Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T09:13:07.338Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Inter-population variation in the intensity of host manipulation by the fish acanthocephalan Pomphorhynchus tereticollis: are differences driven by predation risk?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 June 2019

M. Fayard*
Affiliation:
Biogéosciences, UMR 6282 CNRS, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, 6 Boulevard Gabriel, 21000 Dijon, France
F. Cezilly
Affiliation:
Biogéosciences, UMR 6282 CNRS, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, 6 Boulevard Gabriel, 21000 Dijon, France
M.-J. Perrot-Minnot
Affiliation:
Biogéosciences, UMR 6282 CNRS, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, 6 Boulevard Gabriel, 21000 Dijon, France
*
Author for correspondence: M. Fayard, E-mail: marion.fayard@u-bourgogne.fr

Abstract

Many trophically-transmitted parasites induce behavioural alteration in their intermediate hosts that tend to increase host vulnerability to predation. Inter-population variability in parasite-induced alterations is expected to arise from variable local opportunities for trophic transmission. Yet, this hypothesis has not been investigated so far. We addressed the issue in four populations of the fish parasite Pomphorhynchus tereticollis (Acanthocephala), using variable fish biomass density as a proxy for transmission opportunities. We found variation in the intensity of parasite-induced changes in phototaxis and refuge use among populations. Two of the populations with the lowest predator biomass exhibited the highest levels of behavioural manipulation and prevalence, as expected at low transmission opportunities. They also exhibited micro-habitat segregation between infected and uninfected gammarids in the field. In addition, infection had variable effects on two physiological defence systems, immunity and antioxidant capacity, and on total protein content. Overall, our study brings partial support to the prediction that host manipulation and prevalence should be higher at low predator biomass. Although stronger evidence should be sought by increasing population replicates, our study points to the importance of the ecological context, specifically transmission opportunities brought about by predation pressure, for the evolution of parasite manipulation in trophically-transmitted parasites.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Introduction

Some parasites have developed the ability to alter the phenotype of their intermediate host in ways that are supposed to increase their own fitness at the expense of that of their hosts, generally through increased parasite transmission (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Adamo and Moore2005). This phenomenon, known as ‘host manipulation by parasites’ (HMP), is currently regarded as one of the most compelling examples of an extended phenotype (sensu Dawkins, 1982). In parasites with complex life-cycles and trophic transmission, such changes are assumed to increase the vulnerability of infected intermediate hosts to predation by definitive hosts (Lafferty, Reference Lafferty1999; Moore, Reference Moore2002; Cézilly and Perrot-Minnot, Reference Cézilly and Perrot-Minnot2005). For instance, the drifting behaviour of Gammarus pulex (Amphipoda: Crustacea) infected with Pomphorhynchus laevis (Acanthocephala) is increased compared to that of uninfected ones, a behavioural change supposedly contributing to the predation bias towards infected prey recorded in the field (Lagrue et al., Reference Lagrue, Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot, Motreuil and Bollache2007).

Following a classical cost-benefit approach to understand the evolution of HMP, it has been predicted that the intensity of changes induced by parasites should vary according to transmission constraints and opportunities (Poulin, Reference Poulin2010; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Brodeur, Maure, Franceschi, Blanchet and Rigaud2011). Such constraints may vary between host–parasite systems according to parasite transmission strategies. In trophically-transmitted parasites, for instance, the probability for a parasite to pass from an intermediate host onto an appropriate final one strongly depends upon the pattern of predation. Transmission constraints may also vary between and within populations within a given host–parasite system (Poulin, Reference Poulin2010; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Brodeur, Maure, Franceschi, Blanchet and Rigaud2011; Hafer-Hahmann, Reference Hafer-Hahmann2019). Consequently, the pattern and magnitude of HMP are expected to vary at multiple scales, between host–parasite systems but also between and within host populations.

Environmental factors may actually play a key role in the intensity of manipulation (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Rigaud and Brodeur2012). For instance, seasonal fluctuations may be responsible for variations in HMP in relation to parasite requirements (to become mature), mainly because habitat quality varies according to season (Gotthard, Reference Gotthard, Atkinson and Thorndyke2001). Spatial variability in microhabitat features, and in the diversity and abundance of definitive hosts with different foraging strategies, may also contribute to variation in HMP (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Rigaud and Brodeur2012). Indeed, parasite manipulation is not expected to be strong in populations where the abundance of definitive hosts is high, as parasites are likely to get transmitted by chance (Lafferty, Reference Lafferty1992). Conversely, high intensity of HMP is expected under low abundance of definitive hosts. However, despite the diversity of host–parasite systems, and of the abiotic and biotic factors possibly modulating HMP, only a few studies have quantified variation in manipulation by trophically-transmitted parasites (Franceschi et al., Reference Franceschi, Cornet, Bollache, Dechaume-Moncharmont, Bauer, Motreuil and Rigaud2010). Moreover, to the best of our knowledge, no study has related variability in manipulation by trophically-transmitted parasites among natural populations to environmental factors.

The aim of the present study was precisely to investigate whether spatial variation in biotic factors modulates the magnitude of intermediate host manipulation by a fish acanthocephalan parasite. Acanthocephalans are well represented in studies on HPM (Moore, Reference Moore2002; Fayard et al., Reference Fayard, Dechaume-Moncharmon, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2019), including reported cases of inter-population variation in the intensity of manipulation. For instance, the intensity of HMP in experimentally-infected G. pulex has been shown to differ according to the geographical origin of P. laevis population (Franceschi et al., Reference Franceschi, Cornet, Bollache, Dechaume-Moncharmont, Bauer, Motreuil and Rigaud2010). Conversely, variation in parasite-induced mortality among populations of G. fossarum harbouring acanthocephalan parasites has been related to the differential susceptibility of host genetic lineages to HMP (Galipaud et al., Reference Galipaud, Bollache and Lagrue2017). Here, we investigate the link between the magnitude of HMP in G. fossarum naturally infected with the fish acanthocephalan P. tereticollis and transmission opportunities to fish hosts at a local scale, using local fish biomass density as a proxy for predation pressure (Maurer et al., Reference Maurer, Stewart and Lorenz2014). The underlying assumption is that local fish biomass provides a proxy for transmission opportunities. This can be an approximation given that the host range of P. tereticollis includes both highly competent hosts and less competent ones (Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Guyonnet, Bollache and Lagrue2019). However, we considered here the whole fish community as a proxy for predation pressure and transmission opportunities, given theoretical evidence that trophic transmission to suboptimal or non-appropriate final hosts does not impose a cost high enough to constrain the evolution of HMP (Seppälä and Jokela, Reference Seppälä and Jokela2008).

We recorded the magnitude of intermediate host manipulation by P. tereticollis in four populations from four rivers varying in local fish biomass. We first estimated the local prevalence of P. tereticollis cystacanths and the density of G. fossarum. We then quantified HMP on both behavioural and physiological traits. Host manipulation by acanthocephalans is known to be multidimensional (Cézilly et al., Reference Cézilly, Favrat and Perrot-Minnot2013), notably involving traits related to taxis, protection and immune system (Fayard et al., Reference Fayard, Dechaume-Moncharmon, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2019). Here, we recorded phototaxis and refuge use, two behavioural traits markedly altered by P. tereticollis (Tain et al., Reference Tain, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2006; Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007). Both traits are also altered under anxiety-like state, i.e. a state of sustained apprehension of the environment, as recently evidenced in two crustacean species, including gammarids (Fossat et al., Reference Fossat, Bacqué-Cazenave, De Deurwaerdère, Delbecque and Cattaert2014; Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Banchetry and Cézilly2017). As defensive physiological traits, we estimated the level of host immunocompetence provided by the (pro) phenoloxidase system, and the level of antioxidant defences. The prophenoloxidase (proPO) cascade is involved in melanization reactions accompanying innate immune responses and is a common response to infection in arthropods (Rigaud and Moret, Reference Rigaud and Moret2003). Suppression of the proPO system has already been reported in Pomphorhynchus-infected gammarids (Rigaud and Moret, Reference Rigaud and Moret2003; Cornet et al., Reference Cornet, Franceschi, Bauer, Rigaud and Moret2009). In addition, oxidative stress has been associated with predation risk in several aquatic species (Slos and Stoks, Reference Slos and Stoks2008; Janssens and Stoks, Reference Janssens and Stoks2013). We predict that the level of HMP should be lower in localities where the high fish biomass enhances transmission probability independently of manipulation (and therefore relaxes selection for HMP) (Fig. 1a). Conversely, we predict that the level of HMP should be high in localities where fish biomass is low or intermediate, in response to selection on parasite for enhancing host vulnerability to predation (Fig. 1). As a consequence, the prevalence of P. tereticollis cystacanths in G. fossarum could be either low, if strong manipulation leads to faster predation of infected individuals relative to uninfected ones (i.e. at intermediate fish biomass, Fig. 1b), or high, if low predator biomass and, hence, predation rate, leads to the accumulation of cystacanth-infected prey despite high HMP (Fig. 1c). Under the assumption that physiological changes are associated with infection, we also expect a decrease in host immunity (Cornet et al., Reference Cornet, Franceschi, Bauer, Rigaud and Moret2009; Fayard et al., Reference Fayard, Dechaume-Moncharmon, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2019).

Fig. 1. Predicted consequences of local fish biomass on the level of host manipulation by parasite (HMP) through predation rate. Transmission opportunities for trophically transmitted fish parasite are assumed to increase with fish biomass when most fish predators are suitable as definitive hosts. Arrow thickness represents the impact of overall predation rate (independently of infection status) and of infected prey vulnerability to predation relative to uninfected ones (HMP); their relative impacts modulate the observed prevalence of infective parasite stages.

Materials and methods

Biological model, field areas and maintenance

Four distinct rivers located in Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, eastern France, were prospected from mid-February to early-April 2017. We chose sampling localities based on previous records of P. tereticollis, and available information on fish communities, obtained through the fish-based ecological assessment achieved by the Office Français pour la Biodiversité (OFB) in the framework of EU Water Directive: Talmay on Vingeanne river, Vadans on Cuisance river, Orgeux on Norges river, and Marandeuil on Bèze river (Table 1). We retrieved information on local fish biomass density (g.100 m2; thereafter fish biomass) from the OFB database. Fish survey was based on one to four replicates at different time periods (Table 1). At each locality, we sampled gammarids twice, at 2-week intervals. We first randomly sampled gammarids in the benthos to estimate P. tereticollis prevalence, gammarid individual size, and gammarid density. Two weeks later, we collected uninfected and P. tereticollis-infected gammarids at the same place, for behavioural and physiological assays. Gammarids were kept in the lab for no longer than 48 h (prevalence estimates) or 24 h (behavioural assays). During this time, they maintained at 16 °C under a 12:12 light regime in tanks filled with well-oxygenated water from the river, and fed with elm leaves.

Table 1. Description of the localities with coordinates, temperature, descriptors of local fish assemblage structure, intermediate hosts (gammarid) density, and prevalence of P. tereticollis cystacanths in gammarids

The most competent fish species (*) are assumed to be five benthic feeders – barbel, dace, bullhead, loach and gudgeon – as reported in Perrot-Minnot et al. (Reference Perrot-Minnot, Guyonnet, Bollache and Lagrue2019). However, other species – notably the chub Squalius cephalus, ranging in relative biomass in these localities from 12 to 35% – also harbours gravid females of P. tereticollis albeit of smaller size (Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Guyonnet, Bollache and Lagrue2019). Gammarid density is only a semi-quantitative estimate, as the standardized protocol used provides relative abundance among populations rather than absolute abundance. Sampling replicates (nb repl.) are the number of kick-sampling replicates on a fixed area of 1.5 m−2.

Parasite prevalence and gammarid density

Both parasite prevalence and gammarid density were assessed over a 6-week period, from mid-February to late March, to allow comparison between localities, thus avoiding the potential confounding effect of seasonality. We randomly sampled gammarids from the benthos in both the bank and the bed of the river, following the kick sampling procedure (Turner and Trexler, Reference Turner and Trexler1997) on fixed areas. The procedure consisted of moving the river substrate (gravel, plants, rocks and sand) by kicking, before harvesting using a fine mesh net downstream. Sampling was semi-quantitative: the surface area of benthos harvesting was standardized to 5 m2 per sample replicate, and three or four replicates were collected in each locality. For each sample replicate, we used a metal frame kick net (0.5 mm mesh size) to collect the benthos in three contiguous passes of 5 m in length and approximately 0.3 m in width. For each locality, the same experimenter collected at least 2000 individuals, to estimate gammarid density and P. tereticollis prevalence. Semi-quantitative estimates of gammarids density using this standardized protocol reflect relative rather than absolute abundance among populations (Davies, Reference Davies2001). Gammarids from the bank and the bed were kept alive and brought back to the laboratory in two separate ice boxes. Assessment of prevalence, under a stereomicroscope, began the very same day the benthos was collected. Once the prevalence was assessed, individuals were pooled according to their status and the habitat they came from (bank or bed of the river), for further size measurement. Body size was measured for all infected individuals, and for a subset of 5–10% of uninfected individuals picked up at random. Body size was estimated from the height of the fourth coxal plate, following Bollache et al. (Reference Bollache, Rigaud and Cézilly2002), using a Nixon SMZ 1500 stereoscopic microscope.

Pattern of HMP: behavioural assays

Behavioural assays were conducted within a short time period, from 13 of March to 19 of April 2017. We collected at least 60 infected and 60 uninfected individuals from each locality for behavioural and physiological tests. Phototaxis and refuge use were recorded by scan and time sampling, under a light intensity of 700 lux. Reaction to light was assessed by scoring the position of a single individual in a two-choice (light/dark) arena every 15 s for 5 min, following Perrot-Minnot et al. (Reference Perrot-Minnot, Sanchez-Thirion and Cézilly2014). The two-choice (light/dark) arena consisted of a 23 cm long and 3 cm diameter closed glass tube, with half side painted in black while the other half was left translucent. A hole was made in the middle to allow the introduction of a single gammarid. Phototaxis score ranged from 0 (always in the lightened side: strongly photophilic) to 20 (always in the darkened side: strongly photophobic). Following Dianne et al. (Reference Dianne, Perrot-Minnot, Bauer, Guvenatam and Rigaud2014), refuge-use was assessed using a 10.5 × 16 cm rectangular box where a refuge (half a terracotta saucer) was placed. The position of a single individual was registered every 30 s for 10 min. An individual was considered to be outside of the refuge when at least half of its body was. Refuge-use score ranged from 0 (always out of refuge) to 20 (always under the refuge). Both phototaxis and refuge use scores were expressed as a proportion of the maximum score (20). To minimize handling stress between tests, phototaxis was scored first and then refuge use, as gammarids were more easily introduced from phototaxis tube to the refuge box than the reverse.

Just after the completion of behavioural assays, we dissected each gammarid in 100 µL PBS – 0.2% Triton X100 pH 7.7 (reagents from Sigma-Aldrich, St. Quentin Fallavier, France) in an Eppendorf cap to remove the parasite, when present. After the addition of 100 µL PBS, samples were quickly frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at −80 °C for subsequent physiological assays. Empty tubes were individually weighed prior to dissection, and then weighed again before biochemical assays, in order to estimate the fresh weight of individual gammarids.

Pattern of HMP: physiological assays

All biochemical assays were performed on batches of 36 samples within five days, after randomizing the samples with respect to population and infection status. Upon biochemical assay, samples were thoroughly grinded using a ball mill (RETSCH MM 400 Mixer Mill) during two rounds of 2 min interspersed with 2 min on ice. The homogenate was then centrifuged at 9000 g at 4 °C for 15 min. Clear supernatant was collected and kept on ice to proceed to biochemical assays right away (detailed below). All dosages were conducted using a microplate spectrophotometer (Spectramax Plus384 Absorbance Microplate Reader; Molecular Devices LLC, Sunnyvale, CA, USA).

Phenoloxydase (PO) and prophenoloxydase (PPO) dosages were performed according to Cornet et al. (Reference Cornet, Franceschi, Bauer, Rigaud and Moret2009), with a few modifications, using 25 µL of supernatant for each. For PO dosage, 20 µL of filtered PBS pH 7.4 0.1 M and 120 µL of L-DOPA at 10 mm were added to the supernatant. The reaction was monitored by reading the optical density (OD) at 490 nm every 15 s for 40 min, and PO enzyme activity was quantified as the slope (Vmax value) of the curve during the linear phase of the reaction. For PPO dosage, the sample was left for 10 min in 5 µL of chymotrypsin at 5 mg mL−1 after addition of 20 µL of filtered PBS, prior to the addition of L-DOPA.

The antioxidant potential was measured using Trolox (6-hydroxy-2,5,7,8-tetramethychroman-2-carboxylic acid) equivalent antioxidant capacity (TEAC) assay, described in Re et al (Reference Re, Pellegrini, Proteggente, Pannala, Yang and Rice-Evans1999). The antioxidant potential of a sample was estimated from its capacity to quench the free radicals of an oxidized ABTS [2,29-azinobis-(3-ethylbenzothiazoline-6-sulfonic acid)] solution. The oxidized ABTS solution (ABTS+) was generated by reacting 7 mm of ABTS solution in water with 2.45 mm of potassium persulfate at obscurity and ambient temperature for at least 14 h. Just before dosage, the absorbance at 734 nm of ABTS+ solution was adjusted to 0.7 by dilution in filtered PBS. Ten microliters of supernatant or of Trolox standard solution was then mixed with 240 µL of the ABTS+ solution in microplate well, and the absorbance at 734 nm was read every minute for 20 min, at 30 °C. The range of standard Trolox concentrations (from 100 to 800 µ m) was prepared by diluting a 2.5 mm stock solution in PBS triton X100 pH 7.7, in order to get from 20 to 80% ABTS+ bleaching. TEAC was estimated by calculating the proportion of change in OD (debleaching) in 10 min. corrected by blanks, both in the samples and in Trolox standard, and by using the Trolox standard curve to derive the Trolox-equivalent antioxidant capacity of the sample in μ m. To assess global investment in both immune system and antioxidant capacity, we corrected PPO-PO and TEAC raw values by the weight of individuals, using residuals from the linear regression between PPO-PO and TEAC raw values and the weight of individuals. All reagents for the PPO-PO and TEAC assays were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich.

Total protein concentration was estimated in 5 µL of supernatant using the DC™ Protein Assay kit (Biorad) and bovine serum albumin (BSA) as standard (from 0.15 to 0.30 mg mL−1), following the manufacturer's instructions.

Analyses

All analyses were conducted using the R software (v. 3.4.3, R Development Core Team, 2018).

We first calculated effect size, in order to quantify the magnitude of behavioural alterations and physiological changes induced by parasites, and then used non-parametric Cliff's delta effect size with 95% confidence interval (CI) (effsize R-package; Torchiano, Reference Torchiano2017). An effect was considered as non-significant when its 95% confidence interval crossed zero. Negligible, small, medium and large effects correspond to an absolute value lower than 0.15, 0.33, 0.47 and higher than 0.47, respectively (Romano et al., Reference Romano, Kromrey, Coraggio, Skowronek and Devine2006).

To assess the contribution of predictor variables to variation in prevalence, behavioural and physiological traits, we used the information theoretical approach based on model comparison, as an alternative to traditional null hypothesis testing (Galipaud et al., Reference Galipaud, Gillingham, David and Dechaume-Moncharmont2014). The risk of multicollinearity among two of the predictor variables – fish biomass and river locality – was avoided, by using the most relevant to variation in the dependent variable (fish biomass for prevalence and behavioural traits, locality for physiological traits). We used general linear model (GLM) with binomial-logistic (logit) regression (lme4 R-package; Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) to analyse variation in prevalence according to environmental variables (gammarid density and total fish biomass) and gammarid size. We used β-regressions (beta-reg R-package; Cribari-Neto and Zeileis, Reference Cribari-Neto and Zeileis2010) to assess the contribution of environmental variables (fish biomass and prevalence) and individual variables (physiology and infection status), to variation in phototaxis and refuge-use (Ferrari and Cribari-Neto, Reference Ferrari and Cribari-Neto2004). Behavioural scores were transformed following Smithson and Verkuilen (Reference Smithson and Verkuilen2006) to exclude 0 and 1. We used GLMs to assess the contribution of infection status, weight and population to variation in the levels of PO-PPO, TEAC and total proteins content. In all regressions, gammarid density, total fish biomass and PPO were log-transformed and TEAC was squared-transformed to meet normality requirements.

For all regressions, we performed model selection (MuMIn R-package, functions ‘dredge’ and ‘subset’; Bartoń, Reference Bartoń2016) based on the Akaike Information Criterion (AICc) (Akaike, Reference Akaike1973). There is currently no consensus about the best cut-off criterion to select models, more specifically to balance the risk of keeping spurious models with that of excluding biologically meaningful models. Here, we proceeded to model selection using as cut-off criterion an AICc weight above 0.01, or a large gap of delta AICc (at least 2) between two consecutive models, relatively to the delta AICc of lower ranking models (M. Galipaud and F.-X. Dechaume-Moncharmont, pers.comm.). Because both quantitative and qualitative predictors were used, we did not use model averaging to calculate averaged coefficients for predictors, because these coefficients would have had no meaning for qualitative predictors (F.X. Dechaume-Moncharmont, pers.comm.). However, we used the subset of ‘best’ models to identify predictors that more likely contribute to variation in the dependent variable, based on the above-mentioned cut-off criterion.

Results

Prevalence

Prevalence of P. tereticollis cystacanth was high in the Norges river (above 10%) compared to the other three localities where it ranged from less than 1% (Vingeanne) to almost 4% (Bèze) (Fig. 2). Significant spatial segregation of infected and uninfected gammarids was observed in river Norges and river Bèze, with a higher proportion of infected individuals found on the river bank compared to the river bed (χ 2 tests: Bèze, χ 2 = 17.60, P < 0.0001; Cuisance, χ 2 = 3.15, P = 0.08; Norges, χ 2 = 17.59, P < 0.0001; Vingeanne, χ 2 = 0.002, P = 0.96) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Prevalence (percentage of cystacanth-infected gammarids) in the study localities from four rivers ordered from the highest to the lowest prevalence of P. tereticollis. Numbers above the bars correspond to the number of gammarids sampled in three (Bèze, Norges and Vingeanne) or four (Cuisance) replicates. Density is expressed as the number of gammarids per m2, and is only a semi-quantitative estimate, as the standardized protocol used reflects relative abundance among populations rather than absolute abundance. Black and grey bars correspond to bank and bed prevalence, respectively.

The initial global model to analyse variation in prevalence included total fish biomass, gammarid density, their interaction, and gammarid body size, as predictor variables. Variation in prevalence was best explained by the model including total fish biomass, gammarid density and gammarid body size (logistic regression: log-likelihood = −1152.91; d.f. = 4; AICc = 2313.80). Considering the subset of best models, local fish biomass and gammarid density appeared to play a key role in driving variation in prevalence in P. tereticollis cystacanth (see Appendix, Table S1).

Pattern of HMP: host behaviour

The magnitude of behavioural changes in infected individuals differed among the four localities (Fig. 3). Phototaxis and refuge use were strongly to moderately altered by infection in the Norges and Bèze rivers, while only phototaxis was altered in the Cuisance river. No parasite-induced behavioural change was evidenced in the Vingeanne river.

Fig. 3. Cliff's delta effect sizes (with 95% CI) for changes in behaviour induced by infection with P. tereticollis in Gammarus fossarum (phototaxis and refuge-use) for Bèze (Bèz.), Cuisance (Cui.), Norges (Nor.) and Vingeanne (Vin.) localities, respectively. The effect of infection was considered as non-significant when its 95% confidence interval crossed zero. Negligible, small, medium and large effects correspond to |d| lower than 0.15, 0.33, 0.47 and higher than 0.47, respectively (Romano et al., Reference Romano, Kromrey, Coraggio, Skowronek and Devine2006). Numbers above the error-bars correspond to the numbers of gammarids used (I, infected; U, uninfected).

The global model prior to model selection included the status of individuals (infected or uninfected), total fish biomass, their interaction, physiological parameters (total PPO-PO activity and TEAC, corrected by the weight of individuals) and prevalence, as predictor variables. Variation in phototaxis was best explained by the model including infection status, total fish biomass, and their interaction (β-regression: log-likelihood = 296.12, d.f. = 6, pseudo R 2 = 0.22, AICc = −580) (Fig. 4; Table S2a). Parasite-induced change in phototaxis was significant in the three localities with the lowest fish biomass, but not in the locality with the highest fish biomass (Figs 3 and 4). For refuge-use, the best model included infection status, total fish biomass and their interaction (β-regression: log-likelihood = 185.27, d.f. = 5, pseudo R 2 = 0.06, AICc = −360.40) (Fig. 4; Table S2b), with parasite-induced change in refuge use being significant only in two of the three localities with lower fish biomass (Fig. 4). However, effect sizes tended to be low for refuse use compared to phototaxis (Fig. 3), partly due to the overall weak use of refuge both in uninfected and infected gammarids (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4. Behavioural scores: (a) phototaxis and (b) refuge-use plotted against total fish biomass in interaction with the status of individuals (infected or uninfected). Phototaxis score ranges from photophilic (0) to photophobic (1), and refuge use score as always outside (0) to always under refuge (1). Grey and white boxes correspond to infected (Bèze, N = 59; Cuisance, N = 51; Norges, N = 46 and Vingeanne, N = 55) and uninfected (Bèze, N = 52; Cuisance, N = 63; Norges, N = 44 and Vingeanne, N = 60) individuals, respectively. Horizontal lines and vertical dashed lines correspond to median and interquartiles, respectively.

Pattern of HMP: host physiological state

Effect sizes of parasite-induced changes were medium to large for all three physiological parameters in river Bèze only. Depressed levels of PO-PPO activity, antioxidant capacity and protein concentration (all corrected for weight) were evidenced in infected individuals compared to uninfected ones from the Bèze river (Fig. 5). The effect of infection in Norges and Cuisance rivers was restricted to a moderate decrease in PO-PPO activity, while in Vingeanne river, infection moderately decreased antioxidant capacity and protein concentration (Fig. 5). Interestingly, these effects of infection status on PO-PPO activity and antioxidant capacity were no longer significant after correction with total protein content instead of gammarid weight, except for PO-PPO in the Cuisance river (Figs S2 and S3).

Fig. 5. Cliff's delta effect sizes (with 95% CI) for changes in physiological state (total (pro)phenoloxydase activity (PPO activity), antioxidant capacity (TEAC) and total protein content (Proteins), all corrected by the weight of individuals) for Bèze (B.), Cuisance (C.), Norges (N.) and Vingeanne (V.) localities, respectively. Negligible, small, medium and large effects correspond to |d| lower than 0.15, 0.33, 0.47 and higher than 0.47, respectively (Romano et al., Reference Romano, Kromrey, Coraggio, Skowronek and Devine2006). Numbers above the error-bars correspond to the numbers of gammarids used (I = infected, U = uninfected).

The initial global model for studying variation in physiological parameters included individual status (infected or uninfected), individual weight, their interaction and locality. The first-ranking model to explain variation in protein content included all four predictors (GLM: protein content: log-likelihood = −1044.40, d.f. = 8, R 2 = 0.22, AICc = 2105.10). The first-ranking model to explain variation in PPO-PO and TEAC included individual status, individual weight, and locality (log-likelihood = −369.79, d.f. = 7, R 2 = 0.18, AICc = 753.80; log-likelihood = −5608.67, d.f. = 7, R 2 = 0.14, AICc = 11 231.60, respectively) (Fig. 6; Table S3). However, the interaction between individual status and weight should not be discarded, as it appears in the subset of best models (Fig. 6; Table S3). Protein content, antioxidant potential and PPO-PO increased with gammarid weight, and were lower in infected gammarids. The difference between infected and uninfected individuals in the three physiological parameters increased with gammarid weight (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6. Physiological parameters: (a) total protein content, (b) antioxidant capacity and (c) total (pro)phenoloxidase activity, all plotted against the weight of individuals, in interaction with the status of individuals (infected or uninfected). Solid circles and lines correspond to infected individuals and empty circles and dashed lines to uninfected ones.

Discussion

The aim of our study was to address the environmental causes of variation in prevalence and host manipulation by a trophically-transmitted fish parasite, the acanthocephalan P. tereticollis. More specifically, we tested the hypothesis that high predation pressure would be associated with low cystacanth prevalence and low level of manipulation in an intermediate host, by comparing four localities with contrasted fish biomass.

We first highlighted variation in both the prevalence of P. tereticollis cystacanth and behavioural alterations in infected gammarids among the four localities. Overall, infected individuals displayed lower photophobia and refuge use than uninfected ones, as previously reported (see Cézilly et al., Reference Cézilly, Favrat and Perrot-Minnot2013; Fayard et al., Reference Fayard, Dechaume-Moncharmon, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2019), with the exception of one locality. Interestingly, the two localities with the highest level of HMP (Norges and Bèze) also exhibited a significant spatial segregation between infected and uninfected gammarids, as previously observed in another fish acanthocephalan (MacNeil et al., Reference MacNeil, Dick, Hatcher and Dunn2003). Our data on these two populations further suggest that lab estimates of HMP could match micro-habitat segregation of infected hosts in the field. However, since the density estimates provided with kick-sampling on fixed area are only semi-quantitative (Davies, Reference Davies2001), it should be emphasized that our conclusion relies on relative abundance for comparison between habitats. Further investigation should consider quantitative methods to specifically test the link between spatial segregation between infected and uninfected gammarids, and the level of HMP.

Strong HMP was associated with high cystacanth prevalence and low fish biomass (Norges river), and conversely, low and non-significant HMP was associated with low prevalence and high fish biomass (Vingeanne river). This result is in agreement with the hypothesis that the magnitude of HMP should partly match predation pressure, and should be associated with variable cystacanth prevalence. Two processes may explain the observed pattern. First, low HMP in population with high predation pressure could be the expression of an optimal parasitic strategy balancing the costs and benefits of host manipulation, independently of cystacanth age. This optimal strategy could be shaped by plastic adjustment and/or adaptive response at a local scale. Alternatively, low HMP in naturally-infected gammarids could be due to cystacanths being too young to manipulate. Indeed, in the course of behavioural switching from ‘protection’ by acanthella to ‘facilitation’ by cystacanth (Dianne et al., Reference Dianne, Perrot-Minnot, Bauer, Gaillard, Léger and Rigaud2011), young cystacanth may induce a low level of manipulation as evidenced in P. laevis infecting G. pulex (Franceschi et al., Reference Franceschi, Bauer, Bollache and Rigaud2008). Under high predation pressure, cystacanth-infected gammarids could be rapidly removed from the population, such that those present would be, on average, younger than in populations with low predation pressure, where cystacanth-infected gammarids tend to accumulate and age. Under this hypothesis, gammarids from the Vingeanne river would have been infected with younger cystacanths, and therefore displayed a lower intensity of or even no parasite-induced manipulation. Because behavioural assays were performed right after sampling, it is still possible that differences in mean parasite age among populations were responsible for variable levels of HMP. Alternatively, under the first hypothesis, selection for high or low HMP would depend on whether manipulation is costly and whether such costs are compensated by the benefits of increased probability of transmission to final hosts. Since transmission probability is depending on predation pressure, predator density or biomass is likely to be a selective force shaping the pattern of HMP. Indeed, a recent model (de Vries and van Langevelde, Reference de Vries and van Langevelde2018) showed that the selective advantage of two manipulative strategies –predation enhancement and predation suppression – depends on host density. Predation enhancement by mature parasites (i.e. infectious to final hosts) would be beneficial at low final host density, whereas at high host density, selection would rather favour predation suppression by immature parasites (i.e. not-yet infectious to final hosts; de Vries and van Langevelde, Reference de Vries and van Langevelde2018). Apart from this ecological context, energetic costs associated with HMP could limit its evolution, although evidence for such costs is still weak (Hafer-Hahmann, Reference Hafer-Hahmann2019). We cannot disentangle the two alternative hypotheses here, because the age of field-collected infected gammarids was unknown. To address this issue, a conditioning period during which parasites can grow older to reach their ‘manipulation endpoint’ could be applied, before assessing the level of HMP. In addition, the evidence for a link between predation pressure, HMP and prevalence should be considered with caution since only two populations matched the predicted pattern. The two other localities with low fish biomass presented intermediate to high levels of HMP (refuge use and phototaxis respectively) as expected, but prevalence was low (Cuisance and Bèze rivers). Therefore, stronger evidence should be sought by increasing population replicates, in order to cover a larger range of fish biomass.

One relevant question to the evolution of HMP in response to predator biomass is the mechanisms by which such fine-tuned response of parasites to variable transmission opportunities can evolve. The more parsimonious assumption would be that the increased predation risk associated with high predator biomass induces chronic stress in uninfected individuals, and thereby activate an anxiety-like state. Indeed, chronic stress might trigger a sustained apprehension of the environment and elevated vigilance, akin a ‘mood-shift’ towards anxiety-like state. Interestingly, phototaxis and refuge use have been recently associated with general anxiety-like state (Fossat et al., Reference Fossat, Bacqué-Cazenave, De Deurwaerdère, Delbecque and Cattaert2014; Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Banchetry and Cézilly2017). The parasite could thus reverse the behavioural response to predation risk by interfering with this modulatory pathway. One possible pathway involved both in anxiety-like state and parasite manipulation is the serotonergic neuromodulatory system (Tain et al., Reference Tain, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2006; Fossat et al., Reference Fossat, Bacqué-Cazenave, De Deurwaerdère, Delbecque and Cattaert2014; Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Sanchez-Thirion and Cézilly2014). A topical injection of serotonin is indeed mimicking parasite-induced behavioural alteration of phototaxis but not that of refuge use (Perrot-Minnot et al., Reference Perrot-Minnot, Sanchez-Thirion and Cézilly2014). Both lab experiments reproducing the chronic effect of parasite and predation stress, and field studies correlating the expression of anxiety-like behaviour to predator biomass across several localities, should therefore be undertaken to test these hypotheses.

We found that three populations exhibiting moderate to high HMP were also immunosuppressed. However, we did not find any effect of immunocompetence on HMP, suggesting that the intensity of HMP was independent of parasite-induced immunosuppression, in agreement with Cornet et al. (Reference Cornet, Franceschi, Bauer, Rigaud and Moret2009). Furthermore, two populations had lower antioxidative defences and protein concentration. Whereas a depressed protein content in then midgut glands of G. pulex infected with P. laevis has been previously reported (Bentley and Hurd, Reference Bentley and Hurd1995), no evidence for an effect of infection with acanthocephalans on host antioxidant capacity has been published so far. Previous studies have focused on energetic metabolism, including lipid and carbohydrate reserves (see for instance Plaistow et al., Reference Plaistow, Troussard and Cézilly2001; Caddigan et al., Reference Caddigan, Pfenning and Sparkes2017). Interestingly, total (pro)phenoloxidase activity and antioxidant capacity were no longer significantly affected by infection when corrected by individual total protein content instead of body weight. This suggests that immunosuppression and decreased antioxidant defences in infected gammarids are the consequences of a more general depression in protein content. Such depression of protein content and physiological defences likely contributes to the ecological and evolutionary cost of infection for the parasite (Cornet et al., Reference Cornet, Franceschi, Bauer, Rigaud and Moret2009). Alternatively, depressed protein content and physiological defences may be the consequences of resource reallocation to the growing parasite. The fact that depressed physiological defences arise from a general depression in protein content instead of a specific effect of infection should therefore be considered when interpreting the evolutionary significance of such changes. For instance, immunosuppression should not be considered as an adaptive parasitic strategy to increase survival by controlling host immune response, but rather as a side-effect of resource acquisition.

In conclusion, our data indicate that among population variation in the intensity of HMP in both behavioural traits could be related to predator biomass. We postulate that the activation of the modulatory pathway of stress and anxiety-like behaviours in prey by predation risk might be exploited by parasites. Therefore, to understand variation in HMP, variation in the intensity of antipredatory and anxiety-like behaviours of uninfected individuals with respect to predation risk should be considered first. Overall our results suggest that considering the ecological context, particularly prey–predator interactions, might be essential for understanding the transmission strategies of trophically-transmitted parasites.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0031182019000520.

Author ORCIDs

M. Fayard, https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7394-1635.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank an anonymous referee for suggestions on a previous version of the manuscript, and Aude Balourdet and Jean-Emmanuel Rollin for help with field sampling and lab experiments.

Financial support

This study was supported by the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), and the Université de Bourgogne-Franche Comté. MF was funded by a Ph.D. grant from the Ministère de l'Education Nationale et de la Recherche, France.

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

Not applicable.

References

Akaike, H (1973) Maximum likelihood identification of Gaussian autoregressive moving average models. Biometrika 60, 255265.Google Scholar
Bartoń, K (2016) Package “MuMIn”: Multi-Model Inference. ‘MuMIn’ R package, Version 1.15.6. Available at https://cran.rproject.org/web/packages/MuMIn/MuMIn.pdf.Google Scholar
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.Google Scholar
Bentley, CR and Hurd, H (1995) Depressed protein and copper content of the midgut gland in an intermediate host, Gammarus pulex (Crustacea), infected with cystacanths of Pomphorhynchus laevis (Acanthocephala). Journal of Invertebrate Pathology 66, 15.Google Scholar
Bollache, L, Rigaud, T and Cézilly, F (2002) Effects of two acanthocephalan parasites on the fecundity and pairing status of female Gammarus pulex (Crustacea: Amphipoda). Journal of Invertebrate Pathology 79, 102110.Google Scholar
Caddigan, SC, Pfenning, AC and Sparkes, TC (2017) Competitive growth, energy allocation, and host modification in the acanthocephalan Acanthocephalus dirus: field data. Parasitology Research 116, 199206.Google Scholar
Cézilly, F and Perrot-Minnot, MJ (2005) Studying adaptive changes in the behaviour of infected hosts: a long and winding road. Behavioural Processes 68, 223228.Google Scholar
Cézilly, F, Favrat, A and Perrot-Minnot, M-J (2013) Multidimensionality in parasite-induced phenotypic alterations: ultimate versus proximate aspects. Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 2735.Google Scholar
Cornet, S, Franceschi, N, Bauer, A, Rigaud, T and Moret, Y (2009) Immune depression induced by acanthocephalan parasites in their intermediate crustacean host: consequences for the risk of super-infection and links with host behavioural manipulation. International Journal for Parasitology 39, 221229.Google Scholar
Cribari-Neto, F and Zeileis, A (2010) Beta Regression in R. Journal of Statistical Software 34, 124.Google Scholar
Davies, A (2001) The use and limits of various methods of sampling and interpretation of benthic macroinvertebrates. Journal of Limnology 60(suppl. 1.1), 16.Google Scholar
de Vries, LJ and van Langevelde, F (2018) Two different strategies of host manipulation allow parasites to persist in intermediate–definitive host systems. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 31, 393404.Google Scholar
Dianne, L, Perrot-Minnot, M-J, Bauer, A, Gaillard, M, Léger, E and Rigaud, T (2011) Protection first then facilitation: a manipulative parasite modulates the vulnerability to predation of its intermediate host according to its own developmental stage. Evolution 65, 26922698.Google Scholar
Dianne, L, Perrot-Minnot, M-J, Bauer, A, Guvenatam, A and Rigaud, T (2014) Parasite-induced alteration of plastic response to predation threat: increased refuge use but lower food intake in Gammarus pulex infected with the acanothocephalan Pomphorhynchus laevis. International Journal for Parasitology 44, 211216.Google Scholar
Fayard, M, Dechaume-Moncharmon, F-X, Perrot-Minnot, M-J and Cézilly, F (2019) Variation in the intensity of host manipulation by parasites: a meta-analysis in Acanthocephalans. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Ferrari, S and Cribari-Neto, F (2004) Beta regression for modelling rates and proportions. Journal of Applied Statistics 31, 799815.Google Scholar
Fossat, P, Bacqué-Cazenave, J, De Deurwaerdère, P, Delbecque, JP and Cattaert, D (2014) Anxiety-like behavior in crayfish is controlled by serotonin. Science 344, 12931297.Google Scholar
Franceschi, N, Bauer, A, Bollache, L and Rigaud, T (2008) The effects of parasite age and intensity on variability in acanthocephalan-induced behavioural manipulation. International Journal for Parasitology 38, 11611170.Google Scholar
Franceschi, N, Cornet, S, Bollache, L, Dechaume-Moncharmont, FX, Bauer, A, Motreuil, S and Rigaud, T (2010) Variation between populations and local adaptation in acanthocephalan-induced parasite manipulation. Evolution 64, 24172430.Google Scholar
Galipaud, M, Gillingham, MAF, David, M and Dechaume-Moncharmont, FX (2014) Ecologists overestimate the importance of predictor variables in model averaging: a plea for cautious interpretations. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 5, 983991.Google Scholar
Galipaud, M, Bollache, L and Lagrue, C (2017) Variations in infection levels and parasite-induced mortality among sympatric cryptic lineages of native amphipods and a congeneric invasive species: are native hosts always losing? International Journal for Parasitology: Parasites and Wildlife 6, 439447.Google Scholar
Gotthard, K (2001) Growth strategies of ectothermic animals in temperate environments. In: Atkinson, D, Thorndyke, M, editors. Environment and animal development: Genes, life histories and plasticity. Oxford: BIOS. pp. 287303.Google Scholar
Hafer-Hahmann, N (2019) Experimental evolution of parasitic host manipulation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286. 20182413, doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.2413.Google Scholar
Janssens, L and Stoks, R (2013) Predation risk causes oxidative damage in prey. Biology Letters 9. 20130350, doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0350.Google Scholar
Lafferty, KD (1992) Foraging on prey that are modified by parasites. The American Naturalist 140, 854867.Google Scholar
Lafferty, KD (1999) The evolution of trophic transmission. Parasitology Today 15, 111115.Google Scholar
Lagrue, C, Kaldonski, N, Perrot-Minnot, MJ, Motreuil, S and Bollache, L (2007) Modification of hosts’ behavior by a parasite: Field evidence for adaptive manipulation. Ecology 88, 28392847.Google Scholar
MacNeil, C, Dick, JTA, Hatcher, MJ and Dunn, AM (2003) Differential drift and parasitism in invading and native Gammarus spp. (Crustacea: Amphipoda). Ecography 26, 467473.Google Scholar
Maurer, KM, Stewart, TW and Lorenz, FO (2014) Direct and indirect effects of fish on invertebrates and tiger salamanders in prairie pothole wetlands. Wetlands 34, 735745.Google Scholar
Moore, J (2002) Parasites and the Behavior of Animals. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perrot-Minnot, MJ, Kaldonski, N and Cézilly, F (2007) Increased susceptibility to predation and altered anti-predator behaviour in an acanthocephalan-infected amphipod. International Journal for Parasitology 37, 645651.Google Scholar
Perrot-Minnot, MJ, Sanchez-Thirion, K and Cézilly, F (2014) Multidimensionality in host manipulation mimicked by serotonin injection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281, 20141915.Google Scholar
Perrot-Minnot, MJ, Banchetry, L and Cézilly, F (2017) Anxiety-like behaviour increases safety from fish predation in an amphipod crustacea. Royal Society Open Science 4, 171558.Google Scholar
Perrot-Minnot, MJ, Guyonnet, E, Bollache, L and Lagrue, C (2019) Differential patterns of definitive host use by two fish acanthocephalans occurring in sympatry: Pomphorhynchus laevis and Pomphorhynchus tereticollis. International Journal for Parasitology: Parasites and Wildlife 8, 135144.Google Scholar
Plaistow, SJ, Troussard, JP and Cézilly, F (2001) The effect of the acanthocephalan parasite Pomphorhynchus laevis on the lipid and glycogen content of its intermediate host Gammarus pulex. International Journal for Parasitology 31, 346351.Google Scholar
Poulin, R (2010) Parasite Manipulation of Host Behavior: An Update and Frequently Asked Questions. Advances in the Study of Behavior 41, 151186.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2018) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for statistical computing. URL: http://www.R-project.org.Google Scholar
Re, R, Pellegrini, N, Proteggente, A, Pannala, A, Yang, M and Rice-Evans, C (1999) Antioxidant activity applying an improved ABTS radical cation decolorization assay. Free radical in biology and medicine 26, 12311237.Google Scholar
Rigaud, T and Moret, Y (2003) Differential phenoloxidase activity between native and invasive gammarids infected by local acanthocephalans: differential immunosuppression? Parasitology 127, 571577.Google Scholar
Romano, J, Kromrey, JD, Coraggio, J, Skowronek, J and Devine, L (2006) Exploring methods for evaluating group differences on the NSSE and other surveys: Are the t-test and Cohen's d indices the most appropriate choices? In annual meeting of the Southern Association for Institutional Research, 14–17 October 2006, Arlington, VA, USA.Google Scholar
Seppälä, O and Jokela, J (2008) Host manipulation as a parasite transmission strategy when manipulation is exploited by non-host predators. Biology Letters 4, 663666.Google Scholar
Slos, S and Stoks, R (2008) Predation risk induces stress proteins and reduces antioxidant defense. Functional Ecology 22, 637642.Google Scholar
Smithson, M and Verkuilen, J (2006) A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychological Methods 11, 5471.Google Scholar
Tain, L, Perrot-Minnot, MJ and Cézilly, F (2006) Altered host behaviour and brain serotonergic activity caused by acanthocephalans: evidence for specificity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273, 30393045.Google Scholar
Thomas, F, Adamo, S and Moore, J (2005) Parasitic manipulation: where are we and where should we go? Behavioural Processes 68, 185199.Google Scholar
Thomas, F, Brodeur, J, Maure, F, Franceschi, N, Blanchet, S and Rigaud, T (2011) Intraspecific variability in host manipulation by parasites. Infection, Genetics and Evolution 11, 262269.Google Scholar
Thomas, F, Rigaud, T and Brodeur, J (2012) Evolutionary routes leading to host manipulation by parasites. In Host Manipulation by Parasites. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 1635.Google Scholar
Torchiano, M (2017) effsize: Efficient Effect Size Computation. R package version 0.7.1.Google Scholar
Turner, AM and Trexler, JC (1997) Sampling aquatic invertebrates from marshes: evaluating the options. Journal of the North American Benthological Society 16, 694709.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Predicted consequences of local fish biomass on the level of host manipulation by parasite (HMP) through predation rate. Transmission opportunities for trophically transmitted fish parasite are assumed to increase with fish biomass when most fish predators are suitable as definitive hosts. Arrow thickness represents the impact of overall predation rate (independently of infection status) and of infected prey vulnerability to predation relative to uninfected ones (HMP); their relative impacts modulate the observed prevalence of infective parasite stages.

Figure 1

Table 1. Description of the localities with coordinates, temperature, descriptors of local fish assemblage structure, intermediate hosts (gammarid) density, and prevalence of P. tereticollis cystacanths in gammarids

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Prevalence (percentage of cystacanth-infected gammarids) in the study localities from four rivers ordered from the highest to the lowest prevalence of P. tereticollis. Numbers above the bars correspond to the number of gammarids sampled in three (Bèze, Norges and Vingeanne) or four (Cuisance) replicates. Density is expressed as the number of gammarids per m2, and is only a semi-quantitative estimate, as the standardized protocol used reflects relative abundance among populations rather than absolute abundance. Black and grey bars correspond to bank and bed prevalence, respectively.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Cliff's delta effect sizes (with 95% CI) for changes in behaviour induced by infection with P. tereticollis in Gammarus fossarum (phototaxis and refuge-use) for Bèze (Bèz.), Cuisance (Cui.), Norges (Nor.) and Vingeanne (Vin.) localities, respectively. The effect of infection was considered as non-significant when its 95% confidence interval crossed zero. Negligible, small, medium and large effects correspond to |d| lower than 0.15, 0.33, 0.47 and higher than 0.47, respectively (Romano et al., 2006). Numbers above the error-bars correspond to the numbers of gammarids used (I, infected; U, uninfected).

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Behavioural scores: (a) phototaxis and (b) refuge-use plotted against total fish biomass in interaction with the status of individuals (infected or uninfected). Phototaxis score ranges from photophilic (0) to photophobic (1), and refuge use score as always outside (0) to always under refuge (1). Grey and white boxes correspond to infected (Bèze, N = 59; Cuisance, N = 51; Norges, N = 46 and Vingeanne, N = 55) and uninfected (Bèze, N = 52; Cuisance, N = 63; Norges, N = 44 and Vingeanne, N = 60) individuals, respectively. Horizontal lines and vertical dashed lines correspond to median and interquartiles, respectively.

Figure 5

Fig. 5. Cliff's delta effect sizes (with 95% CI) for changes in physiological state (total (pro)phenoloxydase activity (PPO activity), antioxidant capacity (TEAC) and total protein content (Proteins), all corrected by the weight of individuals) for Bèze (B.), Cuisance (C.), Norges (N.) and Vingeanne (V.) localities, respectively. Negligible, small, medium and large effects correspond to |d| lower than 0.15, 0.33, 0.47 and higher than 0.47, respectively (Romano et al., 2006). Numbers above the error-bars correspond to the numbers of gammarids used (I = infected, U = uninfected).

Figure 6

Fig. 6. Physiological parameters: (a) total protein content, (b) antioxidant capacity and (c) total (pro)phenoloxidase activity, all plotted against the weight of individuals, in interaction with the status of individuals (infected or uninfected). Solid circles and lines correspond to infected individuals and empty circles and dashed lines to uninfected ones.

Supplementary material: File

Fayard et al. supplementary material

Figures S1-S3 and Tables S1-S3

Download Fayard et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.2 MB