INTRODUCTION
The view that meaning in language is represented in modality-specific brain regions (e.g., Binder & Desai, 2011) contrasts with proposals that meaning is stored in abstract, amodal systems (e.g., Landauer & Dumais, Reference Landauer and Dumais1997). There now exist a large number of studies demonstrating action simulation during language comprehension (for review see Fischer & Zwaan, Reference Fischer and Zwaan2008). A critical question, however, is the level of abstraction of action simulations; to what extent do they mirror real-world action? Embodied theories are underspecified in terms of how much information is contained in a simulation (Sanford, Reference Sanford2008), or at what grain information is represented.
Action simulations include the specific effector used in the action (Hauk, Johnsrude, & Pulvermüller, Reference Hauk, Johnsrude and Pulvermüller2004), or the specificity of the action (van Dam, Rueschemeyer, & Bekkering, Reference van Dam, Rueschemeyer and Bekkering2010). Recent research suggests even fine-grained temporal information is represented in action simulations: whether the action is fast or slow (Speed & Vigliocco, Reference Speed and Vigliocco2014, Reference Speed and Vigliocco2015; van Dam, Speed, Lai, Vigliocco, & Desai, Reference van Dam, Speed, Lai, Vigliocco and Desai2017). Speed and Vigliocco (Reference Speed and Vigliocco2014) showed online simulations are sensitive to speed: when listening to sentences describing fast and slow actions (e.g., The lion dashed to the balloon vs. The lion ambled to the balloon) looking time toward a concurrent visual scene was longer for slow actions compared to fast actions. Related, mental simulation is also sensitive to the degree of effort suggested in a sentence (e.g., pushing the piano vs. pushing the chair) (Moody & Gennari, Reference Moody and Gennari2010).
Studies of patients with motor deficits can provide strong tests of causality. If parts of the motor circuit of the brain are crucial to understanding action language, at least some types of deficits in the motor system should lead to difficulties comprehending action language. The present study examines whether action simulations of speed play a crucial role in comprehension of language about speed, by testing impairment in comprehension of fast action in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) compared to age-matched healthy controls. PD is a neurodegenerative disease caused by a deficiency in the dopaminergic pathway leading to basal ganglia atrophy and dopaminergic striatal loss (see Helmich, Hallett, Deuschl, Toni, & Bloem, 2012; Rodriguez-Orzo et al., Reference Rodriguez-Oroz, Jahanshahi, Krack, Litvan, Macias, Bezard and Obeso2009; Samii, Nutt, Ransom, & Sampaio, Reference Samii, Nutt, Ransom and Sampaio2004), resulting in reduced activation in brain areas involved in motor planning and execution, including primary motor cortex and the supplementary motor area (Rascol et al., Reference Rascol, Umberto, Chollet, Celsis, Montastruc, Marc-Vergnes and Rascol1992). PD is characterized by a range of motor problems including bradykinesia and rigidity, that is, slow and difficult movement. We predict that since PD patients move at a slower speed and, therefore, have difficulty with fast motion, they should similarly be impaired with language about fast motion.
Previous studies have assessed comprehension of action language broadly in patients with motor deficits (e.g., Bak, O’Donovan, Xuereb, Boniface, & Hodges, Reference Bak, O’Donovan, Xuereb, Boniface and Hodges2001; Boulenger et al., Reference Boulenger, Mechtouff, Thobois, Broussolle, Jeannerod and Nazier2008; Fernandino et al., Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a, Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013b, but see Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber, & Tranel, 2013; York et al., Reference York, Olm, Boller, McCluskey, Elman, Haley and Grossman2014). Boulenger et al. (Reference Boulenger, Mechtouff, Thobois, Broussolle, Jeannerod and Nazier2008) found priming effects for action verbs in PD varied as a function of Levodopa uptake (medication improving motor impairment in PD). Fernandino et al. (Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a) removed the grammatical confound in noun-verb comparisons by comparing PD patients and healthy controls on action verb and abstract verb processing. Compared to healthy controls, patients performed worse with action verbs than abstract verbs, reflecting impairment in processing action language rather than the grammatical category of verbs.
Kemmerer et al. (Reference Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber and Tranel2013) suggest that the accuracy results of Fernandino et al. (Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a) can be explained in terms of a slight deficit in action verb comprehension, or as a slight enhancement in abstract verb comprehension. However, their response time results cannot be explained in this manner, as slowing of response time was observed for both action and abstract verbs relative to controls, with greater slowing for action verbs, with a significant interaction. Considering speed-accuracy tradeoff, abstract verb accuracy benefitted from slower response times, but not action verb accuracy even after greater slowing of response time, supporting the conclusion of a specific deficit for action verb comprehension.
Fernandino et al. (Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013b) also report similar results for verbs in sentence contexts, where slowing of response time was observed with a relatively greater effect for action verb sentences, resulting in an interaction. Cardona et al. (Reference Cardona, Kargieman, Sinay, Gershanik, Gelormini, Amoruso and Ibáñez2014) found that the action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE) was abolished in early PD patients, but not in patients with peripheral motor deficits. The lack of an ACE effect in PD has been shown to be due to reduced motor potential, aberrant frontotemporal connectivity, and overall volume of basal ganglia atrophy (Melloni et al., Reference Melloni, Sedeño, Hesse, García-Cordero, Mikulan, Plastino and Ibáñez2015).
Action language deficits have also been observed in language production: Bocanegra et al. (Reference Bocanegra, García, Pineda, Buriticá, Villegas, Lopera and Ibáñez2015) found disruptions of action verb production in PD patients compared to controls. Moreover, this deficit was unrelated to deficits in executive function or to mild cognitive impairment. Deficiencies in action language have also been observed in spontaneous speech. Using computerized analyses of brief monologues produced by patients and controls, García et al. (Reference García, Carrillo, Orozco-Arroyave, Trujillo, Vargas Bonilla, Fittipaldi and Cecchi2016) found that action-related concepts were less dominant semantic fields in PD discourse than controls (i.e., weighted lower as semantic fields following latent semantic analysis). Thus, several studies have demonstrated that the type of motor system pathology seen in PD can cause specific impairments in action language processing.
Here, we examine whether speed of actions is also represented or simulated during semantic judgments on action verbs. One possibility is that, while action concept processing in general is impaired in PD relative to that of abstract concepts, action simulations used in the service of comprehension are not detailed enough to contain information about motion speed, and speed has no effect on comprehension. If speed is part of the simulations, one may expect it to modulate comprehension performance, especially for verbs describing relatively fast movements, because this form of movement is most difficult in PD. Disembodied approaches to language comprehension would predict comprehension of speed is not affected since sensorimotor systems have no functional role in language understanding.
Here, we used a semantic similarity judgment task (SSJ) where participants judged which of two words were most similar to a target word. Words denoted actions similar in speed, that is, a fast or slow action, or a static action. Actions could be performed with the hand/arm (e.g., grasp) or with the whole body (e.g., run). We compared PD patients with age-matched controls. We predicted an interaction between group (PD vs. control) and verb speed (fast vs. slow).
METHOD
Participants
Eighteen patients with PD were recruited from the Columbia Parkinson’s support group and the Palmetto Health Richmond hospital in Columbia, South Carolina. Six patients were removed for having low scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (≤21), a cutoff for detecting dementia, leaving 12 patients (1 female, M age=67.4; SD=8.97). All patients were on medication, with an average of 181.56 min since last medication (except three patients for whom this information was not recorded). Table 1 presents summary demographics. Fifteen healthy age-matched controls were recruited. Three controls were removed for having a low MoCA score (≤21), leaving 12 control participants (M age=68; SD=9.10; M MoCA=26.92). All participants were paid for their participation. Research was completed according to a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of South Carolina.
Material
Sixteen fast (e.g., to run), 16 slow (e.g., to shuffle) and two sets of 16 verbs of no movement (e.g., to stand) were used in the experiment. Half of the fast and slow verbs were full-body actions (e.g., to run) and half were hand/arm movements (e.g., to grasp). Verbs were rated by a separate group of participants in terms of speed (1 being very slow, 7 being very fast, with an option of “none” available). Fast verbs had an average speed rating of 5.8 (SD=0.8; range=3.71–6.83; fast full body M=6.0, SD=1, range=3.71–6.83; fast hand M=5.45; SD=0.7; range=4.67–5.90) and slow verbs had a mean speed rating of 2.7 (SD=0.5; range=1.83–3.43; slow full body M=2.83; SD=0.5, range=2–3.43; slow hand M=2.51; SD=0.5, range=1.8–3.20) (see the Appendix for a full list of verbs).
Verbs were matched in terms of word frequency, number of letters, number of orthographic neighbors, number of phonemes, and number of syllables. In a subsequent rating task, a separate group of participants were instructed to rate each verb in terms of the extent to which the arms, hands, legs, feet, and torso are involved in the action, and how much effort is required to perform the action (1 being low involvement or low effort, 5 being high involvement or high effort). Full-body verbs were rated higher on involvement of legs and feet, and hand verbs were rated higher on involvement of hands and arms, confirming our categorization (see Table 2).
a Indicates significant difference between full-body verb and hand verb.
Furthermore, verifying our verbs of no movement did sufficiently reflect static action, we found that static verbs were rated as having lower involvement of the arms, hands, and legs than hand verbs, lower involvement of the legs and feet than full-body verbs, and as involving less effort than hand verbs and full-body verbs (see Table 2). Items were divided into two sets to serve as separate blocks within the experiment. The two sets also reflected the independent variable of speed: fast actions and static actions (fast judgments) and slow actions and static actions (slow judgments). That is, on each trial, a participant had to distinguish fast actions from static actions, or slow actions from static actions. The items were then divided into 32 triplets per block with each item serving as the target, match and foil once.
Procedure
Participants responded with two colored Ablenet Jelly Bean buttons (www.ablenetinc.com), which is easier than other types of button response (e.g., keyboard press) because they are much larger. Participants were instructed to respond with their dominant hand and to rest it between the two buttons between responses.
For each trial, three verbs were presented in a triangular arrangement. Each verb was presented with the word “to” to its left to ensure disambiguation of grammatical class. Participants were to indicate which of the two bottom words was most similar in meaning to the top and to press the right button for the word on the right and the left button for the word on the left. The position of the matching verb was counterbalanced across trials.
Participants were instructed that verbs would be similar in terms of whether they described movement or not. The stimuli stayed on screen until the participant had responded or the trial had timed out (after 5000 ms). Participants were not instructed to respond as quickly as possible, but they were aware that the trial would time out after 5 s. Participants first completed six practice trials with words that denoted facial expressions (e.g., to grin) versus words that denoted vocalizations (e.g., to yell) with feedback given on each trial. The task took around 10 min to complete.
RESULTS
Two items were removed because accuracy was 50% or less in the control group. Individual trials were removed if responses were faster than 250 ms or outside of 1.5 SD of a participant’s mean response time (11% of correct trials: 3% fast body, 2.7% slow body, 2.6% fast hand, 2.7% slow hand, leaving 1138 trials). One patient was removed for having overall accuracy less than 50%. Response time analyses were conducted only on accurate trials.
We first conducted 2×2×2 mixed analyses of variance on accuracy and response time with speed type (fast vs. slow) and body part (hand vs. full body) as within subjects factors, and group (PD vs. control group) as a between subjects factor. Fast verb judgments had higher accuracy than slow verb judgments, F(1,22)=5.17, p=.03, η2 p=.19, and the control group was more accurate than the PD group, F(1,22)=6.57, p=.02, η2 p=.23. There was also a significant interaction between speed and body part, F(1,22)=17.94, p<.001, η2 p=.449, reflecting higher accuracy for slow compared to fast full-body verbs, but the opposite pattern for hand verbs. There was no interaction between group and speed or between speed, body part, and group, F<1, and no effect of body part, F(1,22)=2.63, p=.12, η2 p=.107. Mean accuracy is displayed in Figure 1.
For response time there was no overall difference between the PD group and control group, F(1,22)=2.24, p=.15, η2 p=.092. Overall, judgments about slow actions took longer than judgments about fast actions, F(1,22)=6.55, p=.02, η2 p=.23, but there was also an interaction between speed and group, F(1,22)=4.56, p=.04, η2 p=.17. In line with our prediction, judgments were slower in the PD group than the control group for fast verbs, t(22)=1.8, p=.04, d=0.77 (one-tailed), but not slow verbs, t(22)=1.13, p=.27, d=0.23, suggesting PD patients have an impairment in comprehending language about fast actions. There was also an interaction between group and body part, F(1,22)=5.68, p=.02, η2 p=.205, with responses to hand verbs slower than responses to full-body verbs in the PD group, t(11)=2.89, p=.02, d=.198, but not the control group, t(11)=.92, p=.38, d=.067. There was no interaction between speed and body part, F(1, 22)=2.31, p=.14, η2 p=.095, or between speed, body part, and group, F(1, 22)=1.53, p=.23, η2 p=.065.
To determine whether age played a role in the two interactions, we reanalyzed the data using an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with age as a covariate. With the additional covariate, however, the interaction between speed and group was still significant, F(1,21)=4.52, p=.046, η2 p=.177, as was the interaction between body part and group, F(1,21)=5.54, p=.028, η2 p=.209 . As a second test, we calculated the average difference in response time of patients to fast and slow trials, and the average difference in response time to full-body verbs and hand verbs, and then conducted linear regressions on these values with age as a predictor. We found no significant effect of age for the speed difference, β=.1, t=.33, p=.75, R2=.01, or the body part difference, β=.27, t=.87, p=.75, R2=.07.
We also conducted regressions with number of years since diagnosis as a predictor. Again the models were not significant, speed difference β=−.53, t=1.26, p=.28, R2=.29, body part difference, β=.44, t=1.57, p=.15, R2=.44, but the high β and R2 values suggest that number of years since diagnosis accounts for a large amount of variance; our data may be underpowered. The pattern suggests that differences in response time between fast and slow trials gets smaller (i.e., responses to fast trials become more impaired) the longer time since PD diagnosis, and by implication, the greater the severity of PD. Similarly, the difference in response time between full-body verbs and hand verbs gets larger the longer time since PD diagnosis, suggesting hand verbs are more difficult to comprehend the greater the severity of PD. We further investigated whether cognitive status played any role in effects, but using MoCA score as a predictor of both values was not significant, β=−.2, t=.64, p=.54, R2=.04; β=−.09, t=.29, p=.78, R2=.09.
Since the first analyses indicated some differences between full-body verbs and hand verbs, we further explored the form of speed simulation impaired in PD by looking at verbs describing actions with the whole body and verbs describing actions with the hands separately.
For full-body verbs, there was no difference in accuracy between fast and slow verbs, F(1,22)=1.25, p=.28, η2 p=.05, and no interaction, F<1, but there was a marginal effect of group with the PD group having lower accuracy than the control group, F(1,22)=4.28, p=.05, η2 p=.16. Mean accuracy is displayed in Figure 1(B). For response time, slow judgments took longer than fast judgments, F(1,22)=10.45, p=.004, η2 p=.32, but this time there was no interaction between speed and group, F<1, and no difference between PD and the control group, F(1,22)=1.40, p=.25, η2 p=.06. Unlike in the first analysis, there was no significant difference between the PD and control group for fast judgments, t(22)=1.34, p=.2, d=.57.
For hand verbs, fast verb trials had higher accuracy than slow verb trials, F(1,22)=18.56, p<.001, η2 p=.46, but there was no difference between the control group and the PD group, F(1,22)=3.84, p=.06, η2 p=.15, and no interaction between group and speed, F<1. Mean accuracy is displayed in Figure 1(C). There was no overall difference between fast and slow judgments in response time, F<1, and no difference between the PD and control group, F(1,22)=3.18, p=.09, η2 p=.13, but there was a significant interaction between speed and group, F(1,22)=5.00, p=.036, η2 p=.19, such that judgments were slower in the PD group than the control group for fast verbs, t(22)=2.19, p=.04, d=0.93, but not slow verbs, t(22)=1.28, p=.21, d=0.54.
Thus, PD patients were specifically impaired at comprehending fast actions performed with the hands, compared to control participants. As before, to determine whether age played a role in this effect, we reanalyzed the data using an ANCOVA with age as a covariate. Again, with the additional covariate the interaction was still significant, F(1,21)=4.85, p=.04, η2 p=.19. We then calculated the average difference in response time of patients to fast and slow trials, and conducted a linear regression on the values with age as a predictor. We found no significant effect of age, β=.11, t=.36, p=.73, R2=.01.
Again we followed with a regression with number of years since diagnosis as a predictor. The model was not significant, β=−.64, t=1.66, p=.17, R2=.41, but again the β and R2 values suggest that number of years since diagnosis accounts for a large amount of variance. As before this suggests that responses to fast trials become more impaired the longer time since PD diagnosis. We further investigated whether cognitive status played any role in the effect, but using MoCA score as a predictor of difference in response time between fast and slow judgments was not significant, β=−.06, t=.18, p=.87, R2=.06. Figure 2 displays average response time.
DISCUSSION
We provide evidence that a fine-grained parameter, speed, is a crucial component of mental simulations of action. Using SSJs on fast and slow action verbs, we found PD patients were impaired in judgments about fast actions but not slow actions, reflected in longer response times to make correct judgments. This was specific to verbs describing actions performed with the hand, and not for verbs describing actions performed with the whole body. We believe that the motor symptoms experienced in PD, such as slowness of movement (bradykinesia) and rigidity, place constraints on the mental simulation of fast actions during language comprehension. Reduced motor cortex activation in PD that leads to symptoms such as bradykinesia, also leads to difficulties simulating actions that require speeded movements. The results parallel recent findings (Desai, Herter, Riccardi, Rorden, & Fridriksson, Reference Desai, Herter, Riccardi, Rorden and Fridriksson2015) where fine-grained parameters of reaching actions were measured in stroke patients. Here, time to perform and initiate the action correlated with speed of processing action verbs and nouns, compared to abstract words.
Why would a deficit in speed processing be found for hand actions but not whole-body actions? Although we cannot exclude this difference reflects idiosyncrasies of the patients’ motor deficits (i.e., more impairment with hand/arm movement compared to movements with the whole body), a plausible account is that the hand action verbs used here differ from the full-body verbs in that they depict actions requiring greater precision. Performing a precise action quickly (e.g., grasping) is likely more problematic for a PD patient than performing a whole-body action (e.g., walking). Thus, if action simulations mirror real-world action in terms of fine-grained features, then PD patients should similarly be more impaired at understanding language about precision actions compared to non-precision actions.
Another possibility is that, since participants were using their hands to respond in the task, the interaction between the motor system and mental simulation of action-related meaning becomes more prominent for hand-related words [see García & Ibáñez, Reference García and Ibáñez2016, Hand-Action-Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model for a discussion of situated coupling between the motor and embodied domains]. To investigate this possibility, future investigations could implement different response methods, or manipulate the type of ongoing action in the task.
SSJs require explicit semantic processing and is, thus, suitable for examining potential semantic deficits. It does not require or encourage artificial mental imagery. If performing mental imagery is part of the process of comprehension and comparison of word meanings, then it is part of conceptual processing. One may ask whether comprehension difficulties for fast speed in PD would be observed for more automatic or shallow language comprehension tasks (e.g., lexical decision with priming c.f. Fernandino et al., Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a). Recent research suggests simulations are dynamic and context dependent (Lebois, Wilson-Mendenthal, & Barsalou, Reference Lebois, Wilson-Mendenthal and Barsalou2015), being relied upon more or less in different linguistic and situational contexts.
For example, because simulations take time to develop (Barsalou, Santos, Simmons, & Wilson, Reference Barsalou, Santos, Simmons and Wilson2008), when a quick response is required, lexical associations (statistical information such as word co-occurrence) are more likely to be recruited than simulations (Louwerse & Jeuniaux, Reference Louwerse and Jeuniaux2008). It is possible that speed information is a secondary feature of the verbs used here (see van Dam et al., Reference van Dam, Speed, Lai, Vigliocco and Desai2017) and simulation may only take into account such features during deeper processing. This is in line with the recent finding that more details are simulated for more explicit semantic tasks (Desai et al., Reference Desai, Herter, Riccardi, Rorden and Fridriksson2015). Fine-grained motor measurements in stroke patients (e.g., action initiation time, movement direction error) correlated with an explicit semantic task (SSJs) but not more implicit tasks (lexical decision, priming), which were correlated with more global action parameters such as total movement time (Desai et al., Reference Desai, Herter, Riccardi, Rorden and Fridriksson2015). Using tasks that manipulate depth of semantic processing could similarly reveal when speed information becomes important.
We note that elsewhere a specific deficit in action language compared to non-action language was not observed, but a slowing down of comprehension more generally (Kemmerer et al., Reference Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber and Tranel2013). Furthermore, both patients and controls responded most slowly and with lowest accuracy to cutting verbs (e.g., cut, slice, hack), which are similar to the present study’s “hand verbs” (although this was not found for hitting verbs, which also suggest hand actions e.g., hit, poke, jab). However, the PD patients in Kemmerer et al. (Reference Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber and Tranel2013) were older than the present group (75.5 vs. 67.4 years) and were diagnosed much earlier (7.6 vs. 4.5 years ago). It is, therefore, likely that the Kemmerer et al. (Reference Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber and Tranel2013) patients have more cognitive decline, making deficits in action semantics harder to discern.
A decline in executive function is especially associated with PD (Higginson et al., Reference Higginson, King, Levine, Wheelock, Khamphay and Sigvardt2003; Levy et al., Reference Levy, Jacobs, Tang, Côté, Louis, Alfaro and Marder2002; McKinlay, Grace, Dalrymple-Alford, & Roger, Reference Mckinlay, Grace, Dalrymple-Alford and Roger2010; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Moberg, Culbertson, Duda, Katz and Stern2005; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Cole, Mengersen, Silburn, Qiu, Graepel and Kerr2014). Kemmerer et al. (Reference Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber and Tranel2013) had a smaller pool of patients (n=10), and half of these patients exhibited mild impairment in executive functions (see their Table 2), potentially diluting any specific effects.
Beyond a general decline, according to some theories, comprehension of abstract words rely on verbal associations, and consequently on executive and control mechanisms more, as they lack a direct referent (Schwanenflugel, Reference Schwanenflugel1991). This is supported by the activation of the inferior frontal gyrus, an area traditionally associated with executive function and control, for abstract relative to concrete words (Wang, Conder, Blitzer, & Shinkareva, Reference Wang, Conder, Blitzer and Shinkareva2010). Hence, an additional potential factor is that mild executive impairment may specifically affect abstract words, reducing any difference between concrete and abstract words.
But on the other hand, our present data suggest effects would be more likely the greater time since diagnosis, at least in the initial phase of the disease. It is possible, however, that there is a peak to this effect, after which the executive decline increases sufficiently to make action comprehension difficulties less discernible. MoCA scores did not predict response difference between fast and slow verbs in the present study. However, all of our patients were without mild cognitive impairment and so present an unsuitable population in which to test the role of cognitive impairment, instead comparing groups with and without mild cognitive impairment would be more appropriate (e.g., Bocanegra et al., 2017).
One interesting finding from Kemmerer et al. (Reference Kemmerer, Miller, MacPherson, Huber and Tranel2013), however, that supports the present data is that patient’s accuracy for cutting verbs correlated with time since diagnosis, with greater disease duration leading to lower accuracy. This is in line with the idea posed above that hand verbs may be more easily affected by motor disorders because they describe actions that require more precision.
York et al. (Reference York, Olm, Boller, McCluskey, Elman, Haley and Grossman2014) also found that PD patients did not perform worse on judgments of action verbs compared to cognition verbs, but did find the expected pattern in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, a condition with atrophy in motor association and prefrontal regions. Of the 22 PD patients in their study, however, 14 of them were not cognitively healthy, but ranged from having mild cognitive impairment to dementia. It is unclear to what extent such impairment could differentially affect the different verb types used in their study.
A point to consider in research regarding action-verb processing in motor disorders (see Bak, Reference Bak2013) is whether deficits should be observed in accuracy measures or response time. In the present study, we observed differences in response time, which is in line with studies showing differences in performance on tasks such as lexical decision (Boulenger et al., Reference Boulenger, Mechtouff, Thobois, Broussolle, Jeannerod and Nazier2008; Fernandino et al., Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a) and sentence comprehension (Fernandino et al., Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013b). However, elsewhere differences have been observed in accuracy with semantic similarity judgments (Fernandino et al., Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a) and verb to picture matching (Bak et al., Reference Bak, O’Donovan, Xuereb, Boniface and Hodges2001), for example.
One suggestion for this difference is that effects may be observed in accuracy for tasks that are particularly difficult, or when participants are under time pressure (e.g., Rueschemeyer, Lindemann, van Rooij, van Dam, & Bekkering, Reference Rueschemeyer, Lindemann, van Rooij, van Dam and Bekkering2010). That we find differences in response time but other studies also using semantic similarity judgments find differences in accuracy (Fernandino et al., Reference Fernandino, Conant, Binder, Blindauer, Hiner, Spangler and Desai2013a) could be due to the difficulty level of the current task being too low, or due the lack of an emphasis on responding quickly. Further research manipulating such variables could test these predictions.
There are possible limitations to the present study. First, with only 12 patients, it is possible that our study is underpowered, which could lead to bias in the data (see Button et al., Reference Button, Ioannidis, Mokrysz, Nosek, Flint, Robinson and Munafo2013). Investigations involving such populations can often be difficult in terms of participant recruitment, but such issues should be considered. We also note that, although our items were matched on various psycholinguistic variables, such as word frequency, there are other variables that may be relevant, such as imageability and semantic-relatedness.
For example, it could be possible that pairs of fast verbs were less semantically related than pairs of slow verbs, making the decision more difficult and hence slower. It could be expected though that such a difference between fast and slow verbs in this direction would also be present in the controls, but it was in fact the opposite. Since our pattern of results in PD patients (slower responses to fast verbs) is in the opposite direction to that of controls who were given identical stimuli (slower responses for slow judgments), we are confident that differences do not reflect an unbalanced item set in this sense.
Furthermore, intuitively fast judgements should be easier than slow judgments because the task required discriminating motion verbs from static verbs, and fast actions are more different in terms of speed to static actions than slow actions are. It is also possible that the current speed verbs may differ on additional dimensions not considered here. We note for example that, in a follow-up rating task, fast actions were rated as involving more effort than slow actions (3.36 vs. 2.55). It is, therefore, possible that the present results instead reflect simulation of effort (see Moody & Gennari, Reference Moody and Gennari2010). However, it is unclear to what extent the meaning of effort and the meaning of speed can be disentangled for the present verbs.
It has been suggested that perceptual simulations are schematic (Barsalou, Reference Barsalou1999) and thus it is conceivable that they only include salient or coarse details. That evidence exists for simulation of action speed shows that simulations can go beyond a schematic reconstruction of action events in general, to a further level of detail, including fine-grained information about the manner of action. By showing that patients with motor problems also have difficulties comprehending language about fast action compared to healthy control participants, we provide evidence that action simulations of speed are a causally involved component in the comprehension of language about speed.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by NIH/NIDCD grant R01DC010783 (R.H.D.) and a UCL Bogue Fellowship to L.J. Speed. The study was written while L.J. Speed was supported by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research: NWO VICI grant. We thank Carl Brzorad and Nicholas Riccardi for assistance with running subjects, and Ilja Croijmans for comments on a draft of the study.
APPENDIX
Verbs and Mean Speed Ratings