Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T09:44:18.293Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Timing is everything: Antiretroviral nonadherence is associated with impairment in time-based prospective memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2009

STEVEN PAUL WOODS*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
MATTHEW S. DAWSON
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
ERICA WEBER
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
SARAH GIBSON
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
IGOR GRANT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
J. HAMPTON ATKINSON
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Psychiatry Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Steven Paul Woods, HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center, Department of Psychiatry (0847), University of California, San Diego, 150 West Washington Street, 2nd floor, San Diego, California 92103. E-mail: spwoods@ucsd.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Nonadherence to combination antiretroviral (ARV) therapies (cART) is highly prevalent and significantly increases the risk of adverse human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) disease outcomes. The current study evaluated the hypothesis that prospective memory—a dissociable aspect of episodic memory describing the ability to execute a future intention—plays an important role in successful cART adherence. Seventy-nine individuals with HIV infection who were prescribed at least one ARV medication underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological and neuromedical evaluation prior to completing a 1-month observation of their cART adherence as measured by electronic medication monitoring. Nonadherent individuals (n = 31) demonstrated significantly poorer prospective memory functioning as compared to adherent persons (n = 48), particularly on an index of time-based ProM (i.e., elevated loss of time errors). Deficits in time-based prospective memory were independently predictive of cART nonadherence, even after considering the possible influence of established predictors of adherence, such as general cognitive impairment (e.g., retrospective learning and memory) and psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., depression). These findings extend a nascent literature showing that impairment in time-based prospective memory significantly increases the risk of medication nonadherence and therefore may guide the development of novel strategies for intervention. (JINS, 2009, 15, 42–52.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © INS 2009

INTRODUCTION

Medication nonadherence is a significant public health problem, particularly among persons living with chronic medical conditions. In fact, it has been estimated that as many as 50% of participants in clinical trials for chronic disorders may not adhere to their prescribed medication regimens (Osterberg & Blaschke, Reference Osterberg and Blaschke2005). Medication nonadherence also presents a significant healthcare cost, accounting for estimated $100 billion in annual expenditures and 33–69% of medication-related hospital admissions (Osterberg & Blaschke, Reference Osterberg and Blaschke2005). Adequate adherence to prescribed medication regimens is particularly important for persons infected with the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), for which optimal disease outcomes are intricately tied to the efficacy of combination antiretroviral (ARV) therapies (cARTs). Over the past decade, cART has altered the landscape of HIV disease management in developed countries by dramatically reducing HIV-associated morbidity and mortality (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2006) and also improving health-related quality of life (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Ostrow, Detels, Hu, Johnson, Kingsley and Jacobson2006). However, approximately 40–50% of patients are not adherent to their cART regimens (e.g., Nieuwkerk et al., Reference Nieuwkerk, Sprangers, Burger, Hoetelmans, Hugen, Danner, van der Ende, Schneider, Schrey, Meenhorst, Sprenger, Kaufmann, Jambroes, Chesney, de Wolf and Lange2001), which has led to adherence being branded as the “Achilles’ heel” of HIV treatment (Simoni et al., Reference Simoni, Frick, Pantalone and Turner2003). Although there is variability among different classes of ARVs, the literature generally shows that adherence levels of 95–100% are needed to ensure optimal treatment effectiveness (Bangsburg, Reference Bangsburg2008). Nonadherence to cART (most commonly classified as less than 90% compliance) is associated with poorer HIV disease outcomes, including higher rates of virologic failure (Perno et al., Reference Perno, Ceccherini-Silberstein, De Luca, Cozzi-Lepri, Gori, Cingolani, Bellocchi, Trotta, Piano, Forbici, Scasso, Vullo, d’Arminio Monforte and Antinori2002), the development of drug-resistant viral mutations (Harrigan et al., Reference Harrigan, Hogg, Dong, Yup, Wynhoven, Woodward, Brumme, Brumme, Mo, Alexander and Montaner2005), and an increased risk of mortality (Lima et al., Reference Lima, Geller, Bangsberg, Patterson, Daniel and Kerr2007).

Considering the numerous adverse clinical outcomes associated with cART nonadherence, the importance of identifying salient risk factors to target for screening and remediation is readily apparent. Prior studies have identified a variety of factors that influence nonadherence, including demographics (e.g., age), psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., depression, substance abuse), psychosocial variables (e.g., attitudes and beliefs related to medications, familial support), and systemic factors (e.g., limited access to healthcare). HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment is also associated with increased risk of cART nonadherence, which is pertinent in that approximately 30–50% of HIV-infected persons demonstrate neuropsychological deficits (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Grant, Butters, White, Kirson, Atkinson, McCutchan, Taylor, Kelly and Ellis1995; Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Smurzynski, Parsons, Wu, Bosch, Wu, McCartur, Collier, Evans and Ellis2007). Although progress has been made toward simplifying cART regimens, successful medication management nevertheless remains a complex cognitive challenge that typically requires tracking multiple drugs (oftentimes including non-ARVs) with varying dosages, administration times, and special instructions. Supporting the hypothesized role of cognitive deficits in cART nonadherence, Chesney et al. (Reference Chesney, Ickovics, Chambers, Gifford, Neidig, Zwickl and Wu2000) found that nearly 70% of individuals who were nonadherent to cART reported that they “simply forgot” to take their medication. Subsequent research demonstrated that HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment is associated with poorer performance on laboratory medication management tasks (e.g., Albert et al., Reference Albert, Weber, Todak, Polanco, Clouse, McElhiney, Rabkin, Stern and Marder1999, Reference Albert, Flater, Clouse, Todak, Stern and Marder2003; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Marcotte, Mindt, Sadek, Moore, Bentley, McCutchan, Reicks and Grant2004), higher rates of self-reported problems with medication management (e.g., Avants et al., Reference Avants, Margolin, Warburton, Hawkins and Shi2001; Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Mexhir, Walsh and Hewitt2000; Waldrop-Valverde et al., Reference Waldrop-Valverde, Ownby, Wilkie, Mack, Kumar and Metsch2006; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b), and nonadherence as measured by electronic medication monitors (e.g., Barclay et al., Reference Barclay, Hinkin, Castellon, Mason, Reinhard, Marion, Levine and Durvasula2007; Hinkin et al., Reference Hinkin, Castellon, Durvasula, Hardy, Lam, Mason, Thrasher, Goetz and Stefaniak2002, Reference Hinkin, Hardy, Mason, Castellon, Durvasula, Lam and Stefaniak2004). For example, Hinkin et al. (Reference Hinkin, Castellon, Durvasula, Hardy, Lam, Mason, Thrasher, Goetz and Stefaniak2002) reported that individuals with neuropsychological impairment experienced a twofold risk of nonadherence, even when the potentially confounding effects of demographic factors and psychiatric comorbidities were considered. Across this literature, the domains of episodic learning and memory, executive functions, and psychomotor speed have emerged as the most robust and reliable cognitive predictors of cART nonadherence.

It has been argued that HIV-associated impairment in the domain of prospective memory (ProM) may be a particularly strong risk factor for cART nonadherence (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Nixon, Pitrak, Weddington, Rains, Nunnally, Grbesic, Gonzalez, Jacobus and Bechara2007; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b). ProM is a dissociable aspect of episodic memory that refers to the execution of a future intention in the face of ongoing distractions (i.e., “remembering to remember”). The cognitive aspects of medication adherence can be readily mapped on a conceptual framework of ProM. Specifically, successful independent medication adherence requires one to (1) encode an intention to take a specific medication at a future occasion (e.g., take medication X with food before going to bed), (2) retain the paired intention (i.e., take medication X) and cue (i.e., at bedtime) vis-à-vis the usual barrage of normal daily events (e.g., work, chores, and recreation), (3) accurately identify the retrieval cue and effectively disengage from an ongoing activity (e.g., preparing for bed), (4) recall the specific intention (i.e., take medication X with food), and (5) execute the intention (i.e., take the correct medication as instructed). In fact, the most commonly cited example of ProM in daily life is remembering to take a medication on schedule, for example after a meal (i.e., event-based ProM) or at a specific time during the day (i.e., time-based ProM). Despite these conceptual similarities, only three prior studies have directly examined the relationship between ProM and medication adherence (Hertzog et al., Reference Hertzog, Park, Morell and Martin2000; Vedhara et al., Reference Vedhara, Wadsworth, Norman, Searle, Mitchel, Macrae, O’Mahony, Kemple and Memel2004; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b).

Beyond its conceptual appeal, ProM may be of particular relevance to cART adherence because HIV infection is associated with an increased risk of ProM impairment. Individuals living with HIV disease report an elevated level of ProM complaints (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Carey, Moran, Dawson, Letendre and Grant2007a) and demonstrate mild-to-moderate deficits on performance-based laboratory (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Nixon, Pitrak, Weddington, Rains, Nunnally, Grbesic, Gonzalez, Jacobus and Bechara2007) and semi-naturalistic (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006) measures of ProM. The profile of HIV-associated ProM impairment is hypothesized to reflect deficits in the strategic aspects of intention encoding and retrieval (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006), including increased errors of omission (i.e., not responding to a cue), commission (e.g., incorrectly responding to a cue), and loss of time (i.e., responding to a cue at the incorrect time) in the setting of normal recognition. HIV-associated ProM impairment correlates with deficits in executive functions, working memory, retrospective episodic memory, and information processing speed (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Nixon, Pitrak, Weddington, Rains, Nunnally, Grbesic, Gonzalez, Jacobus and Bechara2007), as well as biological markers of neuroaxonal injury and macrophage activation (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Morgan, Marquie-Beck, Carey, Grant and Letendre2006b).

Only one prior study has examined the role of HIV-associated ProM impairment in medication management. Woods et al. (Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b) reported that higher frequency of ProM complaints and objective deficits on laboratory and semi-naturalistic ProM measures were associated with poorer self-reported medication management in HIV. Of particular note, HIV-associated ProM impairment demonstrated incremental validity as a predictor of medication management, above-and-beyond established risk factors for nonadherence, including psychiatric distress, psychosocial variables, environmental structure, and deficits in retrospective memory and executive functions. Although this study provided promising evidence that ProM may play a unique role in medication management, it nevertheless possessed several methodological limitations. For example, the study design was exclusively cross-sectional and therefore only provided evidence of concurrent, rather than predictive validity. Another significant limitation of this study was its use of a self-report measure of general medication management (i.e., Beliefs Related to Medications [BERMA] Survey; McDonald-Miszczak et al., Reference McDonald-Miszczak, Maris, Fitzgibbon and Ritchie2004). Although there is no gold standard for adherence (Osterberg & Blaschke, Reference Osterberg and Blaschke2005), self-report measures tend to overestimate actual adherence (Levine et al., Reference Levine, Hinkin, Marion, Keuning, Castellon, Lam, Robinet, Longshore, Newton, Myers and Durvasula2006). Moreover, this particular self-report measure was not specific to ARVs, but rather assessed all currently prescribed medications. Use of this measure also limited the prior study’s ecological validity by not allowing for an objective cut-point to identify individuals who were nonadherent to cART, which is the classification of greatest clinical relevance.

Accordingly, the current study was undertaken to determine whether baseline indicators of ProM functioning accurately predict cART adherence as measured in a subsequent 1-month observation period using electronic medication monitors. It was hypothesized that HIV-associated ProM impairment would be associated with an increased risk of cART nonadherence independent of demographics, HIV disease severity, psychiatric comorbidity, and psychosocial factors.

METHOD

Participants

The study sample included 79 participants with HIV infection who were recruited from the San Diego community (e.g., via newspaper advertisements) and local HIV treatment clinics. All participants provided written, informed consent prior to enrolling in this study, which was approved by the institution’s human research protections program. To be considered for inclusion, participants must have been prescribed at least one ARV medication. Study exclusions at enrollment included severe psychiatric illness (e.g., schizophrenia), neurological disease (e.g., seizure disorders, stroke, closed head injuries with loss of consciousness for more than 15 min, and central nervous system neoplasms or opportunistic infections), estimated verbal IQ scores <70 (based on the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading [WTAR]; Psychological Corporation, 2001), a recent diagnosis of substance dependence (i.e., within 6 months of baseline evaluation), and a urine toxicology screen positive for illicit drugs on the day of testing. A positive toxicology screen for marijuana (n = 17) was not a basis for exclusion since its metabolites remain detectable in urine for as long as 1 month and several drugs commonly used in the management of HIV (e.g., efavirenz, marinol) are known to produce positive toxicology results.

Participants were classified as either Adherent or Nonadherent based on the outcome of a 4-week continuous observation period using the (non-alarmed) Medication Event Monitoring System (MEMS; Aprex Corporation, Union City, CA). The MEMS observation period began on the day following participants’ neuropsychological evaluation (described below). The MEMS cap system uses a medication bottle cap (Trackcap®) microchip device that recorded the time, date, and frequency with which the participants opened their medication bottle over the 4-week period. Participants were instructed to use only the MEMS bottle to dispense the target ARV and to remove only one dose at a time. Nonadherence was determined by a blind clinical review of the MEMS protocols and was defined as <90% adherence to their target ARV on any of the following variables: (1) percent days correct number of doses taken, (2) percent prescribed number of doses taken, and (3) percent prescribed doses taken on schedule. Table 1 displays the demographic, HIV disease, and MEMS ARV adherence characteristics of the Adherent (n = 48) and Nonadherent (n = 31) groups.

Table 1. Participants’ demographic, disease, and medication characteristics

a Verbal IQ (M = 100, SD = 15) was derived from the WTAR.

b Median (interquartile range).

Materials and Procedure

The day prior to the beginning of their MEMS observation period, all participants underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological, psychiatric, and medical research evaluation.

Prospective memory assessment

The primary measure of interest was the Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST; Raskin, Reference Raskin2004), which is a 30-min, eight-trial test during which participants engage in a word search puzzle as the distractor task. Consistent with prior studies (e.g., Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006), we examined the following MIST variables: (1) summary score, (2) time-based scale, (3) event-based scale, (4) distractor total, (5) recognition total, (6) a retrieval index, and (7) a 24-hr delay trial for which examinees were instructed to leave a voicemail message for the examiner the day after the examination indicating the number of hours the participant slept the night after the evaluation (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006). In addition, the following error types were coded: (1) no response (i.e., response omission errors), (2) task substitutions (e.g., replacement of a verbal response with an action or vice versa), (3) loss of content (e.g., acknowledgment that a response is required to a cue, but failure to recall the content), and (4) loss of time (i.e., performance of an intention greater than ±15% of the target time). Participants also completed the eight-item Prospective Memory Scale from the Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Della Sala, Logie and Maylor2000), which was used to assess self-reported ProM complaints.

Basic neuropsychological assessment

A standardized battery of clinical tests of neuropsychological functioning was also administered to each participant. This battery was designed to be consistent with National Institutes of Health guidelines for assessing the cognitive domains that are most sensitive to HIV (Antinori et al., Reference Antinori, Arendt, Becker, Brew, Byrd, Cherner, Clifford, Cinque, Epstein, Goodkin, Gisslen, Grant, Heaton, Joseph, Marder, Marra, McArthur, Nunn, Price, Pulliam, Robertson, Sacktor, Valcour and Wojna2007; Butters et al., Reference Butters, Grant, Haxby, Judd, Martin, McClelland, Pequegnat, Schacter and Stover1990), including retrospective learning and memory, executive functions, information processing speed, attention/working memory, verbal fluency, and motor coordination. Raw scores were converted to population-based z scores derived from the entire sample, then averaged across the tests in that domain to create a mean domain z score. The specific tests that comprised each of these domains (and their associated descriptive data) are displayed in Table 3 (Benton et al., Reference Benton, Hamsher and Sivan1994; Culbertson & Zillmer, Reference Culbertson and Zillmer2001; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000; Kløve, Reference Kløve1963; Morgan et al., in press; Psychological Corporation, 1997; Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1985; Shimamura & Jurica, Reference Shimamura and Jurica1994; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Javorsky, Singer, Singer Harris, Somerville, Duke, Thompson and Kaplan1999; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Scott, Sires, Grant, Heaton and Tröster2005).

Table 2. Prospective memory performance in the Adherent and Nonadherent groups

Note.

ProM, prospective memory.

Psychiatric assessment

Structured psychiatric interviews were conducted using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (version 2.1; World Health Organization, 1998), from which lifetime and current (i.e., within 1 month of evaluation) diagnoses of major depressive disorder (MDD), generalized anxiety disorder, and substance-related disorders were generated per Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria. Participants also completed the profile of mood states (POMS; McNair et al., Reference McNair, Lorr and Droppleman1981) to assess current affective distress across four areas (i.e., depression/dejection, fatigue/inertia, vigor/activity, and tension/anxiety) and a Total Mood Disturbance score, for which higher scores indicate greater distress.

Psychosocial and environmental factors

Participants were administered the BERMA (McDonald-Miszczak et al., Reference McDonald-Miszczak, Maris, Fitzgibbon and Ritchie2004) questionnaire, from which three scales were derived. The 23-item Dealing with Health Professionals Scale is intended to assess the strength of participants’ relationship with their medical providers (e.g., “I have difficulty talking openly with my physician”). The 20-item Medication Management Efficacy Scale was designed to assess general medication management abilities (e.g., “I am less efficient at adhering to my medication regimen than I used to be”). Finally, the 10-item Attitudes About Medications Scale comprises items that measure participants’ general health beliefs (e.g., “I am taking too much medication for my medical conditions”). All subscale items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

In addition, participants completed the Prospective Memory for Medications Questionnaire (PMMQ; Gould et al., Reference Gould, McDonald-Miszczak and King1997). The PMMQ is a 33-item questionnaire that assesses the frequency with which an individual uses different internal (e.g., “Do you regularly repeat to yourself the instructions for taking a prescription …?”) and external (e.g., “Do you use a clock or watch alarm to remind you when it is time to take your medications?”) medication-taking strategies. Participants are asked to rate how often they use each strategy on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (always), such that higher scores indicate more frequent strategy use.

Data Analyses

The MIST variables were nonnormally distributed (i.e., negatively skewed), and therefore, a series of Wilcoxon rank-sum tests were conducted and complemented by Cohen’s d effect size estimates. Group differences on a priori selected measures of neuropsychological functioning, psychiatric variables, psychosocial and environmental factors, and disease and treatment status were conducted using either Wilcoxon rank-sum tests or a chi-square test. We then conducted a planned follow-up binary logistic regression analysis to evaluate the relative independence of ProM as a predictor of nonadherence as compared to the other cognitive (e.g., retrospective memory) factors. As noted above, there are numerous noncognitive factors that also increase the risk of nonadherence (e.g., demographics, psychiatric disease, substance-related disorders, and psychosocial variables). As such, we also conducted a follow-up binary logistic regression to examine the uniqueness of ProM as a predictor of nonadherence relative to salient noncognitive variables that differentiated the groups. A critical alpha level of .05 was used for all analyses.

RESULTS

Nonadherent participants performed significantly worse than the Adherent group on the MIST summary score (p < .05). As shown in Table 2, accounting for this overall effect were the Nonadherent group’s lower scores on the time-based scale (p < .05), which were primarily driven by an elevation in loss of time (LoT) errors (p < .01). Although significantly correlated with all three indicators of adherence (ps < .05), LoT errors were most strongly associated with the proportion of ARV doses taken on schedule (Spearman’s ρ = −0.29, p = .011). The Adherent and Nonadherent groups did not differ in their performance on the MIST event-based scale, other error types, the distractor test, the recognition posttest, or 24-hr delay trial (all ps > .05). Similarly, the Adherent and Nonadherent groups reported similar levels of ProM complaints on the PRMQ (ps > .10). As such, we conducted a series of focused, nonparametric (i.e., descriptive) classification accuracy statistics on the MIST LoT error score. As shown in Figure 1, a normative cutoff score of ≥1 LoT error (Woods et al., in press) afforded adequate overall predictive power, characterized by excellent specificity, but rather poor sensitivity. Of greater clinical value, positive and negative predictive powers for LoT were each approximately 70%. In fact, the nonparametric odds ratio (Bieliauskas et al., Reference Bieliauskas, Fastenau, Lacy and Roper1997) associated with elevated LoT errors was 5.8 (95% confidence interval = 1.9–17.5).

Fig. 1. Descriptive classification accuracy statistics for LoT errors on the MIST as an indicator of Nonadherence. A normative cut-point of ≥1 LoT error was used (see Woods et al., in press) to classify Nonadherent (n = 31) versus Adherent (n = 48) participants. OPP, overall predictive power; NPP, negative predictive power; PPP, positive predictive power; Sens, sensitivity; Spec, specificity.

Table 3. Descriptive data on the standard neuropsychological tests in the Adherent and Nonadherent groups

Note.

Values reflect raw scores unless indicated. CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test (2nd ed.); BQSS, Boston Qualitative Scoring System; ToL, Tower of London, Drexel version; SOPT, self-ordered pointing test.

Post hoc analyses were undertaken to examine the possible correspondence between MIST LoT errors and a measure of time estimation (Mimura et al., Reference Mimura, Kinsbourne and O’Connor2000). Participants were asked to estimate how much time had elapsed during four brief intervals (i.e., 15, 30, 45, and 90 s in a randomized order), without the aid of a clock. The discrepancy between the actual time elapsed and the participants’ response did not differ between the Adherent (M = 49.3, SD = 37.2) and Nonadherent (M = 42.3, SD = 23.2) groups (p > .10). LoT errors did not correlate with time estimation in either study group (ps > .10).

Table 3 displays the descriptive data for the two study samples on the basic battery of neuropsychological tests. The Adherent group performed significantly better than the Nonadherent sample on RetM Learning and RetM Memory domain z scores (ps < .05), which were primarily attributable to group differences on the Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.) (WMS-III) Logical Memory I and II subtests (ps < .05). In a follow-up binary logistic regression that included the RetM Learning and Memory z scores and MIST LoT errors, (χ2[3, N = 79] = 15.0, p = .002), only LoT errors emerged as an independent predictor of Nonadherence (p = .002). Findings did not differ if only the significant RetM variables (i.e., WMS-III Logical Memory) or clinical ratings (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Rippeth, Frol, Levy, Ryan, Soukup, Hinkin, Lazzaretto, Cherner, Marcotte, Gelman, Morgello, Singer, Grant and Heaton2004) were included in the regression instead of the domain z scores.

Descriptive data regarding the various noncognitive variables associated with nonadherence are displayed in Table 1 (i.e., demographics and HIV disease and treatment characteristics) and Table 4 (i.e., psychiatric, substance-related, psychosocial, and environmental factors). The Nonadherent sample had significantly lower nadir CD4 counts and a larger proportion of individuals with diagnoses of acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (ps < .05), but there was no association between ARV pill burden and adherence (Table 1). With regard to psychiatric predictors of adherence, Table 4 shows that the Nonadherent participants were significantly more likely to have lifetime diagnoses of MDD, endorsed higher levels of acute distress on the POMS Tension/Anxiety Scale, and reported more difficulties in their general ability to manage their medications (BERMA Medication Management Scale; ps ≤ .05). A planned follow-up logistic regression analysis that included all these noncognitive factors (χ2[6, N = 77] = 33.9, p < .0001) showed that MIST LoT errors (p < .0001) and the POMS Tension/Anxiety scale (p = .03) were the sole independent predictors of Nonadherence. Additionally, the independence of the MIST LoT errors did not waiver if the various trend-level findings (e.g., sex, WTAR VIQ, BERMA dealing with health professionals) were included in the statistical model.

Table 4. Psychiatric and psychosocial characteristics of the study groups

a Reflects any prior substance dependence diagnosis.

DISCUSSION

Nonadherence to cART is highly prevalent and significantly increases the risk of poor HIV disease outcomes, thus underscoring the value of identifying salient cognitive predictors of nonadherence that may inform the development of effective interventions. Results from the current study indicate that HIV-infected individuals with deficits in prospective memory (ProM) are at elevated risk of cART nonadherence as measured by electronic pill monitoring. At a group level, Nonadherent individuals demonstrated significantly poorer ProM functioning as compared to Adherent sample, particularly on an index of time-based ProM. These findings were associated with a medium-to-large effect size and were primarily driven by an elevated rate of LoT errors in the Nonadherent participants, meaning that although Nonadherent individuals remembered to perform the prescribed intention, they did so at an incorrect time (i.e., >15% away from the target execution time). Although slightly less than 50% of Nonadherent persons made one or more LoT errors (sensitivity = 45.2%), the corresponding—and arguably more clinically relevant (Ivnik et al., Reference Ivnik, Smith, Petersen, Boeve, Kokmen and Tangalos2000)—positive (70%) and negative (71%) predictive values of such errors were considerably better. In fact, individuals who committed one or more LoT errors were almost six times more likely to be classified as Nonadherent at 1-month follow-up (odds ratio = 5.8).

Notably, ProM LoT errors were a unique and independent predictor of nonadherence when considered alongside well-established predictors of adherence. Consistent with prior research, impairment in retrospective learning and memory (e.g., Hinkin et al., Reference Hinkin, Castellon, Durvasula, Hardy, Lam, Mason, Thrasher, Goetz and Stefaniak2002), psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., DiIorio et al., in press), HIV disease severity (Nieuwkerk et al., Reference Nieuwkerk, Sprangers, Burger, Hoetelmans, Hugen, Danner, van der Ende, Schneider, Schrey, Meenhorst, Sprenger, Kaufmann, Jambroes, Chesney, de Wolf and Lange2001), and psychosocial factors (e.g., Wagner, Reference Wagner2002) were also associated with nonadherence. Nevertheless, ProM LoT errors remained a significant predictor of nonadherence, even when these other factors were included in the statistical model. The independence of ProM as a predictor of nonadherence suggests that this construct may play a unique role in successful medication management, as has also been demonstrated with general instrumental activities of daily living (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Iudicello, Moran, Carey, Dawson and Grant2008a) and in other clinical populations (e.g., schizophrenia; Twamley et al., in press). In this way, assessment of ProM may augment the ecological relevance of neuropsychological evaluations of persons infected with HIV.

Time-based ProM, and particularly LoT errors, demonstrated the strongest association with medication nonadherence in this cohort. LoT errors are rare in healthy adults (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006; Woods et al., in press) but are mildly elevated in individuals with HIV infection (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006), as well as in those with schizophrenia (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Twamley, Dawson, Narvaez and Jeste2007b). Post hoc analyses revealed that the occurrence of LoT errors was not a function of deficient basic time perception, as LoT errors did not correlate with time estimation (and moreover, the Adherent and Nonadherent groups did not differ in time estimation). An alternate hypothesis is that LoT errors reflect difficulties monitoring time concurrently with an ongoing task. Numerous studies, including those on healthy older adults and individuals with central nervous system disease (for a review, see Mäntylä & Carelli, Reference Mäntylä, Carelli, Glicksohn and Myslobodsky2006) show that better performance on time-based ProM measures is associated with more frequent time monitoring. For example, Shum et al. (Reference Shum, Ungvari, Tang and Leung2004) demonstrated that healthy adults engaged in clock monitoring more frequently during a time-based ProM task than individuals with schizophrenia, particularly as the time for execution neared. Moreover, time monitoring was positively correlated with performance on the time-based ProM task (Shum et al., Reference Shum, Ungvari, Tang and Leung2004). In this way, the current findings converge with the profile of HIV-associated ProM impairment, which is thought to reflect difficulties in the strategic allocation of cognitive resources to properly manage the simultaneous burden of the cue monitoring (i.e., time) and ongoing foreground activities (e.g., Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton and Grant2006). In more applied terms, HIV-infected individuals with impaired ProM might not notice important time-based cues to take their medications during the course of day-to-day activities, thereby delaying (or missing) scheduled doses, which decreases the likelihood of maintaining adequate virologic control and favorable disease outcomes.

Results from this study extend a surprisingly small, but growing literature supporting the relationship between ProM impairment and medication nonadherence (Hertzog et al., Reference Hertzog, Park, Morell and Martin2000; Vedhara et al., Reference Vedhara, Wadsworth, Norman, Searle, Mitchel, Macrae, O’Mahony, Kemple and Memel2004), which includes only one prior investigation in HIV (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b). Building on the limitations of the existing ProM and adherence literature, the present study employed a prospective, longitudinal design and used an electronic monitoring device to assess medication adherence. Although not without its limitations (Bova et al., Reference Bova, Fennie, Knafl, Dieckhaus, Watrous and Willimas2005), this methodology provides an objective, behavioral indication of adherence patterns (Paterson et al., Reference Paterson, Potoski and Capitano2002) that is more sensitive to nonadherence than self-report (Levine et al., Reference Levine, Hinkin, Marion, Keuning, Castellon, Lam, Robinet, Longshore, Newton, Myers and Durvasula2006). Indeed, in contrast to our prior study using a generic self-report measure of medication management (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b), the current data did not show an association between cART nonadherence and either ProM complaints or the 24-hr semi-naturalistic ProM task. Importantly, however, both studies demonstrated that a laboratory-based measure of time-based ProM functioning (i.e., the MIST) was independently predictive of medication management in HIV.

A few methodological limitations should be considered when interpreting findings from this study. Most importantly, little is known about the psychometric properties of LoT errors, which tend to be infrequent, raising concerns about their reliability and possible floor and ceiling effects. Another limitation of this study is the absence of a measure of time production (Barkley et al., Reference Barkley, Murphy and Bush2001) or time monitoring during ProM (e.g., clock checking), both of which would allow for a better characterization of the relationship between time-based ProM and adherence. Considering the hypothesized relationship between time perception and frontal systems (see Meck, Reference Meck2005, for a review), future studies may wish to examine brief and extended time estimation and production intervals in HIV more generally, as well as in the specific context of cART adherence. The external validity of the study is restricted because the sample was predominantly male (85%) and had generally mild HIV disease (median current CD4 count = 559). Although these sample characteristics are fairly representative of the HIV epidemic in the United States, whether they generalize to specific subpopulations (e.g., older women with advanced HIV disease) remains to be determined. In addition, we excluded individuals with active substance dependence, which, prior research shows, is a strong predictor of nonadherence (e.g., Hinkin et al., Reference Hinkin, Barclay, Castellon, Levine, Durvasula, Marion, Myers and Longshore2007). Finally, other important predictors of adherence were not available in this cohort, including such psychosocial factors such as access to healthcare, socioeconomic status, and health literacy.

In summary, findings from this study indicate that HIV-associated impairment in time-based ProM increases the risk of cART nonadherence independent of psychiatric comorbidity, HIV disease severity, general cognitive impairment, demographics, and select psychosocial factors. Together with prior literature on ProM and adherence (Hertzog et al., Reference Hertzog, Park, Morell and Martin2000; Vedhara et al., Reference Vedhara, Wadsworth, Norman, Searle, Mitchel, Macrae, O’Mahony, Kemple and Memel2004; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Moran, Carey, Dawson, Iudicello, Gibson, Grant and Atkinson2008b), such findings suggest that interventions that target ProM may be effective in improving adherence. For example, cognitive techniques such as goal management training (Levine et al., Reference Levine, Robertson, Clare, Carter, Hong, Wilson, Duncan and Stuss2000), which uses structured exercises designed to teach individuals to engage in an “executive review” of their plans and intentions for the day (e.g., “What am I doing right now?”, “What else do I have to do today and when?”) may be effective in improving ProM, as was recently shown in patients with traumatic brain injury (Fish et al., Reference Fish, Evans, Nimmo, Martin, Kersel, Bateman, Wilson and Manly2007). Other intervention approaches might focus on reducing the need for strategic monitoring, perhaps by reducing cognitive load (i.e., reducing the number and complexity of intentions held “online”; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Dawson, Carey, Morgan, Scott and Grant2006a) and/or minimizing ongoing distraction (e.g., McDaniel & Einstein, Reference McDaniel and Einstein2007). Relatedly, a noninvasive and relatively inexpensive (cf. caregivers) intervention option might involve a programmable electronic device (e.g., a watch) that prominently notifies the patient when it is time to take a medication with a detailed text message that includes the medication name, dosage, and particular conditions under which it should be taken (e.g., Andrade et al., Reference Andrade, McGruder, Wu, Celano, Skolasky, Selnes, Huang and McArthur2005; Leirer et al., Reference Leirer, Morrow, Tanke and Pariante1991; van den Broek et al., Reference van den Broek, Downes, Johnson, Dayus and Hilton2000). Prospective, theory-driven controlled trials of the effectiveness of these various strategies (perhaps as well as combined, individualized therapeutic approaches) as treatments for HIV-associated ProM impairment and nonadherence are needed.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center Group is affiliated with the University of California, San Diego, the Naval Hospital, San Diego, and the Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System and includes Director: Igor Grant, M.D.; Co-Directors: J. Hampton Atkinson, M.D., Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D., and J. Allen McCutchan, M.D.; Center Manager: Thomas D. Marcotte, Ph.D.; Naval Hospital San Diego: Braden R. Hale, M.D., M.P.H. (P.I.); Neuromedical Component: Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D. (P.I.), J. Allen McCutchan, M.D., Scott Letendre, M.D., Edmund Capparelli, Pharm.D., Rachel Schrier, Ph.D.; Neurobehavioral Component: Robert K. Heaton, Ph.D. (P.I.), Mariana Cherner, Ph.D., David J. Moore, Ph.D., Steven Paul Woods, Psy.D.; Neuroimaging Component: Terry Jernigan, Ph.D. (P.I.), Christine Fennema-Notestine, Ph.D., Sarah L., Archibald, M.A., John Hesselink, M.D., Jacopo Annese, Ph.D., Michael J. Taylor, Ph.D.; Neurobiology Component: Eliezer Masliah, M.D. (P.I.), Ian Everall, FRCPsych., FRCPath., Ph.D., T. Dianne Langford, Ph.D.; Neurovirology Component: Douglas Richman, M.D., (P.I.), David M. Smith, M.D.; International Component: J. Allen McCutchan, M.D., (P.I.); Developmental Component: Ian Everall, FRCPsych., FRCPath., Ph.D. (P.I.), Stuart Lipton, M.D., Ph.D.; Clinical Trials Component: J. Allen McCutchan, M.D., J. Hampton Atkinson, M.D., Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D., Scott Letendre, M.D.; Participant Accrual and Retention Unit: J. Hampton Atkinson, M.D. (P.I.), Rodney von Jaeger, M.P.H.; Data Management Unit: Anthony C. Gamst, Ph.D. (P.I.), Clint Cushman, B.A. (Data Systems Manager), Daniel R. Masys, M.D. (Senior Consultant); Statistics Unit: Ian Abramson, Ph.D. (P.I.), Christopher Ake, Ph.D., Florin Vaida, Ph.D.

This research was supported by grants MH073419 and MH62512 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Department of Defense, or the U.S. government. No relationships exist, financial or otherwise, that could be interpreted as a conflict of interest affecting this manuscript. No information from this manuscript or the manuscript itself has ever been published either electronically or in print. The authors thank Dr. Catherine L. Carey and Lisa M. Moran for their substantial contributions to the parent grant, Ofilio Vigil for managing the psychiatric aspects of this study, Nancy Anderson for her assistance with data entry, and Dr. Sarah Raskin for providing us with the MIST.

References

REFERENCES

Albert, S.M., Flater, S.R., Clouse, R., Todak, G., Stern, Y., & Marder, K. (2003). Medication management skill in HIV: I. Evidence for adaptation of medication management strategies in people with cognitive impairment. II. Evidence for a pervasive lay model of medication efficacy. AIDS and Behavior, 7, 329338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, S.M., Weber, C.M., Todak, G., Polanco, C., Clouse, R., McElhiney, M., Rabkin, J., Stern, Y., & Marder, K. (1999). An observed performance test of medication management ability in HIV: Relation to neuropsychological status and medication adherence outcomes. AIDS and Behavior, 3, 121128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Andrade, A.S., McGruder, H.F., Wu, A.W., Celano, S.A., Skolasky, R.L. Jr., Selnes, O.A., Huang, I.C., & McArthur, J.C. (2005). A programmable prompting device improves adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy in HIV-infected subjects with memory impairment. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 41, 875882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antinori, A., Arendt, G., Becker, J.T., Brew, B.J., Byrd, D.A., Cherner, M., Clifford, D.B., Cinque, P., Epstein, L.G., Goodkin, K., Gisslen, M., Grant, I., Heaton, R.K., Joseph, J., Marder, K., Marra, C.M., McArthur, J.C., Nunn, M., Price, R.W., Pulliam, L., Robertson, K.R., Sacktor, N., Valcour, V., & Wojna, V.E. (2007). Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology, 69, 17891799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avants, S.K., Margolin, A., Warburton, L.A., Hawkins, K.A., & Shi, J. (2001). Predictors of nonadherence to HIV-related medication regimens during methadone stabilization. The American Journal on Addictions, 10, 6978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bangsburg, D.R. (2008). Preventing HIV antiretroviral resistance through better monitoring of treatment adherence. Journal of Infectious Diseases, 197(Suppl 3), 272278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, T.R., Hinkin, C.H., Castellon, S.A., Mason, K.I., Reinhard, M.J., Marion, S.D., Levine, A.J., & Durvasula, R.S. (2007). Age-associated predictors of medication adherence in HIV-positive adults: Health beliefs, self-efficacy, and neurocognitive status. Health Psychology, 26, 4049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R.A., Murphy, K.R., & Bush, T. (2001). Time perception and reproduction in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15(3), 351360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedict, R.H.B., Mexhir, J.J., Walsh, K., & Hewitt, R.G. (2000). Impact of human immunodeficiency virus type-1-associated cognitive dysfunction on activities of daily living and quality of life. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 529534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K., & Sivan, A.B. (1994). Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.Google Scholar
Bieliauskas, L.A., Fastenau, P.S., Lacy, M.A., & Roper, B.L. (1997). Use of the odds ratio to translate neuropsychological test scores into real-world outcomes: From statistical significance to clinical significance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 889896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bova, C.A., Fennie, K.P., Knafl, G.J., Dieckhaus, K.D., Watrous, E., & Willimas, A.B. (2005). Use of electronic monitoring devices to measure antiretroviral adherence: Practical considerations. AIDS and Behavior, 9, 103110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butters, N., Grant, I., Haxby, J., Judd, L.L., Martin, A., McClelland, J., Pequegnat, W., Schacter, D., & Stover, E. (1990). Assessment of AIDS-related cognitive changes: Recommendations of the NIMH workshop on neuropsychological assessment approaches. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 963978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, C.L., Woods, S.P., Rippeth, J.D., Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group (2006). Prospective memory in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 536548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2006). HIV/AIDS surveillance Report, 2005, Vol. 17. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Center for Disease Control and Prevention.Google Scholar
Chesney, M.A., Ickovics, J.R., Chambers, D.B., Gifford, A.L., Neidig, J., Zwickl, B., & Wu, A.W. (2000). Self-reported adherence to antiretroviral medications among participants in HIV clinical trials: The AACTG adherence instruments. Patient Care Patient Care Committee & Adherence Working Group of the Outcomes Committee of the Adult AIDS Clinical Trials Group (AACTG). AIDS Care, 12, 255266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culbertson, W.C. & Zillmer, E.A. (2001). The Tower of London DX (TOL-DX) manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). The California Verbal Learning Test (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
DiIorio, C., McCarty, F., Depadilla, L., Resnicow, K., Holstad, M.M., Yeager, K., Sharma, S.M., Morisky, D.E., & Lundberg, B. (in press). Adherence to antiretroviral medication regimens: A test of a psychosocial model. AIDS and Behavior.Google Scholar
Fish, J., Evans, J.J., Nimmo, M., Martin, E., Kersel, D., Bateman, A., Wilson, B.A., & Manly, T. (2007). Rehabilitation of executive dysfunction following brain injury: “Content-free” cueing improves everyday prospective memory performance. Neuropsychologia, 45(6), 13181330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, O.N., McDonald-Miszczak, L., & King, B. (1997). Metacognition and medication adherence: How do older adults remember? Experimental Aging Research, 23, 315342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrigan, P.R., Hogg, R.S., Dong, W.W., Yup, B., Wynhoven, B., Woodward, J., Brumme, C.J., Brumme, Z.L., Mo, T., Alexander, C.S., & Montaner, J.S. (2005). Predictors of HIV drug-resistance mutations in a large antiretroviral-naïve cohort initiating triple antiretroviral therapy. Journal of Infectious Diseases, 191, 339347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., Butters, N., White, D.A., Kirson, D., Atkinson, J.H., McCutchan, J.A., Taylor, M.J., Kelly, M.D., & Ellis, R.J. (1995). The HNRC 500: Neuropsychology of HIV infection at different disease stages. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 231251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R.K., Marcotte, T.D., Mindt, M.R., Sadek, J., Moore, D.J., Bentley, H., McCutchan, J.A., Reicks, C., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (2004). The impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment on everyday functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 317331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzog, C., Park, D., Morell, R.W., & Martin, M. (2000). Ask and ye shall receive: Behavioural specificity in the accuracy of subjective memory complaints. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 257275.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinkin, C.H., Barclay, T.R., Castellon, S.A., Levine, A.J., Durvasula, R.S., Marion, S.D., Myers, H.F., & Longshore, D. (2007). Drug use and medication adherence among HIV-1 infected individuals. AIDS and Behavior, 11, 185194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinkin, C.H., Castellon, S.A., Durvasula, R.S., Hardy, D.J., Lam, M.N., Mason, K.I., Thrasher, D., Goetz, M.B., & Stefaniak, M. (2002). Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Neurology, 59, 19441950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinkin, C.H., Hardy, D.J., Mason, K.I., Castellon, S.A., Durvasula, R.S., Lam, M.N., & Stefaniak, M. (2004). Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS, 18, S19S25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivnik, R.J., Smith, G.E., Petersen, R.C., Boeve, B.F., Kokmen, E., & Tangalos, E.G. (2000). Diagnostic accuracy of four approaches to interpreting neuropsychological test data. Neuropsychology, 14, 163177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kløve, H. (1963). Grooved pegboard. Indiana: Lafayette Instruments.Google Scholar
Leirer, V.O., Morrow, D.G., Tanke, E.D., & Pariante, G.M. (1991). Elders’ nonadherence: Its assessment and medication reminding by voice mail. The Gerontologist, 31, 514520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, A.J., Hinkin, C.H., Marion, S., Keuning, A., Castellon, S.A., Lam, M.M., Robinet, M., Longshore, D., Newton, T., Myers, H., & Durvasula, R.S. (2006). Adherence to antiretroviral medications in HIV: Differences in data collected via self-report and electronic monitoring. Health Psychology, 25, 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, B., Robertson, I.H., Clare, L., Carter, G., Hong, J., Wilson, B.A., Duncan, J., & Stuss, D.T. (2000). Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental-clinical validation of goal management training. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 299312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lima, V.D., Geller, J., Bangsberg, D.R., Patterson, T.L., Daniel, M., & Kerr, T. (2007). The effect of adherence on the association between depressive symptoms and mortality among HIV-infected individuals first initiating HAART. AIDS, 21, 11751183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C., Ostrow, D., Detels, R., Hu, Z., Johnson, L., Kingsley, L., & Jacobson, L.P. (2006). Impacts of HIV infection and HAART use on quality of life. Quality of Life Research, 15, 941949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäntylä, T. & Carelli, M.G. (2006). Time monitoring and executive functioning: Individual and developmental differences. In Glicksohn, J. & Myslobodsky, M.S. (Eds.), Timing the future: The case for a time-based prospective memory (pp. 191211). River Edge, NJ: World Scientific Publishing Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, E.M., Nixon, H., Pitrak, D.L., Weddington, W., Rains, N.A., Nunnally, G., Grbesic, S., Gonzalez, R., Jacobus, J., & Bechara, A. (2007). Characteristics of prospective memory deficits in HIV-seropositive substance-dependent individuals: Preliminary observations. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 496504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, M.A. & Einstein, G.O. (2007). Prospective memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging field. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald-Miszczak, L., Maris, P., Fitzgibbon, T., & Ritchie, G. (2004). A pilot study examining older adults’ beliefs related to medication adherence: The BERMA Survey. Journal of Aging and Health, 16, 591614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNair, D.M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L.F. (1981). Manual for the profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
Meck, W.H. (2005). Neuropsychology of timing and time perception. Brain and Cognition, 58, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mimura, M., Kinsbourne, M., & O’Connor, M. (2000). Time estimation by patients with frontal lesions and by Korsakoff amnesics. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 517528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, E.E., Woods, S.P., Weber, E., Dawson, M.S., Carey, C.L., Moran, L.M., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (in press). HIV-associated episodic memory impairment: Evidence of a possible differential deficit in source memory for complex visual stimuli. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences.Google Scholar
Nieuwkerk, P.T., Sprangers, M.A., Burger, D.M., Hoetelmans, R.M., Hugen, P.W., Danner, S.A., van der Ende, M.E., Schneider, M.M., Schrey, G., Meenhorst, P.L., Sprenger, H.G., Kaufmann, R.H., Jambroes, M., Chesney, M.A., de Wolf, F., Lange, J.M., & The ATHENA Project. (2001). Limited patient adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy for HIV-1 infection in an observational cohort study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 19621968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterberg, L. & Blaschke, T. (2005). Adherence to medication. New England Journal of Medicine, 353, 487497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, D.L., Potoski, B., & Capitano, B. (2002). Measurement of adherence to antiretroviral medications. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome, 15, S103S106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perno, C.F., Ceccherini-Silberstein, F., De Luca, A., Cozzi-Lepri, A., Gori, C., Cingolani, A., Bellocchi, M.C., Trotta, M.P., Piano, P., Forbici, F., Scasso, A., Vullo, V., d’Arminio Monforte, A., Antinori, A., & The AdICoNA Study Group. (2002). Virologic correlates of adherence to antiretroviral medications and therapeutic failure. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome, 31, S118S122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychological Corporation. (1997). WAIS-III and WMS-III technical manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Psychological Corporation. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Raskin, S. (2004). Memory for intentions screening test [abstract]. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(Suppl 1), 110.Google Scholar
Reitan, R.M. & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Robertson, K.R., Smurzynski, M., Parsons, T.D., Wu, K., Bosch, R.J., Wu, J., McCartur, J.C., Collier, A.C., Evans, S.R., & Ellis, R.J. (2007). The prevalence and incidence of neurocognitive impairment in the HAART era. AIDS, 21, 915921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimamura, A.P. & Jurica, P.J. (1994). Memory interference effects and aging: Findings from a test of frontal lobe function. Neuropsychology, 8, 408412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shum, D., Ungvari, G.S., Tang, W.K., & Leung, J.P. (2004). Performance of schizophrenia patients on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30, 693701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simoni, J.M., Frick, P.A., Pantalone, D.W., & Turner, B.J. (2003). Antiretroviral adherence interventions: A review of current literature and ongoing studies. Topics in HIV Medicine, 11, 185198.Google Scholar
Smith, G., Della Sala, S., Logie, R.H., & Maylor, E.A. (2000). Prospective and retrospective memory in normal aging and dementia: A questionnaire study. Memory, 8, 311321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, R.A., Javorsky, D.J., Singer, E.A., Singer Harris, N.G., Somerville, J.A., Duke, L.M., Thompson, J.A., & Kaplan, E. (1999). The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (BQSS): Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Twamley, E.W., Woods, S.P., Zurhellen, C.H., Vertinski, M., Narvaez, J.M., Mausbach, B.T., Patterson, T.L., & Jeste, D.V. (2008). Neuropsychological substrates and everyday functioning implications of prospective memory impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 106, 4249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Broek, M.D., Downes, J., Johnson, Z., Dayus, B., & Hilton, N. (2000). Evaluation of an electronic memory aid in the neuropsychological rehabilitation of prospective memory deficits. Brain Injury, 14, 455462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vedhara, K., Wadsworth, E., Norman, P., Searle, A., Mitchel, J., Macrae, N., O’Mahony, M., Kemple, T., & Memel, D. (2004). Habitual prospective memory in elderly patients with type 2 diabetes: Implications for medication adherence. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 9, 1727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, G.J. (2002). Predictors of antiretroviral adherence as measured by self-report, electronic monitoring, and medication diaries. AIDS Patient Care STDs, 16, 599608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldrop-Valverde, D., Ownby, R.L., Wilkie, F.L., Mack, A., Kumar, M., & Metsch, L. (2006). Neurocognitive aspects of medication adherence in HIV-positive injecting drug users. AIDS and Behavior, 10, 287297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Carey, C.L., Moran, L.M., Dawson, M.S., Letendre, S.L., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (2007a). Frequency and predictors of self-reported memory complaints in individuals infected with HIV. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 187195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Dawson, M.S., Carey, C.L., Morgan, E.E., Scott, J.C., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (2006a). Increased cognitive load exacerbates HIV-1-associated time-based prospective memory impairment [abstract]. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(Suppl 1), 148.Google Scholar
Woods, S.P., Iudicello, J.E., Moran, L.M., Carey, C.L., Dawson, M.S., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (2008a). HIV-associated prospective memory impairment increases risk of dependence in everyday functioning. Neuropsychology, 22, 110117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Moran, L.M., Carey, C.L., Dawson, M.S., Iudicello, J.E., Gibson, S., Grant, I., Atkinson, J.H., & The HNRC Group. (2008b). Prospective memory in HIV infection: Is “remembering to remember” a unique predictor of self-reported medication management? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 257270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, S.P., Moran, L.M., Dawson, M.S., Carey, C.L., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (2008). Psychometric characteristics of the Memory for Intentions Screening Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22, 864878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Morgan, E.E., Marquie-Beck, J., Carey, C.L., Grant, I., Letendre, S.L., & The HNRC Group. (2006b). Markers of macrophage activation and axonal injury are associated with prospective memory in HIV-1 disease. Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology, 19, 217221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Rippeth, J.D., Frol, A.B., Levy, J.K., Ryan, E., Soukup, V.M., Hinkin, C.H., Lazzaretto, D., Cherner, M., Marcotte, T.D., Gelman, B.B., Morgello, S., Singer, E.J., Grant, I., & Heaton, R.K. (2004). Interrater reliability of clinical ratings and neurocognitive diagnoses in HIV. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 759778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Scott, J.C., Sires, D.A., Grant, I., Heaton, R.K., Tröster, A.I., & The HNRC Group. (2005). Action (verb) fluency: Test-retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 408415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Twamley, E.W., Dawson, M.S., Narvaez, J.M., & Jeste, D.V. (2007b). Deficits in cue detection and intention retrieval underlie prospective memory impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 90, 344350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (1998). Composite international diagnostic interview (CIDI, version 2.1). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participants’ demographic, disease, and medication characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Prospective memory performance in the Adherent and Nonadherent groups

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Descriptive classification accuracy statistics for LoT errors on the MIST as an indicator of Nonadherence. A normative cut-point of ≥1 LoT error was used (see Woods et al., in press) to classify Nonadherent (n = 31) versus Adherent (n = 48) participants. OPP, overall predictive power; NPP, negative predictive power; PPP, positive predictive power; Sens, sensitivity; Spec, specificity.

Figure 3

Table 3. Descriptive data on the standard neuropsychological tests in the Adherent and Nonadherent groups

Figure 4

Table 4. Psychiatric and psychosocial characteristics of the study groups