Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T23:58:38.009Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Articulation rate across dialect, age, and gender

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2009

Ewa Jacewicz
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
Robert A. Fox
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
Caitlin O'Neill
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
Joseph Salmons
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Whether some languages or dialects are spoken faster or slower than others constitutes a gap in the understanding of sociolinguistic variation. Speech tempo is interconnected with the social, physical and psychological marking of speech. This study examines regional variation in articulation rate and its manifestations across speaker age, gender, and speaking situations (reading vs. informal talk). The results of an experimental investigation show that articulation rate differs significantly between two regional varieties of American English examined here. A group of Northern speakers (from Wisconsin) spoke significantly faster than a group of Southern speakers (from North Carolina). With regard to age and gender, young adults read faster than older adults in both regions; in informal talks, however, only Northern young adults spoke faster than older adults. Effects of gender were smaller and less consistent; men generally spoke slightly faster than women did. As the body of work on the sociophonetics of American English continues to grow in scope and depth, we argue that it is important to include fundamental phonetic information as part of our catalog of regional differences and patterns of change in American English.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Humans vary in how they produce speech, and those differences can depend on a number of factors. For instance, speech tempo can be inherently speaker-specific, pertaining to the inherent speed of articulatory movements that define unique speaker characteristics along with other variables such as voice, use of prosody, or pausing. The within-speaker variation in speech tempo is systematically affected by the length of the utterance, discourse complexity, formality, affect, mood, and communication style in noisy environments or over a longer distance, to name a few. Yet, in addition to the complex interaction of the within-speaker factors, there are other powerful sources of variation in speech tempo related to social variables, most notably speaker age, gender, geographic region of origin, place of residence, education, occupation, and socioeconomic status. The latter group of factors determines the between-speaker variation in speech tempo, which has been a topic of fruitful investigation (e.g., Byrd, Reference Byrd1994; Hewlett & Rendall, Reference Hewlett and Rendall1998; Smith, Wasowicz, & Preston, Reference Smith, Wasowicz and Preston1987).

Researchers have long recognized that variation in speech tempo is a way of marking individual speaker characteristics. Abercrombie (Reference Abercrombie1967:7–9) and Laver and Trudgill (Reference Laver, Trudgill and Laver1991:237ff.) have proposed “a typology of markers of identity in speech,” including three types:

  1. 1. Social markers that are associated with, among other things, regional affiliation and social status.

  2. 2. Physical markers that correlate with age, sex, and health status.

  3. 3. Psychological markers that tell us something about “psychological characteristics of personality and affective state.”

This typology emphasizes that there are several quite divergent aspects of speaker identity about which speech tempo may convey information. In contrast, numerous of the most familiar sociolinguistic variables—negative concord, “g-dropping,” /ai/ monophthongization, /æ/ raising, quotative like or “uptalk”—are perceived by speakers in terms of relatively focused social meanings, correlating primarily with social status or educational background (negative concord or g-dropping), regional speech (monophthongization or raising), or age (like or uptalk). This complexity alone makes speech tempo an important subject for language variation and change. It is necessarily interconnected with social, physical, and psychological marking of speech. However, there is a basic lacuna in our understanding of speech tempo. Even the fundamental patterns of regional variation in how fast people speak have not been securely demonstrated.

This paper presents the results of an experimental investigation of speech tempo that focused primarily on regional variation in American English. In particular, the present experiments examined one pervasive stereotype about American dialects: the notion of “slow-talking Southerners” and “fast-talking Northerners” (Niedzielski & Preston, Reference Niedzielski and Preston1999).Footnote 1 The examination of Southern and Northern speech is carried out in a controlled setting (reading task) as well as in an uncontrolled condition (informal talk). To gain a fuller account of potential differences in speech tempo between the speakers from the South and the North, the study includes two additional between-speaker variables: age and gender. It is of interest whether the speech of Southerners (men and women, younger and older) is indeed slower than the speech of Northerners under two different levels of formality (reading vs. speaking). On the other hand, some of the between-speaker effects are predicted to remain unaffected by regional variation, based on previous reports which we discuss later. This includes the effects of age (speech tempo to be faster for younger than older speakers) and gender (speech tempo to be faster for men than for women). We may also expect slower tempo in reading as compared with speaking. How well the present results conform to these predictions is assessed by measuring the articulation rate for each individual speaker in the study.

In this paper, we operationally define speech tempo as the articulation rate. In the phonetics literature, the terms “speaking rate,” “speech rate,” and “articulation rate” were used interchangeably to indicate speech tempo: the pace at which a stretch of connected discourse is delivered by the speaker. The consensus today is that even though speaking/speech rate and articulation rate are both defined as “the number of output units per unit of time” (Tsao, Weismer, & Iqbal, Reference Tsao, Weismer and Iqbal2006:1156), speaking/speech rate includes pause intervals but articulation rate does not. Articulation rate determines the pace at which speech segments are actually produced and does not take into account speaker-specific ways of conveying information, such as hesitations, pausing, emotional expressions, and so on. Speech (or speaking) rate, on the other hand, captures more “global” speaker characteristics including frequency of pausing, use of laughter or fillers such as “you know” or “I mean” that, inserted in a stretch of discourse, cause an interruption of fluency and define a speaker-specific communication style. In this paper, measuring the articulation rate rather than speaking/speech rate will give us a better estimate of cross-dialectal differences in speech tempo as it will eliminate the use of pauses as an additional within-speaker variable. In this regard, the paper follows a more recent approach to study the regional variation in speech tempo in terms of measuring the articulation rate rather than speaking rate (e.g., Quené, Reference Quené2008; Verhoeven, De Pauw, & Kloots, Reference Verhoeven, De Pauw and Kloots2004).

THE EFFECTS OF SOCIAL AND PHYSICAL MARKERS ON VARIATION IN SPEECH TEMPO

Regional differences

Speech tempo has been shown to vary not only across individuals but to have roots in regional varieties of the same language. Byrd (Reference Byrd1994) provided the most compelling evidence on regional differences in speech rate (including the pauses) in American English, drawing on the TIMIT database.Footnote 2 She found a slower speech rate for Southerners and a faster rate for Northerners, broadly speaking, whereas “Army Brats” (who had lived in three or more areas) spoke the fastest. Southern and South Midland speakers produced the most pauses and North Midland, Western, and Army Brat speakers produced fewer pauses than expected given a random distribution determined by a chi-square test. As will be discussed, however, the TIMIT database has serious limitations that do not always allow for a conclusive assessment of regionally defined differences in speech rate in American English. Ultimately, few studies to date have provided direct evidence that overall speech rate differs regionally.

As an example, Ray and Zahn (Reference Ray and Zahn1990) investigated speech rate with 93 speakers from Utah, Oregon, and Washington in the Pacific Northwest; Texas and Louisiana in the Southwest; and Wisconsin and Ohio in the Upper Midwest. The speech samples were taken from public speaking and group discussion, both in university settings. Context category (public speaking vs. discussion) was the only significant factor in the variation in speaking rate to the exclusion of gender and region. The division of speakers was done entirely by the university they attended (i.e., without evidence of where they actually grew up). Even aside from that, the regions do not align with current views of American dialect areas. For instance, in Labov's maps (e.g., Labov, Ash, & Boberg, Reference Labov, Ash and Boberg2006:142, 148), Wisconsin typically includes large areas corresponding to North Central/“North” and Inland North, though some other dialect maps also show a small patch of Midlands. In contrast, Ohio is mostly Midlands, with a band of the Inland North, along with various transition areas and small areas belonging to other dialect areas, including Southern (especially along the Ohio River).

Speaker age and gender

As pointed out in a number of studies, young adults tend to speak faster than older adults do (e.g., Quené, Reference Quené2008; Verhoeven et al., Reference Verhoeven, De Pauw and Kloots2004). This has been also discussed by Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Wasowicz and Preston1987) and treated more recently by Yuan, Cieri, and Liberman (Reference Yuan, Cieri and Liberman2006, with references to earlier work). Seeking an explanation of such patterns, Ramig (Reference Ramig1983:224) cited physiological factors such as “visual acuity, processing time, general neuromuscular slowing, peripheral degeneration of the speech mechanism, and psychosocial variables” as possible reasons why the physical condition of an elderly person affected their speaking rate. The findings by Quené (Reference Quené2008) pointed to the fact that older speakers produce relatively shorter phrases than younger speakers and the difference in phrase length may explain some of the age-related effects.

With regard to gender, most available evidence suggests that men actually speak somewhat faster than women do, as found for example by Byrd (Reference Byrd1994). Yuan et al. (Reference Yuan, Cieri and Liberman2006) reported a small but significant difference in the same direction. Outside of North America, Whiteside (Reference Whiteside1996) examined the characteristics of read speech in three women and three men with a British General Northern accent. Whiteside noted that the small sample limits the conclusions that can be drawn, but results showed that women had longer mean sentence durations and higher standard deviations; they also paused more frequently than the men did.Footnote 3

Urban and rural speech

“Rate of speaking” in the informal sense is involved in a cluster of other stereotypes, including the view that some languages are spoken faster than others (Roach, Reference Roach, Bauer and Trudgill1998) or that urban speech is faster than rural speech. Hewlett and Rendall (Reference Hewlett and Rendall1998) examined the claim that urban dwellers speak more quickly than rural dwellers do by comparing the speech rates of 24 speakers from the rural Orkney Islands, north of Scotland, and from urban Edinburgh. The speakers read a passage and were interviewed for several minutes about their lives. From these data, both speaking and articulation rates were calculated in syllables per second (counting only turns with 10 or more syllables). They found little difference in speaking rate while the speakers were reading, and in conversation mode the difference was not significant. Articulation rate also showed no significant differences in reading rate of the two groups, but the Orkney group had a significantly faster rate in conversation than the Edinburgh group. Thus, the results failed to support the claim that urban speech is faster than rural speech.

Speaking versus reading

Speaking rate and reading rate were directly compared by Crystal and House (Reference Crystal and House1982) who noted that speakers using “less formal production” (p. 706)—that is, informal conversations rather than reading—showed more temporal syllable reductions in their speech, which increased their speaking rates. In another study, Hirose and Kawanami (Reference Hirose and Kawanami2002) examined dialogue versus read speech and focused on the prosodic differences that exist between the two, noting that “dialogue speech generally shows wider dynamic ranges in its prosodic features” (p. 97), such as in tone and rhythm, as well as a higher speech rate than read speech. Howell and Kadi-Hanifi (Reference Howell and Kadi-Hanifi1991) also examined prosodic differences in reading and speaking. Six speakers spoke spontaneously (describing a room of their choice), and then three months later, after transcribing their response, investigators had those speakers read what they had responded, as well as the response of two other speakers, in order to compare speaking styles. They found that speakers produced a “larger number of short tone units” (p. 166) while reading, indicating more fragmentation and a more formal style of speech. They concluded that this variation in stress placement did not produce significant differences in speaking rate, but did observe that speaking rate “changes with the mode of delivery” (p. 168).

As this brief review indicates, factors such as regional affiliation of the speaker, age, gender, and speaking style have been found to produce differences in speech tempo in a systematic way. In this study, we are primarily interested in regional differences in articulation rate and their manifestations across speaker age, gender, and speaking situations (reading vs. speaking). At present, we only focus on the selected social and physical markers as potential predictors of differences in the articulation rate and do not address the contribution of psychological markers. The questions underlying our investigation center on the strength of a given predictor in making broader generalizations. Specifically, can we legitimately claim that certain regional varieties of American English are spoken faster (or slower) than others? If differences do exist, are they manifested in both speaking and reading? Do older adults always speak slower than the younger ones? Do men always speak faster than women?

The present paper has one additional aim. Studies by Crystal and House (Reference Crystal and House1988a, Reference Crystal and House1988b) and Jacewicz, Fox, and Salmons (Reference Jacewicz, Fox and Salmons2006, 2007) indicated that Northern vowels are in fact shorter than Southern vowels. This may suggest that there is a relationship between vowel duration and variation in speech tempo so that Southern vowels reflect the overall slower tempo of the Southern speech whereas the shorter Northern vowels correspond to the faster tempo of Northern speech. However, this correspondence may not hold for all vowels as pointed out by Clopper, Pisoni, and de Jong (Reference Clopper, Pisoni and de Jong2005:1665) who found that vowel duration differences are “selective in nature,” with Southern lax vowels significantly longer than those of other Americans. The present study seeks to find a more conclusive account of regional variation in speech tempo that may also be carried on to smaller temporal units such as phrasal or segmental durations.

METHODS

In the present paper, we report on a study that compares articulation rates of Northern speakers born and raised in south central/southeastern Wisconsin with those of Southern speakers born and raised in westernmost North Carolina. Subjects read sentences aloud and produced a short stretch of speech on a topic of their choice, providing two very different samples. Although our primary focus is on regional difference in speech tempo, our sample yields data on age—contrasting young adults (20 to 34 years old) with older adults (51 to 65 years old)—and gender—contrasting men's and women's speech.

Speakers

A total of 94 speakers participated in the study. All were born, raised, and spent most of their lives in either South-central Wisconsin or Western North Carolina. The speakers fell into two age groups. There were 40 older adults aged 51 to 65 years, 20 from Wisconsin and 20 from North Carolina who were evenly divided by gender (10 men and 10 women), in each region. There were also 54 young adults aged 20 to 34 years. In this group, 36 speakers were the original participants of the study (18 speakers from Wisconsin and 18 from North Carolina, 9 men and 9 women in each region) and 18 additional speakers (6 from Wisconsin and 12 from North Carolina) were recorded at a later time. All Wisconsin speakers came from the Madison area and areas east, well within the parts of the state defined as Labov et al.'s (2006) “Inland North” dialect, the area characterized by the Northern Cities Shift. The North Carolina speakers came from the Sylva, Cullowhee, and Waynesville areas (Jackson, Swain, and Haywood counties). Geographically, these participants yielded a highly homogenous sample of varieties of Northern and Southern speech, respectively.

The speakers were also comparable in terms of educational background and socioeconomic status. Most of the young adults were students at either the University of Wisconsin–Madison or Western Carolina University. The older adults mostly had a college education except for two Wisconsin speakers and six North Carolina speakers, two of whom worked as teacher's assistants and one as a librarian. As Tables 1 and 2 summarize, the occupations of the older adults in this study do not reflect a sharp regional difference in terms of socioeconomic status. To a limited extent, Wisconsin speakers are somewhat more metropolitan than North Carolina speakers are. Still, these groups do not reflect a clear urban/rural split, something that has been argued to correlate with speech differences in the South (see Thomas, Reference Thomas and Schneider2008; Tillery & Bailey, Reference Tillery, Bailey and Schneider2008). Although some of the Wisconsin speakers grew up in Madison (which, according to U.S. Census data reached a population of 203,704 in 2005), many of the younger and older subjects came from small towns such as Beaver Dam, Fond du Lac, Stoughton, West Bend, and New Glarus. The small towns in North Carolina where our subjects grew up (Dillsboro, Waynesville, Sylva, Webster, Cullowhee, and Whittier) are located close to the Great Smoky Mountains National Park, a popular tourist destination, and these small towns experience a steady flow of visitors during the summer and autumn months. Moreover, the entire region encountered increased in-migration in recent years and the picturesque area continues to attract new residents from other states. Overall then, the subject populations in this study are generally similar in basic demographic terms.

Table 1. Educational and professional background of older adults (51 to 65 years) from Wisconsin as reported in a background questionnaire

For years of higher education, the questionnaire displayed the following options: college (1–2), college (3–4), college (4+). Gender: F = female, M = male. IT = Information Technology department; UW = University of Wisconsin.

Table 2. Educational and professional background of older adults (51 to 65 years) from North Carolina

See Table 1 legend for details. MBA = masters of business administration.

Stimulus materials and recording procedure

Two types of recorded speech were obtained: read sentences and spontaneous talks. Each speaker read a set of 240 contextually and prosodically constrained sentences that were constructed to elicit variable emphasis in vowel production, which was a focus of a larger project in our lab. In these sentences, main sentence stress was systematically manipulated as in the examples (see Appendix 1 for a complete set of the recorded sentence material):

JANE knows the small bits are sharp.

No! JOHN knows the small bits are sharp.

John FEELS the small bits are sharp.

No! John KNOWS the small bits are sharp.

John knows the small SCREWS are sharp.

No! John knows the small BITS are sharp.

The complete sentence material was recorded by 76 speakers (40 older adults and 36 young adults). One advantage of collecting read sentences instead of a longer passage of read discourse was that it led to better control of the stress placement and fluency in reading. That is, the sentence material created a testing condition in which all speakers emphasized a particular word and read the phrase without a pause or hesitation. More within-speaker variability can be expected in reading a longer text that introduces considerable differences in reading style, tempo, repetitions, corrections, hesitations, and such. Because we were interested in articulation rate, which is linked to the speed of movement of articulators in a unit of time, fluency in reading was essential to obtaining a representative sample of speech tempo.

The second type of recorded speech consisted of a short informal and unconstrained talk, ranging in duration from 10 to 15 minutes. Most speakers recounted stories from their lives or spoke about their families, friends, hobbies, and their daily activities. They were instructed to speak at their typical tempo and mode, and that the topic of their talk was not of interest to the research. Rather, they were told that the focus of the study was to examine variation in pronunciation across different regions in the United States. Recordings of a talk were obtained from each of the older speakers. However, not all young speakers who read the sentence material were recorded producing an informal talk. For that reason, six new Wisconsin speakers and 12 new North Carolina speakers were brought to the study. These speakers recorded the informal talks only. Altogether, the recordings of the talks were obtained from 40 older speakers and 40 young speakers (20 from Wisconsin and 20 from North Carolina in each age group, evenly divided by gender: 10 men and 10 women) for a total of 80 speakers.

Recording of sentences was controlled by a custom program written in Matlab. The sentence pairs appeared on a computer monitor in random order. The participant read the sentence pair speaking to a head-mounted microphone (Shure SM10A), placed at a 1-inch distance from the lips. The sentences were recorded directly onto a hard drive disk at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz. Only fluently read sentences with a proper stress placement were accepted by the experimenter. The recordings were repeated as many times as needed to obtain satisfactory productions. The spontaneous talks were recorded in the same session using Adobe Audition speech analysis program. Two female research assistants helped with data collection, one in Wisconsin, and one in North Carolina. All participants in either state were thus recorded by the same experimenter. In general, the informal talks produced by North Carolina speakers were mostly uninterrupted by the experimenter. The speakers clearly enjoyed sharing stories from their lives and mostly did not require prompting. Wisconsin speakers ran out of topics more often and needed a leading question when they stopped talking.

Data analysis

Articulation rate in read sentences

Only 120 sentences from each participant were analyzed for the present study for a total of 9120 sentences. The second sentence in the pair was chosen because initial analyses indicated significant differences in the number of hesitations and pauses between the two sentences. The second sentence in the pair was also produced more fluently than the first by most of the participants. All acoustic waveform analyses were done using the Adobe Audition waveform editing program. The locations of sentence onsets and offsets were determined by hand and these values served as input to a Matlab program that calculated the overall sentence duration automatically, displaying the onset and offset markings in the waveform for the researcher to examine. A reliability check was performed by a second researcher on all measurements using the same Matlab program with graphical display of sentence onsets and offsets. Agreement between these two researchers was essentially 100% as all disagreements in measurements were noted and resolved. The average articulation rate for each sentence was measured in syllables per second, which was calculated by dividing the sentence duration by the total number of syllables. There were seven syllables in each sentence and the syllable count for each sentence was verified by a researcher who performed the reliability check on the whole dataset. The word No was excluded from the analyses. Because the focus of the study was to obtain fluent productions from each speaker, the pauses between words were very sparse throughout the entire sample. Nevertheless, if found, they were edited out and subtracted from the duration of the sentence.

Articulation rate in informal talks

To calculate articulation rate for informal speech, the talks were first transcribed. Two types of orthographic transcripts were created. In the first transcript, all words and sounds (such as hesitations or laughing) coming from the speaker were written down. The second transcript focused on fluent phrases only and eliminated all nonfluent productions identified in the first transcript. For the present purposes, the phrase was defined as a string of words containing five or more syllables uttered without a pause. These phrases were numbered consecutively for each subject and the articulation rate was calculated for these fluent phrases only. An example of the second transcript is given in Appendix 2. Next, the onset and offset of each phrase was measured using waveform editor (Adobe Audition) and the articulation rate was calculated in the same manner as for read sentences: by dividing the duration of each phrase by the number of spoken syllables as determined by the experimenter. After listening to each phrase and marking its temporal onsets and offsets, the experimenter counted the number of syllables it contained based on the spoken utterance (and not on its orthographic notation). A total of 4930 phrases were analyzed from all speakers and the number of syllables per phrase varied (mean = 12.7 syllables per phrase, SD = 6.9). As for the read sentences, a reliability check was performed by a second researcher using a dedicated custom Matlab program.

RESULTS

The overall mean articulation rate in read sentences was 3.40 syllables/second (SD = 0.42), whereas the rate in informal talks was 5.12 syllables/second (SD = 0.59), that is, 51% faster. The large difference between the read and spoken productions indicated that, in general, articulation rate in reading is much slower than in informal speech. Of interest to the study, however, was whether there were significant differences within each production type as a function of speaker dialect, age, and gender. The results were initially assessed by two separate univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA). In the first ANOVA, the dependent variable was articulation rate in reading and the between-subject factors were dialect, age, and gender. In the second ANOVA, the dependent variable was articulation rate in talks and the between-subject factors remained the same. Additional analyses, if necessary, were conducted as detailed in the following.Footnote 4

The effects of speaker dialect

In reading, the overall means were 3.54 syllables/second (SD = 0.34) for Wisconsin speakers and 3.27 syllables/second (SD = 0.44) for North Carolina speakers. The main effect of dialect was significant (F(1, 68) = 12.7, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.157), indicating that Wisconsin speakers demonstrated a faster articulation rate in reading as compared with North Carolina speakers. Using the same reading material and the same measurement criteria, the Wisconsin speakers read at a rate 8% faster than North Carolina speakers did. For informal talks, the second ANOVA also revealed a significant main effect of dialect (F(1, 72) = 28.8, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.286). The overall mean articulation rates were 5.41 syllables/second (SD = 0.48) for Wisconsin speakers and 4.81 syllables/second (SD = 0.54) for North Carolina speakers. Thus the articulation rate for Wisconsin speakers was 12.5% faster than for North Carolina speakers. These findings clearly show that the articulation rate, whether in reading or speaking, is faster for the regional variety of American English spoken in Wisconsin as compared with that spoken in North Carolina.

The effects of speaker age

Figure 1 shows the mean articulation rate for young and older adults in Wisconsin and North Carolina for the two types of productions. As can be seen, young adults tend to speak faster than older adults in both reading and informal talks in Wisconsin. However, North Carolina young adults tend to speak faster only in reading but not in informal talks. The ANOVA results for read sentences showed a significant main effect of age (F(1, 68) = 20.9, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.235). Young adults' articulation rate in reading was 11% faster than that of older adults (3.58 syllables/second [SD = 0.44] vs. 3.23 syllables/second [SD = 0.32]). For informal talks, however, the main effect of age was not significant, indicating no differences in articulation rate between the young and older adults (the overall means were 5.18 syllables/second [SD = 0.58] for young adults and 5.04 syllables/second [SD = 0.60] for the older adults). Because the results for Wisconsin suggested some differences between the two groups (see Figure 1), we conducted additional two-way ANOVAs with the between-factors age and gender separately for Wisconsin and North Carolina talks. The results for Wisconsin showed a significant main effect of age (F(1, 36) = 5.19, p = 0.029, η2 = 0.126) although the effect size was small. Young Wisconsin adults were shown to speak 6% faster than older adults (5.58 syllables/second [SD = 0.41] vs. 5.25 syllables/second [SD = 0.50]). For North Carolina, the main effect of age was not significant.

Figure 1. The effects of age on articulation rate for Wisconsin and North Carolina speakers in read sentences denoted here as reading style (RD) and informal talks as conversational style (CS).

Altogether, the results show that the articulation rate in reading is faster for young adults as compared with that for older adults in both Wisconsin and North Carolina. However, in informal speech, the results are less consistent. Young adults tend to speak faster than older adults do in Wisconsin but not in North Carolina, where young and older adults do not show differences in the articulation rate (4.79 syllables/second [SD = 0.44] and 4.83 syllables/second [SD = 0.64], respectively).

The effects of speaker gender

As Figure 2 shows, the differences in articulation rate as a function of speaker gender were very small. As a general tendency, men tended to speak slightly faster than women both in reading (3.48 syllables/second [SD = 0.43] vs. 3.33 syllables/second [SD = 0.40]) and in informal talks (5.2 syllables/second [SD = 0.57] vs. 5.03 syllables/second [SD = 0.61]). For read sentences, the articulation rate for men was 4.5% faster than for women. Although the ANOVA results showed a significant effect of gender (F(1, 68) = 4.06, p = 0.048, η2 = 0.056), its small effect size indicates that gender contributed very little to the variance accounted for. A near-significant interaction between gender and age (F(1, 68) = 3.85, p = 0.054, η2 = 0.054) pointed to the difference between the rates of young men and young women (3.74 syllables/second [SD = 0.42] vs. 3.43 syllables/second [SD = 0.41]). However, although young men tended to read faster than young women did, the size of this effect was again very small. For informal talks, the effect of gender was not significant nor were there any significant interactions between gender and any other factor.

Figure 2. The effects of gender on articulation rate for Wisconsin and North Carolina speakers in read sentences denoted here as reading style (RD) and informal talks as conversational style (CS).

We also conducted separate two-way ANOVAs for each dialect with the between-subject factors gender and age. The main effect of gender on either articulation rate (reading or talks) was again not significant for either Wisconsin or North Carolina speakers. However, there was one significant age by gender interaction for read sentences for North Carolina although its effect size was small (F(1, 34) = 4.8, p = 0.035, η2 = 0.124). This interaction indicated greater differences in the articulation rate between young men and young women as compared with older men and older women. A subsequent two-tailed independent samples t test showed that the difference for young adults was significant (t = 2.7, p = 0.015), showing that in North Carolina, young men read 17% faster than young women did (3.67 syllables/second [SD = 0.48] vs. 3.14 syllables/second [SD = 0.34]). The difference in speech rate between older men and older women was not significant (3.11 syllables/second [SD = 0.27] and 3.15 syllables/second [SD = 0.47], respectively).

Overall, the effects of speaker gender on articulation rate were small and were found mainly in reading where the articulation rate for men was slightly faster than for women, with the exception of North Carolina young men as compared with North Carolina young women. However, no differences as a function of speaker gender were found in the informal speech where all speakers, whether young or older, spoke equally fast.

The choice of the univariate ANOVAs in the present study was motivated by the fact that not all speakers for the young adults group participated in both tasks: reading and informal talks. Thus, the results for the articulation rate in read sentences and talks could not be compared directly. However, the older speakers did participate in both tasks. To address the question of whether there are in fact significant differences between the articulation rate in reading and in informal speech a within-subject ANOVA was conducted for the older adults only. In this analysis, the speaking condition (reading and talk) was the dependent variable, and dialect and gender were the between-subject factors. The results showed a strong significant effect of speaking condition (F(1, 36) = 495.26, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.932). For the older adults, the articulation rate in reading was significantly slower than that in informal talks (3.23 syllables/second [SD = 0.32] vs. 5.04 syllables/second [SD = 0.60]). The effect of dialect was also significant (F(1, 36) = 6.69, p = 0.014, η2 = 0.157) indicating that Wisconsin speakers demonstrate a faster articulation rate than North Carolina speakers do (4.29 syllables/second [SD = 0.44] vs. 3.98 syllables/second [SD = 0.41]). Across the present speaking conditions, Wisconsin speakers spoke about 8% faster. The effect of gender was not significant. There were also no significant interactions.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

The present results provide a broad set of data to allow detailed comparison of articulation rate in two regional varieties of American English, one Northern and the other Southern. The study provides strong evidence that, in both reading and informal speech, the articulation rate of the Northern speakers was higher than that of the Southern speakers, as much as 8% in reading and 12.5% in informal talks. Considering other factors such as age and gender, it was found that young adults read faster than older adults in both the North and the South. However, in informal speech, young adults tended to speak faster than older adults do in the Northern dialect but not in the Southern dialect where the articulation rates of young and older adults did not differ. The effects of gender were smaller and less consistent. In general, men tended to speak a little faster than women but the differences were negligible. There was one exception to this trend among young adults in North Carolina. In reading, the articulation rate of young Southern men was significantly higher than of young Southern women.

Comparing the present results with those in previously published studies, we detect numerous important similarities as well as a few differences. In particular, Byrd (Reference Byrd1994) reported notable differences in speech rate in the TIMIT database across eight broadly defined dialect regions in the United States. The corpus included read sentences only and pauses were included in the calculation of speech rate. The speakers were mostly young and mostly male. It was found that the speech rate among Southern speakers tended to be slower than among the Northern speakers. In terms of speaker gender, men spoke on average 6% faster than women did (4.69 syllables/second vs. 4.42 syllables/second). It must be underscored that these general trends in the TIMIT database need to be interpreted with caution, given the unbalanced design in terms of speaker gender (69.5% men and 30.5% women) and broad regional distribution of speakers classified as Northern and Southern (some shortcomings of the TIMIT database are discussed in Keating, Byrd, Flemming, & Todaka [Reference Keating, Byrd, Flemming and Todaka1994]). Nonetheless, trends in that study and the present study are similar in terms of finding that speakers from the North spoke faster, on average, than speakers from the South did. Although the effects of speaker gender were more variable in our study, the articulation rate for men was also slightly faster than for women.

The tendency for men to speak faster than women was also found in Whiteside (Reference Whiteside1996) for British Northern English. Results showed a higher articulation rate for men than for women (4.10 syllables/second vs. 3.38 syllables/second). The much higher rate (21%) for men may be attributed to a small sample size and perhaps to differences in the ages of the speakers, which were not reported in that study. Our results from a much larger subject pool and an extensive corpus of data do not support this finding. However, there was one exception in our data that corresponds to the finding in Whiteside (Reference Whiteside1996). In particular, the articulation rate in reading for young North Carolina men was 17% higher as compared with young North Carolina women, approximating the rate reported in Whiteside (Reference Whiteside1996). This suggests that the tendency for men to speak faster may be more variable across regional varieties, and in some regions, the articulation rate for men may be much higher than for women.

Turning to the articulation rate in informal speech, Verhoeven et al. (Reference Verhoeven, De Pauw and Kloots2004) addressed the effects of dialect, gender, and age on the articulation rate (excluding silent pauses) and speaking rate (including the pauses) in free conversations by 160 Dutch-speaking teachers in the Netherlands and Belgium. Verhoeven et al. (Reference Verhoeven, De Pauw and Kloots2004) contrasted with the just-discussed work by Byrd and others in regard to areal/dialect distribution: previous studies have typically taken speakers from across broadly defined regions (Byrd, Reference Byrd1994; Clopper et al., Reference Clopper, Pisoni and de Jong2005), whereas Verhoeven et al. provided a picture of regional variation in two national standards and a comparison over those two broader groups. The variables in that study were comparable with those in the present study although the number of speakers per region in the former was smaller (10 men and 10 women). Assessing the articulation rate, the study found significant effects of country (the Netherlands and Belgium), region, age, and gender. Men spoke 6% faster than women did (4.79 syllables/second vs. 4.50 syllables/second) and young adults (aged below 40 years) spoke 5.4% faster than older adults (aged over 45 years). On average, articulation rate in the Netherlands was 16.2% faster than in Belgium (5.05 syllables/second vs. 4.23 syllables/second). Interestingly, there were no statistical differences in articulation rate between the regional varieties in Belgium. In the Netherlands, there was only one region that differed significantly from the remaining three. The significant effect of region disappeared in the assessment of the speaking rate, however; whereas men still spoke faster than women (5.2%) and younger adults spoke also 5.2% faster than older adults did.

Comparing our present results with those in Verhoeven et al. (Reference Verhoeven, De Pauw and Kloots2004) for the national standard varieties of Dutch in the Netherlands and Belgium, we found a much stronger effect of region that was manifested in both read sentences and informal talks of Wisconsin and North Carolina speakers. Our results for gender are generally consistent, although men in our study spoke only 3.3% faster than women did and the main effect of gender was not significant. In terms of age, we found a consistency only in Wisconsin where the present young adults spoke about 6% faster than older adults did in informal talks. (Recall that there were no differences between the two age groups in North Carolina.)

The question arises whether our reported variation in the articulation rate is of any perceptual relevance or whether the differences were too small to be detected by an ordinary listener. The results from Quené (Reference Quené2007) may shed some light on this issue. In his study, listeners detected a change in the articulation rate when the difference was about 5%. This just noticeable difference (JND) indicates that a 5% or more difference of rate in speech fragments could be perceived by the listener as faster (or slower). Most of the articulation rate differences reported here are clearly well above Quené's reported JND of 5%, ranging for most of our measures from 6% to 17%. Except for the effects of speaker gender, the perceptual cues in our data should be available to listeners in creating impressions of how fast others speak.

For informal talks, one issue has not been controlled for in the present study. As indicated in Quené's recent statistical model (2008), speech tempo is strongly influenced by the length of the phrase. Because longer phrases contain more syllables than shorter phrases, they tend to be spoken faster, which shortens syllable durations and increases the articulation rate. In that study, phrase length was found to vary with the speaker's age. Older speakers produced shorter phrases than the younger speakers did and they also tended to vary the length of their phrases more often. If so, speech tempo may be only weakly affected by speaker's age if differences in phrase length are accounted for. This implies that the differences in the articulation rate between the Northern and Southern speakers (or differences due to speaker age and gender) may be significant but they will diminish if phrase length is included in assessing the results.

Although we did not consider phrase length in our analyses of informal talks, the results for read sentences can be interpreted in the light of Quené's findings. Those sentences contained a fixed number of syllables per phrase. Because each speaker uttered seven syllables in a phrase, it was clearly the differences in the phrase duration that affected the articulation rate. The present Wisconsin speakers read faster than the North Carolina speakers did (the mean phrase durations were 2.01 sec and 2.20 sec, respectively), young adults read faster than older adults (2.0 sec vs. 2.20 sec), and men read a little faster than women (2.06 sec vs. 2.15 sec). Given the fixed number of syllables, a longer phrase indicates a slower articulation rate and shorter phrase indicates a faster rate. The present results for reading thus clearly show that articulation rate varies as a function of speaker dialect, age, and, to some extent, gender.

Can we assume that Wisconsin speakers as well as young adults and men are simply better readers than North Carolina speakers, older adults, and women? Given the large sample and significant statistical effects, we have to exclude that these results came about by chance. The reading data show a systematic effect of between-subject factors. Because all reading productions were fluent, it is unclear what would account for the notion of a “better reader” (or perhaps a “faster reader”) if no pauses or hesitations were present in a prosodically constrained read phrase. Although we can only speculate at present in the absence of articulatory data, these effects could be due to differences in the articulatory paths for older and younger adults as well as for women and men, the former exhibiting more careful productions than the latter. However, the dialectal differences cannot be easily explained by the same reasoning. It might be the case that some other dialect-specific segmental properties play a role here. So far, we have found significant effects of vowel duration between the Northern and Southern speech (Jacewicz et al., Reference Jacewicz, Fox and Salmons2007) and significant differences in stop closure durations, Wisconsin closures being longer than North Carolina closures (Jacewicz, Fox, & Lyle, Reference Jacewicz, Fox and Lyle2008). However, these findings are far from making conclusive statements about the dialect-specific features with regard to articulation rate. More work is needed to explain which features contribute mostly to the perception of the Southern speech being slower than that of Northern speakers.

A reanalysis of the present data for informal talks would be necessary to determine whether phrase length is a predictor of differences in articulation rate as a function of speaker dialect, age, and gender. As a general rule, do Wisconsin speakers produce longer phrases that shorten syllable durations as compared with North Carolina speakers or does the dialectal difference originate in dialect-specific temporal differences between speech segments? Because the present results for reading and informal talks do not overlap for the effects of age (see the differences in informal speech for young and older adults in Wisconsin and North Carolina), we may infer that these differences are due to the lack of inclusion of phrase length as a predictor in the analysis of informal talks. However, more work is necessary to prove the validity of this interpretation.

In conclusion, this study yielded several robust findings with regard to the articulation rate in read and informal speech in the productions of mostly the same participants. First, the articulation rate of Wisconsin speakers was distinctly faster than that of North Carolina speakers. Second, young adults in Wisconsin spoke and read faster than older Wisconsinites did. For North Carolina, articulation rates in reading followed the same pattern. However, in informal speech, no significant differences due to speaker age were found.Footnote 5 Finally, even though the effects of gender were present, they were weak. It can only be suggested that men may speak faster than women do under some circumstances, namely reading. This limited finding is consistent with most previous research.

As the body of work on the sociophonetics of American English continues to grow in scope and depth, we argue that it is important to include fundamental phonetic information, such as these results on articulation rate, as part of our catalog of regional differences and patterns of change in American English.

APPENDIX 1

The following sets of sentences were recorded by each speaker. All two-set sentences were randomly presented to the subject in two stimulus lists. The sentences in which the main stressed falls on the first and second word position, respectively, served as distracters and were not included in the final analyses.

Vowels before a voiceless consonant in a word

bits

JANE knows the small bits are sharp.

No! JOHN knows the small bits are sharp.

John FEELS the small bits are sharp.

No! John KNOWS the small bits are sharp.

John knows the SOFT bits are sharp.

No! John knows the SMALL bits are sharp.

John knows the small SCREWS are sharp.

No! John knows the small BITS are sharp.

John knows the small bits are DULL.

No! John knows the small bits are SHARP.

baits

MOM said the dull baits are best.

No! DAD said the dull baits are best.

Dad THINKS the dull baits are best.

No! Dad SAID the dull baits are best.

Dad said the BRIGHT baits are best.

No! Dad said the DULL baits are best.

Dad said the dull HOOKS are best.

No! Dad said the dull BAITS are best.

Dad said the dull baits are WORST.

No! Dad said the dull baits are BEST.

bets

FRANK said the small bets are low.

No! JOHN said the small bets are low.

John THOUGHT the small bets are low.

No! John SAID the small bets are low.

John said the BIG bets are low.

No! John said the SMALL bets are low.

John said the small POTS are low.

No! John said the small BETS are low.

John said the small bets are HIGH.

No! John said the small bets are LOW.

bats

SAM said the small bats are fast.

No! DOC said the small bats are fast.

Doc THINKS the small bats are fast.

No! Doc SAID the small bats are fast.

Doc said the LARGE bats are fast.

No! Doc said the SMALL bats are fast.

Doc said the small BIRDS are fast.

No! Doc said the small BATS are fast.

Doc said the small bats are SLOW.

No! Doc said the small bats are FAST.

bites

JANE thinks the small bites are deep.

No! SUE thinks the small bites are deep.

Sue KNOWS the small bites are deep.

No! Sue THINKS the small bites are deep.

Sue thinks the LARGE bites are deep.

No! Sue thinks the SMALL bites are deep.

Sue thinks the small CUTS are deep.

No! Sue thinks the small BITES are deep.

Sue thinks the small bites are WIDE.

No! Sue thinks the small bites are DEEP.

Vowels before a voiced consonant in a word

bids

BOB thinks the fall bids are low.

No! TED thinks the fall bids are low.

Ted KNOWS the fall bids are low.

No! Ted THINKS the fall bids are low.

Ted thinks the SPRING bids are low.

No! Ted thinks the FALL bids are low.

Ted thinks the fall SALES are low.

No! Ted thinks the fall BIDS are low.

Ted thinks the fall bids are HIGH.

No! Ted thinks the fall bids are LOW.

bades

(The nonsense word bade was explained to the speaker as indicating “a brand of knife, a brand name.”)

TOM says the dull bades are cheap.

No! TED says the dull bades are cheap.

Ted THINKS the dull bades are cheap.

No! Ted SAYS the dull bades are cheap.

Ted says the SHARP bades are cheap.

No! Ted says the DULL bades are cheap.

Ted says the dull FORKS are cheap.

No! Ted says the dull BADES are cheap.

Ted says the dull bades are WEAK.

No! Ted says the dull bades are CHEAP.

beds

TOM said the tall beds are warm.

No! ROB said the tall beds are warm.

Rob THINKS the tall beds are warm.

No! Rob SAID the tall beds are warm.

Rob said the SHORT beds are warm.

No! Rob said the TALL beds are warm.

Rob said the tall CHAIRS are warm.

No! Rob said the tall BEDS are warm.

Rob said the tall beds are COLD.

No! Rob said the tall beds are WARM.

bads

(The speaker was told that bad refers to “an error or mistake.” For example, if someone makes an error, he or she might say “my bad” instead of “my mistake.”).

NICK thinks the small bads are worse.

No! MIKE thinks the small bads are worse.

Mike KNOWS the small bads are worse.

No! Mike THINKS the small bads are worse.

Mike thinks the BIG bads are worse.

No! Mike thinks the SMALL bads are worse.

Mike thinks the small GOODS are worse.

No! Mike thinks the small BADS are worse.

Mike thinks the small bads are BEST.

No! Mike thinks the small bads are WORSE.

bides

(The nonsense word bide was explained to the speaker as indicating “a small animal, a type of dog.”)

SUE thinks the small bides are cute.

No! JANE thinks the small bides are cute.

Jane KNOWS the small bides are cute.

No! Jane THINKS the small bides are cute.

Jane thinks the SHORT bides are cute.

No! Jane thinks the TALL bides are cute.

Jane thinks the small CATS are cute.

No! Jane thinks the small BIDES are cute.

Jane thinks the small bides are GROSS.

No! Jane thinks the small bides are CUTE.

APPENDIX 2

Example of a transcript used in calculating the articulation rate in informal talks. The fluent phrases were numbered consecutively and the articulation rate was calculated for these phrases only. All hesitations, pauses, and fillers (marked here in italics) were excluded from analyses.

Older North Carolina female speaker

52) and to tell a funny story about,

uh, my speech,

we have a,

53) a mountain pasture where we keep our cows, our cattle in the uh,

spring and summer,

and this,

uh,

54) man from Florida came up and bought the place next to it,

55) and so, we were up there checking on the cattle one day and he,

56) we stopped and started talking to him and,

57) he got acquainted with us

and uh,

58) he was just delighted with my speech and I didn't think there was anything wrong with it but,

59) I did not get the feeling that he was making fun of me and,

60) he kept saying that the next time they came up from Florida that he was going to,

um,

have,

61) bring his wife and let her hear me talk,

Footnotes

1. This popular stereotype is confirmed by media attention (see www.pbs.org/speak/speech/prejudice/attitudes). Coverage of the 2008 U.S. presidential campaign reinforced this stereotype. Simple Google searches (December 22, 2007) for “fast-talking” plus the names of two Northern candidates (Rudy Giuliani of New York, Mitt Romney of Massachusetts) yielded a total of 9260 hits and similar searches with “slow-talking” plus two Southern candidates (Mike Huckabee of Arkansas, Fred Thompson of Tennessee) yielded 3370.

2. The TIMIT database is named after Texas Instruments and Massachusetts Institute of Technology, both of which worked on the database.

3. Other work on gender and speech rate has been reviewed recently by Heffernan (Reference Heffernan2007).

4. These analyses were carried out on articulation rate means for individual speakers in either the read sentence or the informal talk conditions. We did not address potential within-speaker variation in articulation rate.

5. Note that possible “age-grading” of speech or articulation rate could raise questions about the apparent time construct.

References

REFERENCES

Abercrombie, David. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Byrd, Dani. (1994). Relations of sex and dialect to reduction. Speech Communication 15:3954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, Cynthia G., Pisoni, David, & de Jong, Kenneth. (2005). Acoustic characteristics of the vowel systems of six regional varieties of American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 118:16611676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crystal, Thomas H., & House, Arthur S. (1982). Segmental durations in connected speech signals: Preliminary results. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 72:705716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crystal, Thomas H., & House, Arthur S. (1988a). The duration of American-English vowels: An overview. Journal of Phonetics 16:263284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, Thomas H., & House, Arthur S. (1988b). Segmental durations in connected-speech signals: Current results. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 83:15531573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heffernan, Kevin M. (2007). Phonetic distinctiveness as a sociolinguistic variable. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Hewlett, Nigel, & Rendall, Monica. (1998). Rural versus urban accent as an influence on the rate of speech. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 28:6371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirose, Keikichi, & Kawanami, Hiromichi. (2002). Temporal rate of change of dialogue speech in prosodic units as compared to read speech. Speech Communication 36:97111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, Peter, & Kadi-Hanifi, Karimai. (1991). Comparison of prosodic properties between read and spontaneous speech material. Speech Communication 10:163169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacewicz, Ewa, Fox, Robert A., & Salmons, Joseph. (2006). Prosodic prominence effects on vowels in chain shifts. Language Variation and Change 18:285316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacewicz, Ewa, Fox, Robert A., & Salmons, Joseph (2007). Vowel duration in three American English dialects. American Speech 82:367385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacewicz, Ewa, Fox, Robert A., & Lyle, Samantha. (2008). Variation in stop consonant voicing in two regional varieties of American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 124:2559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, Patricia, Byrd, Dani, Flemming, Edward, & Todaka, Yuichi. (1994). Phonetic analyses of word and segment variation using the TIMIT corpus of American English. Speech Communication 14:131142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William, Ash, Sharon, & Boberg, Charles. (2006). Atlas of North American English: Phonetics, phonology, and sound change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laver, John, & Trudgill, Peter. (1991) Phonetic and linguistic markers in speech. In Laver, J. (ed.), The Gift of Speech: Readings in the analysis of speech and voice. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. 235264Google Scholar
Niedzielski, Nancy, & Preston, Dennis R. (1999). Folk linguistics. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Quené, Hugo. (2007). On the just noticeable difference for tempo in speech. Journal of Phonetics 35:353362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quené, Hugo (2008). Multilevel modeling of between-speaker and within-speaker variation in spontaneous speech tempo. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123:11041113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramig, Lorraine. (1983). Effects of physiological aging on speaking and reading rates. Journal of Communication Disorders 16:217226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ray, George, & Zahn, Christopher. (1990). Regional speech rates in the United States: A preliminary analysis. Communication Research Reports 7:3437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roach, Peter. (1998). Myth 18: Some languages are spoken more quickly than others. In Bauer, L. & Trudgill, P. (eds.), Language Myths. London: Penguin. 150158.Google Scholar
Smith, Bruce L., Wasowicz, Jan, & Preston, Judy. (1987). Temporal characteristics of the speech of normal elderly adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30:522529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, Erik. (2008). Rural Southern white accents. In Schneider, E. W. (ed.), Varieties of English 2: The Americas and the Caribbean. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 87114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tillery, Jan, & Bailey, Guy. (2008).The urban South: Phonology. In Schneider, E. W. (ed.), Varieties of English 2: The Americas and the Caribbean. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 115128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsao, Ying-Chiao, Weismer, Gary, & Iqbal, Kamran. (2006). Interspeaker variation in habitual speaking rate: Additional evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 49:11561164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, Jo, De Pauw, Guy, & Kloots, Hanne. (2004). Speech rate in a pluricentric language: A comparison between Dutch in Belgium and the Netherlands. Language and Speech 47:297308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiteside, Sandra P. (1996). Temporal-based acoustic-phonetic patterns in read speech: Some evidence for speaker sex differences. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 26:2340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuan, Jiahong, Cieri, Chris, & Liberman, Mark. (2006). Towards an integrated understanding of speaking rate in conversation. Paper presented at the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (Interspeech 2006), Pittsburgh. (Full paper available at http://ldc.upenn.edu/myl/llog/icslp06_final.pdf.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Educational and professional background of older adults (51 to 65 years) from Wisconsin as reported in a background questionnaire

Figure 1

Table 2. Educational and professional background of older adults (51 to 65 years) from North Carolina

Figure 2

Figure 1. The effects of age on articulation rate for Wisconsin and North Carolina speakers in read sentences denoted here as reading style (RD) and informal talks as conversational style (CS).

Figure 3

Figure 2. The effects of gender on articulation rate for Wisconsin and North Carolina speakers in read sentences denoted here as reading style (RD) and informal talks as conversational style (CS).