat to religious identity could be perceived as being paramount because of the “eternal” significance of religious group membership and the highly revered belief system to which it is attached (Kinnvall Reference Kinnvall2004).

It is common for elites to organize voters around cultural issues in U.S. elections (Leege et al. Reference Leege, Wald, Krueger and Mueller2009). Elites present themselves as defending cultural norms, thereby separating themselves from their opponents, who are usually defined as being against such values. Multiple studies indicate that this strategy is likely to prove effective in politically mobilizing culturally aggrieved religious identifiers. Perceptions of threat can motivate white evangelicals to support Republican candidates at higher rates in national elections. Likewise, Campbell and Monson (Reference Campbell and Monson2008) demonstrate how threat perceptions can motivate other theologically conservative religious groups such as Latter Day Saint (LDS) members to become politically active.

As the percentage of Americans who do not identify with an organized religion has increased (Putnam et al. Reference Putnam, Campbell and Garrett2012), White Christians are now a minority in American politics, a trend which has fed into perceptions of declining influence among many white evangelicals (Jones Reference Jones2016). As America becomes an increasingly plural nation along religious lines, elites such as Trump have come to view these changes as an opportunity to activate cultural threat and mobilize support among those who feel as though their beliefs are becoming increasingly marginalized as a result of consequence of these changes. It is with these developments in mind that lead me to state my first hypothesis:

H1: White evangelicals will have a high probability of considering themselves minorities because of their beliefs.

Still, Trump's ability to mobilize white evangelical support seems puzzling when we consider the fact that he appears to know very little about the bible (Merritt 2016) or that some see his personal behavior as unbecoming of a devout Christian. Does Trump's behavior, as well as an apparent lack of religiosity, not matter at all to white evangelicals? This question beget a point made by numerous scholars regarding Trump's relationship with white evangelicals; Trump may not share their religion and lifestyle, but does share—and, more importantly, articulates on their behalf—many of the same grievances (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018; Posner Reference Posner2020). Indeed, and perhaps the most crucial grievance articulated by Trump, is the idea that the aforenoted changes that have occurred since the 50s robbed white evangelicals of the Christian America that many believed was God's intention for America's founding (Gorski Reference Gorski, JL and JC2019; Whitehead and Perry Reference Whitehead and Perry2020).

Even if white evangelicals think that Trump is not particularly religious, there is reason to suspect that they support him on a political level because of the many policies championed by Trump that have sought to further white Christian interests. Crucially, these policy “wins” may overpower concerns related to his lack of religiosity or personal behavior, since they seek to arrest the apparent decline in Christian influence that has led many white evangelicals to feel as though they are now, themselves, minorities. While public beliefs do not crystalize into well-formed policy preferences (Achen and Bartels Reference Achen and Bartels2017) or are simply not attuned to which elites favor which policy, evidence suggests that policy does matter to white evangelicals (Keenan and Pereira Reference Keenan and Pereira2020). This observation is important because Trump has largely delivered in policy areas such as court appointments, which public opinion data indicate matters highly to evangelical voters (Renard Reference Renard2017).Footnote 9

Given Trump's articulation of the Christian nation myth and his relative success in enacting policies to arrest the apparent decline in white Christian influence, it is perhaps not surprising to find that some 73% of white evangelicals believe that God played some role in his election in 2016.Footnote 10 The idea that God played some role in Trump's election has been especially prevalent among evangelical leaders including Ralph Reed (Reference Reed2020) and Robert Jeffress (Mooney Reference Mooney2019). White evangelicals' specific rationale for divine influence varies. Some common rationales include the idea that Trump is fulfilling America's destiny or restoring America as a Christian nation (Posner Reference Posner2020), which fits directly with the Christian nationalism thesis (Whitehead and Perry Reference Whitehead and Perry2020). An addendum to this rationale is the idea that God chooses unlikely leaders to represent his followers (Posner Reference Posner2020). The allegory here being that, though Trump does not exude religiosity despite self-identifying as a Christian, his followers nonetheless see him as restoring a version of a Christian America just as biblical characters such as King Cyrus, the Persian King, restored Jerusalem.

This rationale is important to consider because it may explain why so many white evangelicals think that God played some role in Trump election even if he is not an overtly devout follower of the Christian faith. Religious psychology scholars posit that threats to religious identity are especially salient because they may lead to individuals adopting “coping mechanisms,” including passive resignation to the threat itself, as well as wishful thinking (Ysseldyk et al. Reference Ysseldyk, Matheson and Anisman2011). The belief that God played some role in Trump's election could be an example of wishful thinking on the part of white evangelicals especially concerned about the decline of a white Christian America.

In this model, white evangelicals perceive that Christian America is declining, making them feel as though they are a religious minority. Political elites then frame issues such as the decline of Christian America in a way that appeals to these threat perceptions. In the specific case of Trump, this includes harks to the restoration of a “Christian America” (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018). In this way, identity threat perceptions are activated by the specific frame regarding the “restoration” of Christian America as promoted by Trump, with the activation of these frames depending on the degree of resonance between the threat perceptions themselves and white evangelicals' belief in Trump's message. Given that religious identity threats explain support for Trump (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018), we therefore have reason to expect that they will also condition their beliefs about God's role in Trump's election.Footnote 11 These developments lead me to state my second hypothesis as:

H2: Perceptions of minority status will condition white evangelical beliefs about God's role in Trump's election.

Definitions, data, and measures

A definitional issue must first be addressed before proceeding to outline my data and measures. Despite being a pervasive group of scholarly interest since Trump's election in 2016, Evangelicals are defined in a number of different ways. How evangelicals are defined is important because different coding schema may elicit diverging inferences about the group in question (Hackett and Lindsay Reference Hackett and Lindsay2008; Ayris Reference Ayris2020). Burge and Lewis (Reference Burge and Lewis2018) posit that a question about broad religious affiliation followed by a question about a respondents' status as a “born again” or evangelical Christian is sufficient. However, this approach may lead to a sample that is smaller and slightly more Republican leaning than other definitions (Burge and Lewis Reference Burge and Lewis2018).Footnote 12 This approach, also used in contemporary studies (Margolis Reference Margolis2020), defines evangelicals by their theological beliefs, and locates Evangelism within Protestantism and views religion as an individual identity.

A separate approach defines evangelicals by the religious tradition that individuals belong to (Steensland et al. Reference Steensland, Robinson, Wilcox, Park, Regnerus and Woodberry2000; Smidt Reference Smidt2019). The RELTRAD method similarly focuses on intra-Protestant variations. However, perhaps the most glaring limitation especially relevant in the context of the current study is that, in the RELTRAD method, a respondents' race is not factored into these categorizations (Steensland et al. Reference Steensland, Robinson, Wilcox, Park, Regnerus and Woodberry2000; Smidt Reference Smidt2019). The RELTRAD method may therefore not be the most appropriate definition to take in studies such as this one, where the aim is to understand white evangelical beliefs and behaviors. Because of my theoretical interest in understanding white evangelical beliefs about God's role in Trump's election, I therefore opt for the former method. Crucially, this method allows for the inclusion of race into my religious classification scheme, and as noted in the previous paragraph, has also been used to study white evangelicals in a number of contemporary studies. Nonetheless, it is important to be aware of the limitations of my chosen definition for the purposes of conducting a transparent investigation.

To test my two hypotheses, data are taken from Wave 61 of Pew's ATP. The ATP is a nationally representative, probability-based online panel of noninstitutionalized persons aged 18 or over living in any of the 48 contiguous U.S. states, as well as AK or HI. Wave 61 of the ATP was conducted on behalf of Pew by the polling company Ipsos from February 4 to 20, 2020. Altogether, N = 6395 ATP panelists completed Wave 61. Survey weights were constructed by Pew for the online sample, which are used here so that results are generalizable to the U.S. adult population.

Dependent variables

To test H1, I use a dichotomous item that asks respondents whether they consider themselves to be a minority because of their religious beliefs. Respondents who consider themselves to be a minority because of their religious beliefs are coded as 1, and those who do not as 0.

The dependent measure to test H2 is a four-point categorical item that asks which statement comes closest to a respondent's view about god's role in the 2016 election, even if none are exactly right. The four statements are: (i) God chose Trump to be President because God approves of Trump's policies, (ii) Trump's election must be part of God's plan but doesn't necessarily mean that God approves of Trump's policies, (iii) God does not get involved in elections, and (iv) I do not believe in God.Footnote 13

Independent variables

Religion measures

Denomination and religiosity are both used to measure religion. Religious denominations are coded using a classification scheme outlined by the Public Religion Research Institute (PRRI) using eight categories: white evangelical Protestant, Black Protestant, Hispanic Protestant, white Catholic, Hispanic Catholic, other Christian, non-Christian, and the religiously unaffiliated, with white mainline Protestants serving as the reference category.Footnote 14

Religiosity is measured with three items

The first is a four-point ordinal item that asks how important religion is to a respondent, with possible responses ranging between 1 = “not at all important,” and 4 = “very important.” The second is a six-point ordinal item that asks how often a respondent attends religious service, with possible responses ranging between 1 = “never,” and 6 = “more than once a week.” The third item is a seven-point ordinal item that asks how often a respondent prays, with possible responses ranging between 1 = “never,” and 7 = “several times a day.” The three items are summed to create a single measure of religiosity (Cronbach's α = 0.87).

Controls

In addition to the measures outlined above, models are also estimated with a number of socio-political, demographic, and structural covariates. Respondents with a partisan or leaned partisan affiliation with the Republican Party are coded 1, and those with a partisan or leaned partisan affiliation with the Democratic Party as 0.Footnote 15 Ideology is a five-point ordinal item ranging between 1 = “very liberal,” and 5 = “very conservative.” Age is a four-point ordinal item ranging between 1 = “18–29,” and 4 = “65 + .”Footnote 16 Male are coded as 1 and females as 0. Education is a six-point ordinal item ranging between 1 = “less than high school,” and 6 = “postgraduate.” Lastly, respondents who reside in the South are coded 1, and those who reside elsewhere as 0. Unless noted, all variables are z-transformed to obtain standardized coefficient estimates.

Results

White evangelicals as religious minorities

I first turn to examine the extent to which minority status perceptions shape evaluations of Trump. In order to understand whether minority status perceptions condition evaluations of Trump, it is first necessary to quantify the extent to which white evangelicals consider themselves to be minorities as a consequence of their religious beliefs (H1). To test this possibility, the dichotomous item for perceptions of minority belief status was regressed against the religion and religiosity measures, as well as the set of covariates. Given that my outcome of interest is dichotomous, I use a binary logit estimator. For brevity, Figure 1 depicts the average marginal effect (AME) of religious identity for the eight respective PRRI classifications. Points to the right of the x axis in Figure 1 indicate a positive marginal effect, or an increase in the probability of a respondent considering themselves to be a minority because of their religious beliefs. Conversely, points to the left of the x axis indicate a negative marginal effect, or a decrease in the probably of a respondent considering themselves to be a minority because of their religious beliefs.

Figure 1. Logit estimates for minority status perceptions, by religious identity.

Notes: Points represent the marginal effect of various religious identities (sorted by size). The capped lines are 95% confidence intervals. Data are weighted. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant marginal effect (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). Full estimates presented in section A3 of the SI file.

Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

As evidenced by Figure 1, being a white evangelical is associated with a 20-point increase in the probability of a respondent considering themselves a minority because of their religious beliefs (AME = 0.201, p ≤ 0.001).Footnote 17 Notably, the size of the AME through white evangelical identity is larger than that for a number of other racial-religious groups including Black Protestants (p ≤ 0.001) and Hispanic Protestants (p ≤ 0.001). Elsewhere, being a non-Christian (p ≤ 0.001), religiously unaffiliated (p ≤ 0.001), and an “other Christian” increase the probability of considering oneself a minority. However, being a white Catholic or a Hispanic Catholic does not substantively affect the probability of considering oneself a minority. In sum, the results from Figure 1 therefore provide robust evidence in favor of the hypothesis that white evangelicals consider themselves to be a marginalized group as a result of their religious beliefs (H1).

Table 3. Minority status perceptions, by religious identity

Notes: Table entries represent the percentage of respondents who consider themselves to be minorities because of their religious beliefs.

Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Elections as divine outcomes

Having established that white evangelicals consider themselves minorities because of their religious beliefs, I now turn to examine whether these beliefs shape perceptions of whether Trump's 2016 election was a divine outcome (H2). To test this possibility, I limit my analytical sample to white evangelicals only, and regress the categorical dependent variable against the dichotomous variable indicating whether respondents consider themselves minorities because of their religious beliefs. Given that my dependent variable of interest is categorical I use a multinomial logit estimator. After estimating the multinomial logit model, I then use postestimation to graph two sets of results. The estimates of the postestimations are presented below in Figures 2 and 3.

Figure 2. White evangelical beliefs on god's role in Trump's election, by minority status perceptions.

Notes: Points represent the predicted probability of a white evangelical respondent response to each category of the multinomial-dependent measure at each level of the minority status perception item. Estimates based on a multinomial probit. All other variables in model hold constant or at their respective mean values. Full model estimates presented in section A5 of the SI file.

Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Figure 3. Marginal effect of minority status perceptions on white evangelical beliefs in god's role in Trump's election.

Notes: Points represent the marginal effect of a white evangelical respondent perceiving that they are a religious minority on each outcome. Estimates based on a multinomial logit model. All other variables in model hold constant or at their respective mean values. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant marginal effect (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). Full model estimates presented in section A5 of the SI file.

Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Figure 2 depicts white evangelical responses to each category of the dependent variable across levels of the minority status perceptions variable. I begin first with those who believe that “God chose Trump to be president.” A white evangelical who does not consider themselves to be a minority as a result of their religious beliefs has just a 0.07 predicted probability of believing that God chose Trump to be President. By contrast, a white evangelical who does consider themselves to be a minority as a result of their religious beliefs has a 0.09 predicted probability of believing that God chose Trump to be President. Therefore, increases in the salience of minority status perceptions are associated with a two-point increase in the predicted probability of a white evangelical believing that God chose Trump to be president.

Turning next to the second response category, a white evangelical who does not consider themselves to be a religious minority has a 0.66 predicted probability of believing that “Trump's election is part of God's plan.” Conversely, a white evangelical who does consider themselves to be a religious minority have a 0.74 predicted probability of believing that Trump's election is part of God's plan. As such, increases in the salience of minority status perceptions are associated with an eight-point increase in the predicted probability of a white evangelical respondent believing that Trump's election is part of God's plan.

Turning finally to the third response category, a white evangelical who considers themselves a minority as a consequence of their religious beliefs has a 0.26 predicted probability of believing that “God does not get involved in elections.” Contrastingly, a white evangelical who considers themselves to be a religious minority has just a 0.16 predicted probability of believing that God does not get involved in elections. Therefore, increases in the salience of minority status perceptions are associated with a 10-point decrease in the predicted probability of a white evangelical respondent believing that God does not get involved in elections.

To aid in substantive interpretation of these effects, Figure 3 graphs the marginal effect of a white evangelical respondent perceiving that they are a religious minority on each response category of the dependent variable. Asterisks indicate whether the changes in the predicted probabilities observed in Figure 2 are statistically distinguishable from zero. When it comes to the belief that God chose Trump to be president, we see that increases in the salience of minority status perceptions do not condition white evangelical respondents' beliefs about elections as divine outcomes; while we observe a two-point increase in the predicted probability, the 95% confidence intervals overlap with the reference line along the y-axis, indicating that changes in the predicted probability are not distinguishable from zero.

By contrast, the most substantive changes in the predicted probability of each response to the dependent variable come from those who are more ambivalent in their views on God's role in Trump's election.Footnote 18 For instance, the eight-point increase in the predicted probability of a white evangelical believing that Trump's election is part of God's plan as minority status perceptions become more salient is statistically significant at the p < 0.05 level, indicating a substantive degree of change. Likewise, the 10-point decrease in the belief that God does not get involved in elections as minority status perceptions become more salient is significant at the p < 0.01 level.

What then, might explain the pattern of results seen in Figures 2–3? Firstly, it is useful to consider why there is (essentially) no change in the probability of a white evangelical believing that God chose Trump to become president as minority status perceptions become more salient. One possibility relates to the fact that, among white evangelicals who believe that God chose Trump to be president, 44.03% of respondents already see themselves minorities as a result of their religious beliefs. Crucially, this figure is higher than overall percentage of white evangelicals (33.25%) who consider themselves minorities as a result of their religious beliefs (see Table 2), suggesting that such views are overrepresented within the first response category. The overrepresentation of white evangelicals who consider themselves religious minorities within the first response category brings to mind scholarship on public opinion which speaks to how ceiling effects often lead to limited changes in mass attitudes (Page and Shapiro Reference Page and Shapiro1983). In other words, minority status perceptions do little to change the belief that God chose Trump to be president because the number of white evangelicals who are already of the belief that they are religious minorities leaves little room for opinions to change.

Discussion

This paper explores why so many white evangelicals support Trump despite his well-publicized personal transgressions and seemingly irreligiosity. As noted in the introduction, this contradiction raises an interesting consideration—namely, whether Trump represents something of a unique case when it comes to white evangelical evaluations of elite religiosity, or whether his support among white evangelicals is indicative of a new era of evangelical support for candidates that do not assess religiosity as it used to.

Overall, the results presented here go some way to addressing this contradiction. First, it was necessary to understand whether white evangelicals exhibited particularly strong preferences for elite religiosity. Despite the burgeoning number of studies highlighting the contradiction between Trump's irreligiosity and distinctly “un-Christian” behavior and his strong support among white evangelicals (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018; Wehner Reference Wehner2019; Ayris Reference Ayris2020; Whitehead and Perry Reference Whitehead and Perry2020), the extent to which they had strong preferences for having a President who was religious was somewhat unclear. However, given the scholarship on the effects of religious identification on evaluations of elites (Castle et al. Reference Castle, Layman, Campbell and Green2017; Smidt and Miles Reference Smidt and Miles2017), we had reason to suspect that religious adherents (including white evangelicals) would exhibit strong, generalized preferences for elite religiosity. The bivariate data from Table 1 thus go some way to answering this question by demonstrating that white evangelicals do exhibit relatively strong, generalized preferences for having a President who exudes religiosity and is willing to stand up for their religious group interests.

Next, the finding regarding white evangelical evaluations of Trump's religiosity (see Table 2) underscores an important point. White evangelicals appear to know and acknowledge that Trump isn't particularly religious. However, they are able to evaluate the perceived religiosity or irreligiosity of a slate of Democratic and Republican elites beyond Trump. This finding suggests that Trump is a unique case when it comes to white evangelical evaluations of the religiosity of elites: instead of projecting their beliefs onto Trump, and thereby supporting him because of his perceived religiosity, white evangelicals support him despite his lack of religiosity.Footnote 19 This finding is noteworthy because it goes some way to answering the contradiction that has piqued the interests of contemporary scholarship at the intersection of politics and religion—the answer is that white evangelicals exhibit strong, generalized preferences for elite religiosity. However, in the specific case of Trump's religiosity, they are notably ambivalent.Footnote 20

While the findings within the bivariate data provide an answer to the first part of the puzzle, they do not tell us what factors might explain white evangelicals' robust levels of support for Trump if his perceived religiosity does not matter to the group. This consideration is further compounded by another interesting finding within the public opinion data—namely, the belief among many white evangelicals that God played some role in Trump's election. Indeed, if white evangelicals do not evaluate Trump as being particularly religious, then why would 73% of them believe that God played some role in his election? Consistent with extant theorizing in the religious psychology scholarship (Ysseldyk et al. Reference Ysseldyk, Matheson and Anisman2011), as well as work attesting to how perceived threats to religious identity may become politically relevant attitudes (Campbell and Monson Reference Campbell and Monson2008), I hypothesized that religious identity threats would condition white evangelicals' beliefs about God's role in Trump's election. Drawing on contemporary debates in the Christian nationalism scholarship (Whitehead et al. Reference Whitehead, Perry and Baker2018), I argued that white evangelicals believed that God played some role in Trump's election to arrest the decline of Christian America. These beliefs are important because they are examples of coping appraisal mechanisms that individuals might adopt when encountering threats to their religious identities (Ysseldyk et al. Reference Ysseldyk, Matheson and Anisman2011).

Largely in line with my expectations, I found that perceptions of minority status do condition white evangelical beliefs regarding God's role in Trump's election. While these perceptions did little to condition to those whose views were the most extreme (i.e., white evangelicals who thought that Trump was directly chosen by God), much greater movement was observed when it came to those whose beliefs were ambivalent about Trump's election as a divine outcome. Here, I found that perceptions of minority status increased the probability of a white evangelical believing that Trump's election was part of God's plan. Crucially, these increases coincided with declines in the probability of white evangelical respondents thinking that God does not get involved in elections.

This finding is important because it offers a rationale for why white evangelicals not only support Trump electorally (Margolis Reference Margolis2020), but also think that God played some role in Trump's election. The findings concerning the salience of identity threats on conditioning white evangelical beliefs also provide an additional explanation for why evaluations on Trump's religiosity might not have mattered when it came to their vote choice in 2016. Namely, because Trump's invocation of the decline of white Christian America proved effective in activating religious identity threat in a way that led to white evangelicals to coalesce around his candidacy. In this way, Trump's ability to articulate white evangelicals' fears about the declining influence of Christianity likely overrode any lingering concerns about his religiosity.

Limitations and future directions

It is also important to point out the limitations of the current study, as well as potential avenues for future research. One limitation of the current study is that I am not able to account for the potential effects of elite religious cues in shaping white evangelical responses to the ATP measures. Elite cues are a powerful heuristic that shape individual beliefs and behaviors. Therefore, the manner in which elites such as Trump use and deploy elite religious cues ought to have noteworthy effects on white evangelical evaluations of his religiosity. As noted in the literature review, Trump's failure to name a single bible passage gives us reason to doubt whether his efforts proved effective in convincing white evangelicals about his religiosity. In this way, ineffective deployment of covert religious cues may explain why white evangelicals do not evaluate Trump as being particularly religious. Future research should look into the effects of similar cues in shaping evaluations of elite religiosity. Experimental research designs would allow for the investigation of the impact of these cues across a variety of similar measures to those used in this paper.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755048322000219.

Jack Thompson is a Postdoctoral Research Associate in the Department of Politics at the University of Exeter. His research interests include misinformation attitudes, LGBTQ+ politics, and political geography.

Footnotes

1. Assessments of what percentage of white evangelicals voted for Trump differ somewhat based on the measures used to assess evangelical identification. Among white evangelicals not accounting for inter-denominational differences, Edison exit polls estimate that 76% voted for Trump in the 2020 election. Elsewhere, the AP VoteCast survey finds that 81% of white evangelicals with a Protestant affiliation voted for Trump in 2020 (Newport Reference Newport2020).

2. While white evangelicals may be willing to support non-traditional candidates such as Romney (Mormon) or Trump (well-documented history of moral transgressions), it is important to qualify that both still identify as Christian. Given the well-documented relationships between Christian identity and negative attitudes toward non-Christian religious minorities (Baker et al. Reference Baker, Perry and Whitehead2020), it is perhaps unlikely that white evangelicals would support non-Christian candidates.

3. Nonetheless, a growing body of scholarship indicates that group identities are endogenous to politics (Egan Reference Egan2020). For instance, research finds Democrats and liberals switching into identification as non-religious (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Layman, Green and Sumaktoyo2018; Margolis Reference Margolis2018).

4. The shortcomings of fusing “white” and “evangelical” into a category are nonetheless noted by scholars. Even though the changing racial demographics of evangelicalism have provided fruitful exploration for scholars (Alexander Reference Alexander2020), non-white evangelical remains a largely unexplored category in public opinion data, thus producing a “racially myopic” view of evangelical Christianity (Ayris Reference Ayris2020).

5. Still, covert cues are not solely citing biblical passages. As noted by McLaughlin and Wise (Reference McLaughlin and Wise2014), covert cues may represent any action where elites refer to their “faith” without specific reference to God.

6. Scala's (Reference Scala2020) recent analysis of voter behavior of the 2016 Republican Presidential primaries lends some weight to this possibility, finding that evangelicals who were less devoted to their religion were more supportive of Trump than highly devout evangelicals (who, in theory, should be the most receptive to covert cues) in the early stages of the primary.

7. Though, it is important to note that these incidents require satisfaction of the ignorability assumption. Moreover, even if religious adherents are aware of such scandals, their reactions are also likely conditioned by negative partisanship (Margolis Reference Margolis2020).

8. The fact that white evangelicals evaluate a slew of Democratic elites are irreligious suggests that evaluations of candidate religiosity are politically motivated.

9. Trump appointed 226 federal judges during his term—the most by any first term President since Jimmy Carter.

10. Estimates based off descriptive analysis of Pew data.

11. When theorizing about the direction of movement as minority status perceptions become more salient, it is useful to consider how white evangelicals view God's role in Trump's election. In Wave 61 of the ATP, 12.22% of white evangelicals believe that God chose Trump to be president, 61.33% believe that Trump's election is part of God's plan, while 26.44% believe that God does not get involved in elections. As these estimates indicate, the belief that Trump was directly ordained by God is something of a minority view even among white evangelicals. As such, if we are to see movement from to state of ambivalence (i.e., God does not get involved in elections) to a state of affirmation, it may be more likely to see movement from a state of ambivalence to a state of weak affirmation (i.e., Trump's election is part of God's plan), as opposed to ambivalence to a state of strong affirmation (i.e., God chose Trump to be president).

12. The primary concern is that only counting Protestants who identify as evangelical excludes other Christian denominations—for instance, Catholics, and Letter Day Saints (LDS)—who may also identify as evangelical. However, it is important to note that the proportion of Catholics who identity as evangelical in public opinion surveys is relatively small (Cassese Reference Cassese2020). Similarly, though some LDS members may also identify as evangelical, the very small number of LDS respondents in public opinion surveys mean that their exclusion has not significantly biased the results of similar studies (Ayris Reference Ayris2020).

13. Only two white evangelical respondents within the ATP reported not believing in God. Given that a cell size of N = 2 is far too small to detect meaningful effects, I performed listwise deletion of the two white evangelical respondents who reported not believing in God. As a result, there are only three response categories when I limit my analytical sample to white evangelical respondents.

14. A comprehensive elucidation of the PRRI coding scheme is presented in section A1 of the SI file.

15. While desirable, Pew datasets do not contain the standard seven-point scale for party ID.

16. Age is modeled as an ordinal covariate. Modeling age by generational cohort does not affect the significance and direction of the results presented here.

17. In the bivariate context, 33.25% of white evangelicals consider themselves minorities as a result of their religious beliefs (see Table 3). Adjusting for covariates, this number increases to 43.74%.

18. That is, among those who either believe that Trump's election is part of God's plan, or who believe that God does get involved in elections.

19. This finding also brings to mind an interesting literature on projection heuristics. While much of the projection heuristics scholarship focuses on ideological projection and not religion (Lerman and Sadin Reference Lerman and Sadin2016), the mechanism (or lack thereof in this case) remains largely the same; individuals are prone to overestimate (i.e., project) the probability of sharing a characteristic with another individual. When applied as a model for elite evaluations, projection is thus moderated by the extent to which individuals are linked to the elite by a shared characteristic. As such, if white evangelicals do project their religious beliefs onto Trump, then we ought to have expected a large number to perceive Trump as being religious. However, this is not what we observe in the bivariate data in Table 2.

20. When evaluations of Trump's religiosity are regressed against my baseline set of covariates in an OLS model, it is apparent that party ID dominates the religion and religiosity measures, suggesting that socio-political beliefs are doing much of the work in driving evaluations of Trump religiosity—a finding congruous with scholarship on the power of partisanship on elite evaluations (Bartels Reference Bartels2002; Hayes Reference Hayes2005).

References

Achen, CH and Bartels, LM (2017) Democracy for Realists: Why Elections Do Not Produce Responsive Government. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Albertson, BL (2015) Dog-whistle Politics: Multivocal Communication and Religious Appeals. Political Behavior 37, 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, NH (2020) Latino Evangelicals: How Their Multiple Identity Influences Their Political Preferences (Doctoral dissertation). The Claremont Graduate University.Google Scholar
Ayris, A (2020) “They'd Vote Against Jesus Christ Himself”: Trump's “White Evangelicals,” the Construction of a Contested Identity, and the Need for a New Narrative. Journal of Church and State 63, 648670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, JO, Perry, SL and Whitehead, AL (2020) Keep America Christian (and White): Christian Nationalism, Fear of Ethnoracial Outsiders, and Intention to Vote for Donald Trump in the 2020 Presidential Election. Sociology of Religion 81, 272293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, LM (2002) Beyond the Running Tally: Partisan Bias in Political Perceptions. Political Behavior 24, 117150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, L (2016) Ted Cruz Quotes Scripture, Thanks God and “Courageous Conservatives” After Iowa Victory. The Christian Post, February 2. Available at https://www.christianpost.com/news/ted-cruz-quotes-scripture-thanks-god-iowa-caucus.html.Google Scholar
Branscombe, NR, Ellemers, N, Spears, R and Doosje, B (1999) The Context and Content of Social Identity Threat. In Social Identity: Context, Commitment, Content, eds. N, Ellemers, R, Spears and B, Doosje, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 3558.Google Scholar
Burge, RP and Lewis, AR (2018) Measuring Evangelicals: Practical Considerations for Social Scientists–CORRIGENDUM. Politics and Religion 11, 920925.Google Scholar
Calfano, BR and Djupe, PA (2009) God Talk: Religious Cues and Electoral Support. Political Research Quarterly 62, 329339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, DE, Layman, GC, Green, JC and Sumaktoyo, NG (2018) Putting Politics First: The Impact of Politics on American Religious and Secular Orientations. American Journal of Political Science 62, 551565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, DE and Monson, JQ (2008) The Religion Card: Gay Marriage and the 2004 Presidential Election. Public Opinion Quarterly 72, 399419.Google Scholar
Cassese, EC (2020) Straying From the Flock? A Look at How Americans’ Gender and Religious Identities Cross-Pressure Partisanship. Political Research Quarterly 73, 169183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castle, JJ (2019) New Fronts in the Culture Wars? Religion, Partisanship, and Polarization on Religious Liberty and Transgender Rights in the United States. American Politics Research 47, 650679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castle, JJ, Layman, GC, Campbell, DE and Green, JC (2017) Survey Experiments on Candidate Religiosity, Political Attitudes, and Vote Choice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 56, 143161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, PJ (2020) Identity as Dependent Variable: How Americans Shift Their Identities to Align with Their Politics. American Journal of Political Science 64, 699716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feuer, A (2018) What We Know About Trump's $130,000 Payment to Stormy Daniels. The New York Times, August 27. Available at https://www.nytimes.com/2018/08/27/nyregion/stormy-daniels-trump-payment.html.Google Scholar
Gorski, P (2019) Why Evangelicals Voted for Trump: A Critical Cultural Sociology. Politics of Meaning/Meaning of Politics, eds. JL, Mast and JC, Alexander, Cham: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 165183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, JC, Rozell, MJ and Wilcox, C (eds) (2000) Prayers in the Precincts: The Christian Right in the 1998 Elections. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Hackett, C and Lindsay, DM (2008) Measuring Evangelicalism: Consequences of Different Operationalization Strategies. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 47, 499514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haider-Markel, DP (2007) Representation and Backlash: The Positive and Negative Influence of Descriptive Representation. Legislative Studies Quarterly 32, 107133.Google Scholar
Hayes, D (2005) Candidate Qualities Through a Partisan Lens: A Theory of Trait Ownership. American Journal of Political Science 49, 908923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hetherington, MJ, Weiler, JD and Weiler, JD (2009) Authoritarianism and Polarization in American Politics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hunter, JD (1992) Culture Wars: The Struggle to Control the Family, Art, Education, Law, and Politics in America. New York City, NY: Avalon Publishing.Google Scholar
Jones, RP (2016) The End of White Christian America. New York City, NY: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Keenan, JP and Pereira, I (2020) For Many White Evangelicals, the Supreme Court Makeup Solidifies Their Support for President Trump. ABC, October 16. Available at https://abcnews.go.com/Politics/white-evangelicals-supreme-court-makeup-solidifies-support-president/story?id=73153818.Google Scholar
Kinnvall, C (2004) Globalization and Religious Nationalism: Self, Identity, and the Search for Ontological Security. Political Psychology 25, 741767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layman, GC (1997) Religion and Political Behavior in the United States: The Impact of Beliefs, Affiliations, and Commitment From 1980 to 1994. Public Opinion Quarterly 61, 288316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leege, DC, Wald, KD, Krueger, BS and Mueller, PD (2009) The Politics of Cultural Differences. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lerman, AE and Sadin, ML (2016) Stereotyping or Projection? How White and Black Voters Estimate Black Candidates’ Ideology. Political Psychology 37, 147163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolis, MF (2018) From Politics to the Pews: How Partisanship and the Political Environment Shape Religious Identity. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolis, MF (2020) Who Wants to Make America Great Again? Understanding Evangelical Support for Donald Trump. Politics & Religion 13, 89118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martí, G (2019) The Unexpected Orthodoxy of Donald J. Trump: White Evangelical Support for the 45th President of the United States.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinez, J and Smith, GA (2016) How the Faithful Voted: A Preliminary 2016 Analysis. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
McDermott, ML (2009) Religious Stereotyping and Voter Support for Evangelical Candidates. Political Research Quarterly 62, 340354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, B and Wise, D (2014) Cueing God: Religious Cues and Voter Support. Politics & Religion 7, 366394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merritt (2016) Trump's Bible Fail. The Atlantic, April 15. Available at https://www.theatlantic.com/politics/archive/2016/04/trumps-bible-fail/478425/.Google Scholar
Mooney, MJ (2019) Trump's Apostle. Texas Monthly, August. Available at https://www.texasmonthly.com/articles/donald-trump-defender-dallas-pastor-robert-jeffress/.Google Scholar
Mouw, T and Sobel, ME (2001) Culture Wars and Opinion Polarization: The Case of Abortion. American Journal of Sociology 106, 913943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, F (2020) Religious Group Voting and the 2020 Election. Gallup, November 13. Available at https://news.gallup.com/opinion/polling-matters/324410/religious-group-voting-2020-election.aspx.Google Scholar
Olson, LR and Warber, AL (2008) Belonging, Behaving, and Believing: Assessing the Role of Religion on Presidential Approval. Political Research Quarterly 61, 192204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, BI and Shapiro, RY (1983) Effects of Public Opinion on Policy. American Political Science Review 77, 175190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center (2012) Election 2012 Postmortem: White Evangelicals and Support for Romney. Pew Research Center, December 7. Available at https://www.pewforum.org/2012/12/07/election-2012-post-mortem-white-evangelicals-and-support-for-romney/.Google Scholar
Popkin, SL (2020) The Reasoning Voter: Communication and Persuasion in Presidential Campaigns. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Posner, S (2020) Unholy: Why White Evangelicals Worship at the Altar of Donald Trump. New York City, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Putnam, RD, Campbell, DE and Garrett, SR (2012) American Grace: How Religion Divides and Unites us. New York City, NY: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Reed, R (2020) For God and Country: The Christian Case for Trump. New York City, NY: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Renard, M (2017) Myths Debunked: Why Did White Evangelical Christians Vote for Trump? The University of Chicago Divinity School, January 17. Available at https://divinity.uchicago.edu/sightings/articles/myths-debunked-why-did-white-evangelical-christians-vote-trump.Google Scholar
Scala, DJ (2020) Polls and Elections: The Skeptical Faithful: How Trump Gained Momentum among Evangelicals. Presidential Studies Quarterly 50, 927947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, DJ (2017) White Attitudes About Descriptive Representation in the US: The Roles of Identity, Discrimination, and Linked Fate. Politics, Groups, and Identities 5, 84106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwadel, P (2017) The Republicanization of Evangelical Protestants in the United States: An Examination of the Sources of Political Realignment. Social Science Research 62, 238254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smidt, CE (2019) Reassessing the Concept and Measurement of Evangelicals: The Case for the RELTRAD Approach. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 58, 833853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smidt, W and Miles, MR (2017) Religious Identity and Descriptive Representation. Politics & Religion 10, 186221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C (2000) Christian America?: What Evangelicals Really Want. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Smith, GA (2020) White Christians Continue to Favor Trump Over Biden, but Support has Slipped. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
Steensland, B, Robinson, LD, Wilcox, WB, Park, JZ, Regnerus, MD and Woodberry, RD (2000) The Measure of American Religion: Toward Improving the State of the art. Social Forces 79, 291318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, HE (1978) Differentiation Between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H and Turner, J (1979) An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Conflict. In Austin, WE and Worchel, S (eds), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole, pp. 3347.Google Scholar
Wald, KD and Calhoun-Brown, A (2014) Religion and Politics in the United States. Washington, DC: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Weber, C and Thornton, M (2012) Courting Christians: How Political Candidates Prime Religious Considerations in Campaign Ads. The Journal of Politics 74, 400413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wehner, P (2019) The Deepening Crisis in Evangelical Christianity. The Atlantic.Google Scholar
Wellman, JK Jr and Tokuno, K (2004) Is Religious Violence Inevitable?. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 43, 291296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, AL and Perry, SL (2020) Taking America Back for God: Christian Nationalism in the United States. New York City, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, AL, Perry, SL and Baker, JO (2018) Make America Christian Again: Christian Nationalism and Voting for Donald Trump in the 2016 Presidential Election. Sociology of Religion 79, 147171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, DK (2012) God's Own Party: The Making of the Christian Right. New York City, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wuthnow, R (1988) The Restructuring of American Religion. Princeton, NY: University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ysseldyk, R, Matheson, K and Anisman, H (2010) Religiosity as Identity: Toward an Understanding of Religion From a Social Identity Perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14, 6071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ysseldyk, R, Matheson, K and Anisman, H (2011) Coping with Identity Threat: The Role of Religious Orientation and Implications for Emotions and Action Intentions. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 3, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, J (2009) Whose America?: Culture Wars in the Public Schools. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Preferences for elite religiosity, by religious identity

Figure 1

Table 2. Perceptions of elite religiosity, by religious identity

Figure 2

Figure 1. Logit estimates for minority status perceptions, by religious identity.Notes: Points represent the marginal effect of various religious identities (sorted by size). The capped lines are 95% confidence intervals. Data are weighted. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant marginal effect (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). Full estimates presented in section A3 of the SI file.Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Figure 3

Table 3. Minority status perceptions, by religious identity

Figure 4

Figure 2. White evangelical beliefs on god's role in Trump's election, by minority status perceptions.Notes: Points represent the predicted probability of a white evangelical respondent response to each category of the multinomial-dependent measure at each level of the minority status perception item. Estimates based on a multinomial probit. All other variables in model hold constant or at their respective mean values. Full model estimates presented in section A5 of the SI file.Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Figure 5

Figure 3. Marginal effect of minority status perceptions on white evangelical beliefs in god's role in Trump's election.Notes: Points represent the marginal effect of a white evangelical respondent perceiving that they are a religious minority on each outcome. Estimates based on a multinomial logit model. All other variables in model hold constant or at their respective mean values. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant marginal effect (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). Full model estimates presented in section A5 of the SI file.Source: American Trends Panel (Wave 61).

Supplementary material: File

Thompson supplementary material

Thompson supplementary material

Download Thompson supplementary material(File)
File 27.9 KB