Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:28:13.333Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurocognitive Outcome Following Recovery from Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome – Coronavirus-1 (SARS-CoV-1)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2021

Farena S. Pinnock*
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology & Cognitive Health, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada
Jill B. Rich
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology & Cognitive Health, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Brandon Vasquez
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology & Cognitive Health, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Occupational Science & Occupational Therapy, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Melanie Wiegand
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
John Patcai
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Medicine, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
Angela K. Troyer
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology & Cognitive Health, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Kelly J. Murphy
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology & Cognitive Health, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Farena Pinnock, PhD, Neuropsychology & Cognitive Health, Baycrest Health Sciences, 3560 Bathurst St, North York, ONM6A 2E1, Canada. Email: fpinnock@baycrest.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) is a highly contagious viral respiratory illness associated with hypoxia and dyspnea. Many of those who contracted and recovered from SARS during the 2002–2003 outbreak reported persistent physical, psychological, and cognitive difficulties. Here, we investigated the residual influences of SARS on cognition for a subset of healthcare professionals who recovered and were referred for neuropsychological evaluation through their workplace insurance.

Method:

Twenty-eight healthcare professionals were evaluated on neuropsychological and mood functioning approximately 1.5 years post-recovery from a severe respiratory illness. Test scores were compared with age-matched normative data, and correlations were examined between mood, self-report memory scales, subjective complaints (e.g., poor concentration, pain, fatigue), illness severity (i.e., length of hospitalization, oxygen use during hospital stay), and cognitive performance.

Results:

Participants performed within age expectations on the majority of cognitive measures including overall memory ability. Although processing speed was generally within normal limits, 43% showed significant speed–accuracy trade-offs favoring accuracy over maintaining speed. Deficits were observed on measures of complex attention, such as working memory and the ability to sustain attention under conditions of distraction. Participants endorsed poorer memory ability than same-age peers on a meta-memory measure and mild to moderate depression and anxiety symptoms. Objective test performance was largely uncorrelated with self-reports, mood, or illness severity, except for moderate correlations between complex attention and participants’ subjective ratings of Everyday Task-Oriented Memory.

Conclusions:

These findings demonstrate specific long-term cognitive deficits associated with SARS and provide further evidence of the cognitive effects of hypoxic illnesses.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2021

INTRODUCTION

Outbreaks of coronavirus are occurring with greater regularity, as indicated by three epidemics within the last two decades. Most recently, severe acute respiratory syndrome – coronavirus-2 (SARS-CoV-2) surfaced in December 2019 and developed into a worldwide pandemic of the clinical syndrome coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). In this study, we focus on the SARS-CoV-1 (or SARS) outbreak of 2002–2003, which infected approximately 8100 people and claimed the lives of 774 people in 29 countries in under 5 months (WHO, 2015). SARS-CoV-1 initially presents with flu-like symptoms that, in some individuals, progress to life-threatening atypical pneumonia associated with dyspnea (difficult or labored breathing) and hypoxemia (deficient oxygenation of the blood; Kamps & Hoffman, Reference Kamps and Hoffman2004; Moldofsky & Patcai, Reference Moldofsky and Patcai2011). Because the mechanism and route of transmission of SARS-CoV-1 were initially unknown, there was an early lack of containment of the disease within hospitals, which resulted in a disproportionate infection rate among healthcare professionals. In Toronto, Canada, where 273 cases were reported, healthcare professionals accounted for over 40% of those affected (Muller et al., Reference Muller, Richardson, McGeer, Dresser, Raboud, Mazzulli and Louie2006; Naylor et al., Reference Naylor, Basrur, Bergeron, Brunham, Butler-Jones and Dafoe2003).

Approximately 30–90% of those who survived SARS-CoV-1 experienced residual effects up to 7 years later, such as shortness of breath, chronic fatigue, nonrestorative sleep, muscle and joint pain, psychological disorders [e.g., depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and an overall reduced quality of health and life (Gardner & Moallef, Reference Gardner and Moallef2015; Herridge et al., Reference Herridge, Cheung, Tansey, Matte-Martyn, Diaz-Granados, Al-Saidi and Slutsky2003; Lam et al., Reference Lam, Wing, Yu, Leung, Ma, Kong and Lam2009; Moallef, Lueke, Gardner, & Patcai, Reference Moallef, Lueke, Gardner and Patcai2021; Moldofsky & Patcai, Reference Moldofsky and Patcai2011)]. A recent meta-analysis supports the neuropsychiatric presentation and revealed similarities among severe coronavirus infections [SARS-CoV-1, SARS-CoV-2 (or COVID-19), and Middle East Respiratory Syndrome (MERS); Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Chesney, Oliver, Pollak, McGuire, Fusar-Poli and David2020]. Importantly, individual factors such as hypoxia, chronic fatigue, pain, and mood can negatively affect cognition (Irani, Barbone, Beausoleil, & Gerald, Reference Irani, Barbone, Beausoleil and Gerald2017; Suhr, Reference Suhr2003). However, little is known about the cognitive profile associated with SARS-CoV-1 or with coronaviruses in general.

One recent study assessed cognitive functioning in a small group of patients that were hospitalized due to symptoms of COVID-19 and found declines in attention, memory, language, and praxis (Negrini et al., Reference Negrini, Ferrario, Mazziotti, Berchicci, Bonazzi, de Sire and Zapparoli2020); of note, the conclusions were clouded by a significant association between impairments and length of stay in the intensive care unit. Another study examined cognitive outcomes in 29 recovered COVID-19 patients using a digital iPad-based online neuropsychological assessment. The authors found a significant deficit in sustained attention compared to controls, according to subcomponents of a continuous performance test (CPT; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Lu, Chen, Wei, Wang, Lyu and Hu2020). Several individuals who contracted SARS-CoV-1 reported problems with memory and attention after acute symptoms had receded, which raises the possibility that SARS-CoV-1 may have persistent effects on cognitive functioning (Chan, Reference Chan2005; Sheng, Cheng, Lau, Li, & Chan, Reference Sheng, Cheng, Lau, Li and Chan2005). To date, there has been no investigation to objectively characterize the nature and degree of the subjectively reported persistent cognitive difficulties described by some SARS survivors.

As the neuropsychological outcome of SARS-CoV-1 has not been well studied, the mechanism responsible for cognitive and psychological changes is also poorly understood. Subjective cognitive changes are reported in many populations (cognitive aging, cancer, stroke, depressed mood); however, these can be weakly correlated with objective cognitive function and are often more strongly related to mood factors and/or fatigue related to illness (Burmester, Leathem, & Merrick, Reference Burmester, Leathem and Merrick2016; O’Farrell, MacKenzie, & Collins, Reference O’Farrell, MacKenzie and Collins2013). The neuropsychological sequelae of SARS may very well result from various interacting factors (Rabinovitz, Jaywant, & Fridman, Reference Rabinovitz, Jaywant and Fridman2020). Hypothesized mechanisms contributing to changes in brain health in this group include indirect effects due to systemic complications (e.g., hypoxia; Ritchie, Chan, & Watermeyer, Reference Ritchie, Chan and Watermeyer2020), psychological trauma (Adhikari et al., Reference Adhikari, Tansey, McAndrews, Matté, Pinto, Cheung and Herridge2011; Mak, Chu, Pan, Yiu, & Chan, Reference Mak, Chu, Pan, Yiu and Chan2009; Wing & Leung, Reference Wing and Leung2012), as well as potential direct neural damage from the virus (Bohmwald, Gálvez, Ríos, & Kalergis, Reference Bohmwald, Gálvez, Ríos and Kalergis2018).

Indeed, considering that one of the critical symptoms of SARS is hypoxemia, its effects may be similar to other hypoxic disorders. Complex attention (sustained, divided, and concentration during interference), mental processing speed, memory, and executive functioning is the most commonly impaired cognitive domains in several hypoxic disorders (see Schultz, Sepehry, & Greer, Reference Schultz, Sepehry and Greer2018 for a review), including sleep apnea (Sforza & Roche, Reference Sforza and Roche2012; Stranks & Crowe, Reference Stranks and Crowe2016), chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (Riordan, Stika, Goldberg, & Drzewiecki, Reference Riordan, Stika, Goldberg and Drzewiecki2020; Schou, Østergaard, Rasmussen, Rydahl-Hansen, & Phanareth, Reference Schou, Østergaard, Rasmussen, Rydahl-Hansen and Phanareth2012; Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Blanc, Julian, Yelin, Katz, Sidney and Eisner2010), hypoxia due to stroke or heart attack (Kim, Reference Kim2016), and other acute respiratory diseases such as acute respiratory distress syndrome (ARDS; Riordan et al., Reference Riordan, Stika, Goldberg and Drzewiecki2020; Mikkelsen et al., Reference Mikkelsen, Christie, Lanken, Biester, Thompson, Bellamy and Angus2012) and hantavirus pulmonary syndrome (HPS; Hopkins, Larson-Lohr, Weaver, & Bigler, Reference Hopkins, Larson-Lohr, Weaver and Bigler1998). Furthermore, there is a complex relationship between the severity of hypoxia and cognition. For example, the degree of cognitive impairment among individuals with obstructive sleep apnea has been associated with the degree of oxygen deprivation in patients who experience hypoxia (Greenberg, Watson, & Deptula, Reference Greenberg, Watson and Deptula1987; Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Blanc, Julian, Yelin, Katz, Sidney and Eisner2010). In addition to these cognitive effects, many hypoxic conditions are also associated with elevated symptoms of depression, anxiety, and PTSD (Kim, Reference Kim2016; Mikkelsen et al., Reference Mikkelsen, Christie, Lanken, Biester, Thompson, Bellamy and Angus2012; Sheng et al., Reference Sheng, Cheng, Lau, Li and Chan2005).

Given the scale of the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic, knowledge of long-term cognitive outcomes following infection from a respiratory virus such as SARS-CoV-1 would be useful for future resource planning of those recovering from coronavirus infection. Here, we report on data from neuropsychological and mood functioning that were collected from healthcare professionals approximately 1.5 years post-recovery from SARS-CoV-1. Based on past research investigating the cognitive consequences of other hypoxic illnesses (Kim, Reference Kim2016; Mikkelsen et al., Reference Mikkelsen, Christie, Lanken, Biester, Thompson, Bellamy and Angus2012; Schou et al., Reference Schou, Østergaard, Rasmussen, Rydahl-Hansen and Phanareth2012; Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Blanc, Julian, Yelin, Katz, Sidney and Eisner2010; see Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Sepehry and Greer2018 for a review), we hypothesized that: (a) survivors of SARS would show impairments on neuropsychological measures of complex attention, memory, and processing speed; (b) survivors would perform normally on less complex measures of attention and memory; (c) degree of cognitive dysfunction would be associated with illness severity; and (d) survivors’ subjective complaints would be weakly correlated (if at all) with their performance on objective cognitive measures.

METHOD

Participants

Thirty healthcare professionals from the Toronto area who were classified as having contracted SARSFootnote 1 were referred for a neuropsychological evaluation at Baycrest Health Sciences from a provincial government agency SARS rehabilitation clinic at St. John’s Rehabilitation Centre. All participants were fully employed healthcare workers, mostly nurses – all were midlevel socioeconomic status. At the time of testing, these healthcare workers were on a leave of absence. They were not dismissed from their employment. During the clinical interview, all participants spontaneously self-reported lingering cognitive, mood, and/or physical changes post-recovery when probed with open-ended questions. Fatigue, low energy, breathlessness, and pain were the most common physical complaints. Poor memory (particularly prospective memory), difficulty attending to complex tasks, and word-finding problems were the most common cognitive complaints (see Figure 1).

Fig. 1. Common difficulties reported by SARS patients 1.5 years post-recovery.

Common cognitive, physical, and mood complaints were reported by 28 healthcare professionals 1.5 years post-recovery from SARS.

The neuropsychological evaluations were conducted to characterize each individual’s cognitive profile, understand the personal impacts associated with having had the illness, and provide recommendations for practical strategies that might help minimize the impacts of reduced cognitive capacity. All participants consented to having their neuropsychological evaluation data analyzed as part of a research study to examine the effects of SARS on cognition. This research was conducted in accordance with the Research Ethics Board at Baycrest Health Sciences and St. John’s Rehabilitation Hospital and in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration. This research is not under review elsewhere.

Of the 30 participants (28 female) who underwent neuropsychological assessment, data from 2 (females) were excluded due to poor proficiency in English. Specifically, they were non-fluent English speakers whose low FSIQ scores (< 70) were likely an underestimate of their true intellectual abilities. Demographic data for the remaining 28 participants are shown in Table 1. The vast majority of the participants were nurses (79%), and the rest were other interprofessional healthcare providers (e.g., orderlies, an ECG technician, respiratory therapist, paramedic, dietician, case manager, and a business project manager). All participants contracted the virus at their place of work. Testing was conducted approximately 1.5 years postinfection (in late 2004 to early 2005). Participants endorsed symptoms consistent with mild to moderate depression on the BDI-II (M = 17.4, with 10 participants falling in the moderate-to-severe range; Beck Depression Inventory-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996), and mild anxiety on the BAI (M = 14.0; Beck Anxiety Inventory; Beck & Steer, Reference Beck and Steer1993).

Table 1. Demographic and psychometric data for SARS patients (n = 28)

FSIQ = Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient, estimated using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) Matrix Reasoning and Vocabulary subtests. We have hospital data for 27 of the 28 participants. Of these 27, all were hospitalized, 15 received oxygen, and 2 were in ICU.

Materials

The neuropsychological battery was selected based on knowledge of the brain regions most vulnerable to hypoxia and the cognitive processes these regions subserve, and consideration of cognitive skills most critical to the performance of health professionals’ work-related duties, such as working memory. See Table 2 for the cognitive domains assessed and the specific tests administered.

Table 2. Neuropsychological test battery

WMS-III = Wechsler Memory Scale – Third Edition; D-KEFS =Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (Stroop = Color–Word Interference Test); TEA = Test of Everyday Attention; PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; RCFT = Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; CVLT-II =California Verbal Learning Test; MAC-S revised = Memory Assessment Clinic Self-Rating Scale; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.

Illness Severity

Illness severity was defined as length of hospital stay and whether or not oxygen was administered.

Data Analysis

Mean scaled scores (SS) were obtained for all neurocognitive measures using age-matched normative data. Scores within one standard deviation of the mean were considered average (i.e., 7 ≤ SS ≤ 13), while those with SS greater than one standard deviation above or below the mean were classified as above average or below expectations, respectively. Pearson product-moment correlations were conducted to investigate whether the degree of memory complaint or depression and anxiety levels from self-report measures were significantly related to performance on the cognitive measures. Similarly, point-biserial correlations or Pearson product-moment correlations were used to examine the relationship between severity of illness (i.e., duration of hospitalization and requirement of oxygen during hospital stay) and cognitive performance. Lastly, chi-square analyses were conducted to examine the relationship between relevant subjective complaints on the interview (i.e., attention, fatigue, and pain levels) and illness severity (i.e., duration of hospitalization and requirement of oxygen during hospital stay).

Of note, not all participants were administered the full complement of tests. This was a clinically informed decision to further streamline the battery for greater efficiency while examining the intended cognitive constructs that would aid in advising return to work decisions. Consequently, the following subtests had missing data (with a total number of participants in parentheses): TEA Lottery subtest (n = 15); PASAT test (n = 25); WMS-III Logical Memory and Faces subtests (n = 17); and MAC-S self-report (n = 27).

RESULTS

Orientation and Estimated Intellectual Function

All participants were oriented to time and place. Estimated general intellectual function was within the normal range (see Table 1), and performance on the Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subtests was equivalent (WASI; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999).

Simple and Complex Attention

Performance was within normal limits on tasks of simple attention and processing speed (Figure 2). Specifically, performance was average for the DKEFS (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001) Number and Letter Sequencing subtests, the Color Naming, and Word Reading subtests of the DKEFS Color–Word Interference Test, the WMS-III forward digit and spatial spans (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997), the Ruff 2 & 7 Test including both automatic and controlled cancelation tasks’ speed and accuracy ratings (Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Niemann, Allen, Farrow and Wylie1992), and the TEA Lottery subtest (Robertson, Ward, Ridgeway, & Nimmo-Smith, Reference Robertson, Ward, Ridgeway and Nimmo-Smith1994). Working memory, as measured by the WMS-III backward digit and spatial spans, was within expectations. Overall, participants showed normal cognitive set shifting on the DKEFS Trail Making Test Switching subtest, Color–Word Interference Test, and fluency switching tasks.

Fig. 2. Neurocognitive profile of SARS patients 1.5 years post-recovery.

Bars represent aggregate neuropsychological test scores from 28 SARS participants. Red dotted line = 1 standard deviation below the expected mean. Green dotted line = 1 standard deviation above the mean. Sp = span; Fwd = forward; Num = number; W = word; C = color; Spd = speed; Acc = accuracy; TEA = Test of Everyday Attention; PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; Con Tri = Consonant Trigrams; Bwd = backward; Switch = switching; Matrix Reason = Matrix Reasoning; RCFT DR = Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test delayed recall; WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale; LM = Logical Memory; Reten = retention; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; SD = short delay; FR = free recall; CR = cued recall; LD = long delay; Recog = recognition; MAC-S = Memory Assessment Clinic Self-Rating Scale; freq = frequency.

Examination of the Ruff 2 & 7 Test revealed that nearly half the participants (43%) showed a significant speed–accuracy trade-off, in which they sacrificed speed to maintain accuracy. Other measures of complex attention showed evidence of mild deficit, particularly those that placed increased demands on working memory and are more vulnerable to distraction. Specifically, performance on tests of complex forms of attention, the PASAT (Gronwall, Reference Gronwall1977) and Consonant Trigrams, was below expectations.

Language

Overall language performance was generally within expectations. Performance on the DKEFS verbal fluency tasks was within normal limits, both when participants were required to generate words beginning with particular letters as well as specific categories. Although performance on the BNT (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, Reference Kaplan, Goodglass and Weintraub1983) was significantly below average, this test is a highly culturally bound measure of word-finding ability. To compensate for this, all participants were on average credited for four items that more than 20% of people with diverse cultural and linguistical backgrounds typically get incorrect (see Chen et al., Reference Chen, Lin, Lin, Yeh, Chen, Wang and Wang2014; Mariën, Mampaey, Vervaet, Saerens, & De Deyn, Reference Mariën, Mampaey, Vervaet, Saerens and De Deyn1998; Roberts & Doucet, Reference Roberts and Doucet2011; Tallberg, Reference Tallberg2005); once doing so, their performance was within normal limits (SS = 8.9).

Memory

Performance was average to above average on most tests of memory, including immediate and delayed recall of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Singer, Duke, Singer, Morey, Daughtrey and Kaplan1994), delayed recall of the Logical Memory and Faces subtests of the WMS-III, all trials of the CVLT-II (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober1987), and both time and event-based tasks of prospective memory (with percentage hits at 89% for time and 93% for event; Event and Time-Based Prospective Memory; Troyer & Murphy, Reference Troyer and Murphy2007).

Performance on a memory ability task mediated by executive cognitive processes was impaired in up to 40% of participants. Specifically, proactive interference as measured by comparison of List B to List A Trial 1 recall on the CVLT-II was generally normal (M = 8.96, SD = 3.04). However, 29% of the sample showed significant proactive interference (i.e., 1.5 standard deviations below average), and 39% had scores (SS ≤ 7) one standard deviation or more below the average. Thus, despite normal performance on the learning trials, a number of participants had difficulty actively filtering out information that was no longer useful. Importantly, performance on the CVLT Forced Choice was 99.5% on average (range: 94–100), and this is above the recommended cutoff of 87.5% for valid responding (Schwartz, Erdodi, Rodriguez, Ghosh, Curtain, Flashman, & Roth, Reference Schwartz, Erdodi, Rodriguez, Ghosh, Curtain, Flashman and Roth2016).

Self-Report Scales

On the MAC-S self-report measure, participants rated their memory as significantly lower than same-age peers and reported an increased frequency of memory problems. However, overall self-ratings were not significantly associated with performance on any of the cognitive measures on which participants showed difficulty (i.e., PASAT, Consonant Trigrams, and the proactive interference measure on the CVLT-II), rs = .01–.37, ps = .067–.975 (see Table 3).

Table 3. Correlations between cognitive and mood ratings and complex attention measures

Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients between participants’ self-ratings of memory and attention ability and frequency, as measured by the Memory Assessment Clinic Self-Rating Scale (MAC-S) questionnaire and their performance on complex attention measures (i.e., PASAT, Consonant Trigrams, and proactive interference on CVLT). Pearson correlation coefficients are also presented between the complex attention measures and participants’ emotional state, as measured by the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) and Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI). Pearson correlation and point-biserial correlation coefficients between complex attention tasks and severity of illness (i.e., duration of hospitalization and oxygen use). Correlation coefficients significant at a .05 two-sided significance level (p < .05) were considered statistically significant.

* Statistically significant at p < .05.

** Statistically significant at p < .01.

Given that some MAC-S items probe behaviors that have a greater attentional involvement (i.e., Everyday Task-Oriented Memory and Attention/Concentration subscales), these items may be more specifically related to the cognitive performance observed in the sample (see Crook & Larrabee, Reference Crook and Larrabee1990). To explore this possibility, a second set of correlations was run with scores averaged from the Everyday Task-Oriented Memory subscale. A combined score of items comprising the Everyday Task-Oriented Memory ability factor of the MAC-S showed a medium-sized correlation with scores on the PASAT 2.4s. Similarly, the Everyday Task-Oriented Memory frequency factor showed medium to large correlations with performance on the PASAT 2.4s and PASAT 2.0s tasks. However, the Attention/Concentration factor itself was not significantly correlated with any of the more challenging complex attention tasks.

Depression and anxiety levels were uncorrelated with cognitive measures, rs = .01 – .39, ps = .054 – .962. Furthermore, depression and anxiety ratings were unrelated to each other, r(26) = .19, p = .342.

Subjective Complaints and Illness Severity

The most prominent cognitive complaints reported by participants on the interview included poor prospective memory, problems with concentration and distractibility, and word-finding difficulties (see Figure 1). Common physical complaints were breathlessness, muscle/joint pain, low energy (with decreased libido), fatigue, poor sleep, and weight loss (15–20 pounds). Overall, correlational analyses revealed that there was generally no significant relationship between subjective cognitive or physical complaints (i.e., problems with attention, fatigue, and pain) and illness severity (length of hospitalization, whether oxygen was given; see Table 4). However, there was a significant moderate relationship between self-reported reduced attention and duration of hospitalization.

Table 4. Correlations between illness severity and subjective complaints from interview

Point-biserial correlation coefficients between duration of hospitalization and subjective cognitive and physical complaints were obtained from the clinical interview. Chi-square coefficients between oxygen use and subjective complaints were obtained from the interview. Coefficients significant at a .05 two-sided significance level (p < .05) were considered statistically significant.

* Statistically significant at p < .05.

An additional analysis was conducted to examine if oxygen status influenced test findings given that a subset (i.e., 15 of 28) of participants had received oxygen. The data revealed that oxygen status significantly affected performance on (two of three) attention switching tasks only. Individuals who had not received oxygen significantly outperformed individuals who had received oxygen on the Switching subtests of DKEFS Verbal Fluency, t(26) =2.16, p = .04 and Stroop, t(26) = 2.92, p = .007, but not on the Trail Making Test, t(26) = .37, p = .71 (see Table 5 for each subtest’s mean SS and standard deviations). Of note, participants’ overall performance on switching tasks was within normal limits. Participants who received oxygen (M = 19.9, SD = 7.5, n = 15) also endorsed more symptoms of depression than those who did not receive oxygen (M = 14.5, SD = 3.8, n = 13), t(26) = −2.35, p = .027.

Table 5. Mean scaled scores for attention switching tasks for individuals who received and did not receive oxygen

D-KEFS = Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System; Stroop = Color–Word Interference Test.

DISCUSSION

This study examined the persistent neuropsychological sequelae among survivors of SARS-CoV-1. Individuals who recovered from the illness showed chronic cognitive difficulties 1.5 years post-illness on tasks of complex attention and working memory that rely on executive thinking skills. Specifically, below-expected performance was observed on complex attention measures that required participants to either maintain concentration in the face of interference or ignore irrelevant but once salient information to accurately complete a task. Poor target/nontarget discrimination and deficits in filtering information is also consistent with executive dysfunction. Additionally, participants who received oxygen had poorer performance on most attention switching tasks, though their overall performance on these tasks was within normal limits.

Furthermore, approximately half of the participants exhibited slower processing speed when performing tasks that required controlled attention and concentration, which is suggestive of increased mental effort. Participants performed within normal limits on simple measures of attention and concentration, immediate and delayed memory, reasoning skills, visuospatial ability, and language. Although some participants endorsed word-finding difficulties, the cultural bias inherent in the measure used to evaluate confrontation naming limited interpretation of performance on this measure. Participants also endorsed poorer memory ability than same-age peers, and mild to moderate symptoms of depression and anxiety. Participants who received oxygen endorsed more symptoms of depression than those who had not received oxygen.

It is worth noting that because testing was conducted in a highly controlled environment, it is possible that functional difficulties could be worse during real-world day-to-day experiences. Indeed, participants’ complaints about everyday attention-related memory slips were significantly correlated with one of the most challenging complex attention tasks given to them (i.e., PASAT). Arguably, complex attention, among other executive functions, is perhaps a more sensitive measure of real-world functioning. The importance of complex attention to daily functioning has been supported in a meta-analysis, which showed that executive function ability (including complex attention) is one of the most reliable predictors of return to work (among individuals with traumatic brain injury; Ownsworth & McKenna, Reference Ownsworth and McKenna2004).

The cause of persistent cognitive symptoms in SARS survivors is not fully understood, but the evidence is highly suggestive of hypoxemia, or hypoxia-induced brain injury. Hypoxic injury has been shown to affect such cognitive domains as attention and vigilance, memory, and executive functions (see Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Sepehry and Greer2018 for a review). Tests of sustained attention and memory are particularly sensitive to hypoxia, and there is a dose-dependent relationship between hypoxia and cognitive deficits (Stuss, Peterkin, Guzman, Guzman, & Troyer, Reference Stuss, Peterkin, Guzman, Guzman and Troyer1997). The current findings of a relative weakness on tasks of vigilance and self-monitoring among SARS survivors suggest that brain regions involved in the control of executive attention are compromised.

The present observed executive deficits can be mapped to Stuss’s theory of frontal lobe functioning that proposes a fractionation of processes: energization, monitoring, and task setting (Stuss, Reference Stuss2006; Henri-Bhargava, Stuss, & Freedman, Reference Henri-Bhargava, Stuss and Freedman2018). According to this theory, energization (or activation) is mediated by the superior medial frontal lobe region and is important for response initiation and maintenance. Individuals with lesions in the superior medial frontal lobe are more likely to have slower reaction times on tasks that recruit significant brain resources (Stuss, Reference Stuss2006). Vigilance and monitoring are important processes of the right lateral frontal lobe, whereas increased errors are observed with lesions in the left lateral prefrontal cortex due to poor criterion setting (Stuss, Reference Stuss2006, Reference Stuss2011). In the present SARS-CoV-1 sample, participants’ difficulty on a complex sustained attention task and slowed processing speed on another (possibly to maintain accuracy), was perhaps indicative of superior medial frontal lobe dysfunction. Indeed, this pattern of slowing is suggestive of difficulty maintaining alertness and is consistent with data on disrupted superior medial frontal lobe function (Stuss, Reference Stuss2006). The superior medial frontal lobe has been shown to be susceptible to dysfunction in hypoxic conditions (Rosenzweig et al., Reference Rosenzweig, Glasser, Polsek, Leschziner, Williams and Morrell2015). Furthermore, participants in the current study showed vigilance and monitoring errors on attentional tasks, which was suggestive of right lateral frontal lobe deficits.

Our results support recent research on the cognitive outcome of SARS-CoV-2, with the primary domain affected being complex attention (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Lu, Chen, Wei, Wang, Lyu and Hu2020). The findings also provide converging evidence with the extant literature on the impact of hypoxic illnesses such as sleep apnea, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), and ARDS on cognition (Hopkins et al., Reference Hopkins, Weaver, Collingridge, Parkinson, Chan and Orme2005; McMorris, Hale, Barwood, Costello, & Corbett, Reference McMorris, Hale, Barwood, Costello and Corbett2017; Mikkelsen et al., Reference Mikkelsen, Christie, Lanken, Biester, Thompson, Bellamy and Angus2012; Schou et al., Reference Schou, Østergaard, Rasmussen, Rydahl-Hansen and Phanareth2012; Sforza & Roche, Reference Sforza and Roche2012). Specifically, the cognitive sequelae of SARS were less severe than other hypoxic disorders, with mild deficits evident during complex attention and memory tasks only. Oxygen saturation levels were not available for all participants, and we were unable to determine if illness severity based on this factor correlated with performance on complex attention and memory tasks. Nonetheless, the data suggest a possible link between subjective reports of attentional errors and illness severity, as measured by duration of hospitalization. In addition, given the findings of reduced attention switching among the subset of participants who received oxygen, we speculate that the requirement of oxygen may represent an additional level of severity that is worth further investigation in future studies.

Importantly, the neurocognitive and mood-related difficulties found among the current sample were consistent with evidence from other studies on hypoxic illnesses (Gardner & Moallef, Reference Gardner and Moallef2015; Moldofsky & Patcai, Reference Moldofsky and Patcai2011; Mikkelsen et al., Reference Mikkelsen, Christie, Lanken, Biester, Thompson, Bellamy and Angus2012; Schou et al., Reference Schou, Østergaard, Rasmussen, Rydahl-Hansen and Phanareth2012; Sforza & Roche, Reference Sforza and Roche2012). The persistence of symptoms reported is in line with severe cases of influenza. That is, one study found that psychological impairment following recovery from H7N9 influenza persisted at 2 years (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Wu, Hao, Yang, Lu, Chen and Li2017), while another study found that a large proportion of individuals with H1N1 influenza were experiencing depression and anxiety, decreased quality of life, and an elevated risk of PTSD 1-year post-discharge (Luyt et al., Reference Luyt, Combes, Becquemin, Beigelman-Aubry, Hatem, Brun and Chastre2012). Although the underlying mechanism for persistent symptoms following recovery from influenza is unclear, Luyt and colleagues proposed that significant respiratory distress leading to hypoxemia may be one possible contributor. Thus, it is reasonable to believe that hypoxemia is an important differential for the SARS-CoV-1 neurocognitive profile.

Another consideration is the direct effect of mood and physical complaints on cognition. Meta-analytic studies have revealed significant effects of depression across measures of attention, processing speed, encoding and retrieval memory, and executive functions (Rock, Roiser, Riedel, & Blackwell, Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014; Zakzanis, Leach, & Kaplan, Reference Zakzanis, Leach and Kaplan1998). Similarly, neurocognitive deficits that are commonly present in individuals with a history of chronic pain include memory, processing speed, and attention (Higgins, Martin, Baker, Vasterling, & Risbrough, Reference Higgins, Martin, Baker, Vasterling and Risbrough2018). The triad of physical, cognitive, and psychological symptoms seen in respiratory illnesses (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Chesney, Oliver, Pollak, McGuire, Fusar-Poli and David2020; Riordan et al., Reference Riordan, Stika, Goldberg and Drzewiecki2020) are also found in clinical syndromes, such as post-concussion syndrome (PCS). Analogous to PCS, a number of moderating factors may influence the persistence of symptoms experienced by some SARS survivors, and also like PCS, it is unclear why some SARS survivors have ongoing symptoms and others do not (Marshall, Reference Marshall2020). PCS research suggests that full recovery may depend on such factors as injury severity, age, comorbidities, cognitive reserve prior to syndrome onset, presence of affective disturbance that prolongs and/or worsens symptoms, level of resiliency, environmental demands, and expectations (Cole & Bailie, Reference Cole, Bailie, Laskowitz and Grant2016; Lezak, Reference Lezak2012). It is possible that similar factors influence recovery from SARS.

It is possible that the persistent effects of mild hypoxia-producing conditions, such as SARS-CoV-1, constitute a syndrome. There may be moderating factors related to vulnerability and resiliency that put individuals at risk of developing persistent symptoms, as well as those that relate to protracted rates of recovery. For example, psychological distress, PTSD symptoms, level of support, and amount of time given to recover before being required to return to normal daily activities could all be potential contributors. Furthermore, the specific environment (e.g., healthcare) and/or sociocultural aspects may represent an additional contextual element. Unfortunately, we were unable to address whether or not there are sex differences in the impact of SARS given that our sample was over 90% female working in a female-dominated profession (80% of our sample were nurses; see Statistics Canada, 2005 for gender-based employment data). To our knowledge, the results from other publications examining the health impacts of SARS on affected individuals have not reported gender differences.

Nonetheless, the experience for SARS survivors in Toronto was found to be more psychologically distressing than for those living in a nearby city where the outbreak was not as significant (Maunder et al., Reference Maunder, Lancee, Balderson, Bennett, Borgundvaag, Evans and Wasylenki2006). Indeed, Moallef and colleagues (Reference Moallef, Lueke, Gardner and Patcai2021) recently published findings from a 4- and 7-year follow-up of Toronto healthcare workers who had contracted SARS. Their data revealed that SARS can have chronic psychological effects, particularly among healthcare workers who had client-facing roles that exposed them to the virus. Of the healthcare workers who had contracted SARS, 78% were still experiencing clinically significant symptoms of PTSD (avoidance and hyperarousal scores were moderately related to adverse functioning), approximately 50% had clinically significant symptoms of depression and anxiety, and 69% scored in the clinically impaired range on a measure of overall well-being (Moallef et al., Reference Moallef, Lueke, Gardner and Patcai2021).

Client-facing healthcare professionals had one of the lowest return-to-work rates among SARS survivors (Gardner & Moallef, Reference Gardner and Moallef2015; Kwek et al., Reference Kwek, Chew, Ong, Ng, Lee, Kaw and Leow2006; Moallef et al., Reference Moallef, Lueke, Gardner and Patcai2021; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Fang, Guan, Fan, Kong, Yao and Hoven2009). Thirty percent of healthcare professionals who survived SARS-CoV-1 did not return to work, whereas others resumed on modified duties because they were unable to keep up with the multiple demands of their role in the workplace (Ngai et al., Reference Ngai, Ko, Ng, To, Tong and Hui2010). Most of the participants in our sample were nurses, a role for which complex attention and executive functions are critical to monitor, track, and carry out intended tasks. In our sample, approximately 64% of healthcare professionals were not working at the time of the assessment. Although this rate is high, the sample is likely biased given it is based on survivors with persistent cognitive complaints that affected their ability to return to functioning at their premorbid level within the work environment. Moallef and colleagues (Reference Moallef, Lueke, Gardner and Patcai2021) revealed that even 7 years postinfection, 66% of healthcare professionals were either unemployed or on disability.

Unfortunately, our study is limited by a relatively small sample size and lack of comparison group. Future studies examining neurocognitive outcomes will benefit from the inclusion of a larger sample and control group in order to replicate these findings and enhance generalizability. As stated earlier, another study limitation is our predominantly female sample, which precluded an examination of gender differences in the health impacts of SARS on affected individuals. Lastly, a worthy investigation is the degree of SARS-related hypoxic injury on neurocognitive sequelae, particularly complex attention. Unfortunately, we did not have oxygen saturation levels to examine this relationship. A longitudinal sample is warranted to explore gender differences and oxygen saturation levels among SARS survivors.

Nonetheless, the current findings provide objective evidence of cognitive deficiencies in SARS survivors who experience subjective difficulties post-recovery. Whereas, the current literature accounts for the long-term physical and psychological effects of SARS, our study points to the need to examine the prevalence and potential persistence of cognitive deficits associated with coronavirus and other severe infectious diseases. Indeed, the past outbreak of another coronavirus, MERS (Butler, Reference Butler2015; Hilgenfeld & Peiris, Reference Hilgenfeld and Peiris2013; WHO, 2013), and SARS-CoV-2, illustrate that similar-acting viruses do sporadically appear, which underscores the relevance of this research. The prediction of the long-term neuropsychological outcomes of coronavirus infections is especially important now given the large number of infections due to the current ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. Importantly, our interpretation of these findings is not based on causal inference, rather it is based on the discovery of an association between ongoing subjective and objective cognitive difficulties and having experienced probable SARS.

Neurocognitive assessments are necessary to ensure that accommodations are set in place for individuals to return to employment that is commensurate with their capacity. The cognitive effects of SARS beyond 2 years are unknown. However, other hypoxic conditions such as obstructive sleep apnea put patients at greater risk of developing mild cognitive impairment at a later age, and it has been suggested that its effects may be due to an acceleration of the aging process (Sforza & Roche, Reference Sforza and Roche2012). Early evaluation of individuals recovering from hypoxic conditions should assess physical, psychological, and neurocognitive functioning. It is with heightened awareness and comprehensive evaluation that we may identify symptoms that benefit from treatment, such as poor mood, sleep deprivation, and pain. Consequently, we can more effectively intervene with the goal of preventing, reversing, or reducing cognitive decline and improving the overall well-being of affected individuals.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Drs. Kathryn Stokes, Dmytro Rewilak, Eva Svoboda, and Brent Hayman-Abello for their contributions to the development of the neuropsychological test battery and/or assessment of participants.

Financial Support

This research did not receive financial support.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

Footnotes

1 In 2002/3, there was “possible SARS” and “probable SARS”, with criteria for each. Mild cases did not meet the criteria. There were also no criteria for a definitive diagnosis of “SARS;” “probable SARS” was the closest to this and all participants met these criteria (i.e., severe respiratory illness with likely exposure to a laboratory-confirmed case; CDC, 2003).

References

REFERENCES

Adhikari, N.K., Tansey, C.M., McAndrews, M.P., Matté, A., Pinto, R., Cheung, A.M., … Herridge, M.S. (2011). Self-reported depressive symptoms and memory complaints in survivors five years after ARDS. Chest, 140(6), 14841493. https://doi.org/10.1378/chest.11-1667 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A.T. & Steer, R.A. (1993). Beck Anxiety Inventory manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G.K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-II . San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation, 78, 490498.Google Scholar
Bohmwald, K., Gálvez, N.M.S., Ríos, M., & Kalergis, A.M. (2018). Neurologic alterations due to respiratory virus infections. In Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience (Vol. 12). Frontiers Media S.A. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2018.00386 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burmester, B., Leathem, J., & Merrick, P. (2016). Subjective cognitive complaints and objective cognitive function in aging: A systematic review and meta-analysis of recent cross-sectional findings. Neuropsychology Review, 26(4), 376393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, D. (2015). South Korean MERS outbreak is not a global threat. Nature. https://www.nature.com/news/south-korean-mers-outbreak-is-not-a-global-threat-1.17709. Accessed 5 Jul 2016.Google Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2003). Public Health Guidance for Community-Level Preparedness and Response to Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome (SARS) Draft–October 2003.Google Scholar
Chan, J.C.K. (2005). Recovery pathway of post-SARS patients. Thorax, 60, 361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Wu, J., Hao, S., Yang, M., Lu, X., Chen, X., & Li, L. (2017). Long term outcomes in survivors of epidemic Influenza A (H7N9) virus infection. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17497-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, T.B., Lin, C.Y., Lin, K.N., Yeh, Y.C., Chen, W.T., Wang, K.S., & Wang, P.N. (2014). Culture qualitatively but not quantitatively influences performance in the Boston Naming Test in a Chinese-speaking population. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 4, 8694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, W.R. & Bailie, J.M. (2016). Neurocognitive and psychiatric symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury. In Laskowitz, D. & Grant, G. (Eds.), Translational Research in Traumatic Brain Injury (Chapter 19). Boca Raton: CRC Press/Taylor and Francis Group.Google ScholarPubMed
Crook, T.H., & Larrabee, G.J. (1990). A self-rating scale for evaluating memory in everyday life. Psychology and Aging, 5, 4857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (1987). California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Gardner, P.J. & Moallef, P. (2015). Psychological impact on SARS survivors: Critical review of the English language literature. Canadian Psychology, 56, 123135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, G.D., Watson, R.K., & Deptula, D. (1987). Neuropsychological dysfunction in sleep apnea. Sleep, 10, 254–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gronwall, D.M. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henri-Bhargava, A., Stuss, D.T., & Freedman, M. (2018). Clinical assessment of prefrontal lobe functions. CONTINUUM: Lifelong Learning in Neurology, 24(3), 704726.Google ScholarPubMed
Herridge, M.S., Cheung, A.M., Tansey, C.M., Matte-Martyn, A., Diaz-Granados, N., Al-Saidi, F., … Slutsky, A.S. (2003). One-year outcomes in survivors of the acute respiratory distress syndrome. The New England Journal of Medicine, 348, 683693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, D.M., Martin, A.M., Baker, D.G., Vasterling, J.J., & Risbrough, V. (2018). The relationship between chronic pain and neurocognitive function: a systematic review. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 34(3), 262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilgenfeld, R. & Peiris, M. (2013). From SARS to MERS: 10 years of research on highly pathogenic human coronaviruses. Antiviral Research, 100, 286295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, R.O., Larson-Lohr, V., Weaver, L.K., & Bigler, E.D. (1998). Neuropsychological impairments following hantavirus pulmonary syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 190196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, R.O., Weaver, L.K., Collingridge, D., Parkinson, R.B., Chan, K.J., & Orme, J.F. (2005) Two-year cognitive, emotional, and quality-of-life outcomes in acute respiratory distress syndrome. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 171, 340347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irani, F., Barbone, J.M., Beausoleil, J., & Gerald, L. (2017). Is asthma associated with cognitive impairments? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 14.Google Scholar
Kamps, B.S. & Hoffman, C. (2004). SARS Reference. http://www.sarsreference.com. Accessed 4 Jan 2016.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Kim, J. S. (2016). Post-stroke mood and emotional disturbances: pharmacological therapy based on mechanisms. Journal Stroke, 18, 244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwek, S.K., Chew, W.M., Ong, K.C., Ng, A.W.K., Lee, L.S.U., Kaw, G., & Leow, M.K.S. (2006). Quality of life and psychological status in survivors of severe acute respiratory syndrome at 3 months postdischarge. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 513519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, M.H.B., Wing, Y.K., Yu, M.W.M., Leung, C.M., Ma, R.C., Kong, A.P., … Lam, S.P. (2009) Mental morbidities and chronic fatigue in severe acute respiratory syndrome survivors: long-term follow-up. Archives of Internal Medicine, 169, 21422147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M.D. (2012). Neuropathology for Neuropsychologists. In Neuropsychological assessment (Fifth edition). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Luyt, C.E., Combes, A., Becquemin, M.H., Beigelman-Aubry, C., Hatem, S., Brun, A.L., … Chastre, J. (2012). Long-term outcomes of pandemic 2009 influenza A(H1N1)-associated severe ARDS. Chest, 142(3), 583592. https://doi.org/10.1378/chest.11-2196 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mak, I.W.C., Chu, C.M., Pan, P.C., Yiu, M.G.C., & Chan, V.L. (2009). Long-term psychiatric morbidities among SARS survivors. General Hospital Psychiatry, 31(4), 318326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mariën, P., Mampaey, E., Vervaet, A., Saerens, J., & De Deyn, P.P. (1998). Normative data for the Boston Naming Test in native Dutch-speaking Belgian elderly. Brain and language, 65(3), 447467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, M. (2020). The lasting misery of coronavirus long-haulers. Nature, 585(7825), 339341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maunder, R.G., Lancee, W.J., Balderson, K.E., Bennett, J.P., Borgundvaag, B., Evans, S., … Wasylenki, D.A. (2006). Long-term psychological and occupational effects of providing hospital healthcare during SARS outbreak. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 12, 19241932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMorris, T., Hale, B.J., Barwood, M., Costello, J., & Corbett, J. (2017). Effect of acute hypoxia on cognition: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 74, 225232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikkelsen, M.E., Christie, J.D., Lanken, P.N., Biester, R.C., Thompson, B.T., Bellamy, S.L., … Angus, D.C. (2012). The adult respiratory distress syndrome cognitive outcomes study: long-term neuropsychological function in survivors of acute lung injury. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 185, 13071315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moallef, P., Lueke, N.A., Gardner, P.J., & Patcai, J. (2021). Chronic PTSD and other psychological sequelae in a group of frontline healthcare workers who contracted and survived SARS. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moldofsky, H., & Patcai, J. (2011). Chronic widespread musculoskeletal pain, fatigue, depression and disordered sleep in chronic post-SARS syndrome; A case-controlled study. BMC Neurology, 11, 37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, M.P., Richardson, S.E., McGeer, A., Dresser, L., Raboud, J., Mazzulli, T., … Louie, M. (2006). Early diagnosis of SARS: lessons from the Toronto SARS outbreak. European Journal of Clinical Microbiology and Infectious Diseases, 25, 230237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naylor, D., Basrur, S., Bergeron, M.G., Brunham, R.C., Butler-Jones, D., & Dafoe, G. (2003). Chapter 2 - SARS in Canada: Anatomy of an Outbreak. In National Advisory Committee on SARS and Public Health. Learning from SARS: renewal of public health in Canada (pp. 2342). Ottawa: Health Canada. http://www.phac-aspc.gc.ca/publicat/sars-sras/naylor/. Accessed 5 Jul 2016.Google Scholar
Negrini, F., Ferrario, I., Mazziotti, D., Berchicci, M., Bonazzi, M., de Sire, A., … Zapparoli, L. (2020). Neuropsychological features of severe hospitalized COVID-19 patients at clinical stability and clues for post-acute rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2020.09.376 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ngai, J.C., Ko, F.W., Ng, S.S., To, K.W., Tong, M., & Hui, D.S. (2010). The long-term impact of severe acute respiratory syndrome on pulmonary function, exercise capacity and health status. Respirology, 15, 543550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Farrell, E., MacKenzie, J., & Collins, B. (2013). Clearing the air: a review of our current understanding of “chemo fog”. Current Oncology Reports, 15, 260269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ownsworth, T. & McKenna, K. (2004). Investigation of factors related to employment outcome following traumatic brain injury: A critical review and conceptual model. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26(13), 765784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabinovitz, B., Jaywant, A., & Fridman, C. B. (2020). Neuropsychological functioning in severe acute respiratory disorders caused by the coronavirus: implications for the current COVID-19 pandemic. Clinical Neuropsychologist. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2020.1803408 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riordan, P., Stika, M., Goldberg, J., & Drzewiecki, M. (2020). COVID-19 and clinical neuropsychology: A review of neuropsychological literature on acute and chronic pulmonary disease. In Clinical Neuropsychologist. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2020.1810325 Google Scholar
Ritchie, K., Chan, D., & Watermeyer, T. (2020). The cognitive consequences of the COVID-19 epidemic: collateral damage? Brain Communications, 2(2). https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcaa069 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, P.M. & Doucet, N. (2011). Performance of French-speaking Quebec adults on the Boston Naming Test. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology & Audiology, 35(3), 254–267.Google Scholar
Robertson, I.H., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). The Test of Everyday Attention (TEA). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Rock, P.L., Roiser, J.P., Riedel, W.J., & Blackwell, A.D. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(10), 2029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, J.P., Chesney, E., Oliver, D., Pollak, T.A., McGuire, P., Fusar-Poli, P., … David, A.S. (2020). Psychiatric and neuropsychiatric presentations associated with severe coronavirus infections: a systematic review and meta-analysis with comparison to the COVID-19 pandemic. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(7), 611627. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30203-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenzweig, I., Glasser, M., Polsek, D., Leschziner, G.D., Williams, S.C., & Morrell, M.J. (2015). Sleep apnoea and the brain: a complex relationship. The Lancet Respiratory Medicine, 3(5), 404414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, R.M., Niemann, H., Allen, C.C., Farrow, C.E., & Wylie, T. (1992). The Ruff 2 and 7 selective attention test: a neuropsychological application. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, Suppl 3, 13111319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schou, L., Østergaard, B., Rasmussen, L.S., Rydahl-Hansen, S., & Phanareth, K. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease–a systematic review. Respiratory Medicine, 106, 10711081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, I.Z., Sepehry, A.A., & Greer, S.C. (2018). Anoxia-Hypoxia in Forensic Neuropsychological Assessment: Cognitive Impact of Pulmonary Injuries, Respiratory Distress, Cerebral Blood Hypoperfusion, and Major Surgeries. Psychological Injury and Law, 11, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, E.S., Erdodi, L., Rodriguez, N., Ghosh, J.J., Curtain, J.R., Flashman, L.A., & Roth, R.M. (2016). CVLT-II forced choice recognition trial as an embedded validity indicator: A systematic review of the evidence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 22(8), 851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sforza, E. & Roche, F. (2012). Sleep apnea syndrome and cognition. Frontiers in Neurology, 3, 87. doi 10.3389/fneur.2012.00087 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheng, B., Cheng, S.K.W., Lau, K.K., Li, H.L., & Chan, E.L.Y. (2005). The effects of disease severity, use of corticosteroids and social factors on neuropsychiatric complaints in severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) patients at acute and convalescent phases. European Psychiatry, 20, 236242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, R.A., Singer, E.A., Duke, L.M., Singer, N.G., Morey, C.E., Daughtrey, E.W., & Kaplan, E. (1994). The Boston qualitative scoring system for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Description and interrater reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 8, 309322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stranks, E.K. & Crowe, S.F. (2016). The cognitive effects of obstructive sleep apnea: an updated meta-analysis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31, 186193.Google ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D.T. (2006). Frontal lobes and attention: processes and networks, fractionation and integration. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 12(2), 261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D.T. (2011). Functions of the frontal lobes: relation to executive functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 17(5), 759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D.T., Peterkin, I., Guzman, D.A., Guzman, C., & Troyer, A.K. (1997). Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: effects of hypoxia on neurological and neuropsychological measures. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(4), 515524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suhr, J.A. (2003). Neuropsychological impairment in fibromyalgia: relation to depression, fatigue, and pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55, 321329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallberg, I.M. (2005). The Boston naming test in Swedish: normative data. Brain and Language, 94(1), 1931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thakur, N., Blanc, P.D., Julian, L.J., Yelin, E.H., Katz, P.P., Sidney, S., … Eisner, M.D. (2010). COPD and cognitive impairment: the role of hypoxemia and oxygen therapy. International Journal of Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease, 5, 263269.Google ScholarPubMed
Troyer, A.K., & Murphy, K.J. (2007). Memory for intentions in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Time-and event-based prospective memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 13, 365369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wing, Y., & Leung, C.M. (2012). Mental health impact of severe acute respiratory syndrome: a prospective study. Hong Kong Medical Journal, 18, 2427.Google ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2013). Novel coronavirus infection - update (Middle East respiratory syndrome- coronavirus). http://www.who.int/csr/don/2013_05_23_ncov/en. Accessed 19 Sept 2016.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2015). Summary of probable SARS cases with onset of illness from 1 November 2002 to 31 July 2003. https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/summary-of-probable-sars-cases-with-onset-of-illness-from-1-november-2002-to-31-july-2003. Accessed 5 Jul 2016.Google Scholar
Wu, P., Fang, Y., Guan, Z., Fan, B., Kong, J., Yao, Z., … Hoven, C.W. (2009). The psychological impact of the SARS epidemic on hospital employees in China: Exposure, risk perception, and altruistic acceptance of risk. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 302311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zakzanis, K.K., Leach, L., & Kaplan, E. (1998). On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 11(3), 111119.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhou, H., Lu, S., Chen, J., Wei, N., Wang, D., Lyu, H., … Hu, S. (2020). The landscape of cognitive function in recovered COVID-19 patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 129, 98102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.06.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Common difficulties reported by SARS patients 1.5 years post-recovery.Common cognitive, physical, and mood complaints were reported by 28 healthcare professionals 1.5 years post-recovery from SARS.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and psychometric data for SARS patients (n = 28)

Figure 2

Table 2. Neuropsychological test battery

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Neurocognitive profile of SARS patients 1.5 years post-recovery.Bars represent aggregate neuropsychological test scores from 28 SARS participants. Red dotted line = 1 standard deviation below the expected mean. Green dotted line = 1 standard deviation above the mean. Sp = span; Fwd = forward; Num = number; W = word; C = color; Spd = speed; Acc = accuracy; TEA = Test of Everyday Attention; PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; Con Tri = Consonant Trigrams; Bwd = backward; Switch = switching; Matrix Reason = Matrix Reasoning; RCFT DR = Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test delayed recall; WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale; LM = Logical Memory; Reten = retention; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; SD = short delay; FR = free recall; CR = cued recall; LD = long delay; Recog = recognition; MAC-S = Memory Assessment Clinic Self-Rating Scale; freq = frequency.

Figure 4

Table 3. Correlations between cognitive and mood ratings and complex attention measures

Figure 5

Table 4. Correlations between illness severity and subjective complaints from interview

Figure 6

Table 5. Mean scaled scores for attention switching tasks for individuals who received and did not receive oxygen