Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:01:47.911Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention and Regional Gray Matter Development in Very Preterm Children at Age 12 Years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2017

Rachel E. Lean
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri
Tracy R. Melzer
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch, New Zealand
Samudragupta Bora
Affiliation:
Mothers, Babies and Women’s Health Program, Mater Research Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
Richard Watts
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, University of Vermont, Burlington, Vermont
Lianne J. Woodward*
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatric Newborn Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Lianne Woodward, Department of Pediatric Newborn Medicine, Harvard Medical School, 77 Avenue Louis Pasteur, Boston, MA 02115. E-mail: ljwoodward@bwh.harvard.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives: This study examines the selective, sustained, and executive attention abilities of very preterm (VPT) born children in relation to concurrent structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures of regional gray matter development at age 12 years. Methods: A regional cohort of 110 VPT (≤32 weeks gestation) and 113 full term (FT) born children were assessed at corrected age 12 years on the Test of Everyday Attention-Children. They also had a structural MRI scan that was subsequently analyzed using voxel-based morphometry to quantify regional between-group differences in cerebral gray matter development, which were then related to attention measures using multivariate methods. Results: VPT children obtained similar selective (p=.85), but poorer sustained (p=.02) and executive attention (p=.01) scores than FT children. VPT children were also characterized by reduced gray matter in the bilateral parietal, temporal, prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices, bilateral thalami, and left hippocampus; and increased gray matter in the occipital and anterior cingulate cortices (family-wise error–corrected p<.05). Poorer sustained auditory attention was associated with increased gray matter in the anterior cingulate cortex (p=.04). Poor executive shifting attention was associated with reduced gray matter in the right superior temporal cortex (p=.04) and bilateral thalami (p=.05). Poorer executive divided attention was associated with reduced gray matter in the occipital (p=.001), posterior cingulate (p=.02), and left temporal (p=.01) cortices; and increased gray matter in the anterior cingulate cortex (p=.001). Conclusions: Disturbances in regional gray matter development appear to contribute, at least in part, to the poorer attentional performance of VPT children at school age. (JINS, 2017, 23, 539–550)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017 

INTRODUCTION

Children born very preterm (VPT; ≤32 weeks gestation) are at increased risk of attention problems and are twice as likely as full term (FT) children to receive a diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever & Oosterlaan, Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009; Bhutta, Cleves, Casey, Cradock & Ananad, Reference Bhutta, Cleves, Casey, Cradock and Ananad2002). Despite higher risks of inattentive ADHD (Jaekel, Wolke & Bartmann, Reference Jaekel, Wolke and Bartmann2013), few follow-up studies have included neuropsychological measures that differentially assess key components of attention. Thus, the nature and extent of attention problems in VPT children are not well understood.

Attention is comprised of functionally related, yet distinct, cognitive and neural processes (Dosenbach, Fair, Cohen, Schlagger, & Petersen, 2008; Petersen & Posner, Reference Petersen and Posner2012). Three key domains of attention include selective, sustained, and executive attention. Selective attention is the capacity to orient to, prioritize, and select task-relevant information (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014). The dorsal and ventral attention networks underlie selective attention, and include the intraparietal sulcus and superior parietal cortex, and the temporal–parietal junction and ventral frontal cortex, respectively (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, Reference Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz and Posner2002; Petersen & Posner, Reference Petersen and Posner2012). Supported by frontal and parietal cortices, sustained attention is the maintenance of a vigilant state to detect incoming/changing task-based information, (Sarter, Givens, & Bruno, Reference Sarter, Givens and Bruno2001; Fan et al., Reference Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz and Posner2002).

Finally, executive attention is maintained by two networks that use top–down processes to control, shift, and divide attention across competing information/tasks (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014; Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Fair, Cohen, Schlaggar and Petersen2008). The fronto-parietal network includes the dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices and the intraparietal sulcus, and modulates task/response selection (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Fair, Cohen, Schlaggar and Petersen2008; Posner & Petersen, Reference Posner and Petersen1990). The cingulo-opercular network, characterized by the anterior prefrontal and medial frontal cortices, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the thalamus, detects task conflict and monitors performance (De Pisapia & Braver, Reference De Pisapia and Braver2006; Posner & Petersen, Reference Posner and Petersen1990; Power et al., Reference Power, Cohen, Nelson, Wig, Barnes, Church and Petersen2011). In children, the right superior temporal gyrus and the bilateral occipital cortex are also involved in executive attention (Konrad et al., Reference Konrad, Neufang, Thiel, Specht, Hanisch, Fan and Fink2005).

Findings from existing follow-up studies suggest that school age children born VPT are at increased risk of poorer sustained and executive attention compared to FT children, while findings relating to selective attention are mixed. In terms of sustained attention, VPT children are less accurate and take longer to complete continuous performance tasks than FT children (de Kieviet, van Elburg, Lafeber, & Oosterlaan, Reference de Kieviet, van Elburg, Lafeber and Oosterlaan2012; Shum, Neulinger, O’Callaghn, & Mohay, Reference Shum, Neulinger, O’Callaghn and Mohay2008). They are also more likely to lapse into absent-minded responding on behavioral measures of inhibitory control (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011; Bayless & Stevenson, Reference Bayless and Stevenson2007; Mulder, Pitchford, & Marlow, Reference Mulder, Pitchford and Marlow2011).

In contrast to sustained attention, links between prematurity and visual selective attention are less clear. For example, Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011) found that 43% of extremely preterm (<28 weeks gestation) children had impaired selective attention compared to 17% of FT children at age 7. Conversely, other studies have not found a significant difference between VPT and FT children in selective attention (Bayless & Stevenson, Reference Bayless and Stevenson2007; Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Pitchford and Marlow2011; Shum et al., Reference Shum, Neulinger, O’Callaghn and Mohay2008). However, these latter studies were characterized by smaller samples, low participant response rates, and/or differences in attention measure used, which may potentially explain differences in study findings.

Compared to sustained attention, stronger links between VPT birth and executive attention have been found at school age. For shifting attention, several studies report that VPT children obtain raw or standardized shifting attention scores at least half a standard deviation below FT children (Bayless & Stevenson Reference Bayless and Stevenson2007; Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Pitchford and Marlow2011; Shum et al., Reference Shum, Neulinger, O’Callaghn and Mohay2008). Extremely preterm children struggle even more on shifting attention tasks, having odds of impairment that are 3.6 times higher than FT children (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011). To date, executive divided attention remains understudied. Although findings from two school age Australian cohorts suggest that 62% of extremely and 41% of VPT born children have impairments in executive divided attention compared to 20–26% of FT children (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Scratch, Thompson, Inder, Doyle, Anderson and Anderson2014).

Follow-up studies have examined the extent to which neonatal clinical factors predict attention impairments, with relatively modest results (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011; Wilson-Ching et al., Reference Wilson-Ching, Molloy, Anderson, Burnett, Roberts, Cheong and Anderson2013). Although diffuse white matter abnormalities on neonatal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) significantly predict cognition and executive function in VPT children (Iwata et al., Reference Iwata, Nakamura, Kihara, Takashima, Matsushi and Iwata2012; Woodward, Anderson, Austin, Howard, & Inder, Reference Woodward, Anderson, Austin, Howard and Inder2006; Woodward, Clark, Bora, & Inder, Reference Woodward, Clark, Bora and Inder2012), cerebral growth and maturation appear to be more strongly associated with attention (Bora, Pritchard, Chen, Inder, & Woodward, Reference Bora, Pritchard, Chen, Inder and Woodward2014). Specifically, Bora et al. (Reference Bora, Pritchard, Chen, Inder and Woodward2014) found that reduced cerebral tissue volumes in dorsal prefrontal, orbitofrontal, sensorimotor, and parieto-occipital regions at term were associated with attention/hyperactivity problems in VPT children from age 4 to 9 years.

Further elucidating the role of early brain development, gray matter abnormalities assessed on neonatal MRI have been related to inattention in the Victorian Infant Brain Studies (VIBeS) cohort of children born <30 weeks gestation. Murray et al. (Reference Murray, Scratch, Thompson, Inder, Doyle, Anderson and Anderson2014) used an abnormality scoring system comprised of qualitative ratings and brain metrics and found that gray matter abnormalities in the caudate heads, lentiform nuclei, and thalami were strong predictors of shifting and divided attention at age 7. Although Murray et al. (Reference Murray, Scratch, Thompson, Inder, Doyle, Anderson and Anderson2014) highlighted subcortical gray matter, concurrent relations between attention and diffusion tensor imaging findings were also found (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Thompson, Pascoe, Leemans, Inder, Doyle and Anderson2016). Specifically, reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) and increased radial diffusivity in the left cingulum bundle was associated with impairments in selective, sustained, and divided attention amongst VPT children. Reduced FA in the corpus callosum was associated with poorer selective attention, while reduced FA in the superior longitudinal fasciculus related to poorer sustained and divided attention. Altered maturation of the corpus callosum between term and age 7 years in the same cohort has also been linked with poorer sustained attention (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Lee, van Bijnen, Leemans, Pascoe, Scratch and Anderson2015).

Together, these studies suggest that early alterations in cerebral maturation and gray matter development place the VPT infant at risk of inattention. Part of the mechanism also appears to reflect ongoing abnormalities in white matter fiber tracts underlying the networks of attention (Leech, Kamourieh, Beckmann, & Sharp, Reference Leech, Kamourieh, Beckmann and Sharp2011; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Thompson, Pascoe, Leemans, Inder, Doyle and Anderson2016; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Lee, van Bijnen, Leemans, Pascoe, Scratch and Anderson2015). However, a remaining issue concerns the extent to which regional gray matter development relates to inattention in VPT children at school age. Thus, the aims of this study were to examine in VPT and FT children at age 12 years: (1) birth-group differences in selective, sustained, and executive attention functioning; (2) birth-group differences in regional gray matter volumes; and (3) the extent to which regional gray matter volumes explain relations between VPT birth and attention functioning in childhood. We hypothesized that VPT birth would be associated with poorer selective, sustained, and executive attention functioning at age 12 years. We also hypothesized that volumetric alterations in key anatomical gray matter regions implicated in the networks of attention, including the prefrontal, temporal, parietal, anterior and posterior cingulate cortices, and deep nuclear gray matter would help to explain links between VPT birth and inattention at age 12 years.

METHOD

Sample

Two groups of children, born 1998–2000, were studied prospectively to age 12 years. The first group consisted of a non-selective regional cohort of 110 VPT infants (≤32 weeks gestation) who were admitted consecutively to the Level-III Neonatal Intensive Care Unit at Christchurch Women’s Hospital, New Zealand (92% recruitment). Excluding deaths (n=3), 97% of VPT children were assessed at corrected age 12 years. The second group included 113 FT infants (38–41 weeks gestation). These infants were identified from hospital records (N=7200 live births) by selecting for each VPT infant, the second-previous or the second-next, same-sex FT born infant in the birth register (62% recruitment) to match infants on date of birth and to enhance random selection of the FT group.

At age 12 years, 96% (109/113) of FT children were assessed. Exclusion criteria for both groups included congenital anomalies, fetal alcohol syndrome, and/or non-English speaking parents. Comparison with regional census data showed that families of FT infants were highly representative of the Canterbury region from which they were recruited (Statistics New Zealand, 2001). Recruited and non-recruited infants did not differ in gestational age, birth weight, sex, family type, ethnicity, or socioeconomic status (p>.05). Sample characteristics are provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Clinical and social background characteristics of FT and VPI (n=206)

a Retinopathy of prematurity data available for 99 preterm infants from hospital records.

b Handedness collected from neurological exam at age 9 years for 201/206 children seen at age 12 years (98%).

Procedures

At age 12, children completed a two-phase evaluation that consisted of a half-day neurodevelopmental assessment at the University of Canterbury and a 60-min MRI scan at the New Zealand Brain Research Institute. Testers and MRI technicians were blind to children’s birth group. There was no significant difference in the age of children at the time of their neurodevelopmental assessment (VPT: 12.1±0.1 years; FT: 12.2±0.1 years; p=.45). However, there was a small tendency for FT children to be 3 weeks older than the corrected age of VPT children at the time of their MRI assessment (p=.02) due to a short-term closure of the MRI facility and scheduling conflicts. All study procedures were approved by the Upper South Regional Ethics Committee (URA/10/05/040). Written and informed consent was obtained from all parents/guardians and assent from all children.

Measures

Attention

Five subtests from the Test of Everyday Attention-Children (TEA-Ch; Manly et al., Reference Manly, Anderson, Nimmo-Smith, Turner, Watson and Robertson2001) were used to assess children’s selective, sustained, and executive attention at age 12 years. These included Sky Search (visual selective attention), Score! (sustained auditory attention), Walk Don’t Walk (sustained response inhibition), Creature Counting (executive shifting attention), and Sky Search Dual Task (DT, executive divided attention). Sky Search requires a child to visually scan and select target spaceships as quickly as possible. The Sky Search Attention Score is reported, which reflects children’s time and accuracy while taking motor control into account. The Score! subtest consists of 10 trials in which a child is required to count laser sounds that are presented at varying intervals without using counting aids. Walk Don’t Walk requires children to maintain a state of awareness and inhibit drawing on a paper trail when directed by an auditory cue. Creature Counting requires the flexible transfer of attention as children count creatures along a trail, changing counting direction when prompted by arrows positioned along the trail. The total number of correct trials is reported for Score! (max=10), Creature Counting (max=7), and Walk Don’t Walk (max=20).

Finally, the Sky Search DT involves a child simultaneously having to complete the Sky Search and Score! task at the same time. The Sky Search DT Decrement Score is reported and represents the extent to which performance is compromised relative to the Sky Search single task condition. The TEA-Ch is psychometrically reliable and has good convergent validity with existing attention tasks (Manly et al., Reference Manly, Anderson, Nimmo-Smith, Turner, Watson and Robertson2001). Raw scores are reported due to narrow and equivalent age range between-groups, selection of a single test battery, and to compare VPT children against a regionally-representative FT comparison group. Attention data were collected for 96% (100/104) of VPT and 97% (106/109) of FT children. Reasons for data loss included global developmental delay (n=2), severe cerebral palsy (n=1), blindness (n=1), overseas relocation (n=1), task refusal (n=1), and file misplaced (n=1).

MRI

Structural images were acquired on a 3 Tesla GE HDxt scanner, with an eight channel head coil and a T1-weighted, three-dimensional inversion recovery-prepared spoiled gradient recalled echo acquisition (250 mm field-of-view, 256×256 acquisition matrix, sagittal acquisition, 186 1-mm-thick slices, ~1 mm3 isotropic voxels, echo time/repetition time=4.8/10.7 ms, flip angle=15°, inversion time=400 ms). MRI scans were obtained for 93% (97/104) of VPT and 89% (97/109) of FT children. Scans were not obtained for 19 children due to orthodontic braces (n=4), anxiety (n=4), relocation (n=2), and declined MRI (n=9). All acquired scans were visually inspected by two blinded reviewers for image quality. Based on this review, eight scans were excluded (motion=6, hydrocephalus/parieto-occipital shunt=1, nifti failure=1) leaving 186 usable scans (VPT=90; FT=96).

T1-weighted images were pre-processed using VBM8 and TOM8 (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/) toolboxes for SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) in Matlab (Matlab R2013a). Images were manually realigned to the anterior commissure. A study-specific DARTEL template was created by performing an initial segmentation of the structural images into gray and white matter segments. Using an affine transformation, segments were normalized to tissue probability maps created in TOM8 using NIH age- and sex-matched priors to provide a-priori knowledge of the spatial distribution of tissue types and improve image registration (Mechelli, Price, Friston, & Ashburner, Reference Mechelli, Price, Friston and Ashburner2005; Waber et al., Reference Waber, Moor, Forbes, Almli, Botteron, Leonard and Rumsey2007). Visual inspection of gray matter segments saw four subjects excluded due to poor segmentation (VPT=89; FT=93).

Structural images were then intensity bias corrected, tissue classified again, and spatially normalized to the study-specific DARTEL template. Next, gray matter segments were modulated by non-linear components of the spatial normalization only (i.e., multiplying warped tissue segments by the Jacobian determinant of the warp) to preserve actual tissue values locally and account for individual brain size globally during spatial normalization. Modulation allows gray matter values to be interpreted as a measure of the regional difference in the absolute volume of gray matter at the voxel-level rather than gray matter concentration (Ashburner, Reference Ashburner2007, Reference Ashburner2009).

The modulated, normalized gray matter segments were smoothed using an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel to improve the signal-to-noise ratio, to ensure that the assumptions underlying Gaussian random fields theory were met, and to account for any spatial normalization imperfections (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2000). To restrict analysis to gray matter voxels, a mask was created from the mean gray matter image of all 89 VPT and the first consecutive 89 FT modulated, normalized gray matter segments (threshold≥0.15). An equal number of VPT and FT gray matter segments were selected for the mask so that the isolation of gray matter voxels would not be biased toward the larger FT group.

Social risk

To assess the socio-familial characteristics of the sample, a social risk index score was created based on five key indicators of children’s family social background assessed over the first 2 years of life (score range: 0–5). These factors were all dichotomized into present (1) or absent (0) and summed. They included: (1) mother <21 years old, (2) child born into single parent family, (3) ethnic minority, (4) mother had no formal high-school qualification, and (5) birth family a low socioeconomic household. The Elly-Irving Socioeconomic Index measured family socioeconomic status (SES; Elly & Irving, Reference Elley and Irving2003), with ratings based on the profession of the highest earning parent. Low SES included semiskilled, unskilled, and unemployed primary breadwinner.

Data Analysis

Data analysis was performed with the largest data set available for attention (VPT=100; FT=106) and gray matter (VPT=89; FT=93) outcomes to enhance detection of small-to-medium effect sizes, rather than limiting analysis to a smaller subset of children who had complete attention and gray matter data (VPT=87; FT=91). Subject numbers, therefore, vary accordingly.

First, birth-group differences in attention were examined using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) which takes into account several dependent variables in combination, thus reducing the probability of Type 1 error. If a significant effect of group is found on the multivariate F statistic, ANOVA describes the pattern of group differences on the individual TEA-Ch subtests. ANOVA results were not corrected for multiple comparisons as this study focuses on the patterns and magnitudes of group differences rather than p-values (see Anderson & Doyle, 2004; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Thompson, Pascoe, Leemans, Inder, Doyle and Anderson2016; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Chen, Beare, Adamson, Ellis, Ahmadzai and Anderson2016). Cohen’s d provided an estimate of effect size. Chi-squared analysis compared rates of attention impairment between-groups, defined as a TEA-Ch score greater than 1SD below the mean of the FT group (see Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011; Nosarti et al., Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg and Murray2008). Odds ratios (ORs; 95% confidence intervals) provided an estimate of effect size for rates of impairment. TEA-Ch results were re-run adjusting for social risk index.

As MANOVA is highly sensitive to statistical outliers, TEA-Ch outliers (>3SD) were reassigned the next poorest non-outlier value in their respective birth group to retain children in the analysis. Outlier scores were reassigned for four children on Sky Search, seven children on Sky Search DT, and one child on Walk Don’t Walk. Reassigning outlier scores did not alter the pattern of significant findings. Furthermore, independent t tests performed without statistical outliers (>3 SD) did not change study findings (please see Table 5 of Online Supplement 1).

Second, whole-brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used to quantify regional gray matter development between VPT and FT children. In SPM8, independent sample t tests were performed on smoothed, modulated, normalized gray matter segments and tested two Contrasts: (1) regional reductions in gray matter volume in VPT relative to FT children, and (2) regional increases in gray matter volume in VPT relative to FT children. Analysis was family-wise error (FWE) rate corrected for multiple comparisons (uncorrected p<.001 followed by voxel-wise FWE-corrected, p<.05). Clusters k>10 voxels are reported. Birth-group differences in regional gray matter volumes were re-examined in SPM8 adjusting for covariates: age at MRI scan, sex, puberty, and social risk index (Table 6 of Online Supplement 1). Puberty was assessed with The Tanner Scale (Marshall & Tanner, Reference Marshall and Tanner1969, Reference Marshall and Tanner1970).

Third, modulated, normalized gray matter volume values were extracted from the VBM clusters surviving FWE-correction in Contrasts 1 and 2, and related to attention measures for all children (n=178) using multivariate linear regression in SPSS (Version 23, IBM New York). Inspection of regional gray matter volume values did not identify any significant (>3 SD) outliers. Regression models were developed in a stepwise manner for each domain of attention. In Step 1, birth group (VPT=1; FT=2), alongside the following four covariates (age at MRI scan, sex [M=1; F=2], puberty, and social risk index), was entered to examine the proportion of variance in attention scores attributed to birth group.

In Step 2, regional gray matter volumes that were significantly associated with prematurity from the VBM results were added to this base model in a stepwise manner to determine which gray matter regions explained birth-group differences in attention outcomes. All gray matter regions associated with prematurity were included as previous developmental research has shown that the neural correlates of attention in children includes regions outside a-priori defined regions-of-interest (Konrad et al., Reference Konrad, Neufang, Thiel, Specht, Hanisch, Fan and Fink2005). Stepwise criteria for variable inclusion was set at Probability of F Entry p=.05, and Removal p=.10. Final models were examined for violations in nonlinearity, distribution of errors, homoscedasticity, and multicollinearity. The total proportion of variance accounted for, regression coefficients, standard errors, and significance levels are reported. A supplementary analysis was conducted including gestational age as a covariate with sex, puberty and age at MRI scan (Tables 7 and 8 of Online Supplement 1).

RESULTS

Attention at Age 12

As shown in Table 2, the effect of birth group on the multivariate F statistic representing the linear combination of the five TEA-Ch subtests was highly significant (F(5,198)=5.12; p=.001). Results for each of the individual subtests are as follows.

Table 2 Mean TEA-Ch scores and rates of impairment for FT and VPT children at age 12 years (n=206)

Note. Raw TEA-Ch scores are reported. λ: Wilks’ Lambda.

a Higher scores indicate poorer performance.

b FT n=104 as MANOVA excluded two cases with partial data.

CI=confidence interval.

Selective attention

VPT children did not differ significantly from FT children in their mean selective attention scores (p=.85; d=.03) or their rates of impairment (p=.41; OR=0.71) on the Sky Search task.

Sustained attention

In terms of sustained auditory attention, VPT children obtained fewer correct items than FT children on the Score! subtest (p=.02; d=.33), indicating that they were less vigilant across the task. They also achieved fewer correct trials than FT children on the Walk Don’t Walk subtest which assessed sustained response inhibition (p=.02; d=.32). Examination of rates of impairment across these two sustained attention tasks showed that the performance of VPT children was more often impaired when inhibiting a response (29% vs. 16%; p=.03; OR=2.09) than when simply being asked to sustain their attention (p=.38; OR=1.33).

Executive attention

VPT children were less accurate than FT children on the Creature Counting subtest, which assesses the ability to shift attention (p=.01; d=.36). They were also twice as likely to have a total correct score in the impaired range (30% vs. 14%; p=.006, OR=2.60). In terms of executive divided attention, both groups showed the expected decrement in performance under dual task conditions, but the impact on performance was greater for VPT children. Specifically, VPT children had higher mean Sky Search DT Decrement Scores compared to FT children (p=.001; d=.63), suggesting greater difficulty dividing their attentional resources. Approximately one quarter of the VPT group (24%) had a Sky Search DT Decrement Score in the impairment range, compared to only 10% of FT children (p=.005; OR=3.03).

To assess the extent to which group differences in attention might be explained by differences in social background, TEA-Ch analyses were re-run adjusting for social risk index. As shown in the last column of Table 2, results remained largely unchanged with the exception of mean scores on Score! (p=.12) and Walk Don’t Walk (p=.10), which were attenuated to marginal non-significance, although the risks of sustained attentional impairment were not.

Regional Gray Matter Volume at Age 12

Contrast 1: VPT<FT

Table 3 and Figure 1 show the VBM results for Contrast 1, showing regions where VPT children had reduced gray matter compared to FT children. These regions included the bilateral parietal cortex (medial juxtapositional lobule, extending from the paracentral lobule to the pre/post-central lobule), right inferior post-central gyrus, right superior temporal sulcus, left superior temporal sulcus, left prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate gyrus, bilateral thalami, left parahippocampus, and right inferior cerebellum (FWE-corrected p<.05).

Fig. 1 Statistical parametric maps showing regional gray matter volume differences between VPT (n=89) and FT (n=93) children at age 12 years (voxel-wise FWE-corrected, p<.05). Blue indicates areas of reduced gray matter volume in VPT relative to FT. Red indicates increased gray matter volume in VPT relative to FT. Color shade indicates t-value, displayed on the colorbar.

Table 3 MNI coordinates for voxels with peak t- and Z-scores from VBM analysis comparing regional differences in gray matter volume between FT (n=93) and VPT (n=89) children at age 12 years

Note. Cluster-extent threshold k>10.

a P-value adjusted in SPM8 for: age at MRI scan, sex, puberty, and social risk index.

MNI=Montreal Neurological Institute; Inf=infinity.

Contrast 2: VPT>FT

Also shown in Table 3 and Figure 1 are the VBM results for Contrast 2 which identified regions where VPT children had increased gray matter relative to FT children. These regions included the medial occipital cortex (supra-calcarine, cuneal, intra-calcarine) and the medial anterior cingulate gyrus (FWE-corrected p<.05). Additional slices from the VBM statistical parametric maps are included in Online Supplement 2.

To account for alternative explanations of VBM findings, group differences in regional gray matter volumes were re-run in SPM8, adjusting for children’s age at MRI scan, sex, puberty, and social risk index. As shown in the last column of Table 3, the results of Contrasts 1 and 2 remained unchanged. Adjusted results are reported in full in Table 6 of Online Supplement 1.

Regional Gray Matter Volume and Attention at Age 12

Whole-brain VBM findings identified regions in which VPT children showed altered cerebral gray matter development compared to FT children at age 12. The final aim of the study was to examine the extent to which regional gray matter volume explains, at least in part, associations between VPT birth status and poor attentional performance at age 12. Normalized gray matter volume values were extracted from the VBM clusters that survived FWE-correction and related to attention measures using multivariate stepwise linear regression. Based upon the non-significant finding for selective attention, Sky Search was excluded from this analysis. Table 4 shows the extent to which regional gray matter volumes explain the sustained and executive attention problems associated with VPT birth. For each model, Step 1 shows the proportion of variance in attention scores accounted for by birth group and any significant covariates (p≤.05). Step 2 shows the gray matter regions that were significantly associated with attention (p≤.05). Regression analysis preformed with VPT and FT groups separately yielded a similar set of gray matter regions associated with attention at age 12 (Tables 7 and 8 of Online Supplement 1).

Table 4 Summary of regression models for the prediction of attention outcomes in all children (FT=91, VPT=87) at age 12 years

Sustained attention

As shown in Table 4, increased gray matter in the anterior cingulate gyrus was associated with poorer sustained auditory attention on the Score! task (β=−.16; p=.04) and helped to, at least partially, explain between-group differences in very preterm and FT children’s performance (p=.02). In contrast, no significant associations were found between any gray matter regions and sustained response inhibition at age 12.

Executive attention

Shown in Table 4, reduced gray matter in the right superior temporal sulcus (β=.19; p=.04) and bilateral thalami (β=.17; p=.05) were significantly related to poorer Creature Counting scores which provided a measure of executive shifting attention. These associations were in the expected direction based on the results of Aims 1 and 2. Inclusion of these variables in the model attenuated between-group differences to non-significance, suggesting that thalamic and right temporal regions may explain associations between preterm birth and poor executive shifting attention outcomes (Step 1 p=.03; Step 2 p=.70).

In terms of executive divided attention, results show that between-group differences in children’s Sky Search DT performance (β=−.32; p=.001) were explained by altered gray matter development in four regions (p=.28). Specifically, poorer performance on this subtest (i.e., higher scores) was associated with reduced gray matter in the posterior cingulate gyrus (β=−.19; p=.02), left superior temporal sulcus (β=−.24; p=.01), and occipital cortex (β=−.27; p=.001). Poor performance on this task was also associated with increased gray matter in the anterior cingulate gyrus (β=.25; p=.001).

DISCUSSION

This study examines the selective, sustained and executive attention outcomes of a regional cohort of VPT children at school age. Of particular interest was the extent to which children’s attention skills were explained by concurrent volumetric MRI measures of regional gray matter development. The key findings of this study are discussed below.

VPT children showed difficulties in sustained and executive attention compared to FT children, while abilities in selective attention were similar between the two birth groups. Importantly, group differences on measures of sustained attention and all measures of executive attention persisted after accounting for family social risk. Specifically, poorer scores and higher rates of impairment persisted for VPT children compared to FT in executive shifting attention and executive divided attention after adjusting for social risk index. For sustained attention, adjusted rates of impairment in sustained response inhibition remained higher in VPT children relative to FT children, although findings suggested that social risk may, at least in part, be contributing to between group differences in test scores on these measures.

Our finding with respect to visual selective attention is consistent with existing reports of similar selective attention abilities between VPT and FT children at school age. Specifically, two studies, Mulder et al. (Reference Mulder, Pitchford and Marlow2011) and Bayless and Stevenson (Reference Bayless and Stevenson2007), found that VPT and FT children were either similarly accurate on Sky Search or obtained equivalent Sky Search Attention Scores. In contrast, two larger Australian studies found that VPT children identified significantly fewer Sky Search targets than FT children at age 7 (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Scratch, Thompson, Inder, Doyle, Anderson and Anderson2014, Reference Murray, Thompson, Pascoe, Leemans, Inder, Doyle and Anderson2016). It is, therefore, possible that effects in selective attention are small and larger samples are needed to detect group differences. Additionally, selective attention impairments may be more prominent in children born at earlier gestational ages (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011).

Regarding sustained attention, the small to medium effect size found on the Score! subtest (d=.33) was similar to Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011) on this task (d=.34). However, it was smaller than that observed by de Kieviet et al. (Reference de Kieviet, van Elburg, Lafeber and Oosterlaan2012) and Shum et al. (Reference Shum, Neulinger, O’Callaghn and Mohay2008) on the Attention Network (d=.55) and Trail Making (d=.50) tasks. These effect size variations may reflect differences in the clinical characteristics of the samples and/or differences in measurement sensitivity or task demands. Attention tasks may emphasize accurate and/or speeded responding, or draw upon information processing or motor control skills, making direct comparisons across studies difficult (Mulder, Pitchford, Hagger, & Marlow, Reference Mulder, Pitchford, Hagger and Marlow2009).

Finally, our results for executive attention were highly consistent with previous research (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, De Luca, Hutchinson, Spencer-Smith, Roberts and Doyle2011; Bayless & Stevenson, Reference Bayless and Stevenson2007). We found that VPT children had odds of impaired attentional shifting that were 2.6 times higher and divided attention 3 times higher than FT children. Murray et al. (Reference Murray, Scratch, Thompson, Inder, Doyle, Anderson and Anderson2014) reported similar odds of impairment for executive attention, although odds for shifting impairments (OR=3.3) were slightly higher than for divided attention (OR=2.9). These findings suggest that VPT children may particularly struggle with the effortful control of attention.

At age 12, VPT children had reduced gray matter in the bilateral parietal and temporal cortices, right inferior post-central sulcus, posterior cingulate gyrus, left prefrontal cortex, left parahippocampus, right inferior cerebellum, and bilateral thalami compared to FT children. Decreased gray matter volume has also been reported in similar regions across several preterm studies (Kesler et al., Reference Kesler, Reiss, Vohr, Watson, Schneider, Katz and Ment2008; Nagy et al., Reference Nagy, Ashburner, Andersson, Jbabdi, Draganski, Skare and Largercrantz2009; Nosarti et al., Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg and Murray2008; Soria-Pastor et al., Reference Soria-Pastor, Padilla, Zubiaurre-Elorza, Ibarretxe-Bilbao, Botet, Costas-Moragas and Junque2009). In contrast, Kesler et al. (Reference Kesler, Ment, Vohr, Pajot, Schneider, Katz and Reiss2004) found that parietal gray matter was 4–5% larger in VPT compared to FT children at age 7–11 years. The discrepancy in findings may reflect differences in approach used to analyze structural MRI measures. Kesler et al. (Reference Kesler, Ment, Vohr, Pajot, Schneider, Katz and Reiss2004) used a large region-of-interest scheme whereas we used VBM to investigate highly localized regions of gray matter. Even so, decreased regional gray matter volume in preterm infants is thought to be an ongoing consequence of injury to pre-oligodendrocytes, poor axonal myelination, and/or trophic anomalies in developing gray matter (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Counsell, Rueckert, Kapellou, Bhatia, Aljabar and Edwards2006; Leviton & Gressens, Reference Leviton and Gressens2007; Volpe, Reference Volpe2009a). Further research is, however, needed to delineate whether gray matter alterations are due to abnormalities in the underlying regional white matter and/or postnatal experiences during the neonatal period (Norsarti et al., 2011; Volpe, Reference Volpe2009b).

In addition to reduced gray matter, study findings also identified increased gray matter volume in the medial anterior cingulate gyrus and occipital cortex in VPT relative to FT children at age 12 (see Nosarti et al., Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg and Murray2008, Reference Nosarti, Mechelli, Herrera, Walshe, Shergill, Murray and Allin2011; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Vohr, Staib, Cannistraci, Dolberg, Schneider and Ment2000). Increased gray matter in the anterior portion of the cingulate cortex, coupled with reductions in the posterior area, suggest regional vulnerability in the development of the cingulum (see Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Inder, Neil, Dierker, Alexopoulos, Anderson and Van Essen2015). Increased gray matter volume in VPT children may indicate impaired or delayed synaptic pruning that should occur during typical recessive maturational processes (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos and Rapoport1999; Murner-Lavanchy et al., Reference Murner-Lavanchy, Steinlin, Nelle, Rummel, Perrig, Schroth and Everts2014; Nosarti et al., Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg and Murray2008).

Current study findings indicate that volumetric alterations in the gray matter of key brain regions helped to explain the attention outcomes of VPT children at age 12. First, gray matter in the cingulum was associated with sustained auditory attention. Overall, the cingulum of VPT children was characterized by increased gray matter in the anterior region and decreased gray matter in the posterior region. Our findings suggest that increased gray matter in the anterior region of the cingulum is more strongly related to poorer sustained attention than the posterior region. The anterior cingulate cortex is thought to play a role in regulating attention and monitoring task performance (Cohen Reference Cohen2014; Sadaghiani & D’Esposito, Reference Sadaghiani and D’Esposito2014; Shenhav, Botvinick, & Cohen, Reference Shenhav, Botvinick and Cohen2013). Of interest, Murray et al. (Reference Murray, Thompson, Pascoe, Leemans, Inder, Doyle and Anderson2016) also linked white matter aberrations in the cingulum bundle to sustained attention, which when taken with current study findings, highlights the vulnerability of the cingulum for sustained attention in VPT children.

Second, reduced gray matter in the right superior temporal sulcus and the bilateral thalami appeared to explain the relationship between VPT birth and poorer executive shifting attention. Thalamic and temporal regions are theoretically linked to executive attention. For example, the thalamus is implicated in the cingulo-opercular network and plays a role in the covert shifting of visual selective attention (Kastner, Saalmann, & Schneider, Reference Kastner, Saalmann and Schneider2012; Posner & Petersen, Reference Posner and Petersen1990) and modulation of arousal during task performance (Kinomura, Larsson, Gulyás, & Roland, Reference Kinomura, Larsson, Gulyás and Roland1996; Petersen & Posner, Reference Petersen and Posner2012). The superior temporal cortex more generally forms part of the ventral information processing system and may support the top–town control of executive attention in children (Konrad et al., Reference Konrad, Neufang, Thiel, Specht, Hanisch, Fan and Fink2005; Shapiro, Hillstrom, & Husain, Reference Shapiro, Hillstrom and Husain2002; Vossel, Gegg, & Fink, Reference Vossel, Geng and Fink2014).

Third, and last, reduced gray matter in the occipital, left temporal, and posterior cingulate regions; and increased gray matter in the anterior cingulate gyrus was associated with poorer executive divided attention. The occipital cortex is an early and higher-order visual cortical area that processes high-priority information in the visual field during selective attention tasks (Davidesco et al., Reference Davidesco, Harel, Ramot, Kramer, Kipervasser, Andelman and Malach2013, Heinze et al., Reference Heinze, Mangun, Burchert, Hinrichs, Scholz, Munte and Hillyard1994; Woldorff et al., Reference Woldorff, Liotti, Seabolt, Busse, Lancaster and Fox2002). Although VPT birth was associated with increased gray matter in the occipital region relative to controls, increased gray matter was associated with lower (i.e., better) Sky Search DT scores. We interpret this finding with caution but speculate that alterations in the neural architecture of gray matter in the cerebral visual processing region of the VPT brain may be a marker for impaired visual selective attention when tested under dual task conditions (cf. Shah et al., Reference Shah, Guinane, August, Austin, Woodward, Thompson and Inder2006).

Although the precise neurobiological mechanism remains unclear, this finding could also indicate neural compensation in such that increased gray matter supports visual attention when neural abnormalities are present in the wider networks of attention. Nonetheless, both the posterior and anterior regions of the cingulate were associated with performance on Sky Search DT. The posterior and anterior cingulate regions are strongly implicated in the default mode and cingulo-opercular networks which are modulated by task demands/conflict (Berg, Alaburda, & Hounsgaard, Reference Berg, Alaburda and Hounsgaard2007; Greicius, Krasnow, Reiss, & Menon, Reference Greicius, Krasnow, Reiss and Menon2003; Power et al., Reference Power, Cohen, Nelson, Wig, Barnes, Church and Petersen2011). The anterior cingulate cortex detects and resolves task conflict (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014; De Pisapia & Braver, Reference De Pisapia and Braver2006; Petersen & Posner, Reference Petersen and Posner2012) and the posterior cingulate cortex acts as a hub region in the executive network integrating information that has been processed by other connected brain regions (Leech, Braga, & Sharp, Reference Leech, Braga and Sharp2012). Therefore, gray matter alterations/abnormalities in regions implicated in the functional networks of attention suggest a neural basis for the sustained and executive attention difficulties experienced by VPT children.

Strengths of this study include the prospective longitudinal design, large non-selective samples and high rates of retention, follow-up into school age, the standardized assessment of attention, and adjustment for several covariates. However, limitations should also be noted. First, TEA-Ch data are not reported for children who failed practice items (n=2), were cognitively delayed (n=2), or who had behavioral difficulties (n=1). Thus, attention impairments may be slightly under-reported (cf. Murray et al., Reference Murray, Scratch, Thompson, Inder, Doyle, Anderson and Anderson2014). Second, selective attention was assessed with one subtest whereas sustained and executive attention was assessed with two subtests each. Third, VBM may be less sensitive to the detection of structural alterations than other methods when neuroanatomical variability is high across subjects (Good et al., Reference Good, Johnsrude, Ashburner, Henson, Friston and Frackowiak2001). This factor may explain why the prefrontal cortex was not related to attention measures (cf. Urben et al., Reference Urben, Van Hanswijck De Jonge, Barishnikov, Pizzo, Monnier, Lazeyras and Hüppi2015). Fourth, the inclusion of movement parameters specifically acquired during structural MRI scans may help minimize the effects of very subtle motion artifacts (Reuter et al., Reference Reuter, Tisdall, Qureshi, Buckner, van der Kouwe and Fischl2015). While movement parameters were collected for the resting state and diffusion scans in the larger study, these data were not collected for structural scans.

Nonetheless, the novel contribution of this study is the reported link between regional gray matter volumes and sustained and executive attention functioning in VPT children. Future directions include the replication of study findings using a sophisticated cross-validation multi-modal approach on an independent data set with analysis extended to examine functional and structural connectivity between brain regions associated with attention (see Kesler et al., Reference Kesler, Reiss, Vohr, Watson, Schneider, Katz and Ment2008; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Chen, Beare, Adamson, Ellis, Ahmadzai and Anderson2016).

In conclusion, school age VPT born children experience lapses in attention, are less flexible when shifting attention, and struggle with multitasking. VPT children also show regionally specific disturbances in gray matter development with volumetric alterations in the parietal, temporal, occipital, cingulate, prefrontal and thalamic regions at age 12. Importantly, reduced gray matter in the temporal, occipital, posterior cingulate, and thalamic regions; and increased gray matter in the anterior cingulate cortex helped to explain attention problems associated with prematurity. Thus, results highlight the importance of altered gray matter development in the pathogenesis of attention problems common among children born VPT.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors have no potential or competing conflicts of interest to disclose. This work was supported by the Neurological Foundation of New Zealand (grant number 022/PG); the Lottery Grants Board of New Zealand; the Canterbury Medical Research Foundation (grant number 05/01); and the Health Research Council of New Zealand (grant number 03/196). The authors thank: The Canterbury Child Development Research Group for study coordination and data collection, especially Nicola Austin, Marie Goulden, and Karelia Levin; Prof. Joseph J. Volpe for manuscript feedback; and the children and families involved in this study.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000388

References

REFERENCES

Aarnoudse-Moens, C., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., van Goudoever, B., & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124, 717728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P.J., & Doyle, L.W. (2004). Executive Functioning in School-Aged Children Who Were Born Very Preterm or With Extremely Low Birth Weight in the 1990s. Pediatrics, 114(1), 5057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P.J., De Luca, C., Hutchinson, E., Spencer-Smith, M., Roberts, G., & Doyle, L. (2011). Attention problems in a representative sample of extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 5773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage, 38, 95113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J. (2009). Computational Anatomy with the SPM Software. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 27, 11631174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K.J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry--The methods. Neuroimage, 11, 805821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayless, S., & Stevenson, J. (2007). Executive functions in school age children born very prematurely. Early Human Development, 83, 247254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, R.W., Alaburda, A., & Hounsgaard, J. (2007). Balanced inhibition and excitation drive spike activity in spinal half-centers. Science, 315(5810), 390393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhutta, A.T., Cleves, M.A., Casey, P.H., Cradock, M.M., & Ananad, K.J.S. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school aged children who were born preterm. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 728737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boardman, J., Counsell, S., Rueckert, D., Kapellou, O., Bhatia, K., Aljabar, P., & Edwards, D. (2006). Abnormal deep grey matter development following very preterm birth detected using deformation-based morphometry. Neuroimage, 32, 7078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, S., Pritchard, V., Chen, Z., Inder, T., & Woodward, L. (2014). Neonatal cerebral morphometry and later risk of persistent inattention/hyperactivity in children born very preterm. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 828838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, R. (2014). The neuropsychology of attention (2nd ed.), New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidesco, I., Harel, M., Ramot, M., Kramer, U., Kipervasser, S., Andelman, F., & Malach, R. (2013). Spatial and object-based attention modulates broadband high-frequency responses across the human visual cortical hierarchy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 12281240.Google Scholar
de Kieviet, J., van Elburg, R., Lafeber, H., & Oosterlaan, J. (2012). Attention problems of very preterm children compared with age-matched term controls at school age. Journal of Pediatrics, 161, 824829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Pisapia, N., & Braver, T. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69, 13221326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dosenbach, N.U.F., Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Schlaggar, B.L., & Petersen, S.E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 99105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elley, W.B., & Irving, J.C. (2003). The Elley-Irving Socio-Economic Index: 2001 Census Revision. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 38, 317.Google Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340347.Google Scholar
Giedd, J., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N., Castellanos, F., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., & Rapoport, J. (1999). Brain development during childhood: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861863.Google Scholar
Good, C., Johnsrude, J., Ashburner, J., Henson, R., Friston, K., & Frackowiak, R. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage, 14, 2136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greicius, M.D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A.L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253258.Google Scholar
Heinze, H.J., Mangun, G.R., Burchert, W., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Munte, T.F., & Hillyard, S.A. (1994). Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature, 272, 543546.Google Scholar
Iwata, S., Nakamura, T., Kihara, H., Takashima, S., Matsushi, T., & Iwata, O. (2012). Qualitative brain MRI at term and cognitive outcomes at 9 years after very preterm birth. Pediatrics, 129, e1138e1147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaekel, J., Wolke, D., & Bartmann, P. (2013). Poor attention rather than hyperactivity/ impulsivity predicts academic achievement in very preterm and full-term adolescents. Psychological Medicine, 43, 183196.Google Scholar
Kastner, S., Saalmann, Y., & Schneider, K. (2012). Thalamic control of visual attention. In G. Mangun (Ed.), The neuroscience of attention: Attention control and selection. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kesler, S., Ment, L., Vohr, B., Pajot, S., Schneider, K., Katz, K., & Reiss, A. (2004). Volumetric analysis of regional cerebral development in preterm children. Pediatric Neurology, 31, 318325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesler, S., Reiss, L., Vohr, B., Watson, C., Schneider, K., Katz, K., & Ment, L. (2008). Brain volume reductions within multiple cognitive systems in male preterm children at age twelve. Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 513520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinomura, S., Larsson, J., Gulyás, B., & Roland, P.E. (1996). Activation by attention of the human reticular formation and thalamic intralaminar nuclei. Science, 271, 512515.Google Scholar
Konrad, K., Neufang, S., Thiel, C., Specht, K., Hanisch, C., Fan, J., & Fink, G. (2005). Development of attentional networks: An fMRI study with children and adults. Neuroimage, 28, 429439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leech, R., Braga, R., & Sharp, D. (2012). Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 215222.Google Scholar
Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C.F., & Sharp, D.J. (2011). Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 32173224.Google Scholar
Leviton, A., & Gressens, P. (2007). Neuronal damage accompanies perinatal white matter damage. Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 473478.Google Scholar
Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 10651081.Google Scholar
Marshall, W.A., & Tanner, J.M. (1969). Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 44, 291303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, W.A., & Tanner, J.M. (1970). Variations in the pattern of pubertal changes in boys. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 45, 1323.Google Scholar
Mechelli, A., Price, C., Friston, K., & Ashburner, J. (2005). Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: Methods and applications. Current Medical Imaging Reviews, 1, 19.Google Scholar
Mulder, H., Pitchford, N., Hagger, M., & Marlow, N. (2009). Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: A systematic review. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 393421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, H., Pitchford, N., & Marlow, N. (2011). Processing speed mediates executive function difficulties in very preterm children in middle childhood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 445454.Google Scholar
Murner-Lavanchy, I., Steinlin, M., Nelle, C., Rummel, W, Perrig, G., Schroth, G., & Everts, R. (2014). Delay of cortical thinning in very preterm born children. Early Human Development, 90, 443450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A., Scratch, S., Thompson, D., Inder, T., Doyle, L., Anderson, J., & Anderson, P. (2014). Neonatal brain pathology predicts adverse attention and processing speed outcomes in very preterm children and/or very low birth weight children. Neuropsychology, 28, 552562.Google Scholar
Murray, A., Thompson, D., Pascoe, L., Leemans, A., Inder, T., Doyle, L., & Anderson, P. (2016). White matter abnormalities and impaired attention abilities in children born very preterm. Neuroimage, 124, 7584.Google Scholar
Nagy, Z., Ashburner, J., Andersson, J., Jbabdi, S., Draganski, B., Skare, S., & Largercrantz, H. (2009). Structural correlates of preterm birth in the adolescent brain. Pediatrics, 214, 964972.Google Scholar
Nosarti, C., Giouroukou, E., Healy, E., Rifkin, L., Walshe, M., Reichenberg, A., & Murray, R. (2008). Gray and white matter distribution in very preterm adolescents mediates neurodevelopmental outcome. Brain, 131, 205217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosarti, C., Mechelli, A., Herrera, A., Walshe, M., Shergill, S., Murray, R., & Allin, P. (2011). Structural covariance in the cortex of very preterm adolescents: A voxel-based morphometry study. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 16151625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, S., & Posner, M. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 7389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, B., Vohr, B., Staib, L., Cannistraci, C., Dolberg, A., Schneider, K., & Ment, L. (2000). Regional volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. Journal of American Medical Association, 284, 19391947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M., & Petersen, S. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, J.D., Cohen, A.L., Nelson, S.M., Wig, G.S., Barnes, K.A., Church, J.A., & Petersen, S.E. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron, 72(4), 665678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter, M., Tisdall, M., Qureshi, A., Buckner, R., van der Kouwe, A., & Fischl, B. (2015). Head motion during MRI acquisition reduces gray matter volume and thickness estimates. Neuroimage, 107, 107115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadaghiani, S., & D’Esposito, M. (2014). Functional characterization of the cingulo-opercular network in the maintenance of tonic alertness. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 27632773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Reviews, 35, 146160.Google Scholar
Shah, D.K., Guinane, C., August, P., Austin, N.C., Woodward, L.J., Thompson, D.K., & Inder, T.E. (2006). Reduced occipital regional volumes at term predict impaired visual function in early childhood in very low birth weight infants. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 47, 33663373.Google Scholar
Shapiro, K., Hillstrom, A.P., & Husain, M. (2002). Control of visuotemporal attention by inferior parietal and superior temporal cortex. Current Biology, 12, 13201325.Google Scholar
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M.M., & Cohen, J.D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79, 217240.Google Scholar
Shum, D., Neulinger, K., O’Callaghn, M., & Mohay, H. (2008). Attentional problems in children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight at 7 – 9 years. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 103112.Google Scholar
Soria-Pastor, S., Padilla, N., Zubiaurre-Elorza, L., Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N., Botet, F., Costas-Moragas, C., & Junque, C. (2009). Decreased regional brain volume and cognitive impairment in preterm children at low risk. Pediatrics, 124, e1161e1170.Google Scholar
Statistics New Zealand. (2001). 2001 Census: Regional Summary. Retrieved from http://www.stats.govt.nz/Census/2001-census-data.aspx.Google Scholar
Thompson, D.K., Chen, J., Beare, R., Adamson, C., Ellis, R., Ahmadzai, Z., & Anderson, P.J. (2016). Structural connectivity relates to perinatal factors and functional impairments at 7 years in children born very preterm. Neuroimage, 134, 328337.Google Scholar
Thompson, D.K., Lee, K.J., van Bijnen, L., Leemans, A., Pascoe, L., Scratch, S.E., & Anderson, P.J. (2015). Accelerated corpus callosum development in prematurity predicts improved outcome: Longitudinal corpus callosum development. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 37333748.Google Scholar
Urben, S., Van Hanswijck De Jonge, L., Barishnikov, R., Pizzo, R., Monnier, M., Lazeyras, F., & Hüppi, P. (2015). Gestational age and gender influence on executive control and its related neural structures in preterm-born children at age 6 years. Child Neuropsychology, 23, 188207.Google Scholar
Volpe, J. (2009a). Brain injury in premature infants: A complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurology, 8, 110124.Google Scholar
Volpe, J. (2009b). The encephalopathy of prematurity—Brain injury and impaired brain development inextricably intertwined. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 16, 167178.Google Scholar
Vossel, S., Geng, J.J., & Fink, G.R. (2014). Dorsal and ventral attention systems. The Neuroscientist, 20, 150159.Google Scholar
Waber, D., Moor, C., Forbes, P., Almli, C., Botteron, K., Leonard, G., & Rumsey, J. (2007). The NIH MRI study of normal brain development: Performance of a population based sample of healthy children aged 6 to 18 years on a neuropsychological test battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 118.Google Scholar
Wilson-Ching, M., Molloy, C., Anderson, V., Burnett, A., Roberts, G., Cheong, J., & Anderson, P.J. (2013). Attention difficulties in a contemporary geographic cohort of adolescents born extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 19, 10971108.Google Scholar
Woldorff, M.G., Liotti, M., Seabolt, M., Busse, L., Lancaster, J.L., & Fox, P.T. (2002). The temporal dynamics of the effects in occipital cortex of visual-spatial selective attention. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 115.Google Scholar
Woodward, L.J., Anderson, P.J., Austin, N.C., Howard, K., & Inder, T.E. (2006). Neonatal MRI to predict neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. New England Journal of Medicine, 355, 685694.Google Scholar
Woodward, L.J., Clark, C., Bora, S., & Inder, T.E. (2012). Neonatal white matter abnormalities. An Important predictor of neurocognitive outcome for very preterm children. PLoS One, 7, e51879.Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Inder, T.E., Neil, J., Dierker, D., Alexopoulos, D., Anderson, P.J., & Van Essen, D. (2015). Cortical structural abnormalities in very preterm children at 7 years of age. Neuroimage, 109, 469479.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Clinical and social background characteristics of FT and VPI (n=206)

Figure 1

Table 2 Mean TEA-Ch scores and rates of impairment for FT and VPT children at age 12 years (n=206)

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Statistical parametric maps showing regional gray matter volume differences between VPT (n=89) and FT (n=93) children at age 12 years (voxel-wise FWE-corrected, p<.05). Blue indicates areas of reduced gray matter volume in VPT relative to FT. Red indicates increased gray matter volume in VPT relative to FT. Color shade indicates t-value, displayed on the colorbar.

Figure 3

Table 3 MNI coordinates for voxels with peak t- and Z-scores from VBM analysis comparing regional differences in gray matter volume between FT (n=93) and VPT (n=89) children at age 12 years

Figure 4

Table 4 Summary of regression models for the prediction of attention outcomes in all children (FT=91, VPT=87) at age 12 years

Supplementary material: File

Lean supplementary material

Figure 2

Download Lean supplementary material(File)
File 334 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lean supplementary material

Tables 5-8

Download Lean supplementary material(File)
File 28.6 KB