Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T12:13:50.408Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infection with acanthocephalans increases the vulnerability of Gammarus pulex (Crustacea, Amphipoda) to non-host invertebrate predators

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2008

N. KALDONSKI
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéocsciences, Dijon, France
M.-J. PERROT-MINNOT
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéocsciences, Dijon, France
S. MOTREUIL
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéocsciences, Dijon, France
F. CÉZILLY*
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéocsciences, Dijon, France
*
*Corresponding author: Equipe Ecologie Evolutive, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéosciences, Université de Bourgogne, 6 Boulevard Gabriel, 21000 Dijon, France. E-mail: frank.cezilly@u-bourgogne.fr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Phenotypic alterations induced by parasites in their intermediate hosts often result in enhanced trophic transmission to appropriate final hosts. However, such alterations may also increase the vulnerability of intermediate hosts to predation by non-host species. We studied the influence of both infection with 3 different acanthocephalan parasites (Pomphorhynchus laevis, P. tereticollis, and Polymorphus minutus) and the availability of refuges on the susceptibility of the amphipod Gammarus pulex to predation by 2 non-host predators in microcosms. Only infection with P. laevis increased the vulnerability of amphipods to predation by crayfish, Orconectes limosus. In contrast, in the absence of refuges, the selectivity of water scorpions, Nepa cinerea, for infected prey was significant and did not differ according to parasite species. When a refuge was available for infected prey, however, water scorpion selectivity for infected prey differed between parasite species. Both P. tereticollis- and P. laevis-infected gammarids were more vulnerable than uninfected ones, whereas the reverse was true of P. minutus-infected gammarids. These results suggest that the true consequences of phenotypic changes associated with parasitic infection in terms of increased trophic transmission of parasites deserve further assessment.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

INTRODUCTION

Several parasites critically rely on trophic transmission from an intermediate host to a final host to complete their complex life-cycle (Moore, Reference Moore2002). Any influence of the parasite on its intermediate host phenotype that results in increased trophic transmission to an appropriate final host is thus thought to be favoured by natural selection (Dawkins, Reference Dawkins1982; Poulin, Reference Poulin1995). Accordingly, several helminth species have been shown to bring about phenotypic changes in their intermediate hosts that appear to enhance trophic transmission to final hosts (Moore, Reference Moore2002; Thomas et al. Reference Thomas, Adamo and Moore2005). However, it is not clear whether such alterations in host phenotype specifically enhance transmission to appropriate final hosts, or actually increase the susceptibility of intermediate hosts to a large range of predators, including non-host species (Cézilly and Perrot-Minnot, Reference Cézilly and Perrot-Minnot2005). Mouritsen and Poulin (Reference Mouritsen and Poulin2003) observed that the intertidal cockle Austrovenus stutchburyi infected with the bird trematode Curtuteria australis was actually exposed to a high risk of attack by a benthic feeding fish, Notolabrus celidotus. Quantitative estimations indicated that only 2·5% of the parasite population in manipulated cockles was transmitted to appropriate bird final hosts, whereas 17·1% was lost to fish (Mouritsen and Poulin, Reference Mouritsen and Poulin2003). Similarly, Ness and Foster (Reference Ness and Foster1999) observed that three-spine sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus, parasitized by the bird tapeworm Schistocephalus solidus, showed increased susceptibility to visually hunting predators, appropriate hosts and non-hosts (e.g. trout) alike. By contrast, Seppälä et al. (Reference Seppälä, Karvonen and Valtonen2006a) found that uninfected rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) and trout infected with the eye fluke Diplostomum spathaceum, did not differ in their susceptibility to predation by a non-host species, the pike, Esox lucius. However, experimental evidence of increased susceptibility of eye fluke-infected fish to predation by appropriate final hosts (e.g. birds) remains ambiguous (Seppälä et al. Reference Seppälä, Karvonen and Valtonen2006b). Cost of predation by non-hosts might be reduced if a fraction of non-host predators are themselves preyed upon by appropriate final hosts (Nickol et al. Reference Nickol, Fuller and Rock2006). Parasites may eventually survive in such paratenic hosts, although the importance of paratenic hosts in the evolution of complex life-cycles remains unclear (Choisy et al. Reference Choisy, Brown, Lafferty and Thomas2003). However, it is likely that in most cases predation of the intermediate host by a non-host species will constitute a dead end for the parasite (Marriott et al. Reference Marriott, Collins, Paris, Gudgin, Barnard, Mcgregor, Gilbert, Hartley and Behnke1989; Mouritsen and Poulin, Reference Mouritsen and Poulin2003). This is particularly true of non-host predators that do not swallow their prey, but feed on them through sucking their body fluids, such as some heteropteran insects, or pull them into pieces such as some crustaceans.

Most previous studies of parasite-induced trophic transmission (i.e. Lafferty and Morris, Reference Lafferty and Morris1996; Bakker et al. Reference Bakker, Mazzi and Zala1997; Perrot-Minnot et al. Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007) relied on a measure of selective predation on infected hosts by appropriate vertebrate hosts, but neglected to assess the vulnerability of infected prey to predation by non-hosts (see, however, Ness and Foster, Reference Ness and Foster1999; Mouritsen and Poulin, Reference Mouritsen and Poulin2003; Seppälä et al. Reference Seppälä, Karvonen and Valtonen2006b; Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007). Amphipods are key elements in the functioning of aquatic ecosystems both as detritivores and predators of other invertebrate species (Dick and Platvoet, Reference Dick and Platvoet1996; Maltby et al. Reference Maltby, Clayton, Wood and McLoughlin2002; Elliott, Reference Elliott2005), and as prey to a wide range of predators (Redmond and Scott, Reference Redmond and Scott1989; Corona et al. Reference Corona, Soto and Sanchez2000; Wilhelm et al. Reference Wilhelm, Hamann and Burns2002; Griffen and Byers, Reference Griffen and Byers2006). They are also used by several trematode, cestode and acanthocephalan parasites as intermediate hosts before reaching birds or fish final hosts. Although the phenotypic alterations brought about by parasites on amphipods appear to be consistent with their respective type of final host (Cézilly et al. Reference Cézilly, Grégoire and Bertin2000; Perrot-Minnot et al. Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007; Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007), little is known about their consequences on susceptibility to predation by other, non-host predators. In particular, the effects of infection on vulnerability to predation by aquatic invertebrates remain undocumented.

In this study, we examine the influence of infection with 3 different acanthocephalan parasites on the susceptibility of the amphipod Gammarus pulex to predation by 2, non-host aquatic invertebrate predators differing in feeding behaviour and habitat-use. Mouritsen and Poulin (Reference Mouritsen and Poulin2003) provided evidence that infection with manipulative parasites can increase the vulnerability of intermediate hosts to predation by non-hosts in the field. Here, using experiments in microcosms, we test the hypothesis that phenotypic changes induced by acanthocephalan parasites increase the susceptibility of amphipod hosts to predation by non-host predators.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study species

Water scorpion, Nepa cinerea, and crayfish Orconectes limosus were used as experimental predators. Water scorpions are predominantly surface predators, whereas crayfish are more benthic. Aquatic Hemiptera such as water scorpions, generally hold an intermediate place in food chains, and are often important predators (McCafferty, Reference McCafferty1981; Ohba and Nakasuji, Reference Ohba and Nakasuji2006). For example, in a study of a fishless spring in Arizona, the heteropteran Belastoma bakeri ate more prey (mostly amphipods) than any other top predator (Runck and Blinn, Reference Runck and Blinn1994). Crayfish can have an important predatory impact on aquatic macroinvertebrates (Crawford et al. Reference Crawford, Yeomans and Adams2006), particularly on amphipods (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Miner and Lowe1998; MacNeil et al. Reference MacNeil, Dick and Elwood1999).

Water scorpions had an average length (from tip of rostrum to tail basis) of 18·76 mm±1·9 mm (n=95) and crayfish had an average length (from tip of rostrum to distal end of telson) of 61·8 mm ±0·7 mm (n=99). Both were collected in the same place using the random kick sampling method (Hynes, Reference Hynes1954). These predators were kept separately and fed with gammarids, but were starved for 24 h prior to the experiments.

In Burgundy, eastern France, the amphipod Gammarus pulex is exploited as intermediate host by 3 acanthocephalan species. Pomphorhynchus laevis and the congeneric Pomphorhynchus tereticollis are fish parasites that modify reaction to both light (Cézilly et al. Reference Cézilly, Grégoire and Bertin2000; Tain et al. Reference Tain, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2006) and fish predator's odour in G. pulex (Baldauf et al. Reference Baldauf, Thünken, Frommen, Bakker, Heupzl and Kullmann2007; Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007; Perrot-Minnot et al. Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007). Polymorphus minutus is a bird acanthocephalan that modifies geotaxis in G. pulex, but has only weak or no effect on reaction to light or fish odour (Cézilly et al. Reference Cézilly, Grégoire and Bertin2000; Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007). All G. pulex used in the experiments (uninfected, P. laevis-, P. tereticollis- and P. minutus-infected) were collected using the kick sampling method (Hynes, Reference Hynes1954) in the River Ouche in Dijon (Eastern France), during the summer of 2004 (from July to September). There, the density of water scorpions was approximately 5 individuals per linear metre along the bank of the river, whereas the density of crayfish ranged between 1 and 4 individuals per square metre. Only healthy and undamaged adults were kept for the experiments, within sizes ranging from 11 to 15 mm long. Gammarids were maintained in large, separated, well-aerated tanks, filled with dechlorinated tap water and fed with elm leaves and pellet fish food. They were acclimatized to room and water conditions for at least 1 week before the experiments.

Predation experiments

Experiments were conducted in a climate room with a constant temperature of 15°C and a light:dark cycle of 12:12 h. Observation tanks (35×15×20 cm) were filled with 7 litres of dechlorinated tap water, oxygenated by an air-stone and illuminated overhead by solar spectra fluorescent tubes (light T°=9600°K). Aquarium sides were screened with brown plastic to avoid any lateral disturbance. Experiments were conducted in either a simple or a complex microcosm habitat set-up. The simple habitat set-up consisted of washed river sand substrate whereas the complex habitat set-up was composed of the same sand substrate plus a piece of air-brick (21·5×10×5 cm, designed with eight 1·5 cm2 holes) sunk into the sand and 2 Apiacea plants where gammarids could find refuge.

To assess the selectivity of predators for uninfected or infected gammarids, predation tests were performed in paired choice experiments involving a 30% proportion of infected gammarids, slightly above natural prevalence of parasites in the field (Lagrue et al. Reference Lagrue, Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot, Motreuil and Bollache2007). Each experimental test consisted of recording the relative numbers of infected and non-infected prey captured by a single predator during a given amount of time. Forty-two uninfected gammarids and 18 infected ones (parasitized by a single acanthocephalan species) were first introduced into the aquarium. After 15 min, 1 individual predator was introduced in the aquarium. The time of exposure of prey to different predators was adjusted to prevent too great a proportion of prey (more than 50%) being eaten (Fullick and Greenwood, Reference Fullick and Greenwood1979). Owing to preliminary experiments, the duration of each trial was set at 3·5 h for crayfish and 72 h for water scorpion. At the end of each trial, predators and remaining prey were removed, surviving gammarids counted, and all material rinsed well with tap water. Only trials where more than 6 prey were eaten were kept for analysis.

Because it was not possible to correctly identify which prey types were captured through direct observation, absolute as well as relative prey densities could not be kept constant over the course of predation trials. Differential of predation between the two prey types was therefore analysed by Manly's alpha probabilistic approach (Manly, Reference Manly1974; Chesson, Reference Chesson1978). This method allows for prey depletion during the course of the trial, and thus for changes in the proportions of available prey classes as prey are eaten. Manly's alpha (αi) for variable prey population is calculated using the equation:

\alpha _{i} \equals {{\log p_{i} } \over {\sum\nolimits_{j \equals \setnum{1}}^{m} {\log p_{j} } }}

where αi=Manly's alpha (preference index) for prey type I, p i and p j=proportion of prey i or j remaining at the end of the trial, and m=number of prey types. Manly's selectivity index ranges from 0 (when only uninfected prey are eaten) to 1 (when only infected prey are eaten), with a value of 0·5 for absence of preference.

Preference index data were transformed using a Box-Cox function to meet the assumptions of parametric tests. The preference index for each group was tested using a t-test to a specified value (Sokal and Rohlf, Reference Sokal and Rohlf1995) to check whether predator significantly preyed upon infected prey. The influence of infection type, refuge availability, and interaction term were analysed using a two-way ANOVA and pair-wise differences were evaluated with Fisher's PLSD. All tests were performed using the Statistica statistical software (v. 6.0, Statsoft Inc.).

RESULTS

A total of 2388 gammarids were eaten, 1127 by the 99 different crayfish and 1261 by the 95 different water scorpions. Crayfish ate fewer gammarids (mean±S.D.=11·38±4·70) than did water scorpions (mean± S.D.=13·27±5·93) per trial (Mann-Whitney test, Z=−2·07, n WS=95, n C=99, P=0·04). However, for both series of tests, the number of prey eaten had no influence on selectivity of predator toward infected preys, neither for crayfish (Pearson correlation, r<0·001, P=0·84) nor for water scorpions (r=0·003, P=0·58).

Crayfish selectivity

Refuge availability had no effect on crayfish selectivity, irrespective of parasite species (Table 1; Fig. 1a). Crayfish selectivity for infected prey varied, however, significantly between groups (two-way ANOVA: F5,93=7·52, P<0·0001; Fig. 1a). Crayfish selectivity for P. laevis-infected prey was highly significant (with refuge: t 1·14=3·44, P=0·004; without refuge: t 1·16=8·96, P<0·001; Fig. 1a), and differed significantly from selectivity for P. tereticollis-infected (Fisher's PLSD: P<0·001; Fig. 1a) or P. minutus-infected prey (Fisher's PLSD: P<0·001; Fig. 1a). By contrast, crayfish showed no selectivity for either P. minutus- (with refuge: t 1·16=−1·86, P=0·081; without refuge: t 1·16=−1·32, P=0·207) or P. tereticollis-infected prey (with refuge: t 1·17=0·29, P=0·726; without refuge: t 1·14=−0·24, P=0·816; Fig. 1a). No difference was observed between selectivity for P. minutus-infected prey and selectivity for P. tereticollis-infected ones (Fisher's PLSD: P>0·05; Fig. 1a).

Fig. 1. Mean (±95% confidence interval) of preference index showing selectivity of (a) crayfish and (b) water scorpions toward uninfected versus gammarids infected by 3 different parasite species, either in a complex microcosm type setting (dark bars) or in a simple one (light bars). Values above the dashed line indicate over consumption of infected prey. Stars above the bars indicate a significant difference from equal susceptibility of infected and infected prey and bars not connected by the same letter are significantly different.

Table 1. Results of a two-way ANOVA testing for differences in mean selectivity of crayfish in relation to parasite species and refuge availability

Water scorpion selectivity

Water scorpion selectivity for infected prey differed between trials (two-way ANOVA: F5,89=15·15, P<0·001; Fig. 1b). A significant interaction between parasite species and refuge availability indicates that infected prey differed in their vulnerability to predation according to microcosm type (Table 2; Fig. 1b). When no refuge was available for prey, water scorpions' selectivity for infected prey was significant (t 1·14=6·66, 4·80, and 6·00 for P. laevis, P. tereticollis and P. minutus respectively, P<0·001 in all cases; Fig. 1b) and did not differ according to parasite species (Fisher's PLSD: P>0·05; Fig. 1b). When a refuge was available for infected prey, however, water scorpion selectivity for infected prey differed markedly between parasite species (Fig. 1b). Both P. tereticollis-infected gammarids (t 1·18=10·74, P<0·001) and P. laevis-infected ones (t 1·14=3·13, P=0·007) were more predated than uninfected individuals, whereas the reverse was true of P. minutus-infected gammarids (t 1·15=−2·74, P=0·015; Fig. 1b).

Table 2. Results of a two-way ANOVA test for differences in mean selectivity of water scorpions in relation to parasite species and refuge availability

DISCUSSION

Our results provide, for the first time, empirical evidence that infection with acanthocephalan parasites can increase the vulnerability of amphipods to non-host predators. However, the magnitude of the effect may depend on complex interactions between parasite species, the physical complexity of the environment, and predator species (see Holmlund et al. Reference Holmlund, Peterson and Hay1990; Corona et al. Reference Corona, Soto and Sanchez2000). In particular, the observed differences between predators in selectivity for infected prey remain difficult to interpret. Water-scorpions, being poor swimmers, stay motionless at the water's surface for long periods of time, and capture prey using the ambush method, whereas crayfish are more benthic. Differences in hunting methods between the two predators might then contribute to explain the observed differences in selectivity. However, the vulnerability of uninfected and infected gammarids to predation by crayfish or water-scorpions is also dependent on their ability to modify their behaviour in response to the perceived risk of predation.

Overall, refuge availability had no influence on the vulnerability of infected gammarids to crayfish. Uninfected G. pulex are known to increase their use of refuge when in the presence of a fish predator (Perrot-Minnot et al. Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007; Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007), but the effect of crayfish on refuge use by G. pulex has not been quantified so far. However, it has been previously observed that uninfected G. pulex decrease their activity when exposed to chemicals from predatory fish, but do not react to chemicals from the crayfish, Pacifascatus leniusculus (Åbjörnsson et al. Reference Åbjörnsson, Dahl, Nyström and Brönmark2000). It is therefore possible that both infected and uninfected gammarids did not react to the presence of crayfish, thus explaining the absence of an effect of refuge availability on selectivity for infected prey. Actually, only gammarids infected by P. laevis showed an increased vulnerability to predation by crayfish compared to uninfected individuals. It has been shown that amphipods infected with P. laevis are more active than uninfected ones (Dezfuli et al. Reference Dezfuli, Maynard and Wellnitz2003), thus making them potentially more exposed to sit and await predators such as crayfish. However, no data on activity levels are available for P. tereticollis- and P. minutus-infected amphipods.

Infection with either P. laevis or P. tereticollis had a highly significant effect on vulnerability of infected prey to predation by water scorpions, independently of refuge availability. In contrast, P. minutus-infected individuals were less vulnerable than uninfected ones to predation by water scorpions only in the presence of refuges, whereas the reverse was true in the absence of refuges. Data from previous studies (Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007; Perrot-Minnot et al. Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007) suggest that P. minutus-infected G. pulex spend more time in refuges, independently of predator presence, compared to P. laevis- and P. tereticollis-infected individuals. This may explain the difference between host-parasite associations in vulnerability to predation by water scorpions in relation to the presence/absence of refuges, although other factors may have contributed to the observed pattern of predation.

Parasite-induced trophic transmission to final hosts is supported by substantial empirical evidence (Moore, Reference Moore2002; Thomas et al. Reference Thomas, Adamo and Moore2005). However, evidence for specificity in manipulation remains equivocal, such that the importance of parasitic manipulation for the population dynamics of both predators and prey remains unclear. Fitness consequences of parasite-induced behavioural alterations are likely to be complex (Vance and Peckarsky, Reference Vance and Peckarsky1997). On the one hand, increased vulnerability of infected prey is supposed to benefit both the parasite and its final host (Lafferty, Reference Lafferty1992; Bakker et al. Reference Bakker, Mazzi and Zala1997). Final hosts may benefit from feeding and specializing on infected prey if the energy gain associated with the capture of easy prey outweighs the costs associated with the risk of transmission of the parasite from prey to predator (Lafferty, Reference Lafferty1992). Even if manipulation does incur a cost to parasites (see Poulin et al. Reference Poulin, Fredensborg, Hansen and Leung2005), this cost might be largely compensated for by the benefit of enhanced transmission to final hosts. On the other hand, when prey capture is facilitated by parasites and risk of transmission of the parasite from prey to predator is non-existent (as in this study), a large range of predators may benefit from selecting infected prey (Lafferty, Reference Lafferty1992), including many non-host species. Acanthocephalan-infected gammarids have previously been shown to be more susceptible than uninfected ones to predation by appropriate final hosts (birds: Bethel and Holmes, Reference Bethel and Holmes1977; fish: Bakker et al. Reference Bakker, Mazzi and Zala1997, Perrot-Minnot et al. Reference Perrot-Minnot, Kaldonski and Cézilly2007, Kaldonski et al. Reference Kaldonski, Perrot-Minnot and Cézilly2007). However, the present results suggest that infection with acanthocephalans might also increase vulnerability to non-host predators, such that the exact balance between benefits and costs of manipulation remains to be evaluated properly. One possibility is that the cost of non-host predation is limited because the overall impact of predation on intermediate hosts by appropriate final hosts is much higher than that by non-host predators. In addition, in the present experiment, each predator was tested in isolation, whereas interactions between predators may affect their selectivity in the wild (Nilsson et al. Reference Nilsson, Nilsson and Nystrom2000) Future work should then address this important question through combining long-term experiments in mesocosms and modelling (see Duffy et al. Reference Duffy, Hall, Tessier and Huebner2005; Fenton and Rands, Reference Fenton and Rands2006).

We thank two anonymous referees for useful comments, and Emilie Guyonnet, Mélanie Jouhanneau, Stéphanie Lecuelle, Arnaud Germond, Romain Gueirrero and Thomas Régnier for help with field sampling and lab work. N.K. was funded by a doctoral grant from the MENRT.

References

REFERENCES

Åbjörnsson, K., Dahl, J., Nyström, P. and Brönmark, C. (2000). Influence of predator and dietary chemical cues on the behaviour and shredding efficiency of Gammarus pulex. Aquatic Ecology 34, 379387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakker, T. C. M., Mazzi, D. and Zala, S. (1997). Parasite-induced changes in behavior and color make Gammarus pulex more prone to fish predation. Ecology 78, 10981104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldauf, S. A., Thünken, T., Frommen, J. G., Bakker, T. C. M., Heupzl, O. and Kullmann, H. (2007). Infection with an acanthocephalan manipulates an amphipod's reaction to a fish predator's odour. International Journal for Parasitology 37, 6165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bethel, W. M. and Holmes, J. C. (1977). Increased vulnerability of amphipods to predation owing to altered behavior induced by larval acanthocephalans. Canadian Journal of Zoology 55, 110115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cézilly, F., Grégoire, A. and Bertin, A. (2000). Conflict between co-occurring manipulative parasites? An experimental study of the joint influence of two acanthocephalan parasites on the behaviour of Gammarus pulex. Parasitology 120, 625630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cézilly, F. and Perrot-Minnot, M. J. (2005). Studying adaptive changes in the behaviour of infected hosts: a long and winding road. Behavioural Processes 68, 223228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chesson, J. (1978). Measuring preference in selective predation. Ecology 59, 211215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choisy, M., Brown, S. P., Lafferty, K. D. and Thomas, F. (2003). Evolution of trophic transmission in parasites: Why add intermediate hosts? American Naturalist 162, 172181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corona, A., Soto, L. A. and Sanchez, A. J. (2000). Epibenthic amphipod abundance and predation efficiency of the pink shrimp Farfantepenaeus duorarum (Burkenroad, 1939) in habitats with different physical complexity in a tropical estuarine system. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 253, 3348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, L., Yeomans, W. E. and Adams, C. E. (2006). The impact of introduced signal crayfish Pacifastacus leniusculus on stream invertebrate communities. Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems 16, 611621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Dezfuli, B. S., Maynard, B. J. and Wellnitz, T. A. (2003). Activity levels and predator detection by amphipods infected with an acanthocephalan parasite, Pomphorhynchus laevis. Folia Parasitologica 50, 129134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, J. T. A. and Platvoet, D. (1996). Intraguild predation and species exclusions in amphipods: The interaction of behaviour, physiology and environment. Freshwater Biology 36, 375383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, M. A., Hall, S. R., Tessier, A. J. and Huebner, M. (2005). Selective predators and their parasitized prey: Are epidemics in zooplankton under top-down control? Limnology and Oceanography 50, 412420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, J. M. (2005). Day-night changes in the spatial distribution and habitat preferences of freshwater shrimps, Gammarus pulex, in a stony stream. Freshwater Biology 50, 552566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenton, A. and Rands, S. A. (2006). The impact of parasite manipulation and predator foraging behavior on predator-prey communities. Ecology 87, 28322841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fullick, T. G. and Greenwood, J. J. D. (1979). Frequency-dependent food selection in relation to 2 models. American Naturalist 113, 762765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffen, B. D. and Byers, J. E. (2006). Intraguild predation reduces redundancy of predator species in multiple predator assemblage. Journal of Animal Ecology 75, 959966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmlund, M. B., Peterson, C. H. and Hay, M. E. (1990). Does algal morphology affect amphipod susceptibility to fish predation. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 139, 6583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hynes, H. B. N. (1954). The ecology of Gammarus duebeni lilljeborg and its occurence in fresh water in western Britain. Journal of Animal Ecology 23, 3884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaldonski, N., Perrot-Minnot, M. J. and Cézilly, F. (2007). Differential influence of two acanthocephalan parasites on the anti-predator behaviour of their common intermediate host. Animal Behaviour 74, 13111317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lafferty, K. D. (1992). Foraging on prey that are modified by parasites. American Naturalist 140, 854867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lafferty, K. D. and Morris, A. K. (1996). Altered behavior of parasitized killifish increases susceptibility to predation by bird final hosts. Ecology 77, 13901397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagrue, C., Kaldonski, N., Perrot-Minnot, M. J., Motreuil, S. and Bollache, L. (2007). Modification of hosts' behavior by a parasite: field evidence for adaptive manipulation. Ecology 88, 28392847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNeil, C., Dick, J. T. A. and Elwood, R. W. (1999). The dynamics of predation on Gammarus spp. (Crustacea: Amphipoda). Biological Reviews 74, 375395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maltby, L., Clayton, S. A., Wood, R. M. and McLoughlin, N. (2002). Evaluation of the Gammarus pulex in situ feeding assay as a biomonitor of water quality: Robustness, responsiveness, and relevance. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry 21, 361368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, B. F. J. (1974). A model for certain types of selection experiments. Biometrics 30, 281294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marriott, D. R., Collins, M. L., Paris, R. M., Gudgin, D. R., Barnard, C. J., Mcgregor, P. K., Gilbert, F. S., Hartley, J. C. and Behnke, J. M. (1989). Behavioral modifications and increased predation risk of Gammarus pulex infected with Polymorphus minutus. Journal of Biological Education 23, 135141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCafferty, W. (1981). Aquatic Entomology. Science Books International, Boston.Google Scholar
Moore, J. (2002). Parasites and the Behavior of Animals, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Mouritsen, K. N. and Poulin, R. (2003). Parasite-induced trophic facilitation exploited by a non-host predator: a manipulator's nightmare. International Journal for Parasitology 33, 10431050.Google Scholar
Ness, J. H. and Foster, S. A. (1999). Parasite-associated phenotype modifications in threespine stickleback. Oikos 85, 127134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickol, B. B., Fuller, C. A. and Rock, P. (2006). Cystacanths of Oncicola venezuelensis (Acanthocephala: Oligacanthorhynchidae) in Caribbean termites and various paratenic hosts in the US Virgin Islands. Journal of Parasitology 92, 539542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, P. A., Nilsson, K. and Nystrom, P. (2000). Does risk of intraspecific interactions induce shifts in prey-size preference in aquatic predators? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48, 268275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohba, S. and Nakasuji, F. (2006). Dietary items of predacious aquatic bugs (Nepoidea: Heteroptera) in Japanese wetlands. Limnology 7, 4143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrot-Minnot, M.-J., Kaldonski, N. and Cézilly, F. (2007). Increased susceptibility to predation and altered anti-predator behaviour in an acanthocephalan-infected host. International Journal for Parasitology 37, 645651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin, R. (1995). “Adaptive” changes in the behaviour of parasitized animals: a critical review. International Journal for Parasitology 25, 13711383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin, R., Fredensborg, B. L., Hansen, E. and Leung, T. L. F. (2005). The true cost of host manipulation by parasites. Behavioural Processes 68, 241244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redmond, M. S. and Scott, K. J. (1989). Amphipod predation by the infaunal polychaete, Nephtys incisa. Estuaries 12, 205207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runck, C. and Blinn, D. W. (1994). Role of Belostoma bakeri (Heteroptera) in the trophic ecology of a fishless desert spring. Limnology and Oceanography 39, 18001812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seppälä, O., Karvonen, A. and Valtonen, E. T. (2006 a). Host manipulation by parasites and risk of non-host predation: is manipulation costly in an eye fluke-fish interaction? Evolutionary Ecology Research 8, 871879.Google Scholar
Seppälä, O., Karvonen, A. and Valtonen, E. T. (2006 b). Susceptibility of eye fluke-infected fish to predation by bird hosts. Parasitology 132, 575579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokal, E. R. and Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry, 3rd Edn.W.H. Freeman, New-York.Google Scholar
Stewart, T. W., Miner, J. G. and Lowe, R. L. (1998). An experimental analysis of crayfish (Orconectes rusticus) effects on a Dreissena-dominated benthic macroinvertebrate community in western Lake Erie. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 55, 10431050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tain, L., Perrot-Minnot, M.-J. and Cézilly, F. (2006). Altered host behaviour and brain serotonergic activity caused by acanthocephalans: evidence for specificity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 273, 30393045.Google ScholarPubMed
Thomas, F., Adamo, S. and Moore, J. (2005). Parasitic manipulation: where are we and where should we go? Behavioural Processes 68, 185199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vance, S. A. and Peckarsky, B. L. (1997). The effect of mermithid parasitism on predation of nymphal Baetis bicaudatus (Ephemeroptera) by invertebrates. Oecologia 110, 147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilhelm, F. M., Hamann, J. and Burns, C. W. (2002). Mysid predation on amphipods and Daphnia in a shallow coastal lake: prey selection and effects of macrophytes. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 59, 19011907.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Mean (±95% confidence interval) of preference index showing selectivity of (a) crayfish and (b) water scorpions toward uninfected versus gammarids infected by 3 different parasite species, either in a complex microcosm type setting (dark bars) or in a simple one (light bars). Values above the dashed line indicate over consumption of infected prey. Stars above the bars indicate a significant difference from equal susceptibility of infected and infected prey and bars not connected by the same letter are significantly different.

Figure 1

Table 1. Results of a two-way ANOVA testing for differences in mean selectivity of crayfish in relation to parasite species and refuge availability

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of a two-way ANOVA test for differences in mean selectivity of water scorpions in relation to parasite species and refuge availability