Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T10:55:58.507Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variation in Norwegian retroflexion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2012

Sverre Stausland Johnsen*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, University of Oslo, Postboks 1102 Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway. stausland.johnsen@iln.uio.no

Abstract

A phonological process in Norwegian takes morpheme-initial alveolar /t d n s/ to retroflex [ʈ ɖ ɳ ʂ] after a morpheme ending in the tap /ɾ/, a process which earlier descriptions in the literature have classified as obligatory. This paper reports on production experiments with both existing and novel words, which show that Norwegian speakers treat retroflexion of morpheme-initial /s/ as optional, and that they are more likely to apply retroflexion to a morpheme in /s-/ when the /s/ is followed by a consonant (/sC-/) than when the /s/ is followed by a vowel (/sV-/).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Nordic Association of Linguistics 2012

1. INTRODUCTION

A phonological process in Norwegian takes morpheme-initial alveolar /t d n s/ to retroflex [ʈ ɖ ɳ ʂ] after a morpheme ending in the tap /ɾ/, a process which earlier descriptions in the literature have classified as obligatory. This paper reports on production experiments with both existing and novel words, which show that Norwegian speakers treat retroflexion of morpheme-initial /s/ as optional, and that they are more likely to apply retroflexion to a morpheme in /s-/ when the /s/ is followed by a consonant (/sC-/) than when the /s/ is followed by a vowel (/sV-/).

The focus of this paper is to document a specific pattern of retroflexion in Norwegian. For a discussion of what the origin of this retroflexion pattern is, see Stausland Johnsen (Reference Stausland Johnsen2012).

2. NORWEGIAN RETROFLEXION

2.1 Norwegian

‘Norwegian’ is generally used as a cover term for varieties of North Germanic traditionally spoken within the borders of Norway. There are roughly five million speakers of Norwegian today. The only variety which will be examined in this paper is the spoken variety used by most speakers in urbanized areas in South-East Norway (for a detailed overview of the phonological properties of this variety, see Kristoffersen Reference Kristoffersen2000). All the Norwegian examples in this paper are based on the production and intuition of the author and his native consultants.

2.2 Retroflexion

Norwegian distinguishes two coronal categories in postvocalic position:

  1. (1)

Footnote 1 The contrast between alveolars and retroflexes in postvocalic position is illustrated in (2):Footnote 2

  1. (2)

A morpheme-final apical alveolar tap /ɾ/ deletes when the following morpheme begins with a consonant (Rykkvin Reference Rykkvin1946; Haugen Reference Haugen1948; Kristoffersen Reference Kristoffersen2000:311ff.):Footnote 3

  1. (3)

If the second morpheme begins with an alveolar consonant /t d n s/, the tap /ɾ/ still deletes, as is shown in (3), but the alveolar surfaces as a retroflex [ʈ ɖ ɳ ʂ]:Footnote 4

  1. (4)

This retroflexion process across morpheme boundaries also takes place when the morpheme boundary is a word boundary. The sentence in (5), for example, will surface with deletion of /ɾ/ before words beginning with a consonant, and with retroflexion of that consonant if it is an alveolar.

  1. (5)

As in Kristoffersen (Reference Kristoffersen2000:96f., 316), the discussion in this paper will focus on retroflexion within nominal compounds like those in (4).

3. VARIATION IN NORWEGIAN RETROFLEXION

Previous literature on Norwegian retroflexion describes the retroflexion process as obligatory, absent only when there is a significant intonational or pausal boundary between the morphemes (Eliasson Reference Eliasson and Andersen1986:282; Kristoffersen Reference Kristoffersen2000:316f.; Torp Reference Torp2007:70). As Kristoffersen puts it, the retroflexion process ‘seems to be beyond speakers’ active control’ (Reference Kristoffersen2000:317). This description is generally correct, as retroflexion seems to be obligatory when a morpheme beginning in an alveolar /t d n/ follows a morpheme ending in the tap /ɾ/:Footnote 5

  1. (6)

On the other hand, when the morpheme begins with an alveolar sibilant /s/, retroflexion seems to be entirely optional:

  1. (7)

Although retroflexion of words in /s-/ is optional, some words in /s-/ appear to undergo retroflexion more readily than others. One factor that seems to contribute to the likelihood of words in /s-/ undergoing retroflexion is what the segment following the /s/ is. Specifically, when the following segment is a consonant, retroflexion to [ʂ] is generally preferred, but when the /s/ is followed by a vowel, retroflexion to [ʂ] is generally not preferred. In (8), the preferred output is indicated with a check mark ✓:

  1. (8)

Another way to characterize the pattern described above is that retroflexion is more common in complex onsets (when there is more than one consonant at the beginning of the word, i.e. a cluster /sC-/) than in simple onsets (when there is only one consonant, the /s-/). I will therefore refer to the distinction noted in (8) as ‘complex onset’ vs. ‘simple onset’.

4. ONSET COMPLEXITY IN PHONOLOGY

From the pattern noted in Section 3 above, a natural hypothesis would be that the phonological grammar of Norwegian makes direct reference to the onset complexity of words in /s-/, such that retroflexion is applied more often to complex onsets than to simple onsets. This section will briefly illustrate that phonology often makes reference to the onset complexity of morphemes. As a result, hypothesizing the same for Norwegian should not be considered an unusual approach.

4.1 Onset complexity in Italian

The masculine singular determiner in Italian is /il/ or /lo/ before a noun beginning with a consonant. When this noun has a simple onset in /sV-/, the determiner is /il/, but when the noun has a complex onset in /sC-/, the determiner is /lo/ (Dardano & Trifone Reference Dardano and Trifone1997:151; Maiden & Robustelli Reference Maiden and Robustelli2007:61):

  1. (9)

4.2 Onset complexity in Vedic Sanskrit

Vedic Sanskrit verbs reduplicate their onsets in the perfect tense forms. When the verbal root has a simple onset /sV-/, the consonant /s/ is copied in the reduplicated syllable. The same holds for any simple onset in /CV-/:

  1. (10)

When the verbal root has a complex onset in /sC-/, only one of the consonants is copied in the reduplicated syllable, either the /s/ or the following /C/:Footnote 6

  1. (11)

5. EVIDENCE FOR RETROFLEXION IN SIMPLE AND COMPLEX ONSETS

The examples in Section 3 indicate that retroflexion is applied more often to complex /s/-onsets than to simple /s/-onsets. Ideally we want confirmation of this claim from a large body of Norwegian data. Retroflexion is, however, never marked in spelling, and this excludes the use of written corpora. Additionally, there are to date no spoken Norwegian corpora where retroflexion is marked in the transcription, which makes the use of existing spoken corpora less practical. For these reasons, data was elicited directly from Norwegian speakers during a reading task. This experiment and its results are presented in the following section.

6. EXPERIMENT A: REAL WORDS

6.1 Participants

Ten native speakers of Norwegian with a mean age of 30 years participated in the experiment, six male and four female. Three speakers were visiting students in the Boston area, and the remaining seven speakers participated from their location in Norway.

6.2 Stimuli

The five most frequent monosyllabic nouns in /st-/ and the five most frequent monosyllabic nouns in /sV-/ were chosen as the stimuli. All ten nouns are among the 15 most frequent monosyllabic nouns in /s-/ in the LBK corpus.Footnote 7 The onset /st-/ was chosen as the complex onset because it is the most frequent complex /s/-onset in Norwegian. The stimulus set also contained five filler items, which are high-frequency monosyllabic nouns beginning in a consonant other than /s-/. The stimuli are listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Experiment A stimuli.

All ten nouns were placed in nominal compounds with a nonce first element <bemmer>, which unambiguously represents /bɛməɾ/, ending in the retroflex triggering tap /ɾ/. All stimuli were presented to participants in their orthographic representations.

6.3 Procedure

The nominal compounds containing the stimuli each appeared six times within a short frame story, which the participants were instructed to read in a normal, casual manner. The internal order of the frame stories was randomized within participants. The participants’ productions were digitally recorded. See the appendix for more details.

6.4 Labeling

All recorded tokens of the stimuli were labeled as retroflex or non-retroflex independently by the author and by a phonetically trained linguist who is a native speaker of Norwegian, and who was kept unaware of the purpose and design of the experiment.Footnote 8 Only tokens that were labeled identically by both were included in the analysis. Of the total of 2423 tokens recorded, seven were excluded due to erroneous and disfluent production, and eight were excluded due to disagreement between the two taggers. In the end, 2408 trials were submitted for analysis.

6.5 Results

Retroflexion was variably applied to every stimulus word in /s-/. None of these words underwent retroflexion 100% of the time, and none of them consistently escaped retroflexion. The optionality of retroflexion for /s/ is thereby confirmed. As can be seen in Figure 1, retroflexion is considerably more frequent for words in /st-/ than for words in /sV-/, with a mean retroflexion rate of 78% for words in /st-/ and a mean retroflexion rate of 44% for words in /sV-/. A mixed effects logistic regression model confirms that the difference between /sV-/ and /st-/ is significant (χ2(6) = 230.95, p < .0001). See the appendix for more details.

Figure 1. Retroflexion for real words in /s-/.

7. PHONOLOGICAL PRODUCTIVITY

The results reported above in Section 6.5 confirm that retroflexion of /s/ is optional and more common in complex onsets than in simple onsets. These results were obtained using highly familiar words of Norwegian. The possibility always exists that a phonological pattern observed in existing words is there only because these words have been inherited from an older stage of the language when some phonological process was applied to yield that pattern. Put differently, it is not always clear from the behavior of existing words whether this behavior is caused by productive operations in the language or by the inherited properties of these words.

This conundrum can be illustrated with the Norwegian palatalization rule. In Middle Norwegian, /k/ was palatalized before front vowels (Indrebø Reference Indrebø1951:230f.). In modern Norwegian, this palatalization product appears as /ɕ/ (Endresen Reference Endresen1991:75f.). From the palatalization rule in Middle Norwegian, there are no native words in modern Norwegian dialects with /k/ before original front vowels, and there is a range of words with a /k/–/ɕ/ alternation between morphologically related forms, as illustrated with a few examples in (12) below:Footnote 9

  1. (12)

It has not been uncommon to conclude from such examples that there is a productive palatalization process in Norwegian which takes an underlying /k/ to a surface realization [ɕ] before front vowels (Fretheim Reference Fretheim1969:87f.; Hovdhaugen Reference Hovdhaugen1969/1971:152; Weinstock Reference Weinstock and Benediktsson1970:586; Standwell Reference Standwell1975:346f.). At the same time, however, it is clear that newer loanwords never undergo palatalization, as seen in (13):

  1. (13)

From the data in (12) and (13) we can conclude that there is no phonologically active palatalization rule in Norwegian (Kristoffersen Reference Kristoffersen2000:112), and that the frequently observed /k/–/ɕ/ alternation is just a lexical property of inherited words.

In the case at hand, we want to be certain that Norwegian speakers apply retroflexion more often to complex /s/-onsets due to a productive phonological process, and not solely due to the inherited properties of existing words. Since Berko (Reference Berko1958), the standard method applied to identify productive phonological processes has been to investigate whether speakers apply these processes to novel native-like words made up for the experiment. For these reasons, an experiment with nonce Norwegian words in /s-/ was designed. This experiment and its results are reported in the next section.

8. EXPERIMENT B: NONCE WORDS

8.1 Participants

The participants in Experiment B were the same ten native speakers of Norwegian as those in Experiment A.

8.2 Stimuli

Three monosyllabic nouns in /sV/, three monosyllabic nouns in /st-/, and three monosyllabic nouns in /sk-/ were created as stimuli. All words are novel nouns with native-like phonotactics. The onsets /st-/ and /sk-/ were chosen as complex onsets because these are the two most frequent complex /s/-onsets in Norwegian. The stimulus set also contained six filler items, which were novel native-like monosyllabic nouns beginning with a consonant other than /s-/. The stimuli are listed in Table 2.

Table 2. Experiment B stimuli.

All nine nouns were placed in nominal compounds with a first element <sommer> /sɔməɾ/ ‘summer’, ending in the retroflex triggering tap /ɾ/. The procedure was otherwise identical to that in Experiment A, and the participants’ productions were digitally recorded.

8.3 Labeling

All recorded tokens of the stimuli were labeled as in Experiment A. Of the total of 3395 tokens recorded, 17 were excluded due to erroneous and disfluent production, and 38 were excluded due to disagreement between the two taggers. In the end, 3340 trials were submitted for analysis.

8.4 Results

Retroflexion was variably applied to every stimulus word in /s-/, with the retroflexion rate for individual words varying between 35% and 72%. This confirms the finding from Experiment A that retroflexion of /s/ is optional. As seen in Figure 2, retroflexion is less common for words with a simple onset /sV/ than for words with a complex onset /sC/, as in Experiment A, with a mean retroflexion rate of 44% for words in /sV/ and a mean retroflexion rate of 64% for words in /sC-/. Additionally, there is less retroflexion for words with a complex onset /st-/ (mean 60%) than for words with a complex onset /sk-/ (mean 69%). Mixed effects logistic regression models confirm that all observed differences between /sV/, /st/, and /sk/ are significant (14).

  1. (14)

Figure 2. Retroflexion for nonce words in /s-/.

9. RETROFLEXION OF ALVEOLAR STOPS AND NASALS

According to the native intuition of the present author and his consultants, retroflexion is obligatorily applied to morphemes beginning in alveolar /t d n/ (see Section 3 above). As a quick ‘sanity check’ that this intuition corresponds well with actual linguistic production, the filler items with a simple alveolar stop and nasal onset from both experiments were also labelled as retroflex or non-retroflex. The retroflexion rate for these items is reported in (15) below.

  1. (15)

In contrast to the items in /s-/, the morphemes beginning in alveolar stops and nasals undergo retroflexion close to 100% of the time, thereby supporting the intuition that these alveolars are required to undergo retroflexion.

10. CONCLUSION

Figures 1 and 2 reveal that real and nonce words behave alike when it comes to retroflexion of words in /s-/, in that both experiments show that retroflexion of /s/ is optional and more likely to apply before a consonant than before a vowel. Experiment B also shows that retroflexion is more likely to apply when this consonant is /k/ than when the consonant is /t/. From this the conclusion can be drawn that the phonological grammar of Norwegian makes direct reference to the phonological makeup of the /s/-onset when it determines whether or not to apply retroflexion to such words.

Combining the patterns of variability in /s/-retroflexion with the (near) obligatory retroflexion applied to words in /t-/, /d-/, and /n-/ (see Sections 3 and 9 above), we can set up the following descriptive hierarchy for the probability of retroflexion to alveolar onsets:

  1. (16) /t/, /d/, /n/ > /sk/ > /st/ > /sV/

The hierarchy in (16) illustrates the fact that the retroflexion rate is the highest for onsets in /t-/, /d-/, /n-/, followed by a lower retroflexion rate for /sk-/, and so on.

The aim of this article has been to support the hypothesis that retroflexion of /s/ is optional in Norwegian, and that the variability of /s/-retroflexion largely depends on the segment following the /s/. A question which therefore lies outside the scope of this article is where the retroflexion pattern in (16) comes from. This question is the focus of Stausland Johnsen (Reference Stausland Johnsen2012), where a perceptual experiment reveals that the perceived distance between an alveolar and a retroflex is inversely related to the retroflexion hierarchy in (16). In other words, the perceived distances between alveolar [t d n] and retroflex [ʈ ɖ ɳ] are the smallest, and the perceived distance between alveolar [s] and retroflex [ʂ] before a vowel is the largest. For a fuller discussion of how this correlation between perception and phonology can best be explained, the reader is referred to Stausland Johnsen (Reference Stausland Johnsen2012).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This paper has greatly benefited from comments and suggestions by Adam Albright, Michael Becker, Cathie Ringen, and two anonymous reviewers. I'm very grateful to the Norwegian speakers who participated in my experiments, and I'm especially grateful to Kjetil Aasen for being the trained linguist who tagged all the experiment tokens.

APPENDIX

The frame story in Experiment A was written in such a way that it gave no semantic content to the first element of the nominal compound, the nonce word <bemmer>. This was to prevent the possibility that the semantic and pragmatic plausibility of the compound could affect the likelihood of retroflexion. To ensure that the participants treated the words in /s-/ as the intended high-frequency nouns (and not as coincidental homonyms), each frame story was introduced with an illustration of the /s/-word appearing in that frame story, as seen in Figure A1.

Figure A1. Experiment A trial.

The Norwegian text in Figure A1 translates into English as follows:

In Wonderland there is a wizard named Bogo. With his magic, he can create whatever he pleases from absolutely nothing. Bogo wants to create a new sol made from bemmer. He is thinking about how to go about creating a bemmersol, a very nice bemmersol at that. Bogo creates a bemmersol by saying ‘Puff! Come, bemmersol!’. Now Bogo has a bemmersol, and he is very happy that he has acquired a bemmersol.

The frame story in Experiment B gave no semantic content to the nominal compound other than being an object suitable for use during the summer (Figure A2). Although some of the phonetic strings functioning as novel nouns in this experiment appear elsewhere in the language as verbal forms (e.g. /sɑː/ is the preterite of /siː/ ‘say’), the frame story immediately introduced these words as representations of tangible objects.

Figure A2. Experiment B trial.

The Norwegian text in Figure A2 translates into English as follows:

It's great to use a su in the summer, so when May comes, I go and get my summersu from the garage. It's a wonderful summersu, almost as nice as the summersu that my grandma had. I put the summersu in the back yard. I made the summersu myself many years ago. It's not easy to make a summersu, but it's the best summersu I've ever had.

The three participants in the Boston area read the frame stories from printed copies, whereas the seven participants in Norway read from their own computer monitors. All participants were instructed to read each page with frame stories four times before proceeding to the next page. A pilot study revealed that speakers often produce careful speech at the beginning of a reading task, at which register retroflexion is almost always absent. Since such speech is of no interest in this study, each participant's production up until the first instance of applied retroflexion was treated as a warm-up to the experiment, and the frame stories contained in the warm-up were then repeated at the end of the experiment.

The productions from two participants were recorded in mono using a KSM27 Shure microphone in a sound-attenuated booth. The signal was amplified through an M-AUDIO Firewire 410 amplifier and recorded digitally with Audacity software (Audacity Team 2006) in .wav format at a sampling rate of 44.100 Hz on an iMac7,1 Macintosh computer. One participant was recorded in mono in a quiet room using an MXL M.A.R.K. microphone, and the signal was recorded digitally with a PMD660 Professional Marantz recorder in .wav format at a sampling rate of 44.100 Hz. Seven participants were recorded in mono in quiet locations with the Voice over Internet Protocol software Skype 4.0 (Skype 2003–2009) and Skype Call Recorder 0.7 (Nikiforov Reference Nikiforov2009) in .mp3 format at a bit rate of 128.000 bit/s and a sampling rate of 44.100 Hz.

In the mixed effects logistic regression models, the dependent variable is the presence or absence of retroflexion for each item produced by a participant. The independent variable of interest here is Onset, which classifies the stimulus items as having an onset in /sV-/, /st-/, or /sk-/. Other independent variables code for vowel features of the stimuli, the position of the stimuli within the frame story, the position of the frame story within the experiment, and, for the real words in Experiment A, the lexical frequency of the stimulus words in the LBK corpus (see Note 7). The models were fitted with the glmer() function from the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler & Bolker Reference Bates, Maechler and Bolker2012) in R (R Core Team 2012), with random intercepts for words and participants (Baayen, Davidson & Bates Reference Baayen, Davidson and Bates2008), using the method of backward elimination (Draper & Smith Reference Draper and Smith1998:339f.; Faraway Reference Faraway2005:131f.). Under this approach, an initial model with all variables and their interactions is built, and then the least significant term is removed from the model, as diagnosed by the dropterm() function from the MASS package (Venables & Ripley Reference Venables and Ripley2002). A model without this term is fitted, and this reduction continues until no insignificant terms remain. The significance of the variable Onset is determined by a likelihood ratio test (Hilbe Reference Hilbe2009:81f.). Separate models were built for each of the three comparisons reported in Experiment B.

Footnotes

1. For articulatory studies of these segments, see Foldvik Reference Foldvik, Bjørkum and Borg1988a, Reference Foldvikb; Simonsen & Moen Reference Simonsen and Moen2004; Simonsen, Moen & Cowen Reference Simonsen, Moen and Cowen2008; Moen & Simonsen Reference Moen, Simonsen, Lee and Zee2011.

2. It was not uncommon in the 1960s and 1970s to analyze morpheme-internal retroflexes as derived from underlying consonant sequences (see e.g. Hovdhaugen Reference Hovdhaugen1969/1971:147; Endresen Reference Endresen1974:75), a view that is less common today (see Kristoffersen Reference Kristoffersen2000:88f.; Stausland Johnsen Reference Stausland Johnsen2012:127). The difference between these two approaches has no bearing on the discussion in this paper.

4. This retroflexion process was first described by Brekke (Reference Brekke1881:18ff.); Storm (Reference Storm1884:96f.); Western (Reference Western1889:275). See Kristoffersen (Reference Kristoffersen2000:96f.) for a recent description.

5. In some marginal cases, retroflexion can be absent when several conditions are met: (i) the word ending in /-ɾ/ is unstressed; (ii) the tap /ɾ/ is in an unstressed grammatical suffix; and (iii) the sequence /ɾ + alveolar/ is in a high-frequency collocation. One such example that appears in the corpus study by Olsen (Reference Olsen2011) is /ˈtɾædʋə kɾuːnəɾ ˈtiːmən/ → [ˈtɾædʋə kɾuːnə ˈtiːmən] ‘thirty kroners an hour’, where /kɾuːnəɾ/ ‘crown-pl’ is unstressed and with /-ɾ/ as the plural suffix. Since this paper only deals with nominal compounds, the /-ɾ/ will in our cases always be part of the stem.

6. For the formation of these perfect tense forms, see Whitney (Reference Whitney1889:222f.); Macdonell (Reference Macdonell1910:363f.). Other phonological changes also apply to the forms illustrated in (10) and (11): the s becomes a after high vowels i and u, high vowels i and u become glides (i)y and (u)v before another vowel, and the aspirated stop th dissimilates to t when there is another aspirated stop in the onset of the next syllable (Whitney Reference Whitney1889:44, 61; Macdonell Reference Macdonell1910:23). These changes are not relevant to the discussion here.

8. All tokens with an alveolar realization were labeled ‘non-retroflex’, irrespective of whether the underlying /ɾ/ had been completely deleted (e.g. [bɛməsɑːk]) or articulatorily reduced (e.g. [bɛməɾsɑːk]). This was mainly for practical purposes, as it proved difficult to distinguish /ɾ/-deletion from /ɾ/-reduction, a problem previously pointed out by Haugen (Reference Haugen1948:121). Since this paper focuses on the application of retroflexion and not on the magnitude of reduction to the /ɾ/, any acoustic or articulatory study of /ɾ/-reduction must be left for future research.

9. The orthography in these examples follows the landsmål standard, which is based on the modern Norwegian dialects (Aasen Reference Aasen1873). The bracketed phonetic transcriptions are based on the spoken Norwegian variety treated in this paper (see Section 2.1).

References

REFERENCES

Aasen, Ivar. 1873. Norsk Ordbog med dansk Forklaring. Omarbeidet og forøget Udgave af en ældre „Ordbog over det norske Folkesprog“. Christiania: P. T. Mallings Boghandel.Google Scholar
Audacity Team. 2006. Audacity. Version 1.2.6. Computer software. http://sourceforge.net/projects/audacity/files/audacity/1.2.6/ (retrieved 21 August 2012).Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H[arald], Davidson, D[ouglas] J. & Bates, D[ouglas] M.. 2008. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59 (4), 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Maechler, Martin & Bolker, Ben. 2012. lme4: Linear Mixed-effects Models Using S4 Classes. Version 0.999999-0. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 (retrieved 23 August 2012).Google Scholar
Berko, Jean. 1958. The child's learning of English morphology. Word 14 (2–3), 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brekke, K[nud]. 1881. Bidrag til dansk-norskens lydlære. In Indbydelsesskrift til den offentlige examen i juni og juli 1881 ved Aars og Voss's latin- og realskole, 166. Kristiania: W. C. Fabritius.Google Scholar
Dardano, Maurizio & Trifone, Pietro. 1997. La nuova grammatica della lingua italiana. Bologna: Zanichelli.Google Scholar
Draper, Norman R[ichard] & Smith, Harry. 1998. Applied Regression Analysis, 3rd edn. (Wiley Series in Probability and Statistics). New York: John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eliasson, Stig. 1986. Sandhi in Peninsular Scandinavian. In Andersen, Henning (ed.), Sandhi Phenomena in the Languages of Europe (Trends in Linguistics: Studies and Monographs 33), 271300. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endresen, Rolf Theil. 1974. On retroflex segments in Norwegian. Norwegian Journal of Linguistics 28, 7378.Google Scholar
Endresen, Rolf Theil. 1991. Fonetikk og fonologi. Ei elementær innføring, 2nd edn.Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Faraway, Julian J[ames]. 2005. Linear Models with R (Texts in Statistical Science). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
Foldvik, Arne Kjell. 1974. Phonetic light on the Endresen–Fretheim controversy. Working Papers in Linguistics 6, 18. University of Oslo.Google Scholar
Foldvik, Arne Kjell. 1977. Realisasjonen av r i norsk. Svenska landsmål och svensk folkliv 100, 110118.Google Scholar
Foldvik, Arne Kjell. 1988a. Artikulasjonen av noen konsonantar i norsk. En palatografisk undersøkelse. In Bjørkum, Andreas & Borg, Arve (eds.), Nordiske studiar. Innlegg på den tredje norske dialektologkonferansen 1986 (Skrifter frå Norsk Målførearkiv XL), 359362. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Foldvik, Arne Kjell. 1988b. Taxonomic problems in Norwegian phonetics. Scandinavian Conference of Linguistics 10 (1), 189195.Google Scholar
Fretheim, Thorstein. 1969. Norwegian stress and quantity reconsidered. Norsk Tidsskrift for Sprogvidenskap 23, 7696.Google Scholar
Haugen, Einar. 1948. Mere om r-bortfall i sørøstlandsk. Maal og Minne, 117–122.Google Scholar
Hilbe, Joseph M. 2009. Logistic Regression Models (Texts in Statistical Science). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovdhaugen, Even. 1969/1971. Transformasjonell generativ grammatikk. Annen utgave med øvelser (Scandinavian University Books). Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Indrebø, Gustav. 1951. Norsk målsoga. Bergen: John Griegs boktrykkeri.Google Scholar
Knutsen, Ragnhild Lucy. 2006. An articulatory description of the liquids of Urban East Norwegian, based on EPG and EMA. MA thesis, University of Oslo.Google Scholar
Kristoffersen, Gjert. 2000. The Phonology of Norwegian (The Phonology of the World's Languages). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macdonell, A[rthur] A[nthony]. 1910. Vedic Grammar (Grundriss der indo-arischen Philologie und Altertumskunde I. Band, 4. Heft). Strassburg: Karl J. Trübner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maiden, Martin & Robustelli, Cecilia. 2007. A Reference Grammar of Modern Italian, 2nd edn.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Moen, Inger & Simonsen, Hanne Gram. 2011. The Norwegian retroflex fricative. In Lee, Wai-Sum & Zee, Eric (eds.), The 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS17), Department of Chinese, Translation & Linguistics, City University of Hong Kong. 13981401.Google Scholar
Moen, Inger, Simonsen, Hanne Gram, Lindstad, Arne Martinus & Cowen, Steve. 2003. The articulation of the East Norwegian apical liquids /ɭ ɾ ɽ/. In Solé, Maria-Josep, Recasens, Daniel & Romero, Joaquín (eds.), The 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS15), Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona. 17551758.Google Scholar
Nikiforov, Alexander. 2009. Skype Call Recorder. Version 0.7.2.1. Computer software. http://voipcallrecording.com/Skype_Call_Recorder (retrieved 21 August 2012).Google Scholar
Olsen, Eirik. 2011. R-bortfall og apikalisering i oslomålet. En studie av distribusjon og frekvenseffekter. Masteroppgave, Universitetet i Oslo.Google Scholar
R Core Team. 2012. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Computer software. Version 2.15.1. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rykkvin, Oddmund. 1946. Om r-bortfall i søraustlandsk. Maal og Minne (3–4), 144150.Google Scholar
Simonsen, Hanne Gram & Moen, Inger. 2004. On the distinction between Norwegian /ʃ/ and /ç/ from a phonetic perspective. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 18 (6–8), 605620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonsen, Hanne Gram, Moen, Inger & Cowen, Steve. 2008. Norwegian retroflex stops in a cross linguistic perspective. Journal of Phonetics 36 (2), 385405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skype (2003–2009). Version 4.0.0.206. Computer software. Skype Limited. http://www.oldapps.com/skype.php?old_skype=78 (retrieved 21 August 2012).Google Scholar
Standwell, G[raham] J. 1975. Norwegian phonology. Acta Linguistica Academiae Scientiarum Hungaricae 23 (3–4), 339377.Google Scholar
Stausland Johnsen, Sverre. 2012. From perception to phonology: The emergence of perceptually motivated constraint rankings. Lingua 122 (2), 125143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storm, Joh[an]. 1884. Norsk Lydskrift med Omrids af Fonetiken. Første Afsnit. Norvegia 1 (1), 19132.Google Scholar
Torp, Arne. 2007. R – ei urokråke i språket. Oslo: Det norske samlaget.Google Scholar
Venables, W[illiam] N. & Ripley, B[rian] D[avid]. 2002. Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th edn. (Statistics and Computing). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstock, John M[artin]. 1970. A sketch of Norwegian phonology. In Benediktsson, Hreinn (ed.), The Nordic Languages and Modern Linguistics: The International Conference of Nordic and General Linguistics, 572598. Reykjavík: Vísindafélag Íslendinga.Google Scholar
Western, Aug[ust]. 1889. Kurze darstellung des norwegischen lautsystems. Phonetische Studien 2, 259282.Google Scholar
Whitney, William Dwight. 1889. A Sanskrit Grammar: Including Both the Classical Language, and the Older Dialects, of Veda and Brahmana, 2nd edn., revised and extended (Bibliothek indogermanischer Grammatiken 2). Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Experiment A stimuli.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Retroflexion for real words in /s-/.

Figure 2

Table 2. Experiment B stimuli.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Retroflexion for nonce words in /s-/.

Figure 4

Figure A1. Experiment A trial.

Figure 5

Figure A2. Experiment B trial.