INTRODUCTION
Over the past few decades, numerous studies have highlighted metacognitive deficits as an important clinical symptom in various neurocognitive disorders including stroke, head injury, Parkinson’s (PD), and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (e.g., Barrett, Eslinger, Ballentine, & Heilman, Reference Barrett, Eslinger, Ballentine and Heilman2005; Broadbent, Cooper, FitzGerald, & Parkes, Reference Broadbent, Cooper, FitzGerald and Parkes1982; Cosentino & Stern, Reference Cosentino and Stern2005; McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Drachman, Folstein, Katzman, Price and Stadlan1984; Nelson & Narens, Reference Nelson and Narens1990; Souchay, Isingrini, Pillon, & Gil, Reference Souchay, Isingrini, Pillon and Gil2003). Metamemory, which is the mainstay of metacognitive research, refers to the processes whereby people are able to examine the content of their memories, either prospectively or retrospectively, and make judgments or commentaries about them (Metcalfe & Dunlosky, Reference Metcalfe and Dunlosky2008).
Of note, there is no single method, or gold standard, for measuring metamemory; rather, metamemory has been operationally defined in a variety of ways, not merely in relation to lack of awareness of memory impairment in the context of a task (e.g., failing to recall items on a specific list learning task), but also to behavioral and functional deficits in everyday life (e.g., forgetting appointments or forgetting what you came to the shop to buy). Moreover, awareness of such memory deficits can occur at multiple levels (Stuss, Picton, & Alexander, Reference Stuss, Picton and Alexander2001; Torres, Mackala, Kozicky, & Yatham, Reference Torres, Mackala, Kozicky and Yatham2016).
Two key components of metacognition that have been identified include metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive experience (Flavell, Reference Reed, Jagust and Coulter1979; Perfect & Schwartz, Reference Perfect and Schwartz2002; Torres et al., Reference Torres, Mackala, Kozicky and Yatham2016). Metacognitive knowledge refers generally to beliefs that an individual has about his or her own cognitive functioning. The classic way of assessing metacognitive knowledge is through evaluation of a person’s perceived cognitive skills or problems, often through the use of general ratings or self-report questionnaires (Bacon, Huet, & Danion, Reference Bacon, Huet and Danion2011; Dixon, Hultsch, & Hertzog, Reference Dixon, Hultsch and Hertzog1988; Goverover, Genova, Griswold, Chiaravalloti, & DeLuca, Reference Goverover, Genova, Griswold, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2014). In contrast, metacognitive experience is tied to a person’s ability to monitor their ongoing or online cognitive performance on a specific task (Torres et al., Reference Torres, Mackala, Kozicky and Yatham2016).
In their seminal papers, Nelson and Narens (Reference Nelson and Narens1990, Reference Nelson and Narens1994) describe four classic types of judgments that have been used to measure memory awareness and have subsequently formed the core of traditional experimental metamemory research: (1) Ease-of-learning (EOL) judgments, which occur in advance of information acquisition, are largely inferential, and pertain to items that have not yet been learned. These judgments are predictions about what will be easy or difficult to learn, either in terms of which items will be easiest or in terms of which strategies will make learning easiest; (2) Judgments of learning (JOL) occur during or after acquisition and are predictions about future test performance on currently recallable items; (3) Feeling-of-knowing (FOK) or tip-of-the-tongue judgments occur during or after acquisition (e.g., during a retention session) and are judgments about whether a currently non-recallable item is known and/or will be identified on a subsequent recognition test; and (4) Confidence/Accuracy judgments, which refer to the relationship between a person’s perception of their performance and their objective ability. These paradigms generate many objective metacognitive metrics that can be calculated to measure the relationship between a person’s predicted or perceived performance and their objective ability.
Another frequently used approach to assessing metacognitive knowledge, which has been implemented by other researchers (e.g., Torres et al., Reference Torres, Mackala, Kozicky and Yatham2016; Graham, Kunik, Doody, & Snow, Reference Graham, Kunik, Doody and Snow2005; Helmstaedter, Hauff, & Elger, Reference Helmstaedter, Hauff and Elger1998) and applied in the current study, is to examine the extent to which individuals are generally over or under confident in their perception of their cognitive functioning. This methodology for evaluating memory awareness involves measuring the discrepancy between a person’s true, objective memory performance, and stated confidence in his or her memory functioning (i.e., assessed offline in the context of a subjective memory complaints questionnaire as is done in the current study). Importantly, metacognitive researchers have suggested there are many flavors of metacognitive report, but all involve the elicitation of subjective beliefs about one’s own cognition (Fleming and Dolan, Reference Fleming and Dolan2014).
In AD, the reported prevalence of impaired metamemory ranges between 25% (Reed, Jagust, & Coulter, Reference Reed, Jagust and Coulter1993) and more than 80% of individuals (Conde-Sala et al., Reference Conde-Sala, Rene-Ramirez, Turró-Garriga, Gascon-Bayarri, Juncadella-Puig, Moreno-Cordon and Garre-Olmo2013); some of whom may engage in activities well beyond their true functional capacity (Starkstein, Jorge, Mizrahi, Adrian, & Robinson, Reference Starkstein, Jorge, Mizrahi, Adrian and Robinson2007). Of note, differences in criteria, point of measurement, and assessment methods, as well as the multidimensional aspect of awareness contribute to variations in the reported prevalence. In fact, there is considerable variability in the presentation and severity of disordered metamemory among those with and without significant cognitive impairment, with unawareness ranging from slight minimization to complete denial of problems (Clare, Marková, Verhey, & Kenny, Reference Clare, Marková, Verhey and Kenny2005; Leicht, Berwig, & Gertz, Reference Leicht, Berwig and Gertz2010; Metcalfe and Dunlosky, Reference Metcalfe and Dunlosky2008).
Deficits in metacognition, in particular metamemory, have been associated with an increase in both dangerous behaviors and neuropsychiatric symptoms in AD (Starkstein et al., Reference Starkstein, Jorge, Mizrahi, Adrian and Robinson2007; Vogel, Hasselbalch, Gade, Ziebell, & Waldemar, Reference Vogel, Hasselbalch, Gade, Ziebell and Waldemar2005; Zanetti et al., Reference Zanetti, Vallotti, Frisoni, Geroldi, Bianchetti, Pasqualetti and Trabucchi1999). For example, studies have shown that overestimation of, or overconfidence in memory functioning may moderate the association between AD and a higher frequency of motor vehicle accidents (Hunt, Morris, Edwards, & Wilson, Reference Hunt, Morris, Edwards and Wilson1993; Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Carr, Duchek, Grant, Buckles and Morris1997). Research has also indicated that memory deficits may result in patients taking repeated doses of potentially toxic medications, forgetting they left the stove on, and leaving the front door open—a dangerous event in cities with a high-crime rate (Duchek, Hunt, Ball, Buckles, & Morris, Reference Duchek, Hunt, Ball, Buckles and Morris1997; Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Morris, Edwards and Wilson1993, Reference Hunt, Carr, Duchek, Grant, Buckles and Morris1997); however, to the extent that patients are accurate in their estimation of their cognitive abilities, they may take steps to prevent such incidents from occurring.
In addition to concerns about patient safety, caregiver burden and mood disturbance also increase with loss of insight as patients become harder to manage, less compliant, and are forced to make the transition to higher levels of care (Kelleher, Tolea, & Galvin, Reference Kelleher, Tolea and Galvin2016; Turró-Garriga et al., Reference Turró-Garriga, Garre-Olmo, Vilalta-Franch, Conde-Sala, Blanco and López-Pousa2013). Impaired self-awareness in the direction of over-confidence can also prevent engagement in rehabilitation from brain injury or stroke (Jenkinson, Preston, & Ellis, Reference Jenkinson, Preston and Ellis2011; Prigatano, Reference Prigatano2005) as a person who is unaware of his or her deficits is likely to be unmotivated or uncooperative in therapy, set unrealistic goals, display poor judgment, and fail to see the need for compensatory strategies (Malec & Moessner, Reference Malec and Moessner2000; Ownsworth, Reference Ownsworth2005; Sherer, Bergloff, Boake, High, & Levin, Reference Sherer, Bergloff, Boake, High and Levin1998; Sherer, Oden, Bergloff, Levin, & High, Reference Sherer, Oden, Bergloff, Levin and High1998; Simmond & Fleming, Reference Simmond and Fleming2003).
Overestimation of memory functioning has been shown to have important clinical implications for both the patient and caregiver; indeed the same may hold true for underestimation of one’s abilities. Disordered metamemory, in the direction of under-confidence, among older adults may have particular implications for the diagnosis of subjective cognitive decline (SCD), a state hypothesized to precede objectively apparent cognitive symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease, and hold promise as a preclinical indicator of AD (Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Villemagne, Masters, Ellis, Rowe, Johnson, Sperling and Amariglio2016). SCD is generally characterized by subjective memory deficits, which may or may not be indicative of an underlying degenerative illness.
Recent work has suggested that, in the context of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), reliance on subjective memory complaints as a diagnostic criterion contributes to false classifications among the worried well (Edmonds, Delano-Wood, Galasko, Salmon, & Bondi, Reference Edmonds, Delano-Wood, Galasko, Salmon and Bondi2014; Jessen et al., Reference Jessen, Amariglio, van Boxtel, Breteler, Ceccaldi, Chetelat and Wagner2014). Furthermore, studies have long shown an inconsistent relationship between subjective memory complaints (SMC) and objective memory performance (OMP) among participants with MCI (Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Saling, Ames, Rowe, Lautenschlager, Macaulay and Ellis2013; Lenehan, Klekociuk, & Summers, Reference Lenehan, Klekociuk and Summers2012; Roberts, Clare, & Woods, Reference Roberts, Clare and Woods2009; Studer, Donati, Popp, & von Gunten, Reference Studer, Donati, Popp and von Gunten2014). Such studies argue that the finding that patient self-reports of cognitive functioning correspond poorly to actual cognitive functioning supports the hypothesis of diminished metacognitive knowledge (Torres et al., Reference Torres, Mackala, Kozicky and Yatham2016).
According to Edmonds and colleagues (Reference Edmonds, Delano-Wood, Galasko, Salmon and Bondi2014), there are multiple factors that could account for this disparity between SMCs and OMP, including the possibility that the relationship between these two cognitive constructs is moderated by emotional factors and personality features (Reid & Maclullich, Reference Reid and Maclullich2006; Studer et al., Reference Studer, Donati, Popp and von Gunten2014). However, it is also worth noting that this discrepancy may be explained in part by poor veridicality or verisimilitude between subjective memory complaints questionnaires and objective neuropsychological measures of memory (See Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecombe, Reference Chaytor and Schmitter-Edgecombe2003 for a review).
Indeed, in non-demented older adults, researchers have demonstrated that psychological variables are associated with the perception of cognition, SMCs, and objective cognitive performance (OCP) individually (e.g., Comijs, Deeg, Dik, Twisk, & Jonker, Reference Comijs, Deeg, Dik, Twisk and Jonker2002; Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Negash, Sammel, Bogner, Harel, Livney and Arnold2013). Specifically, meta-analyses have revealed associations between increased SMCs and affective distress, depression, and anxiety, even when objective cognitive measures were normal (Balash et al., Reference Balash, Mordechovich, Shabtai, Giladi, Gurevich and Korczyn2013; Binder, Storzbach, Rohlman, Campbell, & Anger, Reference Binder, Storzbach, Rohlman, Campbell and Anger1999; Pereira et al., Reference Pereira, Yassuda, Oliveira, Diniz, Radanovic, Talib and Forlenza2010). Due to the multifarious approaches to personality research, the literature is large, but poorly integrated (e.g., Pearman & Storandt, Reference Pearman and Storandt2004).
There does not appear to be consensus regarding the relationship between SMCs and personality traits; however, many studies have suggested that individuals low in extraversion and high in neuroticism, for example, tend to report more complaints about their memory (Geerlings, Jonker, Bouter, Adèr, & Schmand, Reference Geerlings, Jonker, Bouter, Adèr and Schmand1999; Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Negash, Sammel, Bogner, Harel, Livney and Arnold2013). Thus, while numerous studies have demonstrated the association between psychological variables and subjective cognition (SC), to the best of our knowledge, no research study has examined the relationship between psychological variables and the accuracy of SC (i.e., metacognition) in non-demented older adults. Clare, Nelils, and colleagues (Reference Clare, Nelis, Martyr, Roberts, Whitaker, Marková and Morris2011) examined this question in dementia, finding that a larger discrepancy score between self-ratings of objective performance and caregiver report (indicating lower awareness) was associated with higher levels of anxiety and depression. However, such studies have typically used only subjective ratings of cognitive functioning (i.e., clinical ratings of Anosognosia or a discrepancy score between caregiver and self-ratings) rather than comparing self-report to an objective measure of performance (Clare, Marková, Roth, & Morris, Reference Clare, Marková, Roth and Morris2011; Clare, Whitaker, et al., Reference Clare, Whitaker, Nelis, Martyr, Marková, Roth and Morris2011).
While emerging work is revealing the neuroanatomic substrates of metamemory in aging and degenerative diseases, little is known about the extent to which premorbid, psychological factors such as mood and personality traits influence metamemory accuracy. Consequently, the current study addresses whether a composite of both personality and mood (CPM) is associated with an offline, objective measure of metamemory in a community-based sample of older adults. We hypothesized that we would uncover distinct patterns of personality and mood in relation to metamemory confidence accuracy.
In consideration of previous research, we postulated that (A) patterns of high extraversion, low anxiety and depression, and low neuroticism would be associated with more accurate metamemory; while (B) patterns of low extraversion, high anxiety and depression, and high neuroticism would be associated with less accurate metamemory in the direction of under-confidence in cognitive functioning. Furthermore, consistent with the literature, we hypothesized that (C) demographic factors, including age, education, and gender might moderate these associations.
METHOD
Participants
Data were obtained from the Nathan Kline Institute Rockland Sample Initiative (NKI-RSI), a community-ascertained lifespan sample (approx. N=1200) from the northern suburban area of New York City, USA. Ethnic and economic demographics of Rockland, and the surrounding counties, resemble those of the United States (U.S. Census Bureau, 2009), increasing the generalizability of the NKI-RSI to the broader U.S. population. Participants were recruited via advertisements flyer mailings, posting of materials in local shops, community talks, street fairs, and various meeting places. Enrollment efforts were used to avoid over-representation of any portion of the community, and to ensure faithful representation of Rockland County. For a full description of the methodology of collection and sampling procedures, see Nooner and colleagues (Reference Nooner, Colcombe, Tobe, Mennes, Benedict, Moreno and Milham2012). This study is in compliance with the Columbia University Institutional Review Board.
NKI-RSI exclusion criteria
General NKI-RSI study criteria included residents of Rockland, Bergen, Orange and Westchester counties, aged 6–85, who were fluent in English with capacity to understand the study and provide informed consent. General NKI-RSI exclusions were assessed over a screening phone call or determined at the time of study participation by the research team, and included chronic medical illness, history of neoplasia requiring intrathecal chemotherapy or focal cranial irradiation, history of leukomalacia or static encephalopathy, other serious neurological (specific or focal) or metabolic disorders, including epilepsy (except for resolved febrile seizures), history of traumatic brain injury, stroke, aneurysm, HIV, carotid artery stenosis, encephalitis, dementia, Huntington’s disease, Parkinson’s, hospitalization within the past month, contraindication for MRI scanning (metal implants, pacemakers, claustrophobia, metal foreign bodies or pregnancy), or inability to ambulate independently.
Individuals with an estimated FSIQ below 66 (WASI-2nd Edition; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011) determined at study visit were excluded from the study. Other exclusionary criteria included acute unipolar depression, bipolar disorder, autism spectrum disorders, psychosis, or suicidal/homicidal ideation; a history of chronic or acute substance dependence disorder; history of psychiatric hospitalization, and suicide attempts requiring medical intervention, which were determined through self-report at screening or at study visit via diagnostic interview (SCID-I/NP) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams2002). The current study participants were drawn from the larger NKI-RSI data set and consist of a community-representative sample of 157 older adults aged 50–85 years, (mean 64.48; standard deviation [SD]=8.79; median=65.00 years). A score of 23 or above on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Trzepacz, Hochstetler, Wang, Walker, & Saykin, Reference Trzepacz, Hochstetler, Wang, Walker and Saykin2015) was also required for inclusion in these analyses. For full demographics and clinical information about the sample, see Table 1.
Table 1 Demographic, cognitive, and clinical information
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20180423060913735-0646:S1355617717001345:S1355617717001345_tab1.gif?pub-status=live)
a RAVLT scores in the sample are calculated based on published, age-adjusted normative data (Roberts & Schmidt, Reference Roberts and Schmidt1996).
RAVLT=Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test.
Measures
Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ)
The CFQ is a 25-item self-report questionnaire assessing failures in perception, memory, and motor function in the completion of everyday tasks in the past 6 months. Individuals are asked to rate the frequency of experiences and behaviors on a 5-point scale: 0-Never, 1-Very rarely, 2-Occasionally, 3-Quite often, 4-Very often. A higher score on the CFQ suggests that the individual has a higher number of cognitive complaints (Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Cooper, FitzGerald and Parkes1982).
Cognitive Failures Questionnaire-Memory (CFQ-M)
For the purposes of the current study, a seven-item subscale was compiled on both theoretical and empirical bases to assess judgments specifically about the frequency of memory failures. Seven items (2, 6, 12, 16, 17, 20, 23) were selected based on their content as memory-related (i.e., Do you find you forget appointments?), and their overlap with items included in a validated memory complaint scale (Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire; Youn et al., Reference Youn, Kim, Lee, Jhoo, Lee, Park and Woo2009).
Additionally, a Confirmatory Factor Analysis in the current study found that these seven items loaded highly on one factor (Table 2). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was .78, above the recommended value of .6, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2(21)=227.43; p<.01). Individuals’ total score was calculated as the sum of the seven items. This score was converted to a Z-score based on the mean and SD of the current sample and, for ease of interpretation, the score was then inverted such that higher Z-scores suggest that the individual perceives him or herself as higher functioning or having fewer memory complaints.
Table 2 Principal component matrix of seven memory-related items (CFQ-M) from the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (N=157)
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20180423060913735-0646:S1355617717001345:S1355617717001345_tab2.gif?pub-status=live)
Note. COGFQ=Cognitive Failures Questionnaire.
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)
The RAVLT (Rey, Reference Rey1941) is a sensitive and commonly used measure of verbal learning and episodic memory, assessing a person’s ability to encode, consolidate, and retrieve verbal information (see Roberts & Schmidt, Reference Roberts and Schmidt1996, for a review; for example, Bigler, Rosa, Schultz, Hall, & Harris, Reference Bigler, Rosa, Schultz, Hall and Harris1989; Butters, Wolfe, Martone, Granholm, & Cermak, Reference Butters, Wolfe, Martone, Granholm and Cermak1985). The measure has been shown to have strong psychometric properties, including convergent reliability and construct validity. Internal consistency is generally above 0.9 and it has a high correlation with other instruments for episodic memory evaluation, such as the California Verbal Learning Test (Stallings, Boake, & Sherer, Reference Stallings, Boake and Sherer1995). Performance on the RAVLT is reported to be generally insensitive to depression and anxiety (e.g., Davidoff et al., Reference Davidoff, Roth, Thomas, Doljanac, Dijkers, Berent and Yarkony1990; Query & Megran, Reference Query and Megran1983; Roberts & Schmidt, Reference Roberts and Schmidt1996; Schoenberg et al., Reference Schoenberg, Dawson, Duff, Patton, Scott and Adams2006).
During the test, the examiner reads aloud a list of 15 words. The participant is then asked to repeat all words from the list that he/she can remember. This procedure is carried out six times to create a total Immediate Recall score. After an interference trial and a 20-minute delay, the participant is again asked to recall as many words as possible from the first list (i.e., Delayed Recall score). Given that the primary variable of interest is the subjective perception of long-term memory, Delayed Recall on the RAVLT was used in our analyses. Raw scores were converted to Z-scores based on the mean and SD of the current sample.
Metamemory Knowledge Index (MKMI)
The global, offline MKMI was computed as the discrepancy between the CFQ-M and delayed RAVLT Z-scores (CFQ-M – RAVLT) with higher scores reflecting over confidence and lower scores reflecting under-confidence. We used an offline evaluation (i.e., a retrospective, crystallized notion of what their memory abilities are) as opposed to an online evaluation (i.e., perception of memory abilities while engaged in a memory task) of metamemory, due to its rich reflection of everyday, real world levels of awareness (Cosentino, Metcalfe, Cary, De Leon, & Karlawish, Reference Cosentino, Metcalfe, Cary, Leon and Karlawish2011; Fleming & Frith, Reference Fleming and Frith2014).
Personality measure
The NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI-3) is a 60-item psychological inventory that was used to assess the five major dimensions of personality: Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. Participants are asked to select the response that best represents their opinion on a 5-point scale: 0-Strongly Agree, 1-Agree, 2-Neutral, 3-Disagree, 4-Strongly Disagree. Scores were standardized, controlling for gender (McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa2010).
State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
The STAI is a 40-Item self-report questionnaire designed to separately evaluate state (A-State) and trait (A-Trait) anxiety in adults. Each 20-question measure uses ratings on a 4-point scale: 1-Almost never, 2-Sometimes, 3-Often, 4-Almost always. Affectivity ranges from immediate, transitory emotional states, through longer-lasting mood states, through dynamic motivational traits, ranging up to relatively enduring personality traits (Boyle, Saflofske, & Matthews, Reference Boyle, Saklofske and Matthews2015).
Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS)
The GDS is a 30-item self-report assessment used to identify depression in the elderly (ages 65 and older). The participant is asked to respond “Yes” or “No” to a series of questions about how they have felt over the past week. Participants are then scored as normal (with total score of 0–9); mild to moderate depressives (10–19); or severe depressives (20–30) (Yesavage et al., Reference Yesavage, Brink, Rose, Lum, Huang, Adey and Leirer1982).
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)
The BDI-II is a 21-item self-report questionnaire assessing the current severity of depression symptoms in adolescents and adults (ages 13 and up). It is not designed to serve as an instrument of diagnosis, but rather to identify the presence and severity of symptoms consistent with the criteria of the DSM-IV. Questions assess typical symptoms of depression such as mood, pessimism, or sense of failure. Participants are asked to pick a statement on a 4-point scale that best describes the way they have been feeling during the past 2 weeks. A total score of 0–10 is considered within the normal range, 11–30 is mild to moderate, and 31+ is severe (Beck, Steer, & Brown, Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996).
Depression Composite Score (DCS)
Due to the nature of the data procedure, 72 participants in our sample were administered the GDS and 80 were administered the BDI-II. Given that studies (Stiles & McGarrahan, Reference Stiles and McGarrahan1998) have demonstrated the high correlation and convergent validity between the GDS and BDI-II, participants were assigned to one of three groups based on the interpretive label they received on either the GDS or BDI-II: normal (0), mild to moderate (1), or severe (2). Both the GDS and BDI-II been shown to have high content, construct, and criterion validity, and high internal consistency.
Data Analysis Procedure
The primary goal of this analysis was to examine the relationship between the CPM and our global, offline metamemory index (MKMI).
We first performed a Latent Class Analysis (LCA) using MPLUS 7.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998) to identify discrete, heterogeneous patterns of personality traits and mood (i.e., anxiety and depression scores). LCA models latent clusters of subjects (or classes) using robust maximum likelihood estimation, such that the classes explain the relationship between multivariate variables (i.e., the probability of the variables scores breaks down into a product of univariate response probabilities conditional on the latent classes). We compared nested unconditional LCA models characterized by a progressive number of classes. All variables were standardized to assist model convergence. The optimal number of classes was estimated combining information from conventional model fit indices (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, Reference Nylund, Asparouhov and Muthén2007), including Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), sample-size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (SSBIC), and Aikaike information criterion (AIC). Greatest weight was placed on the BIC and SSBIC due to evidence that they are the strongest indicator of relative fit under these analytic circumstances (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2006). The least weight was placed on the AIC because of evidence that it tends to favor over specification (Henson, Reference Henson2007).
Additionally, relative Entropy indicated the clarity of class specification, with scores ranging from 0 to 1. Entropy values closer to 1 suggest better fit of the data into the prescribed class structure (Duncan, Duncan, & Strycker, Reference Duncan, Duncan and Strycker2006). Furthermore, the nested models were compared using the Lo-Mendell-Rubin Adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test (LMR), and Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT). The LMR compares improvement of fit between nested model solutions with an increasing k number of classes (Lo, Mendell, & Rubin, Reference Lo, Mendell and Rubin2001). LMR significance is derived from the comparison of the derivatives of a k class model’s likelihood ratio chi-square test with those of the k-1 class model. Similarly, the BLRT (McLachlan, & Peel, Reference McLachlan and Peel2004) is a bootstrapped comparison of the loglikelihood difference of the nested models (Nylund et al., Reference Nylund, Asparouhov and Muthén2007). Non significance of either the LMR or BLRT suggests that the one less class solution is a better fit for the data (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2012). Lastly, explanatory properties, parsimony, and interpretability were also considered to determine the optimal number of classes (Muthén, Reference Muthén2004).
We then performed a generalized linear model (GLM) to examine whether MKMI (as a continuous variable) differed as a function of the participants’ LCA class membership assignments. We investigated the associations of metamemory knowledge (confidence/accuracy judgments) with CPM by regressing MKMI on the derived classes while controlling for age, gender, and education. Finally, we ran sample t tests on metamemory scores in the LCA classes, to determine whether the scores for each class differed significantly from zero (i.e., whether each class was significantly over, under, or accurate in their perception of memory). We used an alpha level of .05 for all statistical tests.
RESULTS
Table 3 displays relative model fit from 1 to 3 class solutions for the unconditional LCA. Results indicated that the LMR likelihood ratio test was not significant for the three class solution. As such, the LMR suggested that the two class model was a more appropriate fit for the data than its neighboring three class solution. Among the remaining solutions, the two class model was the one with the best fit in terms of AIC, BIC, SSBIC, and higher relative entropy; furthermore, the LMR and BLRT for the two class solution were both significant. Taken together, information from the fit indices proved that the two class model was the optimal solution. Figure 1 displays the two class solution, characterized by distinct patterns of mood and personality scores. The best log-likelihood value of the model replicated, indicating successful convergence. The first CPM Class (N=118; 74.4% sample) was characterized by predominantly low neuroticism, high extraversion, high conscientiousness, and low anxiety. The second CPM Class (N=39; 24.6%) was characterized by predominantly high neuroticism, low extraversion, low conscientiousness, and high anxiety.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20180423060913735-0646:S1355617717001345:S1355617717001345_fig1g.jpeg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 1 NEUR, Neuroticism; CONS, Conscientiousness, EXTR, Extraversion; OPEN, Openness, AGREE, Agreeableness, State-A, State, Anxiety; Trait-A, Trait Anxiety; DCS, Depression Composite Scale.
Table 3 Model fit indices for one to three class solutions of the mood and personality measures (N=157)
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20180423060913735-0646:S1355617717001345:S1355617717001345_tab3.gif?pub-status=live)
Note. AIC=Akaike information criterion; BIC=Bayesian information criterion; SSBIC=sample size adjusted Bayesian information criterion.
A GLM was performed to examine whether MKMI differed as a function of the emergent CPM class membership, while covarying for age, education, and gender (for full estimates, see Table 4). The model was significant (F(4,151)=5.42; p<.001) and accounted for 13% of the variance (η2=0.13). Separate one sample t tests indicated that individuals assigned to Class 1 demonstrated accuracy (M=0.21; SD=1.31; t(111)=1.70; p=.092), while individuals in Class 2 demonstrated under-confidence (M=−0.59; SD=1.39; t(38)=−2.64; p=.01; d=.47).
Table 4 General linear model examines metamemory as a function of the composite of personality and mood, covarying for age, gender, and education
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20180423060913735-0646:S1355617717001345:S1355617717001345_tab4.gif?pub-status=live)
b. Alpha=.05
Results suggest that individuals who exhibit characteristics of low neuroticism and anxiety and high extraversion and conscientiousness tend to be accurate in their perception of their memory functioning; whereas, individuals who exhibit characteristics of high neuroticism and anxiety, and low extraversion and conscientiousness tend to underestimate their cognitive functioning. Further analysis revealed gender differences such that men significantly overestimated their memory abilities (M=.44; SD=1.45), t(44)=2.01, p=.05, whereas women tended to be accurate in their perception of their memory abilities (M=−.18; SD=1.31), t(105)=−1.38, p=.17; d=.45. Levene’s test of equality of error variance was not significant (F(1,149)=.02; p=0.91).
DISCUSSION
The present study investigated the association between metamemory and characteristics of personality and mood (CPM). The ability to estimate one’s own performance accurately has been shown to be important for managing everyday situations safely and effectively, and for remaining independent (Clare, Marková, et al., Reference Clare, Marková, Roth and Morris2011; West, Dennehy-Basile, & Norris, Reference West, Dennehy-Basile and Norris1996). Metamemory may also be important for psychological well-being, since overestimation of ability carries risk of failure, while underestimation could lead to avoidance of activities and further loss of skills and confidence.
For individuals in the mild to severe stages of AD, when distortions in evaluative judgments intensify, interventions (i.e., provision of contextual cues and environmental support) could aim to improve accuracy of performance monitoring in real life situations to support more effective coping with deficits (Clare, Marková, et al., Reference Clare, Marková, Roth and Morris2011). For those who significantly underestimate their functioning, and express greater concerns that they may not be remembering as well as before, clinicians should pay careful attention to objective measures of performance (i.e., to reduce the prevalence of misdiagnosis of amnestic MCI), and might consider explicitly conveying the differences between normal and abnormal aging to their patients.
Ultimately, we examined metamemory by comparing offline, global estimations (i.e., assessments of general performance in a given area) to objective memory performance to determine the extent to which individuals overestimated, underestimated, or made accurate estimations of their memory abilities. The multi-faceted and complex nature of awareness calls for a broad conceptual framework, encompassing a diverse range of possible influences when seeking to understand awareness-related phenomena in clinical situations.
This study was designed to explore whether latent classes of mood and personality explain a significant proportion of the variance in metamemory. Although previous research in non-demented elders has found that individual psychological factors are associated with SMCs (e.g., Balash et al., 2013; Binder et al., Reference Binder, Storzbach, Rohlman, Campbell and Anger1999), we sought to take a holistic approach and examine both the stable and changeable psychological characteristics that might influence the accuracy of SMCs (i.e., metamemory). The current results indeed yielded two primary mood and personality patterns, the first of which was characterized by high extraversion, with low anxiety and neuroticism. Contrary to hypotheses, rather than low depression, the final component of the profile was high conscientiousness (i.e., all of which together comprises Class 1). The second class was characterized by high anxiety and neuroticism, and low extraversion and conscientiousness.
Our findings substantiated our primary hypothesis that distinct patterns of personality and mood are associated with metamemory accuracy, as we found that Class 1 and Class 2 differed significantly in this regard. Moreover, we found that individuals who exhibit characteristics of low neuroticism and anxiety and high extraversion and conscientiousness tend to be accurate in their perception of their memory functioning. In contrast, individuals who exhibit characteristics of high neuroticism and anxiety and low extraversion and conscientiousness tend to underestimate their cognitive functioning. These results underscore the need for clinicians to take into consideration psychological characteristics, like mood and personality in their evaluation of the factors that may contribute to variability in patients’ metamemory.
Our findings regarding latent classes of mood and personality are generally consistent with the literature—that is, studies have repeatedly demonstrated a positive correlation between anxiety and neuroticism, and a negative correlation between extraversion and these two constructs across the lifespan (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Timoney & Holder, Reference Timoney and Holder2013). In Clark and Watson’s (Reference Clark and Watson1991) tripartite model, they assert that anxiety and depression not only have a shared component of negative affect or general distress (potentially corresponding to the construct of neuroticism) but also maintain the unique components of “anxious arousal” (autonomic hyperactivity) and “anhedonic depression” (low positive affect). A likely interpretation of the high correlations observed among indices of anxiety and neuroticism is that they tap, at least in part, into a common underlying construct (Bishop & Forster, Reference Bishop and Forster2012).
Previous research has also suggested that mood and personality traits (as measured by the NEOFFI) may be associated with different styles by which individuals guide their self-evaluation schemas (Judge, Locke, Durham, & Kluger, Reference Judge, Locke, Durham and Kluger1998). The stable patterns of mood and personality elucidated in this study may represent or map onto biases in self-evaluation, which may in turn distort the accuracy of estimations about cognitive functioning. Although previous studies provide important insights into how different traits are associated with SMCs and self-evaluation schemas, they fail to establish what the association is between these premorbid characteristics and the integrity of metamemory processes.
Furthermore, studies examining metamemory with online, objective paradigms (e.g., FOK and JOL) have largely dismissed these factors in their studies. Thus, we attempted to bridge the gap, and examine the extent to which personality traits and mood factors could explain variance in memory knowledge. We discuss in brevity the relationships between these premorbid characteristics, self-evaluation schemas, and metamemory accuracy.
Neuroticism and STAI
The domain of neuroticism consists of such negative affect states as anxiety, angry hostility, self-consciousness, vulnerability, and depression. Individuals who are highly neurotic may possess limited social networks and demonstrate difficulties coping with psychological stress and regulating negative affect (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992). At the opposite end of the spectrum, individuals who score low on neuroticism are more emotionally stable, less reactive to stress, and although low in negative emotion, not necessarily high in positive emotion; importantly, these individuals tend to report higher levels of well-being and life satisfaction (Passer & Smith, Reference Passer and Smith2009).
Research on self-evaluation schemas suggest that individuals with high anxiety and elevated levels of neuroticism (i.e., Class 2) may develop a greater sensitivity to negative outcomes, and subsequently maintain a negative self-appraisal. Studies have demonstrated that neuroticism and anxiety are highly correlated with pessimism, and negatively correlated with optimism and self-esteem (Amirkhan, Risinger & Swickert, Reference Amirkhan, Risinger and Swickert1995; Williams, Reference Williams1992). Data from the current study may suggest that persons high on neuroticism and anxiety may be more likely to draw on negative conceptions about themselves (i.e., regarding declining memory), irrespective of their actual level of functioning, and may subsequently underestimate their true ability.
Conscientiousness
Individuals high in conscientiousness (Class 1) are distinguished as being organized, persistent, goal-oriented, and disciplined (McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa2003). Although extraversion (discussed below) and neuroticism seem to have the largest effect on subjective well-being, conscientiousness has also been shown to correlate with positive affect and optimism (DeNeve, & Cooper, Reference Deneve and Cooper1998; Steel, Schmidt, & Shultz, Reference Steel, Schmidt and Shultz2008). Conscientiousness, which often develops in childhood and remains unchanged in adulthood, manifests in characteristic behaviors such as being thorough and deliberate in one’s actions (McCrae, Reference McCrae2004). When taken to extremes, these individuals are often labeled as “perfectionists,” “workaholics,” or compulsive in their behavior (Carter, Guan, Maples, Williamson, & Miller, Reference Carter, Guan, Maples, Williamson and Miller2015). Through a self-evaluation lens, those who are higher on conscientiousness may be more meticulous in nature, and subsequently accurate in their perception of their cognitive abilities.
Extraversion/Introversion
Extraversion refers to a wide variety of traits including warmth, gregariousness, assertiveness, activity, excitement seeking, and of greatest relevance: a tendency to experience positive affect (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa2003). Conversely, individuals who score lower on extraversion or higher on introversion (i.e., Class 2) are typically more reserved and self-reflective (Sipps & Alexander, Reference Sipps and Alexander1987). Psychologists like Jung have characterized introverts as people whose energy tends to expand through reflection and dwindle during interaction.
Eysenck (Reference Eysenck1963; Reference Eysenck1991) similarly characterized extraversion/introversion as the degree to which a person is outgoing and interactive with the world. He postulated that these behavioral variances are presumed to be the result of underlying differences in brain physiology (Eysenck, Reference Eysenck1963). While extraverts seek excitement and social activity in an effort to heighten their arousal level and positive affect, introverts tend to avoid social situations in an effort to keep such arousal to a minimum. He theorized that extraversion is a combination of two major tendencies: impulsiveness and sociability, later adding such traits as liveliness, activity level, and excitability. Extraversion has been repeatedly understood as a facilitator of social interactions (McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa1991; Lucas, Le, & Dyrenforth, Reference Lucas, Le and Dyrenforth2008; Zelenski & Larsen, Reference Zelenski and Larsen1999), since low cortical arousal may result in extraverts seeking more social situations in an effort to increase arousal (Eysenck, Reference Eysenck1967).
In accordance with Eysenck (Reference Eysenck1967), McCrae and Costa (Reference McCrae and Costa1991) put forth the social activity hypothesis to explain the greater subjective well-being among extraverts. They suggest that higher extraversion (as seen in Class 1) helps in the creation of life circumstances, which in turn promote higher levels of positive affect and a more positive self-evaluation schema than does high introversion, high anxiety, and/or high neuroticism (Class 2). The strong correlation demonstrated in numerous studies between extraversion and both positive self-schemas and optimism aligns with our findings that individuals with a more extraverted profile, although accurate in their metacognitive abilities, tend more toward overestimation rather than underestimation (Amirkhan et al., Reference Amirkhan, Risinger and Swickert1995; Williams, Reference Williams1992).
Investigating the long-standing or “premorbid” characteristics of an individual that influence biases in self-related judgments, not only provides information about how self-judgments are formed, but informs the factors that contribute to impaired metamemory in the context of aging and disease (Cosentino, Metcalfe, Holmes, Steffener, & Stern, Reference Cosentino, Metcalfe, Holmes, Steffener and Stern2011). The present study supports a biopsychosocial approach to understanding self-assessment and examining awareness in a healthy aging population (Clare, Marková, et al., Reference Clare, Marková, Roth and Morris2011).
In the context of SCD, our research lends credence to concerns that SMCs alone are not sufficient indicators of cognitive decline, and may contribute to misdiagnosis of MCI. Indeed, Jessen and colleagues (Reference Jessen, Amariglio, van Boxtel, Breteler, Ceccaldi, Chetelat and Wagner2014), in their consensus report on the diagnosis of SCD, point to the association between SCD and both psychological conditions and personality traits. They acknowledge the previously reported link between SCD and neuroticism and anxiety, and the inverse association with openness and conscientiousness. The current work provides additional information in this regard, directly showing that not only are certain psychological variables associated with cognitive complaints, but such complaints are on average, inaccurate underestimations of actual functioning. Current results highlight the psychological variables that come into play within the biopsychosocial framework and provide evidence for the hypothesis that metamemory processes are subject to a range of influencing factors that may constitute barriers to accurate self-knowledge (Greenwald, Reference Greenwald1980; Gergen, Reference Gergen1984).
LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The current study has both strengths and limitations. A significant strength, which we discussed earlier, is that this is the first attempt to our knowledge to both uncover discrete classes of mood and personality in an aging population, and to study whether metamemory is associated with distinct patterns of mood and personality. However, there are a few limitations to this study that are important to address.
First, our selected subjective memory instrument was a modified version of an existing SCC questionnaire (i.e., CFQ), thus potentially raising questions about the reliability and validity of the modified scale. However, in creating the CFQ-M, we used factor analysis to ensure the items plotted onto a single construct (memory) and also ensured that the items selected were consistent with those found in a reliable and validated memory complaint scale (Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire) developed by Youn et al. (Reference Youn, Kim, Lee, Jhoo, Lee, Park and Woo2009).
Second, although a strength of this study is that it uses an objective measure of metamemory by comparing memory ratings directly to memory performance, we acknowledge that the metamemory rating was an offline evaluation (i.e., a crystallized notion of what their memory abilities are) as opposed to an online evaluation of metamemory experience (i.e., perception of memory abilities while engaged in an ongoing memory task). It remains unknown in which way the emergent classes of mood and personality would relate to an online metamemory evaluation like FOK or JOL.
That said, it has been shown that online and offline measures are correlated in individuals with AD, and that there may be significant benefits to using an offline, global subjective measure (Cosentino, Metcalfe, Butterfield, & Stern, Reference Cosentino, Metcalfe, Butterfield and Stern2007). For example, the offline measure is rich in its reflection of everyday, real world levels of awareness. In some respects, offline scores may be better able to inform practically and clinically relevant issues including the extent to which participants appreciate their need for assistance or devise strategies for completing cognitively demanding activities (Fleming & Frith, Reference Fleming and Frith2014; Cosentino, Metcalfe, Cary, et al., Reference Cosentino, Metcalfe, Cary, Leon and Karlawish2011).
An area of debate related to this limitation is that there remains some question as to the convergent validity between subjective cognitive questionnaires (e.g., CFQ-M or an Activities of Daily Living Questionnaire) and task-specific, neuropsychological measures of cognition (e.g., RAVLT). This is an issue that unfortunately plagues many metacognitive studies, which use neuropsychological measures with questionable ecological validity. It remains unclear to what extent the clinically-relevant questions in our CFQ-M map conceptually and directly onto the RAVLT; said differently, a reasonable area of contention is whether there is adequate convergence between a long term list-learning task and a comprehensive evaluation of common, everyday memory complaints. For this reason, we do not operationalize our MKMI as memory monitoring, but instead characterize the construct we are measuring as a more generalized knowledge of memory abilities.
Nonetheless, the significant difference, and the direction of the difference in average metamemory knowledge across LCA groups, suggests that individuals with certain personality features tend more toward underestimation of abilities than those without such features, regardless of the absolute agreement that is to be expected between the subjective and objective measurements of memory in the general population.
Third, our use of a DCS rather than a single, consistent measure of depression is a limitation in this retrospective study, and any extrapolation should be made carefully. Ideally, the BDI or a more comprehensive measure of depression would have been available in all participants. Our decision to group participants based on the interpretive label they received on the GDS and BDI-II aligns with numerous studies that have demonstrated a high correlation and convergent validity between these two measures of depression.
In addition, our sample was fairly homogeneous (see Table 1), and results may not generalize to a larger and more diverse population. A final limitation is the cross sectional nature of the study. Based on previous personality literature, we made the assumptions that (1) personality is a stable, lifelong construct that is not subject to change significantly without deliberate intervention and (2) metacognition is something that can be changed in the context of the aging brain. Future research might consider exploring CPM and metamemory at multiple points in time to determine whether change in one predicts change in the other.
These issues notwithstanding, our results reveal that discrete patterns of mood and personality are associated with metamemory knowledge accuracy within an aging population. More research is needed to explore questions about the extent to which metamemory changes as a function of these premorbid characteristics both across the lifespan and across a range of neurodegenerative diseases, including dementias and movement disorders. Key theoretical challenges for future research will be to identify the distinct influences and contributions of both neurocognitive and socio-environmental factors, and to clarify which awareness phenomena are amenable to appropriate and sensitive intervention.
Moreover, to properly account for their heterogeneous contributions, future research should measure the influence of these factors on individuals directly within the mixture model estimations. Obstacles for clinical practice will be to identify where it is appropriate to attempt to increase awareness, where it is preferable to find ways of managing unawareness, and to help caregivers understand the nature and extent of the person’s awareness, and tailor their interactions accordingly. By understanding and working effectively with awareness phenomena, there is a strong potential to reduce disability and enhance well-being. Therefore, further knowledge in this area should be vigorously pursued.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Principal support for the enhanced NKI-RSI open data-sharing project (http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/enhanced/) is provided by the NIMH BRAINS R01MH094639 (PI Milham); NIMH 1R01MH101555 (PI Craddock); R01AG047596 (PI Colcombe). Project support also provided by the NKI Center for Biomedical Imaging and Neuromodulation (CBIN), the NKI Outpatient Research Department, the Brain Research Foundation, and the Stavros Niarchos Foundation. We have no conflicts of interest to disclose.