Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T08:21:37.879Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of Post-concussive Symptoms in Young Children: Injury versus Non-injury Related Factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2016

Coco O. Bernard*
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Jennie A. Ponsford
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Audrey McKinlay
Affiliation:
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Australia
Dean McKenzie
Affiliation:
Epworth HealthCare and School of Public Health and Preventive Medicine, Melbourne, Australia
David Krieser
Affiliation:
Sunshine Hospital Emergency Department, Melbourne, Australia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Coco O. Bernard, Doctorate of Clinical Neuropsychology (candidate), School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, Australia 3800. E-mail: coco.bernard@monash.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives: A notable minority of children will experience persistent post-concussive symptoms (PCS) following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), likely maintained by a combination of injury and non-injury related factors. Adopting a prospective longitudinal design, this study aimed to investigate the relative influence of child, family, and injury factors on both acute and persistent PCS in young children. Methods: Participants were 101 children aged 2–12 who presented to an Emergency Department, with either mTBI or minor bodily trauma (control). PCS were assessed at time of injury, 1 week, and 1, 2, and 3 months post-injury. Predictors included injury and demographic variables, premorbid child behavior, sleep hygiene, and parental stress. Random effects ordinal logistic regression models were used to analyze the relative influence of these predictors on PCS at early (acute – 1 week) and late (1–3 month) post-injury phases. Results: Presence of mTBI was a stronger predictor of PCS in the early [odds ratio (OR)=18.2] compared with late (OR=7.3) post-injury phase. Older age at injury and pre-existing learning difficulties were significant predictors of PCS beyond 1 month post-injury. Family factors, including higher levels of parental stress, higher socio-economic status, and being of Anglo-Saxon descent, consistently predicted greater PCS. Conclusions: Injury characteristics were significantly associated with PCS for 3 months following mTBI but the association weakened over time. On the other hand, pre-existing child and family factors displayed an increasingly strong association with PCS over time. Follow-up for these “at-risk” children which also addresses family stress may minimize longer-term complications. (JINS, 2016, 22, 793–803)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2016 

INTRODUCTION

Mild traumatic brain injuries (mTBI) are extremely common during childhood, with incidence rates from Emergency Department (ED) presentations averaging around 300 per 100,000 in children aged 0–17 (Koepsell et al., Reference Koepsell, Rivara, Vavilala, Wang, Temkin, Jaffe and Durbin2011). These rates peak in pre-school children (421 per 100,000) where most injuries result from falls (Koepsell et al., Reference Koepsell, Rivara, Vavilala, Wang, Temkin, Jaffe and Durbin2011). Since the majority of children return to pre-morbid functioning relatively quickly with little intervention, routine follow up is neither feasible nor always necessary (Lloyd, Wilson, Tenovuo, & Saarijarvi, Reference Lloyd, Wilson, Tenovuo and Saarijarvi2015). Yet for the notable minority (11–17%) who experience ongoing post-concussive symptoms (PCS) beyond 3 months post-injury (Barlow et al., Reference Barlow, Crawford, Stevenson, Sandhu, Belanger and Dewey2010; Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, Borg, von Holst, Holm and Pepin2004; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Willmott, Rothwell, Cameron, Ayton, Nelms and Ng1999; Zemek, Barrowman, et al., Reference Zemek, Barrowman, Freedman, Gravel, Gagnon, McGahern and Osmond2016), early intervention and follow-up may be pivotal in reducing long-term complications (Winkler & Taylor, Reference Winkler and Taylor2015). Therefore, primary care clinicians need guidelines with which to discern those children at greater risk of ongoing PCS.

Few studies have prospectively examined the relative influence of injury and non-injury factors on PCS in younger children following mTBI (Kirkwood et al., Reference Kirkwood, Yeates, Taylor, Randolph, McCrea and Anderson2008; Zemek, Farion, Sampson, & McGahern, Reference Zemek, Farion, Sampson and McGahern2016). PCS refer to subjective somatic, cognitive, affective, and sleep-related symptoms that are commonly, albeit not exclusively, reported by children and their parents following mTBI (Barlow et al., Reference Barlow, Crawford, Stevenson, Sandhu, Belanger and Dewey2010; Mittenberg, Wittner, & Miller, Reference Mittenberg, Wittner and Miller1997; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Willmott, Rothwell, Cameron, Ayton, Nelms and Ng1999). The fact that PCS can be experienced by children with no evidence of neurological changes (i.e., post-traumatic amnesia, loss of consciousness (LOC), abnormal neuroimaging findings), has sparked ongoing debate around the etiology of PCS and the relative influence of injury and non-injury related factors.

Whereas measures of injury severity such as the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) and duration of post-traumatic amnesia (PTA) have been shown to be significant predictors of outcome following moderate and severe TBI, research examining their predictive utility in the mTBI population has yielded mixed findings (Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, Borg, von Holst, Holm and Pepin2004). This may be due to the heterogeneous nature of mTBI samples, poor sensitivity of the GCS in this population [i.e., 90% have a GCS of 15/15; Melo et al. (Reference Melo, Lemos-Junior, Reis, Araujo, Menezes, Santos and Oliveira-Filho2010)], or unreliable assessment of PTA (Shores et al., Reference Shores, Lammel, Hullick, Sheedy, Flynn, Levick and Batchelor2008). However, advanced neuroimaging techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), have demonstrated an association between pathophysiological changes following mTBI and persistent PCS (Bigler, Reference Bigler2008; Wilde et al., Reference Wilde, McCauley, Hunter, Bigler, Chu, Wang and Levin2008).

The inclusion of trauma control groups has also been used to study the influence of other trauma-related non-brain injury factors, and is now recommended for mTBI research (Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus, & Coronado, Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus and Coronado2004). Research in adolescents and adults has shown that mTBI consistently predicts acute PCS but shows a decreasing association with outcome over time (McNally et al., Reference McNally, Bangert, Dietrich, Nuss, Rusin, Wright and Yeates2013; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Cameron, Fitzgerald, Grant, Mikocka-Walus and Schonberger2012). This suggests that persistent PCS are at least partially accounted for by non-injury factors such as pre-morbid demographic, child or family functioning, or post-injury reactions to the event such as stress and anxiety (Anderson, Godfrey, Rosenfeld, & Catroppa, Reference Anderson, Godfrey, Rosenfeld and Catroppa2012; Babikian, McArthur, & Asarnow, Reference Babikian, McArthur and Asarnow2013; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Cameron, Fitzgerald, Grant, Mikocka-Walus and Schonberger2012).

With regard to demographic factors, age, gender, and history of mTBI have been associated with outcome following mTBI in previous studies. The relationship between age at injury and outcome following mTBI appears complex and the mechanisms responsible are likely multifactorial (Kolb, Reference Kolb1999; Kolb & Teskey, Reference Kolb and Teskey2012; Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997). Earlier age-at-injury has often been associated with poorer academic and intellectual outcomes following both moderate–severe (Lloyd et al., Reference Lloyd, Wilson, Tenovuo and Saarijarvi2015; Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997) and mild TBI (Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2001), particularly when injuries occur in the first 5–7 years of life.

There is also a suggestion that during “critical” periods of cognitive development children are more vulnerable to insult, with one study finding that early primary-aged children (i.e., 7–8 years) were more vulnerable than pre-school and older children (Crowe, Catroppa, Babl, Rosenfeld, & Anderson, Reference Crowe, Catroppa, Babl, Rosenfeld and Anderson2012). A recent systematic review of PCS outcomes following mTBI, however, suggested that children older than 13 were at greater risk of persistent PCS (Zemek, Barrowman, et al., Reference Zemek, Barrowman, Freedman, Gravel, Gagnon, McGahern and Osmond2016a). Similarly, school-aged children describe a greater PCS burden than pre-school children, both immediately (McKinlay, Ligteringen, & Than, Reference McKinlay, Ligteringen and Than2014), and several months (Barlow et al., Reference Barlow, Crawford, Stevenson, Sandhu, Belanger and Dewey2010), after mTBI. The challenges of accurate PCS assessment in pre-school children must be considered when interpreting these results, however (Gioia, Schneider, Vaughan, & Isquith, Reference Gioia, Schneider, Vaughan and Isquith2009).

Research examining the effects of gender on outcomes following pediatric mTBI has yielded mixed results, but recent research has highlighted that females report more PCS following mTBI (Babcock et al., Reference Babcock, Byczkowski, Wade, Ho, Mookerjee and Bazarian2013; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Dietrich, Nuss, Wright, Rusin, Bangert and Yeates2010; Zemek, Barrowman, et al., Reference Zemek, Barrowman, Freedman, Gravel, Gagnon, McGahern and Osmond2016). Gender differences in behavioral and psychiatric outcomes have also been noted in more general TBI samples (Scott et al., Reference Scott, McKinlay, McLellan, Britt, Grace and MacFarlane2015; Whelan-Goodinson, Ponsford, Schonberger, & Johnston, Reference Whelan-Goodinson, Ponsford, Schonberger and Johnston2010). The influence of previous mTBI is unclear (Kirkwood et al., Reference Kirkwood, Yeates, Taylor, Randolph, McCrea and Anderson2008), although a recent multi-center study suggested previous mTBI increases risk of persistent PCS (Zemek, Barrowman, et al., Reference Zemek, Barrowman, Freedman, Gravel, Gagnon, McGahern and Osmond2016). Given mixed findings, further work is needed to delineate the influence of these factors on PCS outcomes (Carroll, Cassidy Holm, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus and Coronado2004).

Pre-injury child factors including pre-morbid learning difficulties, behavioral adjustment (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Rusin, Bangert, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2012) and pre-existing learning difficulties (Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Willmott, Rothwell, Cameron, Ayton, Nelms and Ng1999) have also been shown to influence PCS. There is also some preliminary evidence in adults suggesting poor pre-morbid sleep quality may be a significant predictor of poorer PCS (Theadom et al., Reference Theadom, Cropley, Parmar, Barker-Collo, Starkey, Jones and Feigin2015), however, this has not been explored in children.

Finally, consideration of the influence of family factors on recovery from TBI is imperative in pediatric research. The period following a child’s head injury can be extremely stressful for the child and their family, even for milder injuries (Wade et al., Reference Wade, Taylor, Yeates, Drotar, Stancin, Minich and Schluchter2006). Lack of adequate social and medical support (Prigatano & Gray, Reference Prigatano and Gray2007), observable changes in cognitive and behavioral functioning, often resulting in a failure of children to immediately return to social and school activities (Ganesalingam et al., Reference Ganesalingam, Yeates, Ginn, Taylor, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2008; Rivara et al., Reference Rivara, Jaffe, Polissar, Fay, Liao and Martin1996; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Burant2001; Wade et al., Reference Wade, Taylor, Yeates, Drotar, Stancin, Minich and Schluchter2006; Yeates, Taylor, Walz, Stancin, & Wade, Reference Yeates, Taylor, Walz, Stancin and Wade2010), and financial burden (Keenan, Runyan, & Nocera, Reference Keenan, Runyan and Nocera2006; Osberg, Baker, & Long, Reference Osberg, Baker and Long1996) have been identified as contributing factors to parental distress following TBI in childhood.

This relationship may also be bi-directional, in that a stressful home environment may also impede a child’s recovery from injury (Ganesalingam et al., Reference Ganesalingam, Yeates, Ginn, Taylor, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2008). As such, assessment of family functioning and stress must be considered when examining the impact of childhood head injury (Anderson & Yeates, Reference Anderson and Yeates1997; Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997). Limited financial, social, and/or psychological family resources have shown a negative impact on recovery from TBI. They are typically measured using socio-economic status (SES) indicators or a composite measure with education and occupation. The influence of family resources may vary depending on the severity of injury, given that children who sustain more severe injuries are likely to require more intervention and rehabilitation (Stavinoha, Butcher, & Spurgin, Reference Stavinoha, Butcher and Spurgin2012).

The current study aimed to investigate the relative influence of demographic, child, parent, and injury factors on PCS at early (acute – 1 week) and late (1–3 months) post injury phases, in young children experiencing trauma. In particular, we were interested whether children who sustained mTBI were at greater risk of early and/or late PCS than children experiencing minor bodily trauma without TBI. To assist with determining “at-risk” children during the acute stage, we also wanted to determine which pre-injury demographic (age, gender), child (pre-morbid behavior and sleep) and family factors (parental stress, SES) were associated with more persistent (i.e., late) PCS. Based on previous studies, it was hypothesized that injury factors would be strongly associated with early PCS but that the strength of that association would decline over time, and that the association of pre-morbid non-injury child and family factors with PCS would be evident early post-injury, but would increase over time post-injury.

METHOD

Participants

Participants were parents of children aged 2–12 who presented to a hospital ED from November 2012 to January 2015, with either a mild TBI (mTBI group) or minor injury not involving the head [trauma controls (TC)]. Parents were approached by nursing staff in the ED either in person while their child was under observation, or via telephone <48 hr after hospital presentation. Following verbal consent, parents were contacted by the primary researcher to complete eligibility screen and conduct initial interview. All data obtained were in compliance with regulations outlined in the Helsinki Declaration, and the research was approved by both the Western Health (Ref. HREC11WH90) and Monash University (Ref. 22012001523) Ethics Committees.

Inclusion criteria for the mTBI group included: (1) recent (<48 hr) history of trauma to the head, resulting in LOC <30 min and a GCS score of 13–15 on presentation to ED, and/or >2 transient neurological symptoms (nausea, vomiting, dizziness, headache, confusion/disorientation, or sensitivity to noise/light; (2) age between 2 years 0 months and 12 years 11 months; (3) English-speaking or presence of a family member suitable to interpret. Participants were excluded if they (1) were intubated or required general anesthesia following injury; (2) had focal neurological signs, seizures, or intracerebral abnormality on computed tomography (CT); (3) had a history of neurological or psychiatric illness; (4) injury resulted from child abuse; or (5) they had a history of moderate-severe TBI.

Inclusion in the control (TC) group required (1) recent (<48 hr) history of minor injury (e.g., laceration, soft tissue injury fracture, or dislocation) to parts of the body other than skull and/or spinal cord, including lacerations to the face and scalp, with no disturbance of consciousness. Other inclusion criteria were the same as for the mTBI group. The inclusion of an injury-control comparison group is now recognized as an important methodological control for the effects of general trauma and pre-morbid behavioral differences (Satz et al., Reference Satz, Zaucha, McCleary, Light, Asarnow and Becker1997). Both mTBI and control groups were further divided by age into preschool and school-aged children, based on whether the child had commenced school at time of injury.

Materials and Procedure

Participants were recruited by nursing staff by one of two methods: (1) face-to-face during post-injury observation period, or (2) through retrospective searching of recent medical admissions (<48 hr). There was no known systematic bias as to who was approached by the hospital staff. Following verbal consent, the primary researcher then contacted participants to complete final screen and obtain written consent. With the exception of two cases that required an interpreter, all interviews were conducted via telephone.

Post-concussive symptoms. Given the lack of PCS scales validated for use in this age group, a decision was made to generate a comprehensive list of items (symptoms) from the most commonly used and available PCS scales where parental report available [Rivermead Post-concussive Questionnaire (Gagnon, Swaine, Friedman, & Forget, Reference Gagnon, Swaine, Friedman and Forget2005), Post-injury Symptom checklist (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Barry, Drotar, Wade and Stancin2001), the Acute Concussion Evaluation (Gioia, Collins, & Isquith, Reference Gioia, Collins and Isquith2008), from review by Gioia et al. (Reference Gioia, Schneider, Vaughan and Isquith2009)], with the aim to capture the widest range of symptoms possible. That is, the physical/somatic, cognitive, behavioral, affective, and sleep-related symptoms from the different scales were cross-checked, with equivalent items matched up (e.g., “was sensitive to noise” and “noise sensitivity”) and additional or unique items all included. This resulted in a 28-item PCS scale, which measured both presence and severity [5-point severity scale ranging from “0” (not experienced at all) to “4” (a severe problem)] of symptoms in the acute phase (within 72 hr of injury), 1 week, 1 month, 2 months, and 3 months post-injury. Preliminary reliability analysis of the scale revealed good internal consistency across follow up time points [Cronbach’s α mean=0.86 (SD=0.02; Range=0.83–0.89), with no single items reducing the α value below 0.84. if deleted], providing justification for inclusion of all 28 items (Cronbach, Reference Cronbach1951).

Items included on the scale measuring retrospective parent ratings of premorbid PCS were also collected during the first interview, and thus PCS scores reflected a change from this pre-morbid level. Given the inherent difficulties in eliciting symptom presence in younger children (Anderson & Yeates, Reference Anderson and Yeates1997), a semi-structured interview was used, whereby concrete behavioral exemplars were supplied for each symptom to assist parents with identification of symptoms in their children and avoid misinterpretation of terminology (e.g., ‘More emotional’: “has your child become more ‘teary’ or ‘sooky’, perhaps crying over things they wouldn’t have before, or to overreacting to situations or throwing tantrums unnecessarily?”; or for ‘Anxiety’: “has your child become more ‘clingy’ or more nervous about leaving you when you drop them off at school/kindergarten/daycare?”.

Demographic factors

These included age, gender, reported history of mild TBI, pre-existing learning difficulties (score on Learning Disability sub-scale on the Clinical Assessment of Behavior) and SES. SES score was calculated by residential postcode using the Socio-economic Indexes data [values range one (most disadvantaged) to 10 (most advantaged) area (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2013)].

Injury related factors

Mechanism of injury and whether a child sustained an mTBI or TC were collected. Measures of injury severity included the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) and parental report of LOC (when witnessed). PTA was not routinely recorded in the ED.

Child factors

Premorbid ratings of children’s behavior were collected during initial interview using the Clinical Assessment of Behavior-Parent version (Bracken & Keith, Reference Bracken and Keith2004) and re-administered at 1 and 3 months post-injury to document behavioral changes across that period. The CAB produces an overall Behavioral Index (CBI) and two clinical scales (Internalizing Behavior and Externalizing behavior).

A baseline of children’s sleep habits was obtained during initial interview using the Owen’s Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ; Owens, Spirito, & McGuinn, Reference Owens, Spirito and McGuinn2000) (CSHQ) and re-administered at 1 and 3 months post-injury. The CSHQ is a 36-item parental-report screening measure designed to assess behaviorally and medically based sleep problems in school children. It has been validated in toddlers and pre-school children (Goodlin-Jones, Sitnick, Tang, Liu, & Anders, Reference Goodlin-Jones, Sitnick, Tang, Liu and Anders2008).

Family factors

The Parenting Stress Index/Short Form (PSI/SF) is a 36-item self-report questionnaire which is a derivative of the Parenting Stress Index full-length test. It aims to identify parent-child problem areas in parents of children ages 1 month to 12 years, and was used as a pre-injury measure in this study (Abidin, Reference Abidin1983). Statistical factor analysis of the PSI/SF yields three constructs or scales, namely Parental Distress, Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction, and Difficult Child. It also generates a Total Stress Score, which reflects overall stresses reported in the areas of personal parental distress, stresses derived from the parent’s interaction with the child, and stresses that result from the child’s behavioral characteristics.

Data Analysis

Univariate outliers with Z score >3.29 (p<.001, two-tailed test) were identified [five cases on Sleep Index, four cases on Behavior (CBI) index, four cases on PCS Scale] and assigned a score one unit larger than the next most extreme score (Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013). There were no negative PCS change scores (premorbid to follow-up). Group differences (mTBI, TC) at baseline were evaluated using t tests (normally distributed data) and chi-square (categorical data) analysis. Unless otherwise specified, statistical significance was set at 0.05. Analysis was conducted using Stata Statistical Software Version 14 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX).

The main outcome variable (total PCS) was re-coded as an ordinal categorical variable (whereby 0=no PCS, 1=1–2 PCS, 2=3–5 PCS, and 3=>6 PCS) and analyzed using random effects ordinal logistic regression (Hosmer, Lemeshow, & Sturdivant, Reference Hosmer, Lemeshow and Sturdivant2013). Important when dealing with ordinal outomes, the proportional odds assumption of ordinal logistic regression was upheld. Proportional odds assumes that the relationship between each pair of (in this case, PCS) outcome groups is the same (i.e., that the coefficients that describe the relationship between the lowest versus all higher categories of the response variable, are the same as those that describe the relationship between the next lowest category and all higher categories). This assumption was tested by running a series of binary logistic regressions on the different levels of PCS.

To examine PCS over time, time was dichotomized into early (acute to 1 week) and late (1 to 3 months) post-injury phases, to simplify interpretation, and because such separation of acute versus persistent PCS was believed to be clinically meaningful.

The advantages of using random effects regression modelling are that it: (1) estimates both individual and injury group trend lines over multiple time points; (2) is less restrictive regarding missing data and uses data from all individuals, and, therefore, does not rely on endpoint analysis, and (3) allows time-invariant variables (e.g., gender) to be absorbed by the intercept (Gibbons et al., Reference Gibbons, Hedeker, Elkin, Waternaux, Kraemer, Greenhouse and Watkins1993).

To identify key variables and reduce the number of variables relative to sample size, initial separate ordinal logistic regression analyses were run to examine the predictive utility of each of the individual 15 predictor variables on PCS3. An interaction term between each variable and time was also included, as this allowed assessment of whether a variables’ influence on PCS varied across time (early vs. late). As recommended by Hosmer and colleagues, a p value of .2 was applied to determine which variables, and/or their interactions with time, would be included in the final model (2013). The use of the more liberal p value was to capture variables which showed weak associations by themselves, but exhibited stronger associations with the outcome variable when combined with other variables.

Based on these criteria, the following variables were included in the final model; age, SES, history of mTBI, learning disorder, reporter education, reporter ethnicity, pre-morbid stress, injury group, and LOC. The following variables were not included; gender, reporter gender, premorbid PCS, pre-morbid child sleep, and pre-morbid behavior.

Two separate random effects ordinal logistic regression models were run to examine the influence of the predictor variables selected in the initial analyses described above, on early and late PCS, respectively. Then, to determine whether each variable’s relationship with PCS was statistically different at early versus late post-injury stages, the same ordered logistic regression models were examined longitudinally using a time variable. Interaction terms that were statistically significant in the preliminary analyses (Table 3) were included; that is, if a variable had a statistically significant interaction (p<.05) with time (early, late) on PCS, then this suggested that variable’s relationship with PCS differed between the early and late post-injury phases. As displayed in Table 4, these variables included age and a history of learning disorder. Given the inter-correlation between age and injury group, injury group was no longer statistically significant when all variables were entered together. It should be noted, however, that if age was removed from the model there was a statistically significant interaction between injury group and time on PCS.

RESULTS

Recruitment ran consecutively for 26 months during which time 322 potential mTBI and 3650 TC were admitted to the ED. Of these, 101 participants were successfully recruited into the study (47 mTBI, 55 TC), all of which remained in the study at 3 months post-injury. Table 1 outlines participant demographics, children’s injury details and child/parent pre-injury factors.

Table 1. Demographics, injury factors, and pre-morbid clinical characteristics

Note. mTBI, mild traumatic brain injury group; TC, trauma control group; ES = effect size (phi (2x2) and Cramer’s V (2x3) reported for categorical variables and Cohen’s d used for continuous variables); SES, socio-economic status; HS, high-school graduate; LOC, loss of consciousness; CAB, Clinical Assessment of Behaviour; CBI; Clinical Behavioural Index; OCSHQ, Owen’s Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire; PSI-SF, Parental Stress Index-Short Form;

a Value represents a decile value determined by postcode (ranging 0 – 10), lower decile value = lower SES. Mann-Whitney test applied to compare this ordinal variable across groups;

b Determined by children who scored in the clinically significant range on the Learning Disorder sub-scale of the CAB;

c All reported LOC was suspected to be less than 10 s.

Injury groups (mTBI, TC) did not differ significantly in gender, socio-economic status, level of parental education or history of previous head injury. Injury groups did differ significantly on age and this was considered a limitation, as discussed below. The majority of injuries were due to falls, as expected in younger mTBI populations. Based on GCS scores, the mTBI group’s injuries were very mild, with 95% scoring the maximum 15 of 15. Baseline assessment of behavior and sleep habits revealed no statistically significant differences in scores between mTBI and TC children (Table 1). Further examination using classification cutoffs also revealed that the percentage of children classified as showing clinically “at risk” behavioral problems also did not differ on the overall behavioral index (13% of mTBI and 11% of TC, χ2 (1)=0.11; p=.74), or on externalizing (24% mTBI and 12% TC, χ2 (1)=2.14; p=.14) and internalizing (9% mTBI and 11% of TC, χ2 (1)=0.14; p=.71) behavior scales. Ratings of parental stress were comparable between injury groups at baseline.

Parental report of PCS by mTBI and TC groups across the post-injury period are displayed in in Table 2. In brief, PCS peaked in the acute post-injury phase for both groups but mTBI children experienced significantly more PCS (i.e., more than half the group reporting at least six PCS). Over the first week post-injury, there was a significant drop in PCS and a steady tapering of symptoms up to 1 month post-injury. PCS persisted in a notable minority of children from 1 to 3 months post-injury, with 18% of mTBI and 5% of TC children remaining symptomatic 3 months post-injury.

Table 2. Percentage of children with PCS within each group (mTBI, TC), across the post-injury period

Note. PCS = post-concussive symptoms mTBI = mild traumatic brain injury group; TC = trauma control group.

Results from the preliminary ordinal logistic regression analyses examining the association of individual variables with PCS are outlined in Table 3. LOC, history of mTBI, parent cultural background, and pre-morbid parental stress levels, were all significantly associated with PCS overall. Pre-existing learning difficulties and injury type (mTBI, TC) had varying associations with PCS across time as evidenced by significant interactions of these variables with time.

Table 3. Preliminary regression analyses examining predictors individually on PCS, and as an interaction with time

Note. PCS = post-concussive symptoms; SES, socio-economic status; mTBI, mild traumatic brain injury group; TC, trauma control group; HS, high-school graduate; LOC, loss of consciousness; Gender (male coded 0; female coded 1).

*p-Value for interaction term was <.2 so was included in final model (age * time, β=0.01 (SE (β) =.01, 95% CI [−0.02, 0.00], p=.09).

**p-Value for interaction term (predictor variable * time) statistically significant at p<.05. This included the following interactions; Learning Disorder * Time, OR =1.05, 95% CI [1.00, 1.10], p=.039; Injury Group (mTBI/TC) * Time, OR=0.44, 95% CI [0.20, 0.97], p=.04).

a Parental report of history of mTBI which included suspected mTBI.

bReporter Ethnicity coded 0 for Anglo-Saxon and coded 1 for ‘other’ (Asian, African, other).

Results from the two separate random effects ordinal regression analysis are outlined in Table 4, showing the association of predictor variables with early PCS (left column) and late PCS (right column). The strongest predictor of early PCS was having sustained an mTBI. These children were 16 times more likely to experience a greater number of PCS than those who sustained a mild bodily injury. Having sustained an mTBI was a statistically significant predictor of both early and late PCS (main effects in Table 4).

Table 4. Results from random effects ordinal regression analyses, predicting PCS at both early (acute – 1 week) and late (1 – 3 months) post-injury stages

Note. OR = odds ratios; # indicates significant interaction with time (early, late) in model, indicating the difference in coefficients between early and late is statistically significant at p<.05, e.g., the coefficient for learning disorder (1.02) at the early stage is statistically significant from the coefficient for learning disorder (1.08) at the late stage.

*p<.05.

**p<.01.

A range of non-injury factors were also significantly associated with PCS. Higher SES and greater levels of pre-morbid stress were statistically significant predictors of early PCS, whilst being of Anglo-Saxon decent was a statistically significant predictor of late PCS. Significant interactions were also found between time and age at injury, β=−0.02 (SE(β)=.01, 95% confidence interval (CI) [−0.03, −0.01], p<.01, and time and pre-existing learning difficulties, β=−0.07 (SE(β)=.03, 95% CI [0.02, 0.12], p<.01, indicating that children with pre-existing learning difficulties and those older in age were more likely to have reported symptoms in the late post-injury phase (i.e., beyond 1 month).

DISCUSSION

This study aimed to identify injury and non-injury predictors of PCS at both early (acute to 1 week) and late (1 to 3 months) post-injury stages following trauma in young children. Consistent with research in older children and adults, children who had sustained mTBI were much more likely to experience PCS during both the acute and non-acute post-injury phase, than demographically similar children with mild trauma to their body (Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Cameron, Fitzgerald, Grant, Mikocka-Walus and Schonberger2012; Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Rusin, Bangert, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2012). This result lends support to the presence of neurophysiological changes occurring in the days to weeks following mTBI, and for some children over extended periods of time (Giza & Hovda, Reference Giza and Hovda2014). Given PTA was not routinely assessed, and 95% of our sample had GCS scores of 15/15, the influence of injury severity within the mTBI group was difficult to determine. An alternative indicator of severity, LOC, also did not predict persistent PCS, however. The failure of traditional TBI severity measures to accurately predict outcome following mTBI may reflect the relatively variable or retrospective application of such measures in mTBI cases, or simply that they lack sensitivity in this group. However, given that presence of an mTBI was such a significant predictor of PCS in this study, attempts to identify reliable injury severity markers and have these consistently collected in adults and children with mTBI should continue.

As hypothesized, a combination of injury, child, and family factors were the strongest predictors of PCS beyond 1 month post-injury. Children who had sustained mTBI, who were older in age, and who had pre-existing learning difficulties were at increased risk of persistent PCS. The predictive utility of these variables increased over the post-injury period. This finding supported our hypothesis that non-injury factors would make an increasing contribution to outcome over time.

This effect of age is in disagreement with findings in moderate to severe TBI samples which support an “early vulnerability hypothesis” that the immature brain is more vulnerable to the deleterious effects of TBI (Lloyd et al., Reference Lloyd, Wilson, Tenovuo and Saarijarvi2015; Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997), but is consistent with other mTBI studies highlighting greater PCS in older children (Barlow et al., Reference Barlow, Crawford, Stevenson, Sandhu, Belanger and Dewey2010; Zemek, Barrowman, et al., Reference Zemek, Barrowman, Freedman, Gravel, Gagnon, McGahern and Osmond2016). This might suggest that below a certain severity threshold the negative outcomes associated with age diminishes. Alternatively, the effect of age on outcomes following mTBI may be better represented through “critical periods.” For example, our results were somewhat consistent with Crowe and colleagues (Reference Crowe, Catroppa, Babl, Rosenfeld and Anderson2012), who identified that early primary-school aged child (i.e., 7 to 8 year olds), may be at greater risk than pre-school and perhaps older children.

This forces consideration of the varying environmental demands placed on children of different ages; although school-aged children must attend school where they are expected to learn and acquire new skills, it could be argued that pre-school children return to a more supportive and familiar environment with less cognitively demanding daily routines. In support of this, there is evidence to suggest that the effects of early brain injury may worsen over time as they interact with developmental processes and impede the acquisition of new skills (Gronwall, Wrightson, & McGinn, Reference Gronwall, Wrightson and McGinn1997; McKinlay, Dalrymple-Alford, Norwood, & Fergusson, Reference McKinlay, Dalrymple-Alford, Norwood and Fergusson2002; Oddy, Reference Oddy1993; Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997). Thus the greater reporting of PCS in older children in this study may reflect the fact that greater demands are placed on these children. It may also be that younger children are less able to self-report such symptoms and their parents may be less aware of them. Taken together, these findings further highlight the complex relationship between age at injury and recovery (Kolb & Teskey, Reference Kolb and Teskey2012).

The influence of pre-existing learning difficulties or conversely, of “brain reserve” on outcomes following mTBI has been relatively well documented (Babikian et al., Reference Babikian, McArthur and Asarnow2013; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Willmott, Rothwell, Cameron, Ayton, Nelms and Ng1999; Satz et al., Reference Satz, Zaucha, McCleary, Light, Asarnow and Becker1997) and was supported in this study of younger children. This adds further weight to the theory of “brain reserve capacity” theory which purports that children with pre-existing learning difficulties have a reduced capacity to cope with cerebral insult (Satz, Reference Satz1993). Research in adults has documented a relationship between pre-existing sleep disturbance and outcomes following mTBI (Chan & Feinstein, Reference Chan and Feinstein2015; Kraus et al., Reference Kraus, Hsu, Schaffer, Vaca, Ayers, Kennedy and Afifi2009; Theadom et al., Reference Theadom, Cropley, Parmar, Barker-Collo, Starkey, Jones and Feigin2015); therefore, this was also examined in our study. Our results did not support this relationship in children; however, we used only a brief subjective measure of sleep. Thus, future research should include more detailed measures of sleep, incorporating both objective (i.e., physiological) and subjective sleep measures on which to base more sounds conclusions.

The hypothesis that injury factors (i.e., having sustained an mTBI) would have a decreasing strength of association with PCS over time was also supported. Despite this decline, however, mTBI remained the strongest predictor of persistent PCS, even after controlling for age, and pre-existing child and family characteristics. This appears to be in conflict with previous research which demonstrates demographic, child, and family factors as stronger determinants of PCS beyond 1 month post-injury (McNally et al., Reference McNally, Bangert, Dietrich, Nuss, Rusin, Wright and Yeates2013; Olsson et al., Reference Olsson, Lloyd, LeBrocque, McKinlay, Anderson and Kenardy2013), and may reflect the heterogeneous nature of mTBI injuries (i.e., varying levels of injury severity). The possibility that our results support the presence of more lasting neuro-metabolic changes (i.e., dysfunctional excitatory neurotransmission) following mTBI, must also be considered (Giza & Hovda, Reference Giza and Hovda2001, Reference Giza and Hovda2014; MacFarlane & Glenn, Reference MacFarlane and Glenn2015).

Higher levels of parental stress were consistently associated with greater PCS across both acute and late post-injury periods. This may suggest that either parental stress leads to greater endorsement of PCS by parents, or alternatively, that environmental stress somehow influences the development or expression of PCS within the child (Ganesalingam et al., Reference Ganesalingam, Yeates, Ginn, Taylor, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2008; Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Walz, Stancin and Wade2010). The impact of family burden on mTBI outcomes has received relatively less attention than in moderate and severe TBI groups, perhaps given the majority of children return to pre-morbid functioning relatively quickly following mTBI. Yet the perceived stress that parents experience following their child’s injury may not necessarily be correlated with the severity of injury and thus should be considered as equally important in this population (Wade et al., Reference Wade, Cassedy, Walz, Taylor, Stancin and Yeates2011). Even for children who are discharged quickly after injury, families remain the major long-term source of support and thus play a critical role in a child’s recovery from a TBI (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Drotar, Wade, Yeates, Stancin and Klein1995). This is especially the case for younger children who remain extremely dependent on their parents and carers in everyday life.

Lower SES and limited family resources have often been associated with poorer outcomes following pediatric TBI, particularly following more severe TBI (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Walz, Stancin and Wade2010). Contrary to expectations, results from this study revealed that higher SES was a statistically significant predictor of parental reporting of PCS. This may reflect methodological differences in SES indicators used. Studies which adopt composite measures of SES value which incorporate parental education level, occupation, and family income, have shown that SES consistently predicts outcome (particularly in moderate and severe TBI populations), whilst those examining one component of SES such as our study, show less consistent findings (Stavinoha et al., Reference Stavinoha, Butcher and Spurgin2012). An alternative explanation may be that the influence of SES on outcome is not well-defined in the mild compared with more severe TBIs. In fact, one previous mTBI study has revealed a similar relationship between higher SES and greater PCS (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Rusin, Bangert, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2012).

With regard to other socio-cultural influences, parents from Anglo-Saxon backgrounds reported more PCS in their children. Whereas the research in adults has suggested that ethnic minorities have worse functional outcomes, the evidence in children is less substantiated (Saltapidas & Ponsford, Reference Saltapidas and Ponsford2007; Stavinoha et al., Reference Stavinoha, Butcher and Spurgin2012). There is some evidence that socio-cultural factors may moderate the effects of pediatric TBI on parents and families, but more research on this is needed to better inform primary care clinicians (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Woodrome, Wade, Stancin and Drotar2002). Given pediatric studies often rely on parental report of outcomes, it is also important for researchers to consider the potential influence of cultural factors on parents’ willingness or openness to report the effect of pediatric TBI or attribution of symptoms to the injury.

An alternative explanation is that observed influence of SES and cultural factors may reflect variations in awareness of mTBI and/or symptoms attributed to mTBI. More educated parents may be more aware of public information/media around mTBI or “concussion,” and as a result may be more either more sensitive to symptoms or more likely to misattribute/over-interpret symptoms in the context of a suspected mTBI.

Limitations of this study must be acknowledged and should be considered when reviewing these results. PCS measures were administered solely to parents of children. This may have resulted in reporting bias whereby more observable or externalizing symptoms were over-endorsed and internalizing symptoms were missed (Ayr, Yeates, Taylor, & Browne, Reference Ayr, Yeates, Taylor and Browne2009; Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Schneider, Vaughan and Isquith2009; Hajek et al., Reference Hajek, Yeates, Taylor, Bangert, Dietrich, Nuss and Wright2011). Whereas inter-rater agreement between children and their parents on symptom report has been moderate, it can be quite variable (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Schneider, Vaughan and Isquith2009; Sady, Vaughan, & Gioia, Reference Sady, Vaughan and Gioia2014). Nevertheless, this highlights the significant challenges of PCS assessment in younger children and the lack of scales valid for use in this population (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Schneider, Vaughan and Isquith2009). It should also be noted that pre-morbid ratings of child (i.e., behavior, sleep) and family (i.e., stress) factors were collected retrospectively. As such, parents’ perception of their child’s pre-morbid characteristics may have been influenced by trauma (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Kadoura, Turley, Crawford, Mikrogianakis and Barlow2014).

The potential for sampling bias must also be considered. Given the majority of children’s injuries were very mild, it could be argued the final sample was composed of parents who were more sensitive or cautious, as they made the decision to admit their child to ED. Furthermore, parents with this tendency may also be more likely to agree to participate in research. Additionally, there were several pre-morbid variables which have been shown to be associated with persistent PCS that were not measured in this study. These include pre-morbid somatization (Grubenhoff et al., Reference Grubenhoff, Currie, Comstock, Juarez-Colunga, Bajaj and Kirkwood2016; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Tarima, LaRoche, Hammeke, Barr, Guskiewicz and McCrea2016), post-injury child coping (Woodrome et al., Reference Woodrome, Yeates, Taylor, Rusin, Bangert, Dietrich and Wright2011), post-injury symptom exaggeration (Araujo et al., Reference Araujo, Antonini, Monahan, Gelfius, Klamar, Potts and Bodin2014; Kirkwood, Peterson, Connery, Baker, & Grubenhoff, Reference Kirkwood, Peterson, Connery, Baker and Grubenhoff2014), and post-injury family litigation (Connery, Peterson, Baker, & Kirkwood, Reference Connery, Peterson, Baker and Kirkwood2016). Finally, given our groups differed on age it proved difficult to tease out the effects of age. Future studies need larger samples sizes to allow for matching methods such as propensity scores (Guo & Fraser, Reference Guo and Fraser2015).

In conclusion, recovery from mTBI depends on a complex interplay of injury-related factors, pre-existing child factors, and the environmental context in which the child recovers from injury. Prolonged symptoms following mTBI can be frustrating for children and challenging for parents, and given most children are discharged home with little or no follow-up, greater emphasis needs to be placed on identifying these “at risk” children upon presentation to primary care facilities. Such identification would allow for early interventions such as provision of education, and outpatient follow up where required, both of which have been shown to be effective in reducing persistent PCS (Winkler & Taylor, Reference Winkler and Taylor2015).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Coco O. Bernard and Jennie A. Ponsford, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Audrey McKinlay, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia; Dean McKenzie, Epworth HealthCare and School of Public Health and Preventive Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; David Krieser, Sunshine Hospital Emergency Department, Melbourne, Australia. The authors have indicated that they have no financial relationships relevant to this article, potential conflicts of interest, to disclose. Research funding was awarded to Professor Jennie A. Ponsford and Dr. Audrey McKinlay by the Jack Brockoff Foundation, and to Coco O. Bernard from the Monash University Doctoral Research Fund. This research was conducted as part of Coco O. Bernard’s Doctoral Thesis (Doctorate of Clinical Neuropsychology).

References

REFERENCES

Abidin, R. (1983). Parental stress index (3rd ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Anderson, V., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. (2001). Outcome from mild head injury in young children: A prospective study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(6), 705717. doi:10.1076/jcen.23.6.705.1015 Google Scholar
Anderson, V., Godfrey, C., Rosenfeld, J.V., & Catroppa, C. (2012). Predictors of cognitive function and recovery 10 years after traumatic brain injury in young children. Pediatrics, 129(2), e254e261. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-0311 Google Scholar
Anderson, V., & Yeates, K. (1997). Pediatric Head Injury: Developmental Implications Introduction from the symposium organizers. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(6), 553554.Google Scholar
Araujo, G.C., Antonini, T.N., Monahan, K., Gelfius, C., Klamar, K., Potts, M., & Bodin, D. (2014). The relationship between suboptimal effort and post-concussion symptoms in children and adolescents with mild traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 28(5), 786801. doi:10.1080/13854046.2014.896415 Google Scholar
Ayr, L., Yeates, K., Taylor, H., & Browne, M. (2009). Dimensions of postconcussive symptoms in children with mild traumatic brain injuries. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(1), 1930. doi:10.1017/S1355617708090188 Google Scholar
Babcock, L., Byczkowski, T., Wade, S.L., Ho, M., Mookerjee, S., & Bazarian, J.J. (2013). Predicting postconcussion syndrome after mild traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents who present to the emergency department. Pediatrics, 167(2), 156161. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.434 Google Scholar
Babikian, T., McArthur, D., & Asarnow, R. (2013). Predictors of 1-month and 1-year neurocognitive functioning from the UCLA longitudinal mild, uncomplicated, pediatric traumatic brain injury study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(2), 145154. doi:10.1017/S135561771200104X Google Scholar
Barlow, K., Crawford, S., Stevenson, A., Sandhu, S.S., Belanger, F., & Dewey, D. (2010). Epidemiology of postconcussion syndrome in pediatric mild traumatic brain injury. Pediatrics, 126(2), 374381. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0925 Google Scholar
Bigler, E. (2008). Neuropsychology and clinical neuroscience of persistent post-concussive syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(1), 122. doi:10.1017/s135561770808017x Google Scholar
Bracken, B., & Keith, L. (2004). Clinical assessment of behaviour. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Brooks, B.L., Kadoura, B., Turley, B., Crawford, S., Mikrogianakis, A., & Barlow, K.M. (2014). Perception of recovery after pediatric mild traumatic brain injury is influenced by the “good old days” bias: Tangible implications for clinical practice and outcomes research. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29(2), 186193. doi:10.1093/arclin/act083 Google Scholar
Carroll, L., Cassidy, J., Holm, L., Kraus, J., & Coronado, V. (2004). Methodological issues and research recommendations for mild traumatic brain injury: The WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 43, 113125. doi:10.1080/16501960410023877 Google Scholar
Carroll, L., Cassidy, J., Peloso, P., Borg, J., von Holst, H., Holm, L., & Pepin, M. (2004). Prognosis for mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 43(Suppl), 84105.Google Scholar
Chan, L.G., & Feinstein, A. (2015). Persistent sleep disturbances independently predict poorer functional and social outcomes 1 year after mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation November/December, 30(6), E67E75.Google Scholar
Connery, A.K., Peterson, R.L., Baker, D.A., & Kirkwood, M.W. (2016). The impact of pediatric neuropsychological consultation in mild traumatic brain injury: A model for providing feedback after invalid performance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 30, 579598. doi:10.1080/13854046.2016.1177596 Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16(3), 297334.Google Scholar
Crowe, L., Catroppa, C., Babl, F.E., Rosenfeld, J., & Anderson, V. (2012). Timing of traumatic brain injury in childhood and intellectual outcomes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 37(7), 745754. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jss070 Google Scholar
Gagnon, I., Swaine, B.R., Friedman, D., & Forget, R. (2005). Exploring children’s self-efficacy related to physical activity performance after a mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 20, 436449.Google Scholar
Ganesalingam, K., Yeates, K.O., Ginn, M.S., Taylor, H., Dietrich, A., Nuss, K., & Wright, M. (2008). Family burden and parental distress following mild traumatic brain injury in children and its relationship to post-concussive symptoms. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33(6), 621629. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm133 Google Scholar
Gibbons, R.D., Hedeker, D., Elkin, I., Waternaux, C., Kraemer, H.C., Greenhouse, J.B., & Watkins, J.T. (1993). Some conceptual and statistical issues in analysis of longitudinal psychiatric data: Application to the nimh treatment of depression collaborative research program dataset. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(9), 739750. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820210073009 Google Scholar
Gioia, G., Collins, M., & Isquith, P.K. (2008). Improving identification and diagnosis of mild traumatic brain injury with evidence: Psychometric support for the acute concussion evaluation. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 23(4), 230242. doi:10.1097/01.HTR.0000327255.38881.ca Google Scholar
Gioia, G., Schneider, J., Vaughan, C., & Isquith, P. (2009). Which symptom assessments and approaches are uniquely appropriate for paediatric concussion? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 43(Suppl. 1), i13i22. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2009.058255.Google Scholar
Giza, C.C., & Hovda, D.A. (2001). The neurometabolic cascade of concussion. Journal of Athletic Training (National Athletic Trainers’ Association), 36(3), 228235.Google Scholar
Giza, C.C., & Hovda, D.A. (2014). The new neurometabolic cascade of concussion. Neurosurgery, 75(Suppl. 4), S24S33. doi:10.1227/NEU.0000000000000505 Google Scholar
Goodlin-Jones, B., Sitnick, S., Tang, K., Liu, J., & Anders, T. (2008). The children’s sleep habits questionnaire in toddlers and preschool children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 29(2), 8288. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e318163c39a Google Scholar
Gronwall, D., Wrightson, P., & McGinn, V. (1997). Effect of mild head injury during the preschool years. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(6), 592597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grubenhoff, J.A., Currie, D., Comstock, R.D., Juarez-Colunga, E., Bajaj, L., & Kirkwood, M.W. (2016). Psychological factors associated with delayed symptom resolution in children with concussion. The Journal of Pediatrics, 174, 2732. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.03.027 Google Scholar
Guo, S., & Fraser, M.W. (2015). Propensity score analysis: Statistical methods and applications (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hajek, C.A., Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H., Bangert, B., Dietrich, A., Nuss, K.E., & Wright, M. (2011). Agreement between parents and children on ratings of post-concussive symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychology, 17(1), 1733. doi:10.1080/09297049.2010.495058 Google Scholar
Hosmer, D., Lemeshow, S., & Sturdivant, R. (2013). Applied logistic regression (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Keenan, H., Runyan, D., & Nocera, M. (2006). Longitudinal follow-up of families and young children with traumatic brain injury. Pediatrics, 117(4), 12911297. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-1883 Google Scholar
Kirkwood, M., Peterson, R.L., Connery, A.K., Baker, D.A., & Grubenhoff, J. (2014). Postconcussive symptom exaggeration after pediatric mild traumatic brain injury. Pediatrics, 133(4), 643650. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-3195 Google Scholar
Kirkwood, M., Yeates, K., Taylor, H., Randolph, C., McCrea, M., & Anderson, V. (2008). Management of pediatric mild traumatic brain injury: A neuropsychological review from injury through recovery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(5), 769800. doi:10.1080/13854040701543700 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koepsell, T.D., Rivara, F.P., Vavilala, M.S., Wang, J., Temkin, N., Jaffe, K.M., & Durbin, D.R. (2011). Incidence and descriptive epidemiologic features of traumatic brain injury in King County, Washington. Pediatrics, 128(5), 946954. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2259 Google Scholar
Kolb, B. (1999). Synaptic plasticity and the organization of behaviour after early and late brain injury. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 6276. doi:10.1037/h0087300 Google Scholar
Kolb, B., & Teskey, G. (2012). Age, experience, injury, and the changing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 54(3), 311325. doi:10.1002/dev.20515 Google Scholar
Kraus, J., Hsu, P., Schaffer, K., Vaca, F., Ayers, K., Kennedy, F., & Afifi, A.A. (2009). Preinjury factors and 3-month outcomes following emergency department diagnosis of mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 24(5), 344354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, J., Wilson, M.L., Tenovuo, O., & Saarijarvi, S. (2015). Outcomes from mild and moderate traumatic brain injuries among children and adolescents: A systematic review of studies from 2008-2013. Brain Injury, 29(5), 539549. doi:10.3109/02699052.2014.1002003 Google Scholar
MacFarlane, M.P., & Glenn, T.C. (2015). Neurochemical cascade of concussion. Brain Injury, 29(2), 139153. doi:10.3109/02699052.2014.965208 Google Scholar
McKinlay, A., Dalrymple-Alford, J., Norwood, L., & Fergusson, D. (2002). Long term psychosocial outcomes after mild head injury in early childhood. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 73(3), 281288. doi:10.1136/jnnp.73.3.281 Google Scholar
McKinlay, A., Ligteringen, V., & Than, M. (2014). A comparison of concussive symptoms reported by parents for preschool versus school-aged children. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 29(3), 233238. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3182a2dd7f Google Scholar
McNally, K.A., Bangert, B., Dietrich, A., Nuss, K., Rusin, J., Wright, M., & Yeates, K.O. (2013). Injury versus noninjury factors as predictors of postconcussive symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury in children. Neuropsychology, 27(1), 112. doi:10.1037/a0031370 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melo, J.R.T., Lemos-Junior, L.P., Reis, R.C., Araujo, A.O., Menezes, C.W., Santos, G.P., & Oliveira-Filho, J. (2010). Do children with Glasgow 13/14 could be identified as mild traumatic brain injury? Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 68(3), 381384. doi:10.1590/S0004-282X2010000300010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittenberg, W., Wittner, M.S., & Miller, L.J. (1997). Postconcussion syndrome occurs in children. Neuropsychology, 11(3), 447452.Google Scholar
Nelson, L.D., Tarima, S., LaRoche, A.A., Hammeke, T.A., Barr, W.B., Guskiewicz, K., & McCrea, M.A. (2016). Preinjury somatization symptoms contribute to clinical recovery after sport-related concussion. Neurology, 86(20), 18561863. doi:10.1212/wnl.0000000000002679 Google Scholar
Oddy, M. (1993). Head injury during childhood. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 3(4), 301320. doi:10.1080/09602019308401444 Google Scholar
Olsson, K.A., Lloyd, O.T., LeBrocque, R.M., McKinlay, L., Anderson, V.A., & Kenardy, J.A. (2013). Predictors of child post-concussion symptoms at 6 and 18 months following mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 27(2), 145157. doi:10.3109/02699052.2012.729286 Google Scholar
Osberg, J.S., Baker, S.P., & Long, W. (1996). Pediatric trauma: Impact on work and family finances. Pediatrics, 98(5), 890897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, J.A., Spirito, A., & McGuinn, M. (2000). The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): Psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep: Journal of Sleep and Sleep Disorders Research, 23(8), 19.Google Scholar
Ponsford, J., Cameron, P., Fitzgerald, M., Grant, M., Mikocka-Walus, A., & Schonberger, M. (2012). Predictors of postconcussive symptoms 3 months after mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 26(3), 304313. doi:10.1037/a0027888 Google Scholar
Ponsford, J., Willmott, C., Rothwell, A., Cameron, P., Ayton, G., Nelms, R., & Ng, K. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral outcome following mild traumatic head injury in children. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 14, 360372.Google Scholar
Prigatano, G., & Gray, J. (2007). Parental concerns and distress after paediatric traumatic brain injury: A qualitative study. Brain Injury, 21(7), 721729. doi:10.1080/02699050701481605 Google Scholar
Rivara, J., Jaffe, K., Polissar, N., Fay, G., Liao, S., & Martin, K. (1996). Predictors of family functioning and change 3 years after traumatic brain injury in children. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 77(8), 754764. doi:10.1016/s0003-9993(96)90253-1 Google Scholar
Sady, M.D., Vaughan, C.G., & Gioia, G.A. (2014). Psychometric characteristics of the Postconcussion Symptom Inventory in children and adolescents. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29(4), 348363. doi:10.1093/arclin/acu014 Google Scholar
Saltapidas, H., & Ponsford, J. (2007). The influence of cultural background on motivation for and participation in rehabilitation and outcome following traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 22(2), 132139. doi:10.1097/01.HTR.0000265101.75177.8d Google Scholar
Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology, 7(3), 273295. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.7.3.273 Google Scholar
Satz, P., Zaucha, K., McCleary, C., Light, R., Asarnow, R., & Becker, D. (1997). Mild head injury in children and adolescents: A review of studies (1970-1995). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 107131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, C., McKinlay, A., McLellan, T., Britt, E., Grace, R., & MacFarlane, M. (2015). A comparison of adult outcomes for males compared to females following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 29(4), 501508. doi:10.1037/neu0000074 Google Scholar
Shores, E., Lammel, A., Hullick, C., Sheedy, J., Flynn, M., Levick, W., & Batchelor, J. (2008). The diagnostic accuracy of the Revised Westmead PTA Scale as an adjunct to the Glasgow Coma Scale in the early identification of cognitive impairment in patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 79(10), 11001106. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2007.132571 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stavinoha, P., Butcher, B., & Spurgin, A. (2012). Premorbid functional considerations in pediatric concussion. In J.N. Apps & K.D. Walter (Eds.), Paediatrics and adolescent concussion: Diagnosis, management and outcomes (pp. 135149). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Taylor, H., & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences in outcomes following childhood brain insults: An introduction and overview. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(6), 555567. doi:10.1017/S1355617797005559 Google Scholar
Taylor, H., Dietrich, A., Nuss, K., Wright, M., Rusin, J., Bangert, B., & Yeates, K. (2010). Post-concussive symptoms in children with mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 148159. doi:10.1037/a0018112 Google Scholar
Taylor, H., Drotar, D., Wade, S., Yeates, K., Stancin, T., & Klein, S. (1995). Recovery from traumatic brain injury in children: The importance of the family. Traumatic head injury in children (pp. 188216). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, H., Yeates, K., Wade, S., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Burant, C. (2001). Bidirectional child-family influences on outcomes of traumatic brain injury in children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(6), 755767. doi:10.1017/S1355617701766118 Google Scholar
Theadom, A., Cropley, M., Parmar, P., Barker-Collo, S., Starkey, N., Jones, K., & Feigin, V.L. (2015). Sleep difficulties one year following mild traumatic brain injury in a population-based study. Sleep Medicine, 16(8), 926932. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.04.013 Google Scholar
Wade, S., Cassedy, A., Walz, N., Taylor, H., Stancin, T., & Yeates, K. (2011). The relationship of parental warm responsiveness and negativity to emerging behavior problems following traumatic brain injury in young children. Developmental Psychology, 47(1), 119133. doi:10.1037/a0021028 Google Scholar
Wade, S., Taylor, H., Yeates, K., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., Minich, N., & Schluchter, M. (2006). Long-term parental and family adaptation following pediatric brain injury. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31(10), 10721083. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsj077 Google Scholar
Whelan-Goodinson, R., Ponsford, J.L., Schonberger, M., & Johnston, L. (2010). Predictors of psychiatric disorders following traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 25(5), 320329. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181c8f8e7 Google Scholar
Wilde, E., McCauley, S., Hunter, J., Bigler, E., Chu, Z., Wang, Z., & Levin, H. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging of acute mild traumatic brain injury in adolescents. Neurology, 70(12), 948955. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000305961.68029.54 Google Scholar
Winkler, R., & Taylor, N. (2015). Do children and adolescents with mild traumatic brain injury and persistent symptoms benefit from treatment? A systematic review. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 30(5), 324333. doi:10.1097/HTR.0000000000000114 Google Scholar
Woodrome, S.E., Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H., Rusin, J., Bangert, B., Dietrich, A., & Wright, M. (2011). Coping strategies as a predictor of post-concussive symptoms in children with mild traumatic brain injury versus mild orthopedic injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 317326. doi:10.1017/S1355617710001700 Google Scholar
Yeates, K., Taylor, H., Rusin, J., Bangert, B., Dietrich, A., Nuss, K., & Wright, M. (2012). Premorbid child and family functioning as predictors of post-concussive symptoms in children with mild traumatic brain injuries. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(3), 231237. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.05.008 Google Scholar
Yeates, K., Taylor, H., Walz, N., Stancin, T., & Wade, S. (2010). The family environment as a moderator of psychosocial outcomes following traumatic brain injury in young children. Neuropsychology, 24(3), 345356. doi:10.1037/a0018387 Google Scholar
Yeates, K., Taylor, H., Woodrome, S., Wade, S.L., Stancin, T., & Drotar, D. (2002). Race as a moderator of parent and family outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27(4), 393403. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/27.4.393 Google Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Barry, C.T., Drotar, D., Wade, S.L., & Stancin, T. (2001). Neurobehavioral symptoms in childhood closed-head injuries: Changes in prevalence and correlates during the first year postinjury. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26(2), 7991. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/26.2.79 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zemek, R., Barrowman, N., Freedman, S.B., Gravel, J., Gagnon, I., McGahern, C., & Osmond, M.H. (2016). Clinical risk score for persistent postconcussion symptoms among children with acute concussion in the ED. Journal of the American Medical Association, 315(10), 10141025. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.1203 Google Scholar
Zemek, R., Farion, K.J., Sampson, M., & McGahern, C. (2016). Prognosticators of persistent symptoms following pediatric concussion. JAMA Pediatrics, 167(3), 259265. doi:0.1001/2013.jamapediatrics.216 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics, injury factors, and pre-morbid clinical characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Percentage of children with PCS within each group (mTBI, TC), across the post-injury period

Figure 2

Table 3. Preliminary regression analyses examining predictors individually on PCS, and as an interaction with time

Figure 3

Table 4. Results from random effects ordinal regression analyses, predicting PCS at both early (acute – 1 week) and late (1 – 3 months) post-injury stages