Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T14:18:52.911Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive Linguistics and interactional discourse: time to enter into dialogue

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 November 2015

ELISABETH ZIMA
Affiliation:
University of Freiburg, Germany
GEERT BRÔNE
Affiliation:
University of Leuven, Belgium
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Usage-based theories hold that the sole resource for language users’ linguistic systems is language use (Barlow & Kemmer, 2000; Langacker, 1988; Tomasello, 1999, 2003). Researchers working in the usage-based paradigm, which is often equated with cognitive-functional linguistics (e.g., Ibbotson, 2013, Tomasello, 2003), seem to widely agree that the primary setting for language use is interaction, with spontaneous face-to-face interaction playing a primordial role (e.g., Bybee, 2010; Clark, 1996; Geeraerts & Cuyckens, 2007; Langacker, 2008; Oakley & Hougaard, 2008; Zlatev, 2014). It should, then, follow that usage-based models of language are not only compatible with evidence from communication research but also that they are intrinsically grounded in authentic, multi-party language use in all its diversity and complexities. This should be a logical consequence, as a usage-based understanding of language processing and human sense-making cannot be separated from the study of interaction. However, the overwhelming majority of the literature in Cognitive Linguistics (CL) does not deal with the analysis of dialogic data or with issues of interactional conceptualization. It is our firm belief that this is at odds with the interactional foundation of the usage-based hypothesis. Furthermore, we are convinced that an ‘interactional turn’ is not only essential to the credibility and further development of Cognitive Linguistics as a theory of language and cognition as such. Rather, CL-inspired perspectives on interactional language use may provide insights that other, non-cognitive approaches to discourse and interaction are bound to overlook. To that aim, this special issue brings together four contributions that involve the analysis of interactional discourse phenomena by drawing on tools and methods from the broad field of Cognitive Linguistics.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2015 

1. On the place of interactional discourse in Cognitive Linguistics

Usage-based theories hold that the sole resource for language users’ linguistic systems is language use (Barlow & Kemmer, Reference Barlow and Kemmer2000; Langacker, Reference Langacker and Rudzka-Ostyn1988; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello1999, Reference Tomasello2003). Researchers working in the usage-based paradigm, which is often equated with cognitive-functional linguistics (e.g., Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2013, Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003), seem to widely agree that the primary setting for language use is interaction, with spontaneous face-to-face interaction playing a primordial role (e.g., Bybee, Reference Bybee2010; Clark, Reference Clark1996; Geeraerts & Cuyckens, Reference Geeraerts, Cuyckens, Geeraerts and Cuyckens2007; Langacker, Reference Langacker2008; Oakley & Hougaard, Reference Oakley and Hougaard2008; Zlatev, Reference Zlatev, Dor, Knight and Lewis2014). It should, then, follow that usage-based models of language are not only compatible with evidence from communication research but also that they are intrinsically grounded in authentic, multi-party language use in all its diversity and complexities. This should be a logical consequence, as a usage-based understanding of language processing and human sense-making cannot be separated from the study of interaction. However, the overwhelming majority of the literature in Cognitive Linguistics (CL) does not deal with the analysis of dialogic data or with issues of interactional conceptualization. It is our firm belief that this is at odds with the interactional foundation of the usage-based hypothesis. Furthermore, we are convinced that an ‘interactional turn’ is not only essential to the credibility and further development of Cognitive Linguistics as a theory of language and cognition as such. Rather, CL-inspired perspectives on interactional language use may provide insights that other, non-cognitive approaches to discourse and interaction are bound to overlook. To that aim, this special issue brings together four contributions that involve the analysis of interactional discourse phenomena by drawing on tools and methods from the broad field of Cognitive Linguistics.

Despite the relative under-representation of interactional studies in CL, the papers in this special issue do not stand alone, isolated from the rest of the field. Rather, they line up with recent developments within CL and neighboring research areas that have adopted a dialogic view on cognition and language use as well as the emergence and the nature of grammatical systems. There is a growing consensus that the prototypically interactive nature of language use as a negotiation process between two or more participants needs to be treated as an inherent part of cognitive models of language (Brône & Oben, Reference Brône, Oben, Veale, Feyaerts and Forceville2013; Brône & Zima, Reference Brône, Zima, Giora and Du Bois2014; Chafe, Reference Chafe1994; Clark, Reference Clark1996; Deppermann, Reference Deppermann, Deppermann, Fiehler and Spranz-Fogasy2002, Reference Deppermann2007, Reference Deppermann2012; Du Bois, Reference Du Bois and Englebretson2007; Langacker, Reference Langacker2001, Reference Langacker2008, Reference Langacker2013; Levinson, Reference Levinson2006; Pickering & Garrod, Reference Pickering and Garrod2004; Verhagen, Reference Verhagen2005; Zima, Reference Zima2013).

Most prominently, this interactional perspective lies at the heart of Michael Tomasello’s (Reference Tomasello1998, Reference Tomasello1999, Reference Tomasello2003) work on shared intentionality and intersubjectivity, which has widely influenced core areas of Cognitive Linguistics. For instance, in Construction Grammar, there is growing awareness of the encoding of interactional meaning and function in the grammatical system (e.g., Boogaart, Colleman, & Rutten, Reference Boogaart, Colleman and Rutten2014; Fried & Östman, Reference Fried and Östman2005; Östman, Reference Östman, Östman and Fried2005; Verhagen, Reference Verhagen2005). A constantly growing body of work challenges the seemingly static concept of constructions as conventionalized pairings of form and meaning, arguing that, in interaction, constructions aren’t simply instantiated as “prepackaged wholes”, but rather they are “tools for the situated construction of context-sensitive turns: schemata which can be flexibly adapted to interactional contingencies” (Deppermann Reference Deppermann, Auer and Pfänder2011a, p. 121). Most notably in the German research context, ‘Interactional Construction Grammar’ has developed into a very vivid research area at the intersection of CL and interactional linguistics. Exemplary studies of this perspective are, among many others, Auer (Reference Auer2009), Auer and Pfänder (Reference Auer and Pfänder2011a), Brône and Zima (Reference Brône, Zima, Giora and Du Bois2014), Deppermann (Reference Deppermann2007, Reference Deppermann, Auer and Pfänder2011a, Reference Depermann, Lasch and Ziem2011b), Fischer (Reference Fischer2010, Reference Fischer, Lasch and Ziem2015, and this issue), Fox and Thompson (Reference Fox and Thompson2007), Günthner, Bücker, and Imo (Reference Günthner, Bücker and Imo2014), Günthner and Imo (Reference Günthner and Imo2006), and Imo (Reference Imo2007, Reference Imo2009).

Moreover, a growing number of studies also focus on the in vivo (Nerlich & Clarke, Reference Nerlich and Clarke2001, Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Nerlich, Todd, Vimala and Clarke2003) or online (Brône & Coulson, Reference Brône and Coulson2010; Coulson, Reference Coulson2000, Reference Coulson2006; Fauconnier & Turner, Reference Fauconnier and Turner2002; Langlotz, this issue; Zima, Reference Zima2013) character of meaning construction in language, and aim to gain further insights into the interaction of language structure and dynamically evolving discourse. In these studies, attention is drawn to the active process of meaning construction in longer stretches of discourse, which is also apparent in CL studies on discourse coherence, the dynamic construction of discourse representations, perspectivity and polyphony in different text types, and the nature of meaning-making in interaction as processes of joint construction and manipulation of mental spaces (a.o. Brône, Reference Brône2008, Reference Brône2010; Chilton, Reference Chilton2005; Coulson, Reference Coulson, Coulson and Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk2005; Dancygier, Reference Dancygier2005; Hougaard & Oakley, Reference Hougaard and Oakley2008; Langacker, Reference Langacker2001Reference Langacker2008, Reference Langacker2013; Pascual, Reference Pascual2014).

This nascent interest in issues of interactional meaning constitution is accompanied by the rapid evolution of cognitively inspired multimodality research. This particularly fruitful research area has enlarged the traditional focus of linguistics on verbal structures to include bodily means of expression, like gesture, posture, and gaze, into the scope of analysis. Drawing on insights from gesture studies, psycholinguistics, and Conversation Analysis, studies in this line have revisited core concepts of Cognitive Linguistics, such as conceptual metaphor (Cienki, Reference Cienki, Cienki and Müller2008; Cienki & Müller, Reference Cienki and Müller2008a, Reference Cienki, Müller and Gibbs2008b; Forceville & Urios-Aparisi, Reference Forceville and Urios-Aparisi2009; McNeill, Reference McNeill1992; Müller, Reference Müller2004), metonymy (Forceville, Reference Forceville, Caballero and Pinar2010; Mittelberg, Reference Mittelberg2006; Mittelberg & Waugh, Reference Mittelberg, Waugh, Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill and Bressem2014), image schemas (Cienki, Reference Cienki2002, Reference Cienki and Pinar Sanz2013), and the notion of constructions as the basic units of language (Schoonjans, Reference Schoonjans2014; Reference Schoonjans, Sambre, Brône and FeyaertsSchoonjans, Sambre, Brône, & Feyaerts, in press; Steen & Turner, Reference Steen, Turner, Borkent, Dancygier and Hinnell2013; Reference Zima, Ruiz de Mendoza and Pérez SobrinoZima, in press). All these studies are intrinsically dialogic in orientation and put the actual, embodied speaker and his/her gestures, gazes, postures, facial expressions, etc. into the focus of attention, i.e., the data are no longer depersonalized corpus instances that are stripped of any reference to actual speakers. It also actively acknowledges that multimodal messages only acquire meanings in situated, dialogic, and necessarily embodied communication by the integration of input from verbal and kinetic modalities. This line of research is represented in this issue by Oben and Brône’s study at the intersection of Cognitive Linguistics, multimodality research, discourse studies, and psycholinguistics (for detailed descriptions of the papers in this special issue, see Section 3) and the theoretical plea for a multimodal reconsideration of the concepts of the ‘usage event’ and the ‘linguistic sign’ by Alan Cienki.

Nonetheless, despite these encouraging developments in Cognitive Linguistics, the observation remains that the study of authentic, interactional discourse data is not common practice in Cognitive Linguistic work. Most CL analyses rely on monological and to a large extent written data samples. As a consequence, Cognitive Linguistics has been criticized for being non-dialogic in orientation (cf. Fischer, Reference Fischer, Fischer and Stefanowitsch2006, and this issue; Langacker, Reference Langacker2008) and for suffering from a written language bias (Cienki, this issue; Linell, Reference Linell1998). The compatibility of CL theories and concepts with the nature and affordances of conversation is also put into question by more immediately dialogic research paradigms, most notably in Conversation Analysis that hardly ever incorporates insights from CL work (see, however, Depperman, Reference Deppermann2012, for an inside-CA perspective that argues for the need to take a more explicit cognitive perspective in research on talk-in-interaction). It seems, however, that both the under-representation of interactional data in CL and the negligence of CL work and ideas by CA and various strands of discourse studies are not so much due to obvious incompatibilities between Cognitive Linguistics and the nature of interactional communication. Rather, we think that the potential of Cognitive Linguistics in the domain of interactional discourse has simply been underexplored and largely remains to be uncovered.

This is particularly unfortunate as, to our mind, three basic tenets of Cognitive Linguistics strongly favor a CL approach to interactional meaning constitution:

  • the recontextualization approach;

  • the construal hypothesis;

  • the usage-based model.

Each of these tenets will be discussed in more detail in the following section.

2. On Cognitive Linguistics’ suitability for interaction research

As Geeraerts (Reference Geeraerts and Geeraerts2006, p. 25) argues, the recontextualization approach that is typical of usage-based approaches in general, and Cognitive Linguistic theories in particular, has put language use back in the focus of attention, from which it had disappeared in much of twentieth-century theoretical linguistics. While isolated, often invented, examples have long dominated as ‘empirical’ evidence in linguistic research, most notably in generative approaches, CL posits that the study of linguistic structure needs to be based on contextualized instances of authentic language use. As a consequence, especially over the past twenty years, the construction and skillful use of corpora has gained more and more importance, and today virtually all of Cognitive Linguistic research involves corpus studies or the use of other empirical methods (Janda, Reference Janda2015). In that vein, Stefanowitsch (Reference Stefanowitsch, Brdar, Gries and Žic Fuchs2011, p. 272) argues that “from this perspective, the linguistic corpus gains a central place in linguistic theory: it becomes a model of linguistic usage (both input and output) of an ‘average’ speaker”. By alluding to the ‘average’ speaker, he implicitly points to a fundamental principle of Cognitive Linguistics: the main interest is the relationship between cognition and the language system. It essentially deals with issues such as conventionalization, routinization, entrenchment, grammaticalization, lexicalization, etc. It is much less concerned with individual speakers or interactants, and as a matter of principle generalizes away from single, situated instances. This is a major difference with many other research paradigms that are much more concerned with the details of particular pieces of interactional data, most notably Conversation Analysis, and which in turn often shy away from generalizing over instances. This fundamental difference in focus and primary interest, among other factors mentioned, most likely contributes to the fact that interactionally minded researchers are by and large keeping their distance to CL and vice versa (for approaches arguing for fruitful synergies between CA and Cognitive Grammar, however, see Deppermann, Reference Deppermann2007, Reference Deppermann2012; Etelämäki & Visapää, Reference Etelämäki and Visapää2014; and this special issue’s papers by Langlotz and Fischer).

However, we believe that instead of regarding the CL interest in the language system as essentially incompatible with an interaction-focused perspective, a synergetic approach which combines insights from both fields will not only yield more encompassing insights in the cognitive underpinnings of interactional language use but, most notably, it will prevent CL from replicating long-established insights from Conversation Analysis and other discourse-oriented fields. In other words, doing interactionally oriented Cognitive Linguistics should neither be tantamount to ‘doing CL all over again’ nor to ‘doing CA / discourse linguistics all over again’. To be sure, by insisting on an interactional turn in CL, we do not wish to argue that the research done thus far in CL or every study that does not deal with interactional phenomena and data is irrelevant to the study of interaction from a CL perspective. On the contrary, the aim should be to gradually take well-established concepts from CL, to confront them with interactional discourse data, and to see how they react to this confrontation. That is, what needs to be seen is whether they do stand the proof or whether and how they need to be modulated/adapted. This approach, of course, may result in the observation that some CL concepts are not quite compatible with interactional language use. This should, however, not be seen as a threat to CL but as an avoidable step towards a plausible cognitive model of (interactional) language use.

Indeed, existing studies that have already applied theoretical concepts and methods from Cognitive Linguistics to the domain of interaction studies (and the papers in this special issues do just that; see also Hougaard and Oakley, Reference Hougaard and Oakley2008, for another interesting collection), in fact show that many concepts do hold in the interactional domain, although probably not un-modulated. Again, this is especially evident in work on Interactional Construction Grammar, where the concept of construction has proven to be generally well-suited to account for spoken language structures (much better than generative approaches), but which has also revealed the need to rethink some aspects of the concept (most notably its degree of fixedness and granularity; see especially Auer & Pfänder, Reference Auer, Pfänder, Auer and Pfänder2011b; Deppermann, Reference Depermann, Lasch and Ziem2011b; Fischer, this issue; Imo, Reference Imo2009).

Next to the recontextualization approach, another cornerstone of Cognitive Linguistics that makes it theoretically well suited for an extension to interactional discourse is the construal hypothesis. One reason is the fact that it regards meaning to be intrinsically speaker-oriented (again in the sense of focusing on an ‘average’ language user). It entails that meaning is not fixed and stable, but is rather construed by conceptualizers. Construal always involves a given perspective (spatial, epistemic, etc.) taken (i.e., ‘construed’) by given language users. In other words, meaning is grounded in the construal relationship between subjects of conceptualization (interactants) and objects of conceptualization (the things talked about or referred to). The construal hypothesis hence puts the language user – again, however, mostly as an abstract entity – at the very center of its account of meaning. The most explicitly dialogic account of construal has been proposed by Verhagen (Reference Verhagen2005, Reference Verhagen, Geeraerts and Cuyckens2007). Drawing on Langacker’s (Reference Langacker1987, Reference Langacker2001) classical viewing arrangement, Verhagen has argued for an extension of the subject of conceptualization (the ‘viewer’ in Langacker’s terms) as crucially involving two subjects – prototypically a speaker and a hearer. By means of language, speaker and hearer (or writer and reader) engage in cognitive coordination with respect to some construed object of conceptualization. In other words, for Verhagen (Reference Verhagen, Geeraerts and Cuyckens2007, p. 60) “the point of a linguistic utterance is, generally speaking, that the first conceptualizer invites the second to jointly attend to an object of conceptualization in some specific way […]; that is, both conceptualizers are involved in coordinating cognition by means of language, with one conceptualizer taking the initiative in each specific utterance”. The very essence of cognitive coordination relies in intersubjective attuning, i.e., while we speak we take into account others’ perspectives, their knowledge, attitudes, and beliefs. That is, we take into account and try to anticipate how others may understand our utterances.

The construal hypothesis, and must notably its intersubjective extension, brings Cognitive Linguistics closer to interactional theories of language use and communication than other linguistic paradigms. But without a doubt the road towards an interactionally plausible theory of cognitive coordination and meaning-making is still long and winding. Indeed, Verhagen’s version of the construal arrangement is being criticized as too static and too close to a meaning-transfer model from speaker to hearer (cf. also Cienki, this issue). This relates to the ongoing discussion as to where cognitive linguists believe that cognition and language reside. Most work in CL, and this is probably also the outside view on CL (Linell, Reference Linell2009), seems to attribute cognition and language to the individual. Work in dialogism, on the other hand, considers language to be essentially interpersonal and social. Cognition as individual thinking and language as a social (and cultural) phenomenon are often taken to be diametrically opposed, incommensurable perspectives (for a similar argument, see Clark, Reference Clark1996; Linell, Reference Linell2009, Reference Linell2014). However, one of the biggest, most vital challenges for any approach to language and communication is to bridge that gap, because it seems safe to say that language is best viewed as both an individual and inter-individual phenomenon.

Finally, the most important cornerstone of Cognitive Linguistics, which we argue provides a stable theoretical basis for an extension to interactional discourse, is the usage-based model. In contrast to traditional, top-down approaches that treat linguistic structures as largely determined by the language (rule) system and independent from language use, Cognitive Linguistics regards linguistic units to be inherently grounded and shaped by usage. This bottom-up spirit of CL inspires the usage-based thesis: speakers’ grammar arises through the abstraction of symbolic units from concrete, fully contextualized language use (Barlow & Kemmer, Reference Barlow and Kemmer2000; Bybee, Reference Bybee1995, Reference Bybee2010; Diessel, Reference Diessel and Aronoff2014; Geeraerts & Cuyckens, Reference Geeraerts, Cuyckens, Geeraerts and Cuyckens2007; Goldberg, Reference Goldberg1995, Reference Goldberg2006; Langacker, Reference Langacker1987, Reference Langacker and Rudzka-Ostyn1988, Reference Langacker2001; Taylor, Reference Taylor2002; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello1999, Reference Tomasello2003). By adopting the usage-based thesis as one of its basic tenets, Cognitive Linguistics has thus put discourse at the very heart of its theory:

All linguistic units are abstracted from usage events: actual instances of language use in the full detail of their contextual understanding.’ (Langacker, Reference Langacker2009, p. 154)

A crucial role in the emergence of linguistic units is attributed to the processes of entrenchment and conventionalization. Whereas entrenchment refers to the storage of a unit in an individual speaker’s long-term memory, conventionalization concerns a unit’s status in the inter-individual grammar of a speech community. Both processes reduce the cognitive effort in language use, allowing for a unit’s “routinized nature of execution’ (Langacker, Reference Langacker2008, p. 17). Crucially, as CL argues against a clear distinction of linguistic and encyclopedic, experience-based knowledge, any usage aspect may get entrenched or conventionalized as part of a symbolic unit’s meaning. The strict distinction between semantics and pragmatics is discarded in favor of a holistic approach to linguistic meaning: “As units emerge from usage, becoming entrenched in the minds of speakers and conventional in a speech community, they retain as part of their value any feature which is constant across the events giving rise to them. This includes any facet of the context and the speaker-hearer interaction” (Langacker, Reference Langacker2009, p. 154). Usage events (as instances of languages use) hence are taken to “involve conceptualization, encompassing ‘the expression’s full contextual understanding’ – including ‘everything evoked as the basis for its apprehension’, as well as a means of expression. [This] includes the full phonetic detail of an utterance, as well as any other signals, such as gestures and body language” (Langacker Reference Langacker2008, pp. 457f.). In recent years, quite a few studies have adopted and developed further this multimodal definition of the linguistic unit and the usage event (Schoonjans, Reference Schoonjans2014; Reference Schoonjans, Sambre, Brône and FeyaertsSchoonjans, Sambre, Brône, & Feyaerts, in press; Steen & Turner, Reference Steen, Turner, Borkent, Dancygier and Hinnell2013; Reference Zima, Ruiz de Mendoza and Pérez SobrinoZima, in press). This special issue adds to this body of research the papers by Cienki and by Oben and Brône.

The usage-based model, as a fundamental tenet of CL, is thus intrinsically interactional (and multimodal) in orientation. However, as mentioned above, despite the interactional foundation and the non-restrictive approach to entrenchment and conventionalization, most CL studies do not explore the full potential of the usage-based thesis by basing the analysis on fully contextualized, dialogic discourse data. The papers in this special issue are thus welcome and much needed exceptions in offering CL-driven approaches to interactional discourse phenomena. The next section briefly presents the contributions to this special issue.

3. The papers in this special issue

This special issue opens with a paper by Alan Cienki on ‘Spoken language usage events’. Cienki’s starting point is Langacker’s theory of Cognitive Grammar (1987, 1991, 2008) and his definition of the linguistic sign as a pairing of phonological form and semantic structure. In Cognitive Grammar – as in all usage-based models – signs (or constructions) are taken to emerge from usage events via “the progressive entrenchment of configurations [of semantic and phonological structures] that recur in a sufficient number of events to be established as cognitive routines” (Langacker, Reference Langacker2008, p. 220). Usage events are fully contextualized pieces of language use, involving a given conceptualization (semantic content) that is expressed by means of meaningful sounds and any other signals, such as gesture and body language (Langacker, Reference Langacker2008, p. 457). Cienki hence concludes that Cognitive Grammar posits that usage events are multimodal events and that the basic usage events of language are spoken language usage events. This, he observes, is, however, fundamentally at odds with the kind of data analyzed in Cognitive Grammar, i.e., constructed, monomodal examples that often take the form of full sentences. In his paper, Cienki hence proposes to take the claims of Cognitive Grammar seriously and to look at prototypical features of spoken language such as non-lexical sounds (like English uh-hm, hmm, mm), intonation, and, in more detail, manual gestures as being symbolic in nature to varying degrees. This multimodal perspective on symbolization, Cienki claims, has implications for the construal hypothesis (cf. supra Section 2). It is argued that gestures, intonation, and non-lexical sounds may provide vital information on a construal’s schematicity, and its degree of focus and prominence, as well as its perspective (character versus observer viewpoint). Cienki concludes from his observations and long experience with the analysis of multimodal language that whether a given pairing of form and meaning is symbolic in nature cannot always be answered with a straight yes or no. Rather, he argues for a more nuanced approach that allows for gradations of entrenchment. The application of the notion of a symbolic unit to spoken usage events, and its extension with non-verbal information, thus provides a novel insight into the flexibility and permeability of the borderlines of symbolic units as traditionally defined in Cognitive Linguistics.

Cognitive Grammar also is one of the theoretical cornerstones of the paper by Andreas Langlotz on ‘Local meaning-negotiation, activity types, and the current-discourse-space model’. Looking at conversations at a tourist information center in Switzerland, Langlotz observes a discourse-analytical dilemma: meaning in interaction is by definition locally managed and established between discourse participants. Yet activity types and speech genres impose restrictions on what can be said and how and what inferences are most likely to be speaker-intended. Hence, discourse structure and meaning are also conventional to some degree. For Langlotz, this entails that communication relies on “bridging the conventional with the situation-specific”, which “pares down to the cognitive challenge of performing the local planning of interactional moves against the background of … norm-abiding communicative conduct”. To resolve the inherent discrepancy between the online perspective that is typically associated with Conversation Analysis and the convention-oriented perspective on discourse as advocated in the Ethnography of Communication, Langlotz proposes a socio-cognitive model of discourse that combines two discourse models from the realm of Cognitive Linguistics / Cognitive Sciences: the ‘current discourse space-model’ as proposed by Langacker (Reference Langacker2001, Reference Langacker2008) and Lawrence Barsalou’s ‘situated conceptualization-model’ (Barsalou, Reference Barsalou, Cohen and Lefebvre2005). By performing a detailed, step-by-step analysis of two short extracts from his corpus of tourist encounters, he shows that successful communication within the tourist information activity frame means that both the tourist-information officer and the tourist need to gradually build up “a shared … conceptualization of the booking [a room]-category”. The trigger (or precondition) to arrive at such a shared simulation, Langlotz argues, is what he calls a “genre simulator”, which “mediates the discursive states in the interaction by simulating relevant conceptualizations for activity-specific meaning-coordination”. The model presented by Langlotz, and illustrated by means of real-life data, thus presents a useful tool for the analysis of meaning construction and negotiation in interaction, with co-participants who can flexibly and creatively manage shared conceptualizations and mutual (genre) knowledge.

In ‘What you see is what you do: on the relationship between gaze and gesture in multimodal alignment’, Bert Oben and Geert Brône are concerned with a very pervasive, yet under-researched feature of communication: the inter-speaker alignment of gesture. Their account is an example of the aforementioned, particularly fruitful, research strand within Cognitive Linguistics that turns to the multimodality of communication and the linguistic system. Although it is epistemologically grounded in CL, the paper has a clearly multi-disciplinary focus. It integrates relevant concepts and methods from CL, gesture studies, and psycholinguistics. More specifically, Oben and Brône study the effect of eye-gaze on gestural alignment. The analyses presented are based on the InSight Interaction Corpus (Brône & Oben, Reference Brône and Oben2015), a corpus of recordings from face-to-face interactions between two participants who both wear head-mounted eye-trackers and perform a targeted collaborative task. Oben and Brône focus on one specific gesture type that occurs in the data, i.e., descriptive gestures that represent objects in an animation video that both participants have watched before retelling the story to each other. They specifically concentrate on the relation between gesture occurrences and the gaze behavior of the participants. In doing so, they address two issues that have previously been hinted at in the literature (Gullberg & Kita, Reference Gullberg and Kita2009): Are speakers who have focused their interlocutors’ hand gestures more inclined to copy these gestures? And does the focus on one’s own hand gestures lead others to reiterate that gesture? Without anticipating their results, their approach provides partly unexpected insights on how eye-gaze and gesture production are intertwined, and confirms that alignment in interaction indeed is a multi- and essentially cross-modal phenomenon.

Kerstin Fischer’s paper ‘Conversation, Construction Grammar, and cognition’ is yet another convincing example of how transdisciplinary approaches that cross the boundaries of interactional and cognitive paradigms can lead to a much deeper understanding of language and/in interaction than any monoparadigmatic account can do. Fischer’s focus is on the intersections between Conversation Analysis (CA) and Construction Grammar (CxG). Her aim is to show that both paradigms are actually more compatible than is often assumed and that a synergetic approach to constructions, combining CA insights and the grammatical perspective of Construction Grammar, gives a far more encompassing picture of the construction under scrutiny than a pure CA or pure CxG analysis does. To make her point, Fischer takes the example of the English interjection oh, studied in great detail by John Heritage from a CA perspective. Despite the fact that Heritage focuses on classical CA parameters such as the sequential contexts and social actions oh is used in, and hence explicitly leaves grammatical or cognitive factors – the CxG focus – out of the picture, Fischer argues that his description can be seen as a grammatical one, showing that oh is a stable form–meaning pairing in a Construction Grammar sense. One by one, Fischer contrasts Heritage findings and claims with central tenets of CxG and convincingly argues that there are many more similarities between the two approaches than other studies within the field of ‘Interactional Construction Grammar’ have previously suggested. She is also careful to elaborate the deeper insights a grammatical perspective can add to the CA analysis, and how CA-relevant conditions of use can and need to be integrated into the constructional description. Finally, Fischer addresses what is often seen as the red flag by conversation analysts but at the same time constitutes a core interest of Construction Grammar: the role of cognition. In doing so, Fischer’s paper is a carefully constructed plea to cross the boundaries of one’s own research field and to approach the inter-relations of cognition and interactional language use from more than one research angle.

Each in its own way, the papers in this special issue present innovative ideas on how Cognitive Linguistic concepts can fruitfully be employed to study aspects of interactional language use. In doing so, they cross the boundaries of their own discipline and search for commonalities with neighboring disciplines like gesture studies, psycholinguistics, and Conversation Analysis. We consider this transdisciplinary approach particularly well-suited for the development of cognitive approaches to interactional language use. By looking at other approaches and their findings in the domain of interactional language use, we may reduce the risk of reinventing the wheel, and instead can explore the full potential of an integrated, Interactional Cognitive Linguistics. The contributions of this special issue offer a glimpse at the possibilities for the use of Cognitive Linguistics to account for interactional conceptualization, suggesting uncountable ideas for future research.

References

references

Auer, P. (2009). On-line syntax: thoughts on the temporality of spoken language. Language Sciences, 31, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auer, P., & Pfänder, S. (Eds.) (2011a). Constructions: emerging and emergent. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auer, P., & Pfänder, S. (2011b). Constructions: Emergent or emerging? In Auer, P. & Pfänder, S. (Eds.), Constructions: emerging and emergent (pp. 121). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Barlow, M., & Kemmer, S. (Eds.) (2000). Usage-based models of language. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (2005). Situated conceptualization. In Cohen, H. & Lefebvre, C. (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (pp. 619650). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boogaart, R., Colleman, T., & Rutten, G. (Eds.), (2014) Extending the scope of Construction Grammar. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Brône, G. (2008). Hyper- and misunderstanding in interactional humor. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(12), 20272061.Google Scholar
Brône, G. (2010). Bedeutungskonstitution in verbalem Humor: Ein kognitiv-linguistischer und diskurssemantischer Ansatz. Frankfurt a. Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Brône, G., & Coulson, S. (2010). Processing deliberate ambiguity in newspaper headlines: double grounding. Discourse Processes, 47(3), 212236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., & Oben, B. (2013). Resonating humour: a corpus-based approach to creative parallelism in discourse. In Veale, T., Feyaerts, K., & Forceville, C. (Eds.), Creativity and the agile mind: a multidisciplinary approach to a multifaceted phenomenon (pp. 181203). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., & Oben, B. (2015). InSight interaction: a multimodal and multifocal dialogue corpus. Language Resources and Evaluation, 49(1), 195214.Google Scholar
Brône, G., & Zima, E. (2014). Towards a dialogic construction grammar: ad hoc routines and resonance activation. In Giora, R. & Du Bois, J. W. (Eds.), special issue of Cognitive Linguistics, 25(3), 457495.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425455.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time: the flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chilton, P. (2005). Vectors, viewpoints, and viewpoint shifts. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3, 78116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A. (2002). Questions about mental imagery, gesture, and image schemas. Journal of Mental Imagery, 26(4), 346.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (2008). Why study metaphor and gesture? In Cienki, A. & Müller, C. (Eds.), Metaphor and gesture (pp. 525). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (2013). Image schemas and mimetic schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. In Pinar Sanz, M. J. (Ed.), Multimodality and Cognitive Linguistics, special issue of Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 417432.Google Scholar
Cienki, A., & Müller, C. (Eds.) (2008a). Metaphor and gesture. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cienki, A., & Müller, C. (2008b). Metaphor, gesture, and thought. In Gibbs, R. W. (Ed.), Handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 483501). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. (2000). Semantic leaps: frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. (2005). Sarcasm and the space structuring model. In Coulson, S. & Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (Eds.), The literal and the nonliteral in language and thought (pp. 129144). Berlin: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. (2006). Constructing meaning. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(4), 245266.Google Scholar
Dancygier, B. (2005). Blending and narrative viewpoint: Jonathan Raban’s travels through mental spaces. Language and Literature, 14(2), 99127.Google Scholar
Deppermann, A. (2002). Von der Kognition zur verbalen Interaktion: Bedeutungskonstitution im Kontext aus Sicht der Kognitionswissenschaften und der Gesprächsforschung. In Deppermann, A., Fiehler, R., & Spranz-Fogasy, T. (Eds.), Be-deuten: Wie Bedeutung im Gespräch entsteht (pp. 1133). Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Deppermann, A. (2007). Grammatik und Semantik aus gesprächsanalytischer Sicht. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Deppermann, A. (2011a). Constructions vs. lexical items as sources of complex meanings: a comparative study of constructions with German verstehen. In Auer, P. & Pfänder, S. (Eds.), Constructions: emerging and emergent (pp. 88126). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depermann, A. (2011b). Konstruktionsgrammatik und Interaktionale Linguistik: Affinitäten, Komplementaritäten und Diskrepanzen. In Lasch, A. & Ziem, A. (Eds.), Konstruktionsgrammatik III: Aktuelle Fragen und Lösungsansätze (pp. 205238). Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Deppermann, A. (2012). How does ‘cognition’ matter to the analysis of talk-in-interaction? Language Sciences, 34, 746767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H. (2014). Usage-based linguistics. In Aronoff, M. (Ed.), Oxford Bibliographies in ‘Linguistics’. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Du Bois, J. W. (2007). The stance triangle. In Englebretson, R. (Ed.), Stancetaking in discourse: subjectivity, evaluation, interaction (pp. 139182). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etelämäki, M., & Visapää, L. (2014). Why blend Conversation Analysis with Cognitive Grammar? Pragmatics, 24(3), 447506.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. (2006). Konstruktionsgrammatik und Interaktion. In Fischer, K. & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.), Konstruktionsgrammatik, Bd. 1: Von der Anwendung zur Theorie (pp. 129140). Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. (2010). Beyond the sentence: constructions, frames and spoken interaction. Constructions and Frames, 2(2), 185207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. (2015). Kognitive Routinen oder soziale Praktiken: Gesprächsanalyse und das Verhältnis von Kognition und Interaktion. In Lasch, A. & Ziem, A. (Eds.), Konstruktionsgrammatik IV: Konstruktionen und Konventionen als kognitive Routinen (pp. 255268). Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Forceville, Ch. (2010). Why and how study metaphor, metonymy, and other tropes in multimodal discourse? In Caballero, R. & Pinar, M. J. (Eds.), Ways and modes of human communicationg (pp. 5776). Cuenca: Ediciones de la Universidad de Castilla La Mancha.Google Scholar
Forceville, Ch., & Urios-Aparisi, E. (2009). Multimodal metaphor. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Fox, B., & Thompson, S. (2007). Relative clauses in English conversation: relativizers, frequency and the notion of construction. Studies in Language, 31, 293326.Google Scholar
Fried, M., & Östman, J.-O. (2005). Construction Grammar and spoken language: the case of pragmatic particles. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(11), 17521778.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (2006). Introduction: a rough guide to Cognitive Linguistics. In Geeraerts, D. (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics: basic readings (pp. 128). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (2007). Introducing Cognitive Linguistics. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 321). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: a Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M., & Kita, S. (2009). Attention to speech-accompanying gestures: eye movements and information uptake. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33(4), 251277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Günthner, S., Bücker, J., & Imo, W. (Eds.) (2014). Grammar and dialogism: sequential, syntactic, and prosodic patterns between emergence and sedimentation. Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Günthner, S., & Imo, W. (Eds.) (2006). Konstruktionen in der Interaktion. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hougaard, A., & Oakley, T. (Eds.) (2008). Mental spaces in interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ibbotson, P. (2013). The scope of usage-based theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 115.Google Scholar
Imo, W. (2007). Construction Grammar und Gesprochene-Sprache-Forschung: Konstruktionen mit zehn matrixsatzfähigen Verben im gesprochenen Deutsch. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Imo, W. (2009). Where does the mountain stop? A granular approach to the concept of constructions-as-signs. Published online: <http://www.unimuenster.de/imperia/md/content/germanistik/lehrende/imo_w/granularityandconstructions.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Janda, L. (2015). Cognitive Linguistics in the year 2015. Cognitive Semantics, 1(1), 131154.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 1: theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1988). A usage-based model. In Rudzka-Ostyn, B. (Ed.), Topics in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 127161). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 2: descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2001). Discourse in cognitive grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2). 143188.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive Grammar: a basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2009). Investigations in Cognitive Grammar. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2013). Interactive cognition: toward a unified account of structure, processing, and discourse. International Journal of Cognitive Linguistics, 3, 95125.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. (2006). Cognition at the heart of human interaction. Discourse Studies, 8(1), 8593.Google Scholar
Linell, P. (1998). Approaching dialogue: talk, interaction and contexts in dialogic perspectives. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Linell, P. (2009). Rethinking language, mind, and the world dialogically. Charlotte, NY: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Linell, P. (2014). A dialogic notebook: some afterthoughts after rethinking. Manuscript. Online: <http://www.ipkl.gu.se/digitalAssets/1475/1475848_163-a-dialogical-note-book.pdf>.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: what gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Mittelberg, I. (2006). Metaphor and metonymy in language and gesture: discourse evidence for multimodal models of grammar. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, I., & Waugh, L. R. (2014). Gestures and metonymy. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J.. (Eds.), Body – language – communication: an international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 17471766). Berlin/Boston: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2004). Metaphors dead and alive, sleeping and waking: a cognitive approach to metaphors in language use. Unpublished Habilitationsschrift, Free University Berlin.Google Scholar
Nerlich, B., & Clarke, D. (2001). Ambiguities we live by: towards a pragmatics of polysemy. Journal of Pragmatics, 33, 120.Google Scholar
Nerlich, B., & Clarke, D. (2003). Polysemy and flexibility: introduction and overview. In Nerlich, B., Todd, Z., Vimala, H., & Clarke, D. (Eds.), Polysemy: flexible patterns of meaning in mind and language (pp. 329). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Oakley, T., & Hougaard, A. (Eds.) (2008). Mental spaces in discourse and interaction. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Östman, J.-O. (2005). Construction discourse. In Östman, J.-O. & Fried, M. (Eds.), Construction Grammars: cognitive grounding and theoretical extensions (pp. 121144). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pascual, E. (2014). Fictive interaction: the conversation frame in thought, language, and discourse. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Towards a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 27, 169225.Google Scholar
Schoonjans, S. (2014). Modalpartikeln als multimodale Konstruktionen: Eine korpusbasierte Kookkurrenzanalyse von Modalpartikeln und Gestik im Deutschen. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Leuven.Google Scholar
Schoonjans, S., Sambre, P., Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K. (in press). Vers une analyse multimodale du sens: Perspectives constructionnelles sur la gestualité co-grammaticale. Langages.Google Scholar
Steen, F., & Turner, M. (2013). Multimodal construction grammar. In Borkent, M., Dancygier, B., & Hinnell, J. (Eds.), Language and the creative mind (pp. 255274). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. (2011). Cognitive linguistics meets the corpus. In Brdar, M., Gries, S., & Žic Fuchs, M. (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics: convergence and expansion (pp. 57290). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. R. (2002). Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1998). The New psychology of language: cognitive and functional approaches to language structure. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based account of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Verhagen, A. (2005). Constructions of intersubjectivity. discourse, syntax and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Verhagen, A. (2007). Construal and perspectivisation. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 4881). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zima, E. (2013). Cognitive Grammar and dialogic syntax: exploring potential synergies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1), 3672.Google Scholar
Zima, E. (in press). Multimodal constructional resemblance: the case of English circular motion constructions. In Ruiz de Mendoza, F. & Pérez Sobrino, P. (Eds.), Constructing families of constructions. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J. (2014). The co-evolution of human intersubjectivity, morality and language. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, D. (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 249266). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar