Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-07T04:52:49.940Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sedentary behaviour establishment in O-group common sole Solea solea: a laboratory video-tracking study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2009

Eric D.H. Durieux*
Affiliation:
IFREMER, Laboratoire Ecologie et Modèles pour l'Halieutique, rue de l'Ile d'Yeu, BP 21105, 44311 Nantes, France IFREMER, Laboratoire Ressources Halieutiques, Place du Séminaire, BP5, 17137 L'Houmeau, France
Mathieu Le Duigou
Affiliation:
IFREMER, Laboratoire Ressources Halieutiques, Place du Séminaire, BP5, 17137 L'Houmeau, France
Sandie Millot
Affiliation:
IFREMER, Laboratoire Ressources Halieutiques, Place du Séminaire, BP5, 17137 L'Houmeau, France
Pierre Sasal
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Biologie et d'Ecologie Tropicale et Méditerranéenne, UMR 5244 CNRS–EPHE–UPVD, Université de Perpignan Via Domitia, 52 Avenue Paul Alduy, 66860 Perpignan, France UMS 2978 CNRS–EPHE, CRIOBE, BP 1013 Moorea, French Polynesia
Marie-Laure Begout
Affiliation:
IFREMER, Laboratoire Ressources Halieutiques, Place du Séminaire, BP5, 17137 L'Houmeau, France
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: E.D.H. Durieux, IFREMER, Laboratoire Ecologie et Modèles pour l'Halieutique, rue de l'Ile d'Yeu, 44311 Nantes, France email: edh.durieux@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Spontaneous swimming activity of O-group common sole (Solea solea) was evaluated using a video-tracking system under laboratory conditions. An experiment was conducted during two consecutive days on individuals sampled in June, July, September and November (2004) in a coastal nursery ground (Pertuis Charentais, Bay of Biscay, France). The measured behavioural variables were: distance travelled and frequency of occurrence of burying, swimming and immobility. O-group sole showed a relatively clear circadian activity in line with the artificial light conditions (day, twilight and night). Swimming activity of O-group sole decreased drastically from June to July, thereafter remaining at a very low level in September and November. Such important changes reflect the transition between the exploratory behaviour of the post-colonization period and a well established sedentary behaviour remaining until the onset of winter. These results highlight the potential limitation in habitat use capacities of O-group sole once settled in coastal nursery grounds.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 2009

INTRODUCTION

Juveniles of many marine fish and especially flatfish inhabit coastal areas (e.g. estuaries and shallow water bays) where they find generally more abundant food, lower predation risks and higher temperatures than in offshore areas (Gibson, Reference Gibson1994). However, in these essential habitats, fish juveniles have also to face highly variable environmental factors and superimposed anthropogenic activities that may jeopardize survival and influence recruitment into the adult population (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Heck, Able, Childers, Eggleston, Gillanders, Halpern, Hays, Hoshino, Minello, Orth, Sheridan and Weinstein2001). Fish habitat utilization reflects trade-offs between spatial distribution of essential resources, the internal state of the organism and tolerance to ambient conditions (Huntingford, Reference Huntingford1993). In order to achieve habitat conservation and sustainable fisheries management, it is therefore critical to understand fish juveniles behaviour in terms of spatio-temporal dynamics within these coastal habitats (Rice, Reference Rice2005).

Common sole Solea solea (L.) is a widely distributed and most economically important flatfish species of the north-east Atlantic region. Most common sole stocks are reported to be overexploited (ICES, 2005). Previously both field and laboratory studies have focused on the behavioural processes occurring in O-group juveniles during the short colonization phase (Champalbert & Koutsikopoulos, Reference Champalbert and Koutsikopoulos1995; Amara et al., Reference Amara, Lagardère, Désaunay and Marchand2000). Once settled in shallow coastal areas individuals have to face a variety of natural and anthropogenic stressors that may impact their survival. However, apart from field studies that have focused on their spatio-temporal distribution until autumn (Dorel et al., Reference Dorel, Koutsikopoulos, Désaunay and Marchand1991; Rogers, Reference Rogers1992; Jager et al., Reference Jager, Kleef and Tydeman1993) and some other rare studies that have inferred indirectly about their movement capacities using conventional (Coggan & Dando, Reference Coggan and Dando1988) or natural (Vinagre et al., Reference Vinagre, Salgado, Costa and Cabral2008) tags, little is known on O-group sole behavioural activities during the post-colonization period.

Therefore, the present study aimed at bringing new insights on O-group sole swimming behaviour during the sensitive post-colonization period in order to better understand dynamics of individuals in shallow coastal areas. A laboratory video-tracking system was developed to measure the spontaneous swimming and burying activity. Since behavioural patterns in fish generally vary on a diel cycle, experimental set up was designed to measure behavioural activities during two consecutive days according to light period of the day (day–twilight–night). O-group common sole were caught throughout four different months after colonization until the onset of winter (i.e. June to November) in an Atlantic nursery ground (Pertuis Charentais, Bay of Biscay, France). In this region, colonization is centred around May so that, it is likely that individuals belong to the same cohort. Therefore, it was hypothesized that sampling month may be used as proxy of age/ontogenetic status. The objectives were to establish the baseline levels of these natural activities in O-group sole (i.e. normal/spontaneous behaviour) and test the effect of the ontogenetic status (sampling month) on these activities according to light periods under constant experimental conditions throughout the post-colonization period.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

This study was conducted under the approval of the Animal Care Committee of France under the official licence of M.L. Bégout (17-010).

Fish origin and maintenance

Thirty-one wild O-group juveniles were caught in the Pertuis Charentais, a main sole nursery ground for the Bay of Biscay (Le Pape et al., Reference Le Pape, Chauvet, Mahevas, Lazure, Guérault and Désaunay2003), in June, July, September and November 2004 in a mussel pole-culture area (46°15′80″N 1°13′40″W) using a push net to avoid injuries to the fish (Durieux et al., Reference Durieux, Galois, Bégout, Sasal and Lagardère2007). At the video-tracking laboratory, fish were maintained in aerated tanks (45×30×35 cm) with filtered sea water (temperature: 20±1°C; salinity: 35±1; oxygen concentration: 100% air saturation) and a sand substratum (light colour). Both temperature and salinity were set to constant value as a mean of disconnecting the experiment from field conditions in a consistent way for all the fish. Fish were fed daily with frozen Tubifex. They were maintained under these conditions for at least 10 days' acclimatization prior experimentation in order to avoid tidal rhythm effects (Gibson, Reference Gibson1973, Burrows, Reference Burrows1994) that could eventually prevent comparisons between months. An artificial photoperiod was applied: daylight period (8:00–21:00), a twilight transition period (7:00–8:00 and 21:00–22:00) and a night period (22:00–07:00).

Video-tracking set-up and experimental protocol

In the video tracking laboratory (isolated from external disturbances): three black circular arenas of 60 cm diameter, with walls drilled regularly with 5 mm diameter holes to allow water circulation, were placed individually in a 400 l tank with aeration and water filters that maintained a continuous water flow around the arena. A 5 cm deep layer of black sand (100 to 300 µm) was placed on the bottom of the arena in order to allow sole to bury and to provide an adequate colour contrast to allow fish detection by the image analysis system. Water was maintained at temperature around 20±1°C (by an air conditioning system in the laboratory); salinity at around 35±1 psu (by regularly changing seawater and when needed adjusting salinity with freshwater to compensate for evaporation); and oxygen concentration at around 100% air saturation (using air aerator systems). The arenas were illuminated laterally by artificial light with the same photoperiod used during maintenance of the fish: daylight period (8:00–21:00; white light: 0.3×1015 Q.cm−2.s−1), a twilight transition period (7:00–8:00 and 21:00–22:00; white light: 0.3×1014 Q.cm−2.s−1) and a night period (22:00–07:00; infrared light (PAR38 IR 175): 0.14×1014 Q.cm−2.s−1). Since fish are not sensitive to infrared light (Douglas & Hawryshyn, Reference Douglas, Hawryshyn, Douglas and Djamgoz1990), nocturnal behaviour was considered un-altered by the artificial lighting used to assess nocturnal swimming. Fish were not fed the day before the experiment nor during the subsequent video recordings.

Above each arena, a digital black and white high sensitivity camera (Sony XCD-X700, XGA) linked to a computer equipped with Visilog 6.2 software (NOESIS, France) was mounted. Image acquisition proceeded with cyclic sub-sampling of each arena with 33 seconds duration sequences of 1 image s−1 and each image was saved in .jpeg format. Successive sequences relative to a same arena were separated by a time lag of 2 minutes because of a 7 seconds delay between each sequence acquisition. The resulting sub-sampling factor (SSF) was 1/3.6. For each month, at 11:00, the day of the beginning of recording, one fish was introduced per arena, so that three fish were recorded at a time. Videos were then recorded for 48 hours duration. After video recording, fish were individually measured (to 1 mm, standard length (SL)) (Table 1). To complete each month group, successive recordings took up to 8 days.

Table 1. Sample size (N), mean standard length (SL±SE (mm)), and size-range of O-group common sole Solea solea sampled in 2004 in the Pertuis Charentais nursery ground (France)

Fish detection was based on the colour contrast between the fish (appearing in white) and the black sandy bottom using image analysis (Visilog 6.2). The XY coordinates (mm) of the barycentre of the detected fish shape were extracted for each image/time. An integrated VBA module to Visilog 6.2 developed by NOESIS for this experiment, allowed automation of the acquisition and image treatment process.

Data analysis

Five to ten individuals per month (June, July, September and November) i.e. around 1500 hours of video-tracking were analysed (Table 1). Based on XY coordinates, four behavioural endpoints could be obtained: the distance travelled between consecutive images (Di) and the frequency of occurrence of three different activities: swimming (Si), burying (Bi), and immobility (Ii). When the fish was not detected, XY coordinates equalled 0, and the fish was considered buried. When fish was detected but distance travelled between two consecutive images was ≤5 mm s−1 (limit of motion detection of distance travelled between two consecutive images) fish was considered immobile. Alternatively when distance travelled was >5 mm s−1 fish was considered swimming. Distances travelled were transformed in body length (Bl) to avoid any effect of size on swimming speed (Peck et al., Reference Peck, Buckley and Bengtson2006). For each individual fish, hourly values of the four variables were calculated:

\eqalign{D_h &= {1 \over SSF} \sum_{i = 1}^n D_i\cr S_h &= {1 \over n} \times 100\sum_{i=1}^n S_i\cr B_h &= {1 \over n} \times 100\sum_{i=1}^n B_i \cr I_h &= {1 \over n} \times 100\sum_{i=1}^n I_i}

where SSF: sub-sampling factor; n: number of observations per hour; i: individual observation per second.

Dh is expressed in Bl h−1 and Sh, Bh and Ih are expressed in percentage.

Using all 48 hours' observations, individual data were averaged for specific light periods (day, twilight and night) prior to analysis. Given the nature of the data, i.e. non-independent light periods, one-way repeated measures ANOVAs were used to test differences for each behavioural variable (D, S, B and I) between month groups (between-subject factor: June, July, September and October) and light periods (within-subject factor: day, night and twilight) as well was interaction of both factors. Data were tested for normality with a Shapiro–Wilk test and for homoscedasticity with a Bartlett's test. Since data could not meet assumptions they were ranked prior to analysis (Kane et al., Reference Kane, Salierno, Gipson, Molteno and Hunter2004). Homogeneous groups were determined with the a posteriori multiple comparisons Newman and Keuls tests. Results are given as averages±SE (standard error). Statistical tests were performed using XLStat software and Systat. Tests were considered significant at P < 0.05.

For derived instantaneous swimming speeds, only values >5 mm s−1 (limit of motion detection of distance travelled between two consecutive images) were considered. Swimming speed frequencies were calculated for the different month groups with individual speed-classes of 2 cm s−1 and a range of 0.5 to 20 cm s−1.

RESULTS

Significant differences were detected for both factors (i.e. sampled months and light periods) for all measured behavioural variables D, B, S and I (Table 2; Figure 1). Independently of the light periods, D and S decreased significantly between June and July, while B increased significantly. Between July and September, D, S and I decreased significantly, while B increased significantly. The pattern observed for these four variables was independent from the light periods. Between September and November, B decreased significantly and I increased significantly independently of the light periods for both variables whereas no difference was detected for D and S. D and S were significantly higher at night and twilight than during the day, B was significantly lowest at night, intermediate at twilight and highest during the day independently of sampled months. I was significantly highest at night independently of sampled months.

Fig. 1. Average distance travelled (Bl h−1) (A) and frequency occurrence (%) for the three different types of activity swimming (B), burying (C) and immobility (D) per sampled month and light period for O-group common sole Solea solea captured in 2004 in the Pertuis Charentais area (France). Artificial light time period was set as: day, 8:00–21:00; twilight, 7:00–8:00 and 21:00–22:00; night, 22:00–7:00. Values were calculated on the total 48 hours' observation period.

Table 2. Results of one-way repeated measures ANOVAs testing differences in mean behavioural variables (distance travelled and swimming, burying and immobility occurrences; ranked transformed data) between months (between-subject factor), light periods (within-subject factor) and interaction of both factors for O-group common sole Solea sole a sampled in the Pertuis Charentais nursery ground (France). Multiple comparisons Newman and Keuls tests post hoc. Jn, June; Jl, July; Sept, September; Nov, November; Twi, twilight period.

For fish sampled in June, swimming speed frequencies displayed a bimodal distribution (Figure 2). The first mode for speed comprised between 0.5 and 2 cm s−1 (0.4 Bl s−1) and a second for speed of 8–10 cm s−1 (2.2 Bl s−1), each representing 25% of the overall swimming speeds. For fish sampled in July, September and November, swimming speed frequencies displayed a unimodal distribution with 50–60% of swimming speeds between 0.5 and 2 cm s−1.

Fig. 2. Swimming speed (cm s−1) frequency distribution calculated on the total 48 hours' observation period for each sampled month of common sole Solea solea captured in the Pertuis Charentais area (France). 10 cm s−1 corresponds to 2.2, 2.0, 1.4 and 0.9 Bl s−1 in June, July, September and November respectively.

DISCUSSION

Our data were obtained under experimental conditions using a video tracking system at a scale of observation enabling the assessment of daily spontaneous swimming activities (levels and rhythms) of O-group common sole for the first time. This method appears to be one of the only non-harmful methods of quantifying free activity of O-group sole. However, caution should be applied in extrapolating these data to field observations. Swimming activity over two days showed relatively high inter-individual variability as often observed for fish (Bourke et al., Reference Bourke, Magnan and Rodriguez1997; Salvanes & Hart, Reference Salvanes and Hart1998; Mehner, Reference Mehner2006). Body size is well known to influence fish swimming activity so that in our study we used relative distance travelled in proportion of body size in order to minimize size effect within and between sampled months. Alternatively it is important to consider that the low sample size routinely employed in behavioural studies (here five to ten replicates per sampled month) and often due to technical constraints might also have the effect of raising the variability. Nevertheless, some clear temporal trends in the behavioural activities from the post-colonization period untill autumn were observed.

O-group sole showed a relatively clear circadian activity in line with the artificial light conditions of the experiment, with higher swimming activity at night and twilight than during the day and alternatively more burying activity during the day than during the night. This nocturnal activity pattern is consistent with previous studies on young O-group sole (Macquart-Moulin et al., Reference Macquart-Moulin, Champalbert, Howell, Patriti and Ravaivoson1991; Champalbert & Marchand, Reference Champalbert and Marchand1994; Champalbert & Koutsikopoulos, Reference Champalbert and Koutsikopoulos1995), older 2-group (Lagardère et al., Reference Lagardère, Ducamp, Frikha and Sperandio1988; Laffargue et al., Reference Laffargue, M-L and Lagardère2006) and in general in other flatfish (Gibson, Reference Gibson1973; Burrows, Reference Burrows1994; Hurst & Duffy, Reference Hurst and Duffy2005). Champalbert et al. (Reference Champalbert, Macquart-Moulin, Patriti and Chiki1991) demonstrated that sole > 30 mm were photonegative at most light intensities except at twilight. In the field, in addition to circadian rhythms, flatfish display tidal rhythms in swimming activity which has been shown for juveniles to correspond to a trade-off between foraging, predator avoidance and the selection of suitable environmental conditions (Burrows, Reference Burrows1994; Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Phil, Burrows, Modin, Wennhage and Nickell1998). In our study, fish were acclimatized for ten days prior to the experiment in order to prevent any confounding effect of tidal rhythms between sampled months.

Burying behaviour represented 80% of the activity in autumn months (September and November). This behaviour is linked to a strategy for predation avoidance (Gibson, Reference Gibson1997) but also energy saving with lower basal metabolism while buried (Howell & Canario, Reference Howell and Canario1987). Sole from June and July buried clearly less than fish from September and November. The latter were often immobile at the surface of the substratum, especially at night. As burying capacity of juvenile flatfish is a strong logistic function of body size relative to the sediment grain size (Gibson & Robb, Reference Gibson and Robb1992), June and July fish, due to their relatively small size, are likely to have been limited by the size of the sand grains used in the experiment (100–300 µm).

The O-group sole showed a strong decline in swimming activity from June to July (around 8 fold in distance travelled and 4 fold in % swimming), with very low activity remaining until November. This drastic change was also observed in term of swimming speed frequency distributions. Individuals sampled in June displayed a high proportion of fast swimming speeds (around 10 cm s−1 or 2 Bl s−1) that typically corresponds to off bottom swimming with large scales movements (Champalbert & Koutsikopoulos, Reference Champalbert and Koutsikopoulos1995; Hurst & Duffy, Reference Hurst and Duffy2005) whereas from July onwards the majority of swimming speeds were lower than 2 cm s−1 which is characteristic of the foraging behaviour of juvenile flatfish searching for benthic prey (Lagardère et al., Reference Lagardère, Bégout, Lafaye and Villotte1994; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Burrows and Hughes2000, Reference Hill, Burrows and Hughes2002). Both environmental (i.e. current, salinity, light, and food availability) and endogenous factors are considered to play a determinant role in nursery colonization (Champalbert & Koutsikopoulos, Reference Champalbert and Koutsikopoulos1995). Here, experimental conditions were the same for all sampled months so that we assume that our observations reflect an ontogenetic change in swimming activity. O-group sole juveniles in the Bay of Biscay colonize nursery grounds around May (Amara et al., Reference Amara, Lagardère, Désaunay and Marchand2000). Around one month post-colonization, O-group sole juveniles still demonstrate a relatively high swimming activity. This behaviour may be a relic of an exploratory behaviour associated with colonization of new habitat (Hurst & Duffy, Reference Hurst and Duffy2005). Low activity from July onwards may reflect the transition to a well established sedentary behaviour. From an ecological point of view, this behaviour may be interpreted as giving a relatively high resiliency of O-group sole in confined areas of the shallow coastal habitat. This is consistent with field observations of potential O-group sole movements in different nursery grounds based on density distributions (Dorel et al., Reference Dorel, Koutsikopoulos, Désaunay and Marchand1991; Rogers, Reference Rogers1992; Jager et al., Reference Jager, Kleef and Tydeman1993), recapture of tagged individuals (Coggan & Dando, Reference Coggan and Dando1988) and use of natural tags (Vinagre et al., Reference Vinagre, Salgado, Costa and Cabral2008).

In the field it has been reported that O-group sole operate migration to deeper waters prior to winter in order to avoid very low water temperature occurring in shallow part of the habitat at this time (Dorel et al., Reference Dorel, Koutsikopoulos, Désaunay and Marchand1991; van der Veer et al., Reference van der Veer, Dapper and Witte2001). Since we did not observe any increase in swimming activity at this time under constant experimental conditions, this suggests that such migratory behaviour prior to winter is mainly influenced by hydroclimatic factors.

In conclusion, using a laboratory video-tracking system this study provides quantified spontaneous swimming activity baseline levels of O-group common sole through their first period of life in coastal shallow areas. O-group sole showed a clear ontogenetic change in swimming activity between June and July, demonstrating the transition between the exploratory behaviour of late colonization and the well established sedentary behaviour until the onset of winter. These results highlight the potential movement limitations of O-group sole in coastal areas during this highly critical period.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

IFREMER and the Conseil Régional Poitou Charentes funded the study. The VBA module for image acquisition and treatment automation was developed in collaboration with D. Péral (NOESIS). J. Grizon was the boat pilot for fish sampling in the field. P. Pineau, M. Prineau and N. Lachaussée helped in the set-up and maintenance of the experimental infrastructures. We are grateful to E. Hunter from the CEFAS Lowestoft laboratory and to anonymous referees for valuable comments and corrections on the manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Amara, R., Lagardère, F., Désaunay, Y. and Marchand, J. (2000) Metamorphosis and estuarine colonisation in the common sole, Solea solea (L.): implications for recruitment regulation. Oceanologica Acta 23, 469484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, M.W., Heck, K.L., Able, K.W., Childers, D.L., Eggleston, D.B., Gillanders, B.M., Halpern, B., Hays, C.G., Hoshino, K., Minello, T.J., Orth, R.J., Sheridan, P.F. and Weinstein, M.P. (2001) The identification, conservation, and management of estuarine and marine nurseries for fish and invertebrates. Bioscience 51, 633641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourke, P., Magnan, P. and Rodriguez, M.A. (1997) Individual variations in habitat use and morphology in brook charr. Journal of Fish Biology 51, 783794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrows, M.T. (1994) Foraging time strategy of small juvenile plaice: a laboratory study of diel and tidal behaviour patterns with Artemia prey and shrimp predators. Marine Ecology Progress Series 115, 3139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Champalbert, G. and Koutsikopoulos, C. (1995) Behaviour, transport and recruitment of Bay of Biscay sole (Solea solea): laboratory and field studies. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 75, 93108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Champalbert, G., Macquart-Moulin, C., Patriti, G. and Chiki, D. (1991) Ontogenic variations in the phototaxis of larval and juvenile sole Solea solea L. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 149, 207225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Champalbert, G. and Marchand, J. (1994) Rheotaxis in larvae and juvenile sole (Solea solea L.): influence of light conditions and sediment. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 177, 235249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coggan, R.A. and Dando, P.R. (1988) Movements of juvenile Dover sole, Solea solea (L.), in the Tamar estuary, south western England. Journal of Fish Biology 33, 177184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorel, D., Koutsikopoulos, C., Désaunay, Y. and Marchand, J. (1991) Seasonal distribution of young sole (Solea solea (L.)) in the nursery ground of the Bay of Vilaine (Northern Bay of Biscay). Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 27, 297306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, R.H. and Hawryshyn, C.W. (1990) Behavioural studies of fish vision: an analysis of visual capabilities. In Douglas, R.H. and Djamgoz, M.B.A. (eds) The visual system of fish. London: Chapman and Hall, pp. 373418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durieux, E.D.H., Galois, R., Bégout, M.-L., Sasal, P. and Lagardère, F. (2007) Temporal changes in lipid condition and parasitic infection by digenean metacercariae of young-of-year common sole Solea solea (L.) in an Atlantic nursery ground (Bay of Biscay, France). Journal of Sea Research 57, 162170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N. (1973) Tidal and circadian activity rhythms in juvenile plaice, Pleuronectes platessa. Marine Biology 22, 379386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N. (1994) Impact of habitat quality and quantity on the recruitment of juvenile flatfishes. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 32, 191206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N. (1997) Behaviour and the distribution of flatfishes. Journal of Sea Research 37, 241256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N. and Robb, L. (1992) The relationship between body size, sediment grain size and the burying ability of juvenile plaice, Pleuronectes platessa L. Journal of Fish Biology 40, 771778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N., Phil, L., Burrows, M.T., Modin, J., Wennhage, H. and Nickell, L.A. (1998) Diel movements of juvenile plaice Pleuronectes platessa in relation to predators, competitors, food availability and abiotic factors on a microtidal nursery ground. Marine Ecology Progress Series 165, 145159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S., Burrows, M.T. and Hughes, R.N. (2000) Increased turning per unit distance as an area-restricted search mechanism in a pause-travel predator, juvenile plaice, foraging for buried bivalves. Journal of Fish Biology 56, 14971508.Google Scholar
Hill, S., Burrows, M.T. and Hughes, R.N. (2002) Adaptive search in juvenile plaice foraging for aggregated and dispersed prey. Journal of Fish Biology 61, 12551267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, B.R. and Canario, A.V.M. (1987) The influence of sand on the estimation of resting metabolic rate of juvenile sole, Solea solea (L.). Journal of Fish Biology 31, 277280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huntingford, F.A. (1993) Can cost–benefit analysis explain fish distribution patterns? Journal of Fish Biology 43, 289308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurst, T.P. and Duffy, T.A. (2005) Activity patterns in northern rock sole are mediated by temperature and feeding history. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 325, 201213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ICES (2005) Report of the ICES Advisory Committee on Fishery Management, Advisory Committee on the Marine Environment and Advisory Committee on Ecosystems, 2005. Volumes 1—11. 1. In ICES Advice, 418 pp.Google Scholar
Jager, Z., Kleef, H.L. and Tydeman, P. (1993) The distribution of O-group flatfish in relation to abiotic factors on the tidal flats in the brackish Dollard (Ems Estuary, Wadden Sea). Journal of Fish Biology 43, 3143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, A.S., Salierno, J.D., Gipson, G.T., Molteno, T.C.A. and Hunter, C. (2004) A video-based movement analysis system to quantify behavioral stress responses of fish. Water Research 38, 39934001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laffargue, P., M-L, B. and Lagardère, F. (2006) Testing the potential effects of shellfish farming on the swimming activity and spatial distribution of sole (Solea solea) in a mesocosm. ICES Journal of Marine Science 63, 10141028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagardère, J.-P., Ducamp, J.-J., Frikha, L. and Sperandio, M. (1988) Ultrasonic tracking of common sole juveniles (Solea vulgaris Quensel, 1806) in a saltmarsh: methods and fish response to some environmental factors. Journal of Applied Ichthyology 4, 8796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagardère, J.-P., Bégout, M.-L., Lafaye, J.-Y. and Villotte, J.-P. (1994) Influence of wind-produced noise on orientation in the sole (Solea solea). Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 51, 12581264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Pape, O., Chauvet, F., Mahevas, S., Lazure, P., Guérault, D. and Désaunay, Y. (2003) Quantitative description of habitat suitability for the juvenile common sole (Solea solea, L.) in the Bay of Biscay (France) and the contribution of different habitats to the adult population. Journal of Sea Research 50, 139149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macquart-Moulin, C., Champalbert, G., Howell, B.R., Patriti, G. and Ravaivoson, C. (1991) La relation alimentation-fixation benthique chez les jeunes soles Solea solea L. metamorphosées. Evidences expérimentales. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 153, 195205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehner, T. (2006) Individual variability of diel migrations in European vendace (Coregonus albula) explored by stationary vertical hydroacoustics. Ecology of Freshwater Fish 15, 146153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peck, M., Buckley, L. and Bengtson, D. (2006) Effects of temperature and body size on the swimming speed of larval and juvenile Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua): implications for individual-based modelling. Environmental Biology of Fishes 75, 419429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, J.C. (2005) Understanding fish habitat ecology to achieve conservation. Journal of Fish Biology 67, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, S.I. (1992) Environmental factors affecting the distribution of sole (Solea solea (L.)) within a nursery area. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 29, 153161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvanes, A.G.V. and Hart, P.J.B. (1998) Individual variability in state-dependent feeding behaviour in three-spined sticklebacks. Animal Behaviour 55, 13491359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Veer, H.W., Dapper, R. and Witte, J.I.J. (2001) The nursery function of the intertidal areas in the western Wadden Sea for O-group sole Solea solea (L.). Journal of Sea Research 45, 271279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinagre, C., Salgado, J., Costa, M.J. and Cabral, H.N. (2008) Nursery fidelity, food web interactions and primary sources of nutrition of the juveniles of Solea solea and S. senegalensis in the Tagus estuary (Portugal): a stable isotope approach. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 76, 255264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample size (N), mean standard length (SL±SE (mm)), and size-range of O-group common sole Solea solea sampled in 2004 in the Pertuis Charentais nursery ground (France)

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Average distance travelled (Bl h−1) (A) and frequency occurrence (%) for the three different types of activity swimming (B), burying (C) and immobility (D) per sampled month and light period for O-group common sole Solea solea captured in 2004 in the Pertuis Charentais area (France). Artificial light time period was set as: day, 8:00–21:00; twilight, 7:00–8:00 and 21:00–22:00; night, 22:00–7:00. Values were calculated on the total 48 hours' observation period.

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of one-way repeated measures ANOVAs testing differences in mean behavioural variables (distance travelled and swimming, burying and immobility occurrences; ranked transformed data) between months (between-subject factor), light periods (within-subject factor) and interaction of both factors for O-group common sole Solea solea sampled in the Pertuis Charentais nursery ground (France). Multiple comparisons Newman and Keuls tests post hoc. Jn, June; Jl, July; Sept, September; Nov, November; Twi, twilight period.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Swimming speed (cm s−1) frequency distribution calculated on the total 48 hours' observation period for each sampled month of common sole Solea solea captured in the Pertuis Charentais area (France). 10 cm s−1 corresponds to 2.2, 2.0, 1.4 and 0.9 Bl s−1 in June, July, September and November respectively.