Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T09:07:02.106Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pre-dementia Memory Impairment is Associated with White Matter Tract Affection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2010

Ramune Grambaite*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway Akershus University Hospital Research Center, Lørenskog, Norway
Ivar Reinvang
Affiliation:
Center for the Study of Human Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Per Selnes
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway Faculty Division Akershus University Hospital, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Anders M. Fjell
Affiliation:
Center for the Study of Human Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Neuropsychology, Oslo University Hospital Ulleval, Oslo, Norway
Kristine B. Walhovd
Affiliation:
Center for the Study of Human Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Neuropsychology, Oslo University Hospital Ulleval, Oslo, Norway
Vidar Stenset
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway Faculty Division Akershus University Hospital, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Neurosurgery, Oslo University Hospital Ulleval, Oslo, Norway
Tormod Fladby
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway Faculty Division Akershus University Hospital, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Ramune Grambaite, Department of Neurology, Akershus University Hospital, Sykehusveien 25, NO-1478 Lørenskog, Norway. E-mail: ramuneg@psykologi.uio.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), especially amnestic, often represents pre-dementia Alzheimer’s disease, characterized by medial temporal lobe atrophy, while white matter (WM) alterations are insufficiently described. We analyze both cortical morphometric and WM diffusivity differences in amnestic versus non-amnestic subtypes and ask if memory and WM tract affection are related independently of cortical atrophy. Forty-nine patients from a university-hospital based memory clinic with a score of 3 on the Global Deterioration Scale aged 43–77 years (45% female) were included. Two neuropsychologists have classified cases as amnestic (aMCI), non-amnestic (naMCI), or less advanced (laMCI), not satisfying criteria for aMCI/naMCI. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) WM tract and morphometric data of the temporal-parietal memory network were compared among patient subtypes and related to story, word list, and visual memory. WM radial and mean diffusivity (DR and MD), underlying the entorhinal cortex, were higher in aMCI compared with laMCI. WM DR and MD, underlying the entorhinal, parahippocampal, and middle temporal cortex, explained unique variance in word list and story memory, and this was not due to secondary effects of cortical thinning. DTI may thus potentially aid diagnosis in early disease stages. (JINS, 2011, 17, 000–000)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) was originally described as a condition with amnesia, representing an intermediate stage between normal aging and Alzheimer’s dementia (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999), but has later been extended to encompass non-amnestic types (Petersen, Reference Petersen2003).

Subjects with amnestic MCI (aMCI) have subjective and objective (verified in cognitive testing or clinical assessment) memory impairment in either memory alone or both memory and other cognitive domains, but do not fulfill criteria for dementia (Petersen, Reference Petersen2004; Petersen, et al., Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999). Both MCI patients with impaired story or visual memory and patients with impaired word list memory convert to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) at a higher rate than patients with only subjective memory loss (Rountree et al., Reference Rountree, Waring, Chan, Lupo, Darby and Doody2007).

The majority of aMCI cases are pre-dementia AD cases (Nordlund et al., Reference Nordlund, Rolstad, Klang, Edman, Hansen and Wallin2010). The etiology of cognitive impairment in these subjects may differ from non-amnestic MCI (naMCI) subtypes who often have deficits of language, attention and executive function (Whitwell, Petersen, et al., Reference Whitwell, Petersen, Negash, Weigand, Kantarci, Ivnik and Jack2007). AD-like neuropathologic changes are more frequently present in aMCI cases (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Parisi, Dickson, Johnson, Knopman, Boeve and Kokmen2006; Schneider, Arvanitakis, Leurgans, & Bennett, Reference Schneider, Arvanitakis, Leurgans and Bennett2009), and 76% of MCI cases progressing to Alzheimer’s dementia have aMCI (Yaffe, Petersen, Lindquist, Kramer, & Miller, Reference Yaffe, Petersen, Lindquist, Kramer and Miller2006). It is also known that approximately 20–40% of MCIs will revert to the normal range upon repeated testing (Bickel, Mosch, Seigerschmidt, Siemen, & Forstl, Reference Bickel, Mosch, Seigerschmidt, Siemen and Forstl2006; Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Jungwirth, Zehetmayer, Weissgram, Hoenigschnabl, Gelpi and Tragl2007). Due to the heterogeneity of the MCI construct, both well-defined MCI-criteria (Jak, Bangen, et al., Reference Jak, Bangen, Wierenga, Delano-Wood, Corey-Bloom and Bondi2009; Jak, Bondi, et al., Reference Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom, Salmon and Delis2009) and knowledge about the clinical amnestic and non-amnestic subtypes are important for identification of pre-dementia cases (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Roberts, Knopman, Boeve, Geda, Ivnik and Jack2009).

Impaired memory and hippocampal atrophy in MCI are commonly recognized indicators of incipient AD (Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Feldman, Jacova, Dekosky, Barberger-Gateau, Cummings and Scheltens2007; Jack et al., Reference Jack, Petersen, Xu, O’Brien, Smith, Ivnik and Kokmen1999). Gray matter loss in aMCI may be found in the medial temporal lobes (MTLs), including the amygdala, anterior hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and the fusiform gyrus 3 years before the diagnosis of AD, and neuropathology spreads to include the middle temporal gyrus, the whole hippocampus and the parietal lobe one year before the diagnosis of AD (Whitwell, Przybelski, et al., Reference Whitwell, Przybelski, Weigand, Knopman, Boeve, Petersen and Jack2007). The risk for AD may increase due to cerebrovascular disease (Snowdon et al., Reference Snowdon, Greiner, Mortimer, Riley, Greiner and Markesbery1997). Small vessel ischemic disease is manifested by white matter (WM) lesions in conventional imaging studies, which are associated both with low cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) Abeta42, suggesting increased risk for AD (Stenset et al., Reference Stenset, Johnsen, Kocot, Negaard, Skinningsrud, Gulbrandsen and Fladby2006), and with AD-independent cognitive impairment (Stenset et al., Reference Stenset, Hofoss, Johnsen, Skinningsrud, Berstad, Negaard and Fladby2008). It has been shown that those aMCI patients, who progressed to AD within 18 month of the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), had bilateral gray matter loss in the medial/inferior temporal lobe, temporoparietal association neocortex, and frontal lobes, as well as higher frequency of lacunar infarcts than the control group (Whitwell et al., Reference Whitwell, Shiung, Przybelski, Weigand, Knopman, Boeve and Jack2008).

Cortical-subcortical and cortical-cortical disconnection may contribute to cognitive dysfunction due to WM tract disruption (Stenset et al., Reference Stenset, Grambaite, Reinvang, Hessen, Cappelen, Bjornerud and Fladby2007). It has been shown that neocortical connections to the hippocampus may deteriorate due to the degeneration of critical MTL and associated fiber pathways in AD (Salat et al., Reference Salat, Tuch, van der Kouwe, Greve, Pappu, Lee and Rosas2010). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is a method for delineating the anatomy of WM pathways by measuring the magnitude (mean diffusivity, MD) and directionality of water diffusion, which provides new insights into AD and MCI (Stebbins & Murphy, Reference Stebbins and Murphy2009). MD is a measure of non-directional diffusion providing an average of the three diffusion tensor eigenvalues (λ1, λ2, and λ3). Directional diffusion can be quantified by fractional anisotropy (FA), calculated from the diffusion tensor eigenvalues (Charlton et al., Reference Charlton, Barrick, McIntyre, Shen, O’Sullivan, Howe and Markus2006; Le Bihan et al., Reference Le Bihan, Mangin, Poupon, Clark, Pappata, Molko and Chabriat2001), which describe diffusivity parallel (λ1) or perpendicular (λ2 and λ3) to the axonal tracts (axial diffusivity, DA, or radial diffusivity, DR). FA reductions, likely reflecting differences in WM integrity, in MCI versus normal controls have been reported in several studies (Fellgiebel et al., Reference Fellgiebel, Muller, Wille, Dellani, Scheurich, Schmidt and Stoeter2005; Medina et al., Reference Medina, DeToledo-Morrell, Urresta, Gabrieli, Moseley, Fleischman and Stebbins2006; Rose et al., Reference Rose, McMahon, Janke, O’Dowd, de Zubicaray, Strudwick and Chalk2006; Stenset et al., Reference Stenset, Bjornerud, Fjell, Walhovd, Hofoss, Due-Tonnessen and Fladby2009; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Schuff, Jahng, Bayne, Mori, Schad and Weiner2007). In addition to FA, other DTI parameters, such as MD and DR, may provide more detailed information about pathological WM diffusivity changes. Parahippocampal WM degradation causes a disruption of multimodal input from entorhinal cortex to the hippocampus in aMCI (Rogalski et al., Reference Rogalski, Murphy, deToledo-Morrell, Shah, Moseley, Bammer and Stebbins2009), and alterations of diffusion properties, namely DR and DA, were found in parahippocampal WM and in regions with both direct and secondary connections to the MTL in AD (Salat et al., Reference Salat, Tuch, van der Kouwe, Greve, Pappu, Lee and Rosas2010). Decreased FA and increased MD of the MTL have been found in aMCI compared with subjects with normal cognition (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Goldstein, Veledar, Levey, Lah, Meltzer and Mao2009). Significantly lower posterior cingulate FA and higher DR values were observed in MCI patients with pathological T-tau levels at risk for AD compared to patients with normal CSF T-tau levels and controls (Stenset et al., Reference Stenset, Bjornerud, Fjell, Walhovd, Hofoss, Due-Tonnessen and Fladby2009). In another study (Huang, Friedland, & Auchus, Reference Huang, Friedland and Auchus2007), MCI subjects also had decreased FA/DA in temporal normal-appearing WM as well as decreased FA and increased DR in parietal WM. Relationships between DTI parameters and neuropathology are not well established, but comparing pre-operative imaging with electron microscopy analyses of surgically resected temporal lobes in cases with epilepsy and mesial temporal sclerosis, increased diffusivity was related to increased extra-axonal fraction, myelin rarefication, and reduced cumulative axonal membrane area (Concha, Livy, Beaulieu, Wheatley, & Gross, Reference Concha, Livy, Beaulieu, Wheatley and Gross2010). Although the literature is limited, these studies suggest that DTI parameters measure neuropathological parameters highly relevant for cognition, and that more attention should be paid to WM diffusivity changes in addition to gray matter changes to better understand pathological mechanisms in MCI.

The temporal-parietal memory network has been shown to be affected in MCI and AD (Chetelat et al., Reference Chetelat, Desgranges, de la Sayette, Viader, Berkouk, Landeau and Eustache2003; De Santi et al., Reference De Santi, de Leon, Rusinek, Convit, Tarshish, Roche and Fowler2001; Fjell et al., Reference Fjell, Amlien, Westlye, Stenset, Fladby, Skinningsrud and Walhovd2010; Jack et al., Reference Jack, Shiung, Gunter, O’Brien, Weigand, Knopman and Petersen2004, Reference Jack, Lowe, Senjem, Weigand, Kemp, Shiung and Petersen2008; Karas et al., Reference Karas, Sluimer, Goekoop, van der Flier, Rombouts, Vrenken and Barkhof2008; Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Amlien, Grambaite, Stenset, Bjornerud and Fladby2009). In a study, where MCI patients from a sample overlapping with the present were compared to normal controls (Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Amlien, Grambaite, Stenset, Bjornerud and Fladby2009), the morphometric variables were found to have superior diagnostic sensitivity when compared to FA of the temporal and parietal areas, comprising the hippocampus, entorhinal, parahippocampal, retrosplenial, posterior cingulate, precuneus, supramarginal, inferior parietal, and middle temporal cortices. FA still explained unique variance in verbal memory function in the MCI group. In the current study, we use the same regions of interest (ROIs), but, differently from the study of Walhovd et al. (Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Amlien, Grambaite, Stenset, Bjornerud and Fladby2009), WM diffusivity data have been processed using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics and FSL method (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Matthews2004), shown to be a promising method in examining the degeneration of neurofiber tracts in MCI and AD (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Spulber, Lehtimaki, Kononen, Hallikainen, Grohn and Soininen2009). This method makes this study unique also when compared to another of our publications (Grambaite et al., Reference Grambaite, Stenset, Reinvang, Walhovd, Fjell and Fladby2010), containing a sample overlapping with the present, where DTI data have been processed using the nICE Basis and Diffusion Module and nine ROIs have been manually drawn in the FA map. In that study, the patients have been stratified by CSF T-tau pathology, and the results indicate that the size of hippocampus volume (HcV) and memory performance may differ according to levels of T-tau pathology. In the current study, we classify our MCI sample according to neuropsychological performance into the aMCI, naMCI and laMCI (less advanced MCI, not satisfying criteria for either aMCI or naMCI) subtypes to characterize typical DTI and morphometric alterations in aMCI compared to other MCI subtypes. To better understand the relationship between memory and chosen temporal-parietal regions, known to be involved in episodic memory retrieval (Buckner & Wheeler, Reference Buckner and Wheeler2001; Wagner, Shannon, Kahn, & Buckner, Reference Wagner, Shannon, Kahn and Buckner2005; Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Amlien, Grambaite, Stenset, Bjornerud and Fladby2009), we relate episodic memory (story, word list, and visual) to morphometric and WM diffusivity measures. Memory-related cortical changes in pre-dementia stages are relatively well described (Fjell, Walhovd, et al., Reference Fjell, Walhovd, Amlien, Bjornerud, Reinvang, Gjerstad and Fladby2008; Karas et al., Reference Karas, Sluimer, Goekoop, van der Flier, Rombouts, Vrenken and Barkhof2008; Whitwell et al., Reference Whitwell, Jack, Przybelski, Parisi, Senjem, Boeve and Josephs2009), and the relations between memory and WM alterations in MCI have been found in a few DTI studies (Bosch et al., Reference Bosch, Arenaza-Urquijo, Rami, Sala-Llonch, Junque, Sole-Padulles and Bartrés-Faz2010; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Friedland and Auchus2007; Rogalski et al., Reference Rogalski, Murphy, deToledo-Morrell, Shah, Moseley, Bammer and Stebbins2009; Rose et al., Reference Rose, McMahon, Janke, O’Dowd, de Zubicaray, Strudwick and Chalk2006; Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Amlien, Grambaite, Stenset, Bjornerud and Fladby2009). Still, these relationships need to be verified across different DTI methods (e.g., ROI-based and TBSS methods) as well as different episodic memory domains, including both verbal and non-verbal recall aspects. Furthermore, it remains unclear to which degree the relations between WM diffusivity and memory may be accounted for by the secondary effects of cortical gray matter atrophy.

The study aims are as follows: (1) to compare an aMCI group with other neuropsychologically derived clinical MCI subgroups (naMCI and laMCI) with respect to cortical morphometric and DTI measures of the underlying WM tracts (FA, MD, and DR) in the temporal-parietal memory network; (2) to determine clinical significance of WM tract affection by investigating the relationships between imaging parameters and story, word list and visual memory performance; and (3) to analyze whether WM diffusivity measures have unique contributions to memory performance, independently of cortical atrophy.

Methods

Research Participants

The study participants are shortly described in a schematic summary, provided in Figure 1.

Fig. 1 Schematic overview of the study participants. In total, 86 patients were candidates for inclusion. Five participants were excluded after initial investigations due to definite diagnosis (i.e., normal pressure hydrocephalus, frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body disease). Patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Global Deterioration Scale score = 3), verified on cognitive screening tests, who have performed both neuropsychological and magnetic resonance imaging/diffusion tensor imaging examinations, were included in the current study (n = 49). The patients were further classified into three neuropsychological subtypes (amnestic [aMCI], non-amnestic [naMCI], and less advanced [laMCI]).

Patients with subjective cognitive complaints lasting 6 months or longer attending a university-based memory clinic between September 2005 and January 2010 were assessed for inclusion.

Inclusion criteria were preserved general intellectual function, no or very mild activities of daily living problems and Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) score 3 (Auer & Reisberg, Reference Auer and Reisberg1997; Reisberg, Ferris, de Leon, & Crook, Reference Reisberg, Ferris, de Leon and Crook1988) as determined from a clinical interview and screening tests. Screening tests included parameters 13–20 (memory, disorientation, abstract thinking, visuospatial ability, language, sensory aphasia, visual agnosia, and apraxia) from the stepwise comparative status analysis—STEP (Edman, Mahnfeldt, & Wallin, Reference Edman, Mahnfeldt and Wallin2001; Wallin et al., Reference Wallin, Edman, Blennow, Gottfries, Karlsson, Regland and Sjögren1996), word fluency, interference, and numeral-letter items from the I-flex (Royall, Mahurin, & Gray, Reference Royall, Mahurin and Gray1992), and items from the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination – Cognistat (Kiernan, Mueller, Langston, & Van Dyke, Reference Kiernan, Mueller, Langston and Van Dyke1987), as well as Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975). To be classified as GDS 3, subjects had to score ≥26 on MMSE, ≤1 on STEP variables, ≤2 on I-FLEX variables, and there were more than one Clinical Dementia Rating (Morris, Reference Morris1993) domain where they scored 0.5, albeit none where they scored 1. Criteria for exclusion were established psychiatric disorder, cancer, drug abuse, solvent exposure, or anoxic brain damage. The project was approved by the South-Eastern Norway committee for medical research ethics. Participants’ consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki (Declaration of Helsinki, 1991).

Neuropsychological Assessment

Neuropsychological examination was performed by a psychologist within three months of the MRI scan date. General cognitive ability, memory, language, attention/executive and visuospatial function have been assessed. For the assessment of general cognitive ability, vocabulary was measured using vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) and nonverbal abstract problem solving was measured using Matrix reasoning either from WASI or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003). T-scores, based on norms for these tests, are reported in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and neuropsychological data comparison among groups

Note. MCI = mild cognitive impairment; aMCI = amnestic MCI; naMCI = non-amnestic MCI; laMCI = less advanced MCI; APOE = apolipoprotein E; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination (max score, 30). T-scores, presented for all neuropsychological tests, except MMSE, are age-corrected test-specific normative data. The comparison of gender, handedness and APOE 4 among groups were made with a χ2 test (Pearson χ2 test p value is shown). Mann-Whitney U-test was used to compare the groups for the remaining variables in the table. p 1 = significance aMCI versus naMCI; p 2 = significance aMCI versus laMCI.

The memory tests included Logical Memory Subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1987), the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Schmidt, Reference Schmidt1996) and visual reproduction from the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT; Meyers & Meyers, Reference Meyers and Meyers1995). T-scores, based on norms for these tests, are provided in Table 1. The language tests used were the Boston Naming Test (BNT; Kaplan Goodglass, & Weintraub, Reference Kaplan, Goodglass and Weintraub1983) and the Controlled Verbal Fluency Task (FAS; Spreen & Strauss, Reference Spreen and Strauss1998). The attention/executive tests were comprised of Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B (Spreen & Strauss, Reference Spreen and Strauss1998), the Digit Symbol subtest of the WAIS-III, D-KEFS (Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System) Color-Word Interference Test (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001), and The Letter-Number Sequencing task from WAIS-III. RCFT copy trial was used to assess visuospatial function.

Subject Classification

Two experienced clinical neuropsychologists, who were blinded for other data than neuropsychological test results, including normative scores, age, and education, have classified the subjects into either aMCI or naMCI or laMCI subtypes. The clinicians worked independently of each other and finally compared their answers to assure that the subjects were classified correctly. Criteria for classification of subjects were similar to those used by Whitwell and colleagues (Whitwell, Petersen, et al., Reference Whitwell, Petersen, Negash, Weigand, Kantarci, Ivnik and Jack2007).

To be classified as aMCI, a subject had to be impaired on at least two memory tests, based on norms for these tests, irrespectively of impairment on other non-memory tests. The T-score had to be ≤37 for the mean score of short- and long-term recall on RAVLT or/and RCFT, or/and ≤ the 10th percentile for the long-term recall of Logical Memory, reflecting a score of 1.3 or more standard deviations (SD) below the control mean. When the results on RAVLT and RCFT were close to the cut-off and doubtful, then the long-term recall was used for classification. To be classified as naMCI, a subject had to be unimpaired on memory tasks, but impaired on at least two of the non-memory tests. The same cut-off (T = 37 or percentile = 10) as for the judgment of amnestic impairment was used. Subjects, who did not meet the criteria for aMCI or naMCI because of relatively intact performance on neuropsychological tests, were classified as laMCI.

Memory Variables

Three memory variables, used in the analyses were termed: story memory (WMS-R), word list memory (RAVLT), and visual memory (RCFT). The mean scores of the short term and 30 min recall were calculated for word list and visual memory and the mean score of the immediate and 30 min recall was calculated for story memory to reduce the number of variables. The standardized residuals, after regressing out age, gender, and education (and also copy trial for visual memory) from the raw scores, were computed and used in all analyses.

ROIs and MRI/DTI Acquisition

The same ROIs as in a previous publication on a subset of this material were used (Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Amlien, Grambaite, Stenset, Bjornerud and Fladby2009): the hippocampus, entorhinal, parahippocampal, retrosplenial, posterior cingulate, precuneus, inferior parietal, supramarginal and middle temporal cortices, and the underlying WM. A three-dimensional illustration of all these regions may be found in Figure 2. For HcV, the sum of the right and the left sides, and for other ROIs, mean scores of the right and the left sides, have been used in the analyses.

Fig. 2 Group values for the different regions of interest for morphometric and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) measurements. The standardized residuals are depicted. The effects of age and gender (and intracranial volume [ICV] for hippocampus volume [HcV]) were regressed out for morphometric variables. The effects of age, gender, and DTI site were regressed out for fractional anisotropy/mean diffusivity/radial diffusivity [FA/MD/DR]. Significance of the differences between amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI, n = 18) and non-amnestic MCI (naMCI, n = 17) is marked: **p < .05 and *p < .10. Significance of the differences between aMCI and less advanced MCI (laMCI, n = 14) is marked: p < .05 and p < .10. Error bars represent standard deviations.

For practical reasons, two different 1.5 Tesla (T) MRI scanners were used in this study (site 1/2: 14/43 subjects), which are described in detail elsewhere (Stenset et al., Reference Stenset, Bjornerud, Fjell, Walhovd, Hofoss, Due-Tonnessen and Fladby2009). In another study, where the same MR scanners were used and six subjects were scanned on both scanners, correlations across scanners for different morphometric measures were close to 1.00, indicating that change of scanner did not introduce bias in the data (Fjell, Walhovd, et al., Reference Fjell, Walhovd, Amlien, Bjornerud, Reinvang, Gjerstad and Fladby2008). In our data set, correlations (the Pearson coefficients) between MR site and the chosen ROIs of cortical thickness and HcV were not significant (p > .05) and varied from −.15 to .26. Contrarily, the Pearson coefficients between DTI-site and FA parameters were highly significant (p < .01) and varied from .39 to .73 in our data set. Therefore, DTI variables were regressed on age, gender and also MRI site, and the residuals were used in all analyses, except for the supplementary analyses provided in Supplementary Table 1. Age, gender, and intracranial volume (ICV) were regressed out for HcV; age and gender were regressed out for cortical thickness.

MRI Segmentations and Analyses

Morphometric and DTI data were processed by the same physician at Akershus University Hospital (P.S.). Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed with the Freesurfer image analysis suite version 4.5.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). This includes segmentation of the subcortical WM and deep gray matter volumetric structures (Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove and Dale2002) and parcellation of the cortical surface (Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, van der Kouwe, Destrieux, Halgren, Segonne, Salat and Dale2004) according to a parcellation scheme (Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker and Killiany2006). This labels cortical sulci and gyri, and thickness values are calculated in the ROIs.

DTI data were processed using FSL version 4.1 (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Matthews2004; Woolrich et al., Reference Woolrich, Jbabdi, Patenaude, Chappell, Makni, Behrens and Smith2009). Initially, an affine registration was done for each DTI volume to the low-b (b = 0) image using FLIRT (Jenkinson & Smith, Reference Jenkinson and Smith2001). Motion between scans and residual eddy currents were corrected for, before FA and eigenvalue maps were created. DR was defined as the mean of the second and third eigenvalues ((λ2 + λ3)/2), and MD as the mean of all three eigenvalues ((λ1 + λ2 + λ3)/3). Further processing of the FA, DR, and MD data was carried out using TBSS (Tract-Based Spatial Statistics) version 1.2 (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Johansen-Berg, Rueckert, Nichols, Mackay and Behrens2006), part of FSL. FA images were created by fitting a tensor model to the raw diffusion data using FDT, and then brain-extracted using BET (Smith, Reference Smith2002). All subjects’ FA data were then aligned into a common space using the nonlinear registration tool FNIRT (Andersson, Reference Andersson, Jenkinson and Smith2007), which uses a b-spline representation of the registration warp field (Rueckert et al., Reference Rueckert, Sonoda, Hayes, Hill, Leach and Hawkes1999). Next, the mean FA image was created and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton which represents the centers of all tracts common to all subjects in the group. Each subject’s aligned FA data were then projected onto this skeleton and the resulting data fed into voxelwise cross-subject statistics. DR and MD data were then extracted from each subject according to the skeletonized FA map. WM ROIs based on the FreeSurfer WM parcellations were then extracted for FA, DR and MD: The FSL FMRIB58_FA template (to which every subjects FA volume initially was registered) was coregistered to the standard space T1 volume MNI152, which subsequently went through the FreeSurfer processing stream to create a volume with WM parcellations. By applying the registration between the FA template and the MNI152 volume to the volume with the WM parcellations, the resulting volume could be used to extract the skeletonized FA, DR, and MD data from each WM ROI (Fjell, Westlye, et al., Reference Fjell, Westlye, Greve, Fischl, Benner, van der Kouwe and Walhovd2008).

To confirm that the voxels in the mean FA skeleton actually were derived from the correct tract-center points in all subjects, these voxels were projected back from their position on the skeleton to the nearby position at the center of the nearest tract in the subject’s FA image in standard space (i.e., after the FA image had been nonlinearly registered to the target image). Furthermore, these points were then “inversely warped” back into the subject’s native space by inverting the nonlinear registration that was originally applied. The voxels were then manually inspected in both standard and native space. There was a generally satisfactory overlap between tract centers and back projected voxels. Furthermore, the selected WM ROIs from the FreeSurfer parcellation of the standard space T1 volume MNI152 were also projected back to both standard and native space and manually inspected for accuracy.

Statistical Analysis

The Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS 16.0) was used for all statistical analyses. To compare group demographic characteristics Mann-Whitney U-test and χ2 test were used. A linear regression model was used to perform a parametric correction to the data. The variables, the effects of which we regressed out, are listed in the sections “Memory variables” and “ROIs and MRI/DTI Acquisition.” The obtained standardized residuals were further treated as a new dataset before the group analyses were performed. The Mann-Whitney U-test was further used to analyze group differences with regard to neuropsychological performance, WM diffusivity and morphometry. Non-parametric approach was used due to small and unequal sample sizes. To estimate effect sizes, Cohen’s d, defined as the difference between two means divided by the pooled standard deviation for the data, was calculated. As pooled standard deviation, the square root of the average of the squared standard deviations was used (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). Pearson’s correlation method was used to test for associations between imaging and cognitive variables. Both uncorrected and Bonferroni-corrected p values for the number of memory variables (story, word list, visual) investigated were obtained for the correlation analyses. Hierarchic regression method was used to analyze the predictive value of WM diffusivity, namely DR and MD of the regions significantly correlating with memory, to memory variance after controlling for possible effects of cortical thickness in the corresponding ROIs. The terms “statistically significant” and “significant,” used in this article, refer to the p value smaller than .05. The term “significant at trend level” refers to the p value smaller than .10.

Results

The patient groups were quite similar according to their demographics as seen from Table 1. None of the listed variables, except the memory variables as expected by the definitions used, differed significantly between the aMCI and naMCI. When aMCI was compared to laMCI, performance on matrix reasoning test as well as memory variables were significantly better in the laMCI group, but performance on the vocabulary subtest as well as other listed demographical variables did not differ significantly between the groups.

HcV, cortical thickness, FA, MD, and DR for different ROIs were compared among the groups, and the results are presented in Figure 2.

Comparison of aMCI and naMCI Groups

Inferior parietal and supramarginal thickness were lower at trend level in aMCI compared to naMCI. Neither HcV nor FA/DR/MD values differed significantly between the groups. To check how much the correction of data and application of standardized residuals impacted the results, the analyses of group differences were performed with the uncorrected data. Significant group differences, the raw score means, standard deviations, and effects sizes are provided in a supplementary table (Supplementary Table 1).

Comparison of aMCI and laMCI Groups

HcV in the aMCI was smaller than in the laMCI group, but the differences were not statistically significant. Parahippocampal cortex was thinner at trend level in the aMCI compared to laMCI. For the underlying WM tracts, parahippocampal and supramarginal FA values were lower at trend level and entorhinal DR/MD were significantly higher in the aMCI group compared to the laMCI group. Group analyses for the uncorrected data may be found in Supplementary Table 1. Relationships between imaging and memory measures are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 The relationships between ROIs and memory

Note. Pearson correlation coefficients between ROIs and memory are listed. N = 49. ROI = region of interest; TH = cortical thickness; V = volume; FA = fractional anisotropy; DR = radial diffusivity; MD = mean diffusivity; ICV = intracranial volume; HcV = hippocampus volume. The effects of age, gender, and education (and copy trial for visual memory) were regressed out for memory variables. The effects of age and gender (and ICV for HcV) were regressed out for morphometric variables. The effects of age, gender and DTI-site were regressed out for FA/MD/DR. Bold characters indicate * p < .05 and ** p < .01 uncorrected, and underlined characters indicate p < .05 Bonferroni-corrected for three comparisons (story, word list, visual).

As shown, parahippocampal MD/DR correlated significantly with story, word list, and visual memory. Parahippocampal FA was significantly related with story and word list memory, and parahippocampal thickness was significantly related with visual memory. Entorhinal and middle temporal thickness/MD/DR, and also entorhinal FA, correlated significantly with story memory. The relationships were negative between memory and MD/DR and positive between memory and cortical thickness/FA. The correlations were adjusted for three comparisons applying the Bonferroni correction to account for the fact that three memory variables were correlated to the imaging variables of the temporal-parietal memory network. Due to carefully chosen imaging variables, which were interdependent, and relatively small sample, stronger corrections were deemed to be too strict. After the Bonferroni corrections, story memory was still significantly related to entorhinal thickness/FA/MD/DR, parahippocampal MD/DR, as well as middle temporal MD/DR. The relationships between word list memory and parahippocampal MD/DR also remained significant.

Hierarchic regression analyses (Table 3) were further performed with cortical thickness and DR/MD of the ROIs, correlating significantly with memory function (shown in Table 2), as the independent variables. The chosen thickness variable was entered in step 1 (the first block) and DR or MD (one per model) in the underlying WM tract was entered in step 2. That was to analyze if DR/MD could serve as unique predictors of the variance in episodic memory after controlling for the possible effect of cortical thickness.

Table 3 Hierarchic regression analyses with memory function as the dependent variable

Note. N = 49. TH = cortical thickness; DR = radial diffusivity; MD = mean diffusivity; Cp = p for F-change. *Indicates significant model (p < .05). The effects of age and gender (and intracranial volume for hippocampus volume) were regressed out for morphometric variables. The effects of age, gender, and diffusion tensor imaging site were regressed out for MD/DR.

In the analyses with story, word list, and visual memory as the dependent variables, both DR and MD in the parahippocampal WM tract explained unique variance (p < .05) in story and word list memory beyond that which could be explained by cortical thickness in the corresponding ROI. Contrarily, parahippocampal MD/DR did not show superior predictive value in explaining unique variance (p > .05) in visual memory beyond that which could be explained by parahippocampal thickness. Further analyses were performed with story memory as the dependent variable and cortical thickness and DR/MD in either the entorhinal or middle temporal WM tracts as the independent variables. DR and MD in these ROIs could also explain unique variance (p < .05) in story memory after controlling for the possible effects of cortical thickness in the corresponding ROIs.

Discussion

Two main conclusions can be drawn from the present study. First, clinical aMCI and laMCI subgroups could be distinguished by DTI WM diffusivity measures, but aMCI and naMCI groups could not be distinguished by these variables. Second, memory performance in the total MCI sample could be explained by diffusion characteristics in specific WM tracts, which was not due to secondary effects of gray matter thinning.

Our results suggest that increased DR and MD, underlying the entorhinal cortex, differentiate the aMCI group from the laMCI, the latter group comprising MCI patients not satisfying criteria for either aMCI or naMCI subtypes. These findings are partially in accord with the results of Pievani and colleagues, who have found increased DR in the limbic network in aMCI patients compared with healthy elderly, although MD and FA values were not different (Pievani et al., Reference Pievani, Agosta, Pagani, Canu, Sala, Absinta and Filippi2010). Adding to the findings concerning group differences, we show that changes in DR and MD of the WM tracts underlying the entorhinal cortex have unique predictive value for story memory performance even after controlling for cortical thickness. Similarly, the results from our study show that DR and MD of the WM tracts, underlying parahippocampal and middle temporal cortex, are unique predictors of story and word list memory, independently of cortical thickness. In another recent study it has been shown that the statistical effect on WM diffusivity changes in temporal and parahippocampal regions remained after controlling for gray matter degeneration as measured by HcV (Salat et al., Reference Salat, Tuch, van der Kouwe, Greve, Pappu, Lee and Rosas2010). Here, by controlling WM tract parameters for cortical thickness in each of the corresponding regions in the temporal-parietal memory network, we were able to show that the relationships between WM integrity and memory parameters were not secondary effects of cortical gray matter atrophy. The results from another DTI study, where TBSS method was applied, showed that most DTI-derived changes, except some MD and DR increases in posterior associative pathways, were secondary to gray matter atrophy in aMCI and AD (Bosch et al., Reference Bosch, Arenaza-Urquijo, Rami, Sala-Llonch, Junque, Sole-Padulles and Bartrés-Faz2010). Furthermore, FA values were the only DTI measure significantly related to memory in aMCI and AD, while we find that both DR and MD serve as significant predictors of episodic memory impairment.

Previously, it has been shown that severe destruction of the entorhinal pre-[alpha] layer, which is accompanied by comparatively minor changes in the hippocampus, occur before major neocortical destruction in AD (Braak & Braak, Reference Braak and Braak1996). Therefore, small HcV differences in groups are probably not surprising in early disease stages. Whitwell, Przybelski, and colleagues (Reference Whitwell, Przybelski, Weigand, Knopman, Boeve, Petersen and Jack2007) found hippocampus atrophy in aMCI compared to the matched healthy subjects, while HcV in aMCI, defined using similar MCI-subtyping procedure, did not differ significantly from HcV of non-amnestic subtypes in our study, where no control group was included. In another study, Whitwell, Petersen, and colleagues (Reference Whitwell, Petersen, Negash, Weigand, Kantarci, Ivnik and Jack2007) found gray matter loss in hippocampus even in the naMCI group. These findings may possibly explain small HcV differences in amnestic versus non-amnestic MCI in our study, although it is important to mention that different MRI post-processing techniques were used for morphometry. Furthermore, the mean age of the aMCI subjects in the studies of Whitwell, Petersen, et al. (Reference Whitwell, Petersen, Negash, Weigand, Kantarci, Ivnik and Jack2007) and Whitwell, Przybelski, et al. (Reference Whitwell, Przybelski, Weigand, Knopman, Boeve, Petersen and Jack2007) were 76–78 years. Our aMCI subjects are younger (mean age is 63 years) and may have different pattern of cortical and WM changes than older aMCI patients. Possible explanation may be that our relatively young aMCI group consists of AD patients very early in the disease progression, and HcV is, therefore, less affected than it is usually expected in older aMCIs. These questions need to be addressed in the future research.

By using comprehensive diagnostic criteria, including three different memory tasks, we have shown pathological differences concerning WM tract affection (DR and MD) in the aMCI compared with the laMCI subtype. The amnestic group probably represents incipient AD. Non-amnestic groups are heterogeneous and may contain both patients with AD or other incipient dementias and normal aging subjects. MCI patients with relatively normal neuropsychological function (laMCI) may possibly represent a group of “normals.” Of interest, the frequency of the apolipoprotein E ε 4 carriers is equal in aMCI and laMCI groups (data are shown in Table 1). The results from the meta-analysis of Wisdom, Callahan, and Hawkins (Reference Wisdom, Callahan and Hawkins2009) show that APOE ε 4 and cognition are negatively related also in healthy subjects. Thus, the laMCI group may also be at risk for developing aMCI. We have focused on an episodic memory network, and naMCI is defined as having non-memory deficits. Memory is complex, and we have chosen a few defining memory parameters in this study, although that does not mean that there may not be deficits in other parameters or in only one test. Therefore, the groups may be overlapping in memory function. It follows that both group analyses and analyses of different memory parameters should be performed together to better understand AD-related pathology.

This study has some important strengths. First, carefully defined inclusion criteria were used. In addition to cognitive screening measures, we used neuropsychological criteria to further divide our “MCI” subjects into subgroups and were able to demonstrate group differences in otherwise similar individuals. Second, by studying cortical morphometric parameters together with diffusivity parameters (FA/DR/MD) of the underlying WM tracts, we were able to provide consistent findings regarding the importance of both cortical and WM changes in the underlying tracts and their relation to different aspects of the episodic memory. To the best of our knowledge, most studies were focused on ROIs and no former data, except for data from this study and recent study of Bosch and colleagues (Reference Bosch, Arenaza-Urquijo, Rami, Sala-Llonch, Junque, Sole-Padulles and Bartrés-Faz2010), are available providing multiple DTI measures (FA, MD, and DR) in aMCI using TBSS, known to improve the sensitivity, objectivity and interpretability of analysis of multi-subject DTI studies (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Johansen-Berg, Rueckert, Nichols, Mackay and Behrens2006). There are also some important limitations. First, MCI subgroups contained relatively small number of participants. Second, Alzheimer and dementia are progressive processes, and using age-corrected residuals can result in failing to detect group differences. As can be seen in Table 1, the aMCI group is 5 years older than the naMCI group. Supplemental Table 1 shows that group differences are significant for some uncorrected variables. Although we have chosen this conservative approach, it is not necessary correct. Third, we do not know how many patients go on to be demented and the results need to be verified with longitudinal data.

The results of the present study showed more pronounced relationships between memory and DR/MD, than with FA. Experimental and clinical studies have shown that increases in DR may be associated with demyelination and myelin degeneration and axonal atrophy (Concha et al., Reference Concha, Livy, Beaulieu, Wheatley and Gross2010; Song et al., Reference Song, Sun, Ju, Lin, Cross and Neufeld2003, Reference Song, Yoshino, Le, Lin, Sun, Cross and Armstrong2005). Interpretation of putative pathological underpinnings of the DR/MD findings should be made with caution as the specific neurobiology relating to DTI parameters is still unclear (Paus, Reference Paus2009).

Memory function is dependent on memory network. In addition to atrophy in this network, the fiber integrity in key areas may contribute to memory dysfunction. Our main findings indicate that DTI DR and MD may differentiate aMCI from laMCI. A few memory aspects are related to these WM alterations in MCI, suggesting that the structural group differences have cognitive correlates, and so verifying the importance of using more than one memory tests and DTI WM diffusivity measures for identification of incipient AD.

Acknowledgments

The study is supported by grants from South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (Helse Sør-Øst) and Akershus University Hospital. The authors thank Erik Hessen, who has contributed to neuropsychological classification of subjects into groups as well as supervising the first author in the clinical work with the study participants, Boo Johansson for his valuable comments concerning presentation of the study results, and Jurate Saltyte-Benth for her assistance with statistical analyses. We are grateful to Paulina Due-Tønnessen, Atle Bjørnerud, Kjersti Træland Hanssen, Turid Hegde, Sissel Bardosen, and Lisbeth Johnsen for their contribution to data collection and all the participants of this study for donating their time. The authors have no financial relationships that could be interpreted as a conflict of interest.

Supplementary Materials

To review these additional data and analyses, please access the online-only supplementary Table 1. Please visit journals.cambridge.org/INS, then click on the link “Supplementary Materials” at this article.

References

REFERENCES

Andersson, J.L.R., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S. (2007). Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. FMRIB Technical Report TR07JA2 from www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.Google Scholar
Auer, S., Reisberg, B. (1997). The GDS/FAST staging system. International Psychogeriatrics/IPA, 9(Suppl 1), 167171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickel, H., Mosch, E., Seigerschmidt, E., Siemen, M., Forstl, H. (2006). Prevalence and persistence of mild cognitive impairment among elderly patients in general hospitals. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 242250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, B., Arenaza-Urquijo, E.M., Rami, L., Sala-Llonch, R., Junque, C., Sole-Padulles, C., Bartrés-Faz, D. (2010). Multiple DTI index analysis in normal aging, amnestic MCI and AD. Relationship with neuropsychological performance. Neurobiology of Aging, [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
Braak, H., Braak, E. (1996). Evolution of the neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica. Supplementum, 165, 312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R.L., Wheeler, M.E. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of remembering. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2, 624634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlton, R.A., Barrick, T.R., McIntyre, D.J., Shen, Y., O’Sullivan, M., Howe, F.A., Markus, H.S. (2006). White matter damage on diffusion tensor imaging correlates with age-related cognitive decline. Neurology, 66, 217222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chetelat, G., Desgranges, B., de la Sayette, V., Viader, F., Berkouk, K., Landeau, B., Eustache, F. (2003). Dissociating atrophy and hypometabolism impact on episodic memory in mild cognitive impairment. Brain, 126, 19551967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, C.K. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Concha, L., Livy, D.J., Beaulieu, C., Wheatley, B.M., Gross, D.W. (2010). In vivo diffusion tensor imaging and histopathology of the fimbria-fornix in temporal lobe epilepsy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 9961002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Santi, S., de Leon, M.J., Rusinek, H., Convit, A., Tarshish, C.Y., Roche, A., Fowler, J. (2001). Hippocampal formation glucose metabolism and volume losses in MCI and AD. Neurobiology of Aging, 22, 529539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis and Kaplan D-KEFS Executive Functions System: Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Desikan, R.S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B.T., Dickerson, B.C., Blacker, D., Killiany, R.J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage, 31, 968980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, B., Feldman, H.H., Jacova, C., Dekosky, S.T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., Scheltens, P. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurology, 6, 734746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edman, A., Mahnfeldt, M., Wallin, A. (2001). Inter-rater reliability of the STEP protocol. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 14, 140144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fellgiebel, A., Muller, M.J., Wille, P., Dellani, P.R., Scheurich, A., Schmidt, L.G., Stoeter, P. (2005). Color-coded diffusion-tensor-imaging of posterior cingulate fiber tracts in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, 11931198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, P., Jungwirth, S., Zehetmayer, S., Weissgram, S., Hoenigschnabl, S., Gelpi, E., Tragl, K.H. (2007). Conversion from subtypes of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer dementia. Neurology, 68, 288291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Salat, D.H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., Dale, A.M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33, 341355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., van der Kouwe, A., Destrieux, C., Halgren, E., Segonne, F., Salat, D.H., Dale, A.M. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fjell, A.M., Amlien, I.K., Westlye, L.T., Stenset, V., Fladby, T., Skinningsrud, A., Walhovd, K.B. (2010). CSF biomarker pathology correlates with a medial temporo-parietal network affected by very mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease but not a fronto-striatal network affected by healthy aging. Neuroimage, 49, 18201830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fjell, A.M., Walhovd, K.B., Amlien, I., Bjornerud, A., Reinvang, I., Gjerstad, L., Fladby, T. (2008). Morphometric changes in the episodic memory network and tau pathologic features correlate with memory performance in patients with mild cognitive impairment. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 29, 11831189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fjell, A.M., Westlye, L.T., Greve, D.N., Fischl, B., Benner, T., van der Kouwe, A.J., Walhovd, K.B. (2008). The relationship between diffusion tensor imaging and volumetry as measures of white matter properties. Neuroimage, 42, 16541668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grambaite, R., Stenset, V., Reinvang, I., Walhovd, K.B., Fjell, A.M., Fladby, T. (2010). White matter diffusivity predicts memory in patients with subjective and mild cognitive impairment and normal CSF total tau levels. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 5869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, J., Friedland, R.P., Auchus, A.P. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of normal-appearing white matter in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer disease: Preliminary evidence of axonal degeneration in the temporal lobe. AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology, 28, 19431948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C.R.J., Lowe, V.J., Senjem, M.L., Weigand, S.D., Kemp, B.J., Shiung, M.M., Petersen, R.C. (2008). 11C PiB and structural MRI provide complementary information in imaging of Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain, 131, 665680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C.R.J., Petersen, R.C., Xu, Y.C., O’Brien, P.C., Smith, G.E., Ivnik, R.J., Kokmen, E. (1999). Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 52, 13971403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C.R.J., Shiung, M.M., Gunter, J.L., O’Brien, P.C., Weigand, S.D., Knopman, D.S., Petersen, R.C. (2004). Comparison of different MRI brain atrophy rate measures with clinical disease progression in AD. Neurology, 62, 591600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jak, A.J., Bangen, K.J., Wierenga, C.E., Delano-Wood, L., Corey-Bloom, J., Bondi, M.W. (2009). Contributions of neuropsychology and neuroimaging to understanding clinical subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. International Review of Neurobiology, 84, 81103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jak, A.J., Bondi, M.W., Delano-Wood, L., Wierenga, C., Corey-Bloom, J., Salmon, D.P., Delis, D.C. (2009). Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 368375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkinson, M., Smith, S. (2001). A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis, 5, 143156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E.F., Goodglass, H., Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Karas, G., Sluimer, J., Goekoop, R., van der Flier, W., Rombouts, S.A., Vrenken, H., Barkhof, F. (2008). Amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Structural MR imaging findings predictive of conversion to Alzheimer disease. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 29, 944949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiernan, R.J., Mueller, J., Langston, J.W., Van Dyke, C. (1987). The Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination: A brief but quantitative approach to cognitive assessment. Annals of Internal Medicine, 107, 481485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.F., Poupon, C., Clark, C.A., Pappata, S., Molko, N., Chabriat, H. (2001). Diffusion tensor imaging: Concepts and applications. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 13, 534546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y., Spulber, G., Lehtimaki, K.K., Kononen, M., Hallikainen, I., Grohn, H., Soininen, H. (2009). Diffusion tensor imaging and Tract-Based Spatial Statistics in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging, [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Medina, D., DeToledo-Morrell, L., Urresta, F., Gabrieli, J.D., Moseley, M., Fleischman, D., Stebbins, G.T. (2006). White matter changes in mild cognitive impairment and AD: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurobiology of Aging, 27, 663672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyers, J.E., Meyers, K.R. (1995). Rey complex figure test and recognition trial. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Morris, J.C. (1993). The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology, 43, 24122414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordlund, A., Rolstad, S., Klang, O., Edman, A., Hansen, S., Wallin, A. (2010). Two year outcome of MCI subtypes and aetiologies in the Goteborg MCI study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 81, 541546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T. (2009). Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: Myelin or axon? Brain and Cognition, 72, 2635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C. (2003). Conceptual overview. In R.C. Petersen (Ed.), Mild cognitive impairment: Aging to Alzheimer’s disease pp. (114). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, R.C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 183194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Parisi, J.E., Dickson, D.W., Johnson, K.A., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Kokmen, E. (2006). Neuropathologic features of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 63, 665672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Roberts, R.O., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Geda, Y.E., Ivnik, R.J., Jack, C.R. Jr. (2009). Mild cognitive impairment: Ten years later. Archives of Neurology, 66, 14471455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pievani, M., Agosta, F., Pagani, E., Canu, E., Sala, S., Absinta, M., Filippi, M. (2010). Assessment of white matter tract damage in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Human Brain Mapping, [Epub ahead of print].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S.H., de Leon, M.J., Crook, T. (1988). Global Deterioration Scale (GDS). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 661663.Google ScholarPubMed
Rogalski, E.J., Murphy, C.M., deToledo-Morrell, L., Shah, R.C., Moseley, M.E., Bammer, R., Stebbins, G.T. (2009). Changes in parahippocampal white matter integrity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Behavioural Neurology, 21, 5161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S.E., McMahon, K.L., Janke, A.L., O’Dowd, B., de Zubicaray, G., Strudwick, M.W., Chalk, J.B. (2006). Diffusion indices on magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77, 11221128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rountree, S.D., Waring, S.C., Chan, W.C., Lupo, P.J., Darby, E.J., Doody, R.S. (2007). Importance of subtle amnestic and nonamnestic deficits in mild cognitive impairment: Prognosis and conversion to dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 24, 476482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royall, D.R., Mahurin, R.K., Gray, K.F. (1992). Bedside assessment of executive cognitive impairment: The executive interview. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40, 12211226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueckert, D., Sonoda, L.I., Hayes, C., Hill, D.L., Leach, M.O., Hawkes, D.J. (1999). Nonrigid registration using free-form deformations: Application to breast MR images. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 18, 712721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salat, D.H., Tuch, D.S., van der Kouwe, A.J., Greve, D.N., Pappu, V., Lee, S.Y., Rosas, H.D. (2010). White matter pathology isolates the hippocampal formation in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 31, 244256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, M. (1996). Rey auditory and verbal learning test. A handbook. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Schneider, J.A., Arvanitakis, Z., Leurgans, S.E., Bennett, D.A. (2009). The neuropathology of probable Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Annals of Neurology, 66, 200208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S.M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping, 17, 143155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T.E., Mackay, C.E., Behrens, T.E. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage, 31, 14871505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M.W., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E., Johansen-Berg, H., Matthews, P.M. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage, 23(Suppl 1), S208S219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowdon, D.A., Greiner, L.H., Mortimer, J.A., Riley, K.P., Greiner, P.A., Markesbery, W.R. (1997). Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study. JAMA, 277, 813817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, S.K., Sun, S.W., Ju, W.K., Lin, S.J., Cross, A.H., Neufeld, A.H. (2003). Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia. Neuroimage, 20, 17141722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, S.K., Yoshino, J., Le, T.Q., Lin, S.J., Sun, S.W., Cross, A.H., Armstrong, R.C. (2005). Demyelination increases radial diffusivity in corpus callosum of mouse brain. Neuroimage, 26, 132140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreen, O., Strauss, E.A. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests, second edition: Administration, norms and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stebbins, G.T., Murphy, C.M. (2009). Diffusion tensor imaging in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Behavioural Neurology, 21, 3949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenset, V., Bjornerud, A., Fjell, A.M., Walhovd, K.B., Hofoss, D., Due-Tonnessen, P., Fladby, T. (2009). Cingulum fiber diffusivity and CSF T-tau in patients with subjective and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging, [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Stenset, V., Grambaite, R., Reinvang, I., Hessen, E., Cappelen, T., Bjornerud, A., Fladby, T. (2007). Diaschisis after thalamic stroke: A comparison of metabolic and structural changes in a patient with amnesic syndrome. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica. Supplementum, 187, 6871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenset, V., Hofoss, D., Johnsen, L., Skinningsrud, A., Berstad, A.E., Negaard, A., Fladby, T. (2008). White matter lesion severity is associated with reduced cognitive performances in patients with normal CSF Abeta42 levels. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 118, 373378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenset, V., Johnsen, L., Kocot, D., Negaard, A., Skinningsrud, A., Gulbrandsen, P., Fladby, T. (2006). Associations between white matter lesions, cerebrovascular risk factors, and low CSF Abeta42. Neurology, 67, 830833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A.D., Shannon, B.J., Kahn, I., Buckner, R.L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 445453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walhovd, K.B., Fjell, A.M., Amlien, I., Grambaite, R., Stenset, V., Bjornerud, A., Fladby, T. (2009). Multimodal imaging in mild cognitive impairment: Metabolism, morphometry and diffusion of the temporal-parietal memory network. Neuroimage, 45, 215223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallin, A., Edman, A., Blennow, K., Gottfries, C.G., Karlsson, I., Regland, B., Sjögren, M. (1996). Stepwise comparative status analysis (STEP): A tool for identification of regional brain syndromes in dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 9, 185199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, L., Goldstein, F.C., Veledar, E., Levey, A.I., Lah, J.J., Meltzer, C.C., Mao, H. (2009). Alterations in cortical thickness and white matter integrity in mild cognitive impairment measured by whole-brain cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 30, 893899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale-revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler adult intelligence scale—third edition: manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Whitwell, J.L., Jack, C.R. Jr., Przybelski, S.A., Parisi, J.E., Senjem, M.L., Boeve, B.F., Josephs, K.A. (2009). Temporoparietal atrophy: A marker of AD pathology independent of clinical diagnosis. Neurobiology of Aging, [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Whitwell, J.L., Petersen, R.C., Negash, S., Weigand, S.D., Kantarci, K., Ivnik, R.J., Jack, C.R. Jr. (2007). Patterns of atrophy differ among specific subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 64, 11301138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitwell, J.L., Przybelski, S.A., Weigand, S.D., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Petersen, R.C., Jack, C.R. Jr. (2007). 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 130, 17771786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitwell, J.L., Shiung, M.M., Przybelski, S.A., Weigand, S.D., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Jack, C.R. Jr. (2008). MRI patterns of atrophy associated with progression to AD in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 70, 512520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisdom, N.M., Callahan, J.L., Hawkins, K.A. (2009). The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: A meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Woolrich, M.W., Jbabdi, S., Patenaude, B., Chappell, M., Makni, S., Behrens, T., Smith, S.M. (2009). Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage, 45, S173S186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Medical Association. Declaration of Helsinki (1991). Law, Medicine & Health Care, 19, 264–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yaffe, K., Petersen, R.C., Lindquist, K., Kramer, J., Miller, B. (2006). Subtype of mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia and death. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22, 312319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y., Schuff, N., Jahng, G.H., Bayne, W., Mori, S., Schad, L., Weiner, M.W. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of cingulum fibers in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 68, 1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Schematic overview of the study participants. In total, 86 patients were candidates for inclusion. Five participants were excluded after initial investigations due to definite diagnosis (i.e., normal pressure hydrocephalus, frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body disease). Patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Global Deterioration Scale score = 3), verified on cognitive screening tests, who have performed both neuropsychological and magnetic resonance imaging/diffusion tensor imaging examinations, were included in the current study (n = 49). The patients were further classified into three neuropsychological subtypes (amnestic [aMCI], non-amnestic [naMCI], and less advanced [laMCI]).

Figure 1

Table 1 Demographic and neuropsychological data comparison among groups

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Group values for the different regions of interest for morphometric and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) measurements. The standardized residuals are depicted. The effects of age and gender (and intracranial volume [ICV] for hippocampus volume [HcV]) were regressed out for morphometric variables. The effects of age, gender, and DTI site were regressed out for fractional anisotropy/mean diffusivity/radial diffusivity [FA/MD/DR]. Significance of the differences between amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI, n = 18) and non-amnestic MCI (naMCI, n = 17) is marked: **p < .05 and *p < .10. Significance of the differences between aMCI and less advanced MCI (laMCI, n = 14) is marked: p < .05 and p < .10. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Figure 3

Table 2 The relationships between ROIs and memory

Figure 4

Table 3 Hierarchic regression analyses with memory function as the dependent variable

Supplementary material: PDF

Grambaite supplementary material

Table.pdf

Download Grambaite supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 73.1 KB