Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:39:16.423Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altered Brain Activation in Military Personnel with One or More Traumatic Brain Injuries Following Blast

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2011

Randall S. Scheibel*
Affiliation:
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
Mary R. Newsome
Affiliation:
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
Maya Troyanskaya
Affiliation:
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
Xiaodi Lin
Affiliation:
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
Joel L. Steinberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas
Majdi Radaideh
Affiliation:
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas Department of Radiology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
Harvey S. Levin
Affiliation:
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Randall S. Scheibel, Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Baylor College of Medicine, 1709 Dryden Road, Suite 1200, Houston, TX 77030. E-mail: scheibel@bcm.tmc.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Explosive blast is a frequent cause of traumatic brain injury (TBI) among personnel deployed to Afghanistan and Iraq. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with an event-related stimulus-response compatibility task was used to compare 15 subjects with mild, chronic blast-related TBI with 15 subjects who had not experienced a TBI or blast exposure during deployment. Six TBI subjects reported multiple injuries. Relative to the control group, TBI subjects had slightly slower responses during fMRI and increased somatic complaints and symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression. A between-group analysis indicated greater activation during stimulus-response incompatibility in TBI subjects within the anterior cingulate gyrus, medial frontal cortex, and posterior cerebral areas involved in visual and visual-spatial functions. This activation pattern was more extensive after statistically controlling for reaction time and symptoms of PTSD and depression. There was also a negative relationship between symptoms of PTSD and activation within posterior brain regions. These results provide evidence for increased task-related activation following mild, blast-related TBI and additional changes associated with emotional symptoms. Limitations of this study include no matching for combat exposure and different recruitment strategies so that the control group was largely a community-based sample, while many TBI subjects were seeking services. (JINS, 2012, 18, 89–100)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Introduction

Explosives are a frequent cause of injury among personnel wounded in Iraq and Afghanistan and blast exposure often results in traumatic brain injury (TBI) (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Kragh, Wenke, Macaitis, Wade and Holcomb2008). Although the majority of blast-related TBIs are mild, many injured individuals remain in theatre and experience multiple blasts (Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008). Civilians who sustain a single, mild TBI unrelated to blast generally show resolution of postconcussive symptoms during the initial weeks following injury and most perform well on standard neuropsychological tests (Binder, Rohling, & Larrabee, Reference Binder, Rohling and Larrabee1997). However, multiple mild injuries may be cumulative (Rabadi & Jordan, Reference Rabadi and Jordan2001) and it is not clear whether the neuropathology and symptoms associated with military, blast-related TBI are the same as those produced by acceleration/deceleration injury in civilians (Courtney & Courtney, Reference Courtney and Courtney2009).

Primary blast injury results from changes in atmospheric pressure experienced as a wave of increased pressure (i.e., blast overpressure) followed by a relative vacuum and high velocity wind (Cernak & Noble-Haeusslein, Reference Cernak and Noble-Haeusslein2010). Tissues that are exposed to the blast wave and have different densities may accelerate and be displaced to different degrees, resulting in stretching and shear strain (Taber, Warden, & Hurley, Reference Taber, Warden and Hurley2006). Secondary blast injury may occur when the individual is impacted by objects and, in some cases, tertiary injury results when the body is thrown (Warden, Reference Warden2006). Tissue damage may result from other causes, as well, such as burns, substance inhalation, and collapsing objects (i.e., quaternary blast injury) (Finkel, Reference Finkel2006).

Most research on primary blast has been conducted with animals and little is known about the mechanisms that cause injury to the brain, but these have been postulated to include whiplash or head rotation (Bhattacharjee, Reference Bhattacharjee2008; Cernak & Noble-Haeusslein, Reference Cernak and Noble-Haeusslein2010), direct passage of the blast wave through the skull or skull deformation (Cernak & Noble-Haeusslein, Reference Cernak and Noble-Haeusslein2010; Ling, Bandak, Armonda, Grant, & Ecklund, Reference Ling, Bandak, Armonda, Grant and Ecklund2009), and the transfer of pressure wave energy to the brain through the thorax and large blood vessels (Cernak, Reference Cernak2005). These mechanisms are not mutually exclusive and some, such as head rotation, may cause diffuse axonal injury in a manner similar to blunt head trauma (Cernak & Noble-Haeusslein, Reference Cernak and Noble-Haeusslein2010). Other possible causes of neural injury, such as energy transfer through the vasculature, may differ from those presumed to underlie acceleration/deceleration injury (Cernak, Reference Cernak2005). Although there is some experimental evidence for a systemic mechanism involving blast-induced vascular changes (e.g., Long et al., Reference Long, Bentley, Wessner, Cerone, Sweeney and Bauman2009), this evidence is limited and the theory is controversial (Bhattacharjee, Reference Bhattacharjee2008). In addition, one study which compared neuropsychological findings in patients with TBI due to blast or blunt force found no differences in cognitive function, but there was a marginally higher incidence of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) following blast-related TBI (Belanger, Kretzmer, Yoash-Gantz, Pickett, & Tupler, Reference Belanger, Kretzmer, Yoash-Gantz, Pickett and Tupler2009).

Several somatic and cognitive postconcussive symptoms overlap with those of PTSD and depression and, in addition to blast exposure, such conditions may be related to other battlefield risk factors that reflect combat intensity and psychological trauma (Fear et al., Reference Fear, Jones, Groom, Greenberg, Hull, Hodgetts and Wesseley2009). Hoge et al. (Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008) found that soldiers with mild TBI were more likely to report increased postconcussive symptoms, missed workdays, and poorer health than those with other injuries. However, with the exception of headache, mild TBI was no longer associated with these symptoms after the data were statistically adjusted for PTSD and depression. The overlap of postconcussive symptoms with depression and PTSD presents challenges for diagnosis and assessment (Brenner, Vanderploeg, & Terrio, Reference Brenner, Vanderploeg and Terrio2009).

One method that has potential for identifying alterations in neural function following TBI is functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Civilians with moderate to severe TBI typically have greater and more distributed brain activation than uninjured subjects while engaging executive functions, such as working memory and inhibition (e.g., Christodoulou et al, Reference Christodoulou, DeLuca, Ricker, Madigan, Bly, Lange and Ni2001; Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009). These changes have included greater activation within the cingulate gyrus during an fMRI stimulus-response compatibility task, but the distribution of this over-activation within the cingulate gyrus and additional brain structures varied with injury severity (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009). Investigations have also described changes in the modulation of activation in response to increased working memory load when a mild TBI was sustained within the past month (McAllister et al., Reference McAllister, Saykin, Flashman, Sparling, Johnson, Guerin and Yanofsky1999, Reference McAllister, Sparling, Flashman, Guerin, Mamourian and Saykin2001). Other studies have examined athletes with mild TBI and have revealed various patterns of increased or reduced activation (Chen, Johnson, Collie, McCrory, & Ptito, Reference Chen, Johnson, Collie, McCrory and Ptito2007; Chen, Johnson, Petrides, & Ptito, Reference Chen, Johnson, Petrides and Ptito2008a; Jantzen, Anderson, Steinberg, & Kelso, Reference Jantzen, Anderson, Steinberg and Kelso2004). Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Johnson, Collie, McCrory and Ptito2007) noted the variability in these findings, which they attributed to differences in the tasks and experimental design, and stated that the most striking result across fMRI studies is that individuals with mild TBI exhibit atypical brain activation. In addition, there is some evidence that depression can modify the activation pattern during working memory in athletes with mild TBI, including reduced activation within task-relevant brain areas (Chen, Johnson, Petrides, & Ptito, Reference Chen, Johnson, Petrides and Ptito2008b).

The current study examined individuals with mild, blast-related TBI using an fMRI stimulus-response compatibility task that has been reported to produce greater activation in civilians with moderate to severe TBI, relative to control subjects (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009). The mild range of severity was selected for study because this is the injury that is most often reported for service personnel wounded in the current conflicts (e.g., Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008) and because this allowed the composition of a more homogenous group. These subjects were compared to individuals who had also served in Afghanistan or Iraq, but had not sustained a TBI or been exposed to blast. Previous research with the block design version of the stimulus-response compatibility task did not reveal consistent activation in civilian orthopedic control subjects (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007), but emotional factors were not considered within that statistical design. Studies with athletes have found that depression can modify the activation pattern during executive functions (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Johnson, Petrides and Ptito2008b) and, since Hoge et al. (Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008) found that the relationship between mild TBI and postconcussive symptoms was eliminated after adjusting for PTSD and depression, the current study included measures of both depression and PTSD symptoms as covariates. We hypothesized that subjects with mild, blast-related TBI would have greater and more distributed brain activation relative to comparison subjects who had not sustained a TBI, including increased activation within the cingulate gyrus, but we also anticipated that the activation pattern may be modified by emotional factors.

Methods

Participants

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board and all subjects provided written informed consent. The participants consisted of 30 right-handed service personnel and veterans who had served in Afghanistan or Iraq. The U.S. Veterans Health Administration (VHA) has instituted a thorough screening and evaluation system to identify post-deployment veterans with possible TBI (Armistead-Jehle, Reference Armistead-Jehle2010) and this is how many veterans get referred for TBI evaluation. Of the 15 subjects in the TBI group, 12 veterans and 1 active duty reservist were enrolled through the TBI clinic at the Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, one veteran was referred by the center's psychiatrist, and another was initially contacted through a database of consented research subjects. The control group consisted of three active duty personnel recruited through advertisements at an army post, one veteran who was examined by the TBI clinic and found not to have experienced head trauma or blast, and 11 veterans who were identified through the research database. Blast exposure was initially assessed using a self-report questionnaire and all TBI subjects reported at least one post-blast period that included loss of consciousness (LOC), confusion, or retrograde amnesia consistent with Department of Defense criteria for TBI (French & Parkinson, Reference French and Parkinson2008). This diagnosis was confirmed in an interview with a clinician who was experienced in the evaluation of individuals with combat injury. Mild TBI was defined as an injury with LOC less than 30 min, post-traumatic amnesia (PTA) duration of less than 24 hr, and no focal lesions on structural imaging. All provided estimates of LOC and PTA duration that met the eligibility criteria. Ten TBI subjects were able to provide quantitative estimates of post-blast PTA duration associated with their most severe injury (M = 14.7 min; SD = 10.6; range = 3 to 31 min). Three claimed to have had no LOC and, of the 12 who did, 7 were able to provide a quantitative estimate of LOC duration (M = 4.7 min; SD = 5.3; range = 1 to 15 min).

Eleven TBI subjects reported exposure to multiple blasts and, of these, six reported two or more separate events where they had experienced post-blast symptoms consistent with TBI (French & Parkinson, Reference French and Parkinson2008). Ten indicated that they had probably experienced at least one secondary blast injury and seven reported tertiary injury. Cause of the blasts included improvised explosive devices (n = 13), mortars (n = 6), rocket propelled grenades (n = 5), and other types of explosives (n = 6). This was a chronic injury sample as reflected by the interval between the most recent blast-related TBI and the fMRI assessment (M = 963.9 days; SD = 333.2; range = 402 to 1511 days).

The comparison group consisted of subjects who had been deployed to Afghanistan or Iraq, but who had not been exposed to blast and reported no TBI. Five of these control subjects had experienced an orthopedic injury during deployment. None of the control or TBI subjects reported current drug or alcohol abuse, and none had a history of previous psychiatric disorder, learning disability, or pre- or post-deployment head injury. There were no significant between-group differences in self-reported medication use at the time of assessment (see Table 1). Also, whenever possible, the medications were withheld on the day of the behavioral and fMRI assessments.

Table 1 Self-reported medication use by group

aMost comparisons were performed using Fisher's exact test because the χ2 test was not valid due to inadequate cell frequencies.

bComparisons where the χ2 was used because it was valid.

Behavioral Measures and Demographic Information

All subjects completed the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) (Derogatis, Reference Derogatis1975), the Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory (NSI) (Cicerone & Kalmar, Reference Cicerone and Kalmar1995), the Short Form (SF-12) Health Survey (Ware, Kosinski, Turner-Bowker, & Gandek, Reference Ware, Kosinski, Turner-Bowker and Gandek2004), and the PTSD Checklist – Civilian (PCL-C) (Dobie et al., Reference Dobie, Kivlahan, Maynard, Bush, McFall, Epler and Bradley2002). The PCL-C is a brief, self-report instrument that does not require specification of a particular event and does not assume that all traumatic experiences are related to combat. In addition, the subjects provided information to support a rating on the Glasgow Outcome Scale – Extended (GOS-E) (Teasdale, Pettigrew, Wilson, Murray, & Jennett, Reference Teasdale, Pettigrew, Wilson, Murray and Jennett1998) and calculation of the Barona IQ (Barona, Reynolds, & Chastain, Reference Barona, Reynolds and Chastain1984).

Arrows Stimulus-Response Compatibility Task

The stimulus-response compatibility task (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007) was administered in the scanner as a rapid-presentation, stochastic event-related fMRI paradigm. Participants viewed arrows presented one at a time for 265 milliseconds (ms), with each followed by a blank screen for 200 ms and then a crosshair fixation point for a mean of 2235 ms, randomly jittered plus or minus 200 ms. Seventy-five percent of the arrows were blue, and 25% were red. For each color, 50% of the arrows pointed left and 50% pointed right. These different stimuli were randomly intermixed throughout each run of 80 arrows. When the arrows were blue the subjects were required to use their right or left index finger to press the response key that was on the same side that the arrow was pointing toward (i.e., stimulus-response compatible). When the arrows were red the subject pressed the response key opposite to the direction of the arrow (i.e., stimulus-response incompatible). Responses were collected between 200 and 1200 ms after stimulus onset and the first two trials in the run were always from the compatible condition. There was a 20 second (s) fixation crosshair before the first trial to allow for magnetization equilibrium to occur and additional fixation rest periods of 2000 ms occurred after 16, 32, 48, and 64 trials. Each of the three runs lasted 244 s, with a 1-min rest period between runs, and the order of the runs was counterbalanced across subjects. Instructions and practice were provided within 2 hr before scanning until the subject performed with 65% accuracy or better in both conditions. During fMRI data acquisition the response accuracy, onset time, and reaction time (RT) were recorded for each stimulus and only correct responses were included in analyses for RT.

Image Data Acquisition

Whole brain imaging was performed using a multi-channel sensitivity encoding (SENSE) head coil on a Philips Achieva 3 Tesla system (Philips Healthcare, Best, The Netherlands). Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) T2* weighted single-shot gradient-echo echoplanar images (EPI) were acquired as 160 volumes with 32 axial slices of 3.75 millimeter (mm) thickness with a .5 mm gap, using a 240 mm field of view (FOV), 64 × 64 matrix, repetition time (TR) of 1700 ms, echo time (TE) of 30 ms, a 73 degree flip angle, and a SENSE factor of 2.0. The first 20 s of each run were discarded to allow for signal magnetization equilibrium to be achieved. A set of high-resolution T1-weighted 3D-turbo field echo anatomical images were also acquired in 132 axial slices of 1.0 mm thickness (no gap) with 240 mm FOV, 256 × 256 matrix, TR of 9.9 ms, TE of 4.6 ms, a 8.0 degree flip angle, and a SENSE factor of 1.2. Additional anatomical series were performed to assess neuropathology and these included T2-weighted gradient echo (25 axial slices, slice thickness = 5.0 mm, TR = 2500, TE = 32, FOV = 224 mm, flip angle = 30 to 40 degrees), T2-weighted fluid attenuated inversion recovery (25 axial slices; slice thickness = 5.0 mm with 1.0-mm gap; TR = 11,000; TE = 105; FOV = 240 mm; flip angle = 90 degrees), and T2-weighted spin echo imaging (25 axial slices, slice thickness = 5.0 mm; TR = 2141; TE = 80; FOV = 230 mm; flip angle = 90). A board certified neuroradiologist examined the anatomical imaging.

Image Post-Processing and Analysis

The fMRI data were subjected to voxel by voxel analyses using statistical parametric mapping (SPM) 2 software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College, London, UK) implemented in Matlab (Mathworks Inc., Sherborn, MA). After slice-timing correction, the fMRI time series were realigned and unwarped to correct for head motion and susceptibility-by-movement interactions. Series with motion greater than 2.0 mm translational or 3.0 degrees rotational were eliminated from analysis. The fMRI time series were coregistered to the high-resolution anatomical scan, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using the normalization parameters from the anatomical scan, resliced to 2 × 2 × 2 mm, and spatially smoothed using a 6 mm isotropic full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter. A high-pass temporal filter with a cutoff period of 128 s was used to reduce low-frequency noise. First-level analyses were then conducted using the general linear model at each voxel for each subject, using only correct response trial events and with incorrect trials modeled as a nuisance regressor, to contrast the amplitude of the hemodynamic response (HRF) associated with the onset of red arrows (i.e., incompatible condition) with the HRF associated with the onset of blue arrows (i.e., compatible condition).

The incompatible minus compatible contrast image for each subject was carried forward into second-level SPM2 random effects analyses. These included t tests to examine the incompatible minus compatible contrast within each group, a t test to perform between-group comparisons (control vs. TBI), and an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model to examine between-group contrasts while controlling for several covariates (i.e., BSI Depression Scale, PCL-C Total Score, blue arrows RT, and red arrows RT). To confirm that the assumptions of ANCOVA were met, a set of preliminary slopes interaction analyses (Kleinbaum, Kupper, Muller, & Nizam, Reference Kleinbaum, Kupper, Muller and Nizam1998) were completed with results indicating parallel slopes within both groups for each covariate. Follow-up analyses using separate SPM2 simple regressions were also performed with those covariates that were significant in the full ANCOVA model.

An additional ANCOVA was performed using scores from the BSI Somatization Scale as the only covariate to address the possibility that the subjects’ somatic concerns may contribute to the between-group differences. The slopes interaction analysis for the BSI Somatization Scale indicated a single posterior cluster of 802 voxels where the assumptions of ANCOVA were not met. However, the results of this additional ANCOVA were considered to be valid for those voxels that did not overlap with that posterior cluster.

The cluster-defining (height) threshold for all second-level analyses was set at voxel-level t = 2.50. Reported clusters were statistically significant (corrected p < .05) at the cluster level of inference using the SPM Random Field Theory family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons over the whole brain volume. The location of statistically significant clusters was visually inspected and their coordinates were then assigned anatomical labels using the SPM anatomy toolbox (http://www.fz-juelich.de/inm/inm-1/spm_anatomy_toolbox).

Results

Demographics and Outcome Measures

There were no significant between-group differences for age, education level, estimated IQ, or the Mental Component Score from the SF-12 (see Table 2). There was a trend favoring higher NSI Cognitive Cluster Scores within the TBI group (p = .06) and there were significant between-group differences indicating greater symptom severity among TBI subjects on the NSI Total Score, the BSI Depression and Somatization Scales, and the Physical Component Score from the SF-12. Scores on the PCL-C were higher within the TBI group and 11 of these subjects exceeded a cutoff score of 50, while four of the control subjects exceeded this cutoff. The TBI subjects also had worse outcomes as reflected by lower ratings on the GOS-E.

Table 2 Demographic characteristics, outcome measures, and cognitive task performance by group

Note. TBI = traumatic brain injury; PCL-C = PTSD Checklist – Civilian; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; NSI = Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory; SF-12 = Short Form Health Survey; GOS-E = Glasgow Outcome Scale – Extended; fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging.

aOne subject in the control group was female.

bAll subjects in the TBI group were male.

cTwo sample t-test.

dThe PTSD Checklist is a 17-item self-report measure of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual IV (DSM-IV) PTSD symptoms. The military version (i.e., PCL-M) asks about reactions to military experiences, while the civilian version (PCL-C) has slightly different wording so that it is more general and can be used with any population. Each item is rated by the subject on a scale ranging from 1 (“Not at all”) to 5 (“Extremely”) for a maximum possible score of 85. A cutoff value of 50 is often used with the total score to indicate elevated symptoms of PTSD (e.g., Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008).

eThe Cognitive Cluster Scale was originally identified in a cluster analysis performed by Cicerone and Kalmar Reference Cicerone and Kalmar(1995). That analysis also identified affective, somatic, and sensory clusters.

fThe Extended Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS-E) uses a structured interview and examiner ratings to assign subjects to one of eight categories: Dead (1), Vegetative State (2), Lower Severe Disability (3), Upper Severe Disability (4), Lower Moderate Disability (5), Upper Moderate Disability (6), Lower Good Recovery (7), and Upper Good Recovery (8).

gThe Wilcoxon two sample test (normal approximation) was used because the data distribution did not meet the assumptions for parametric statistical analysis.

hPercentage of correct responses.

iAccuracy during the first trial of the pre-scan training.

Anatomical MR Imaging

The 15 comparison subjects and the 15 TBI subjects had normal anatomical imaging. Two additional TBI subjects were also screened, but these had evidence of shear injury or gliotic lesions and were excluded from this study.

Behavioral Results

Initial red arrows accuracy during the pre-scan training did not differ between the groups (see Table 2). There were also no significant between-group differences for blue or red arrows accuracy during fMRI image acquisition, but there were nonsignificant trends favoring slightly slower responding among the TBI subjects for both the blue (p = .06) and red (p = .08) arrows. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated slower responding among TBI subjects when both arrows conditions were combined [F(1,28) = 4.35; p = .05], slower responding for the red than for the blue arrows condition when subjects from both groups were pooled [F(1,28) = 108.12; p = .001], and no significant interaction [F(1,28) = 0.00; p = .95].

Separate analyzes were also performed to examine whether there were any between-group differences in red arrows RT after statistically controlling for the symptoms of PTSD and depression. For both models, the preliminary slope interaction analyses indicated that the assumptions of ANCOVA were met. The analysis using PCL-C scores as a covariate indicated increased RT for the TBI subjects relative to the control subjects [F(1,27) = 4.29; p = .05] and, within this model, the PCL-C covariate was not statistically significant [F(1,27) = 0.99; p = .33]. Similarly, an ANCOVA controlling for scores on the BSI Depression Scale also revealed slower responding for the TBI group [F(1,27) = 4.62; p = .04] and the covariate was not statistically significant [F(1,27) = 1.42; p = .24].

fMRI Results

Within-group activation

A within-group analysis indicated no significant activation for the control subjects. However, TBI subjects had significant task-related activation within the temporal and parietal lobes, the posterior cingulate gyrus, cerebellum, and other areas within the posterior cerebrum (see Table 3, part A, Figure 1).

Table 3 Coordinates and anatomical region definitions for the results of the image analyses.

Note. MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute; TBI = traumatic brain injury; R = right side; L = left side; B = both sides or bilateral; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; PCL-C = PTSD Checklist – Civilian; RT = reaction time.

aNumber of contiguous 2 × 2 × 2 mm voxels that exceed threshold.

bLabels obtained through the SPM Anatomy Toolbox.

cA preliminary analysis found a posterior cluster (i.e., bilateral precuneus, right superior parietal lobule) of 802 voxels where the assumptions of ANCOVA were not met, but these overlapped with only 225 (2.31%) of the 9,960 voxels reported here.

dResults of the simple regression of brain activation with the PCL-C Total Score. Data from both groups were pooled for this analysis.

Between-group comparisons

A between-group t test indicated there were no areas where control subjects had greater activation than the TBI group, but TBI subjects had significantly greater activation within the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, medial frontal areas, the parietal lobes, and other regions as summarized in Table 3, part B, and Figure 1.

An ANCOVA using the BSI Depression Scale, the PCL-C Total Score, blue arrows RT, and red arrows RT as covariates revealed significantly higher activation for the TBI subjects, relative to the control group, within areas that had also exhibited differences with the between-group t test. These regions included the anterior and posterior cingulate, medial frontal, and parietal cortex (e.g., inferior parietal lobule). There were other areas, as well, where this ANCOVA revealed higher activation for the TBI group, including additional temporal and parietal structures (see Table 3, part C, and Figures 1 and 2).

Fig. 1 Brain surface images displaying cortical areas with significant t test and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) results: (A) significant activation in subjects with traumatic brain injury (TBI), (B) areas where the TBI group had greater activation than the control group, and C) areas where the TBI group had greater activation than the control group after controlling for blue arrows reaction time (RT), red arrows RT, and scores on the BSI Depression Scale and PCL-C.

Fig. 2 Brain structures where the traumatic brain injury (TBI) group had greater activation than the control group when the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model included the BSI Depression Scale, PCL-C total score, blue arrows reaction time (RT), and red arrows RT as covariates, as well as areas where there was a significant negative relation between brain activation and scores on the PCL-C (all subjects). The statistical parametric maps are overlaid onto high resolution T1-weighted images from a typical control subject.

A separate ANCOVA using scores from the BSI Somatization Scale as the only covariate revealed between-group differences that were similar to the model that had controlled for depression, PTSD, and RT. In a preliminary analysis using the BSI Somatization Scale, there was a posterior cluster of 802 voxels where this covariate failed to meet the assumptions of ANCOVA, but only 225 of these overlapped with the 9690 voxels that exhibited significant between-group differences. Thus, this ANCOVA provided valid results that generally replicated the findings of the primary image analysis (see Table 3, part D).

Simple regression analyses for the BSI Depression Scale and PCL-C

The blue and red arrows RT covariates were not significant when examined as part of the full ANCOVA model, but the BSI Depression Scale and the PCL-C covariates were significantly related to brain activation and follow-up analyses were performed for these. Preliminary analyses indicated no significant covariate × group interactions and, consequently, data from both groups were pooled for the separate simple regressions. There was no significant correlation between brain activation and scores on the BSI Depression Scale. However, there was a significant negative relation between activation and the PCL-C within posterior regions that included the cerebellum, right medial parietal cortex, and structures within both occipital lobes (see Table 3, part E, and Figure 2).

Discussion

Impairments in executive functions, including cognitive control, are common following TBI (e.g., Cicerone, Levin, Malec, Stuss, & Whyte, Reference Cicerone, Levin, Malec, Stuss and Whyte2006) and the present investigation used a stimulus-response compatibility task to examine military personnel who had sustained one or more blast-related injuries during deployment. This simple cognitive control paradigm has several characteristics that make it desirable for fMRI studies of TBI, including the ability to train neurologically impaired patients to perform at levels equivalent to healthy subjects (Price & Friston, Reference Price and Friston2002), relatively short scan duration, and previous reports of increased activation in civilians with moderate to severe TBI (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009).

Relative to a post-deployment control group, the current study found increased brain activation during stimulus-response incompatibility in subjects with mild, chronic blast-related TBI after statistically controlling for differences in RT and symptoms of PTSD and depression. Using this ANCOVA model, the TBI subjects had greater activation within the anterior cingulate gyrus and medial frontal cortex. Increases were also found within posterior cerebral areas, including some (e.g., inferior parietal lobule, posterior cingulate gyrus) that are thought to be involved in visual perception, visual attention, and visual-spatial functions (e.g., Galati, Pelle, Berthoz, & Committeri, Reference Galati, Pelle, Berthoz and Committeri2010; Orban, Van Essen, & Vanduffel, Reference Orban, Van Essen and Vanduffel2004). Areas of increased activation within the cingulate gyrus and medial prefrontal cortex are generally consistent with those previously reported by Scheibel el al. (2007) for a group of civilians with moderate to severe TBI, but the lateral posterior activation is more similar to that reported by Scheibel et al. (Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009) for civilians with only severe TBI (i.e., GCS ≤ 8). These finding may reflect increased usage of neural substrates for visual processing and attention following mild, blast-related TBI in military personnel and in civilians with severe acceleration-deceleration injuries. Caution should be exercised while making these between-studies comparisons due to differences in the study design, the use of different statistical thresholds, and because the types of stressors and associated emotional factors are likely to differ for these populations. The results of these studies are clearly consistent, however, in that they reveal greater and more diffuse task-related activation following TBI.

Activation increases during cognitive fMRI paradigms are frequently observed in association with neuropathology and proposed interpretations for this finding have included the disinhibition of duplicate neural systems, learning-related neuroplasticity, and cognitive reorganization (Price & Friston, Reference Price and Friston2002). When task performance is equated in comparisons with a control group the over-activation may reflect a higher level of effort, perhaps as a consequence of inefficient processing or as a form of compensation involving the allocation of additional cognitive and neural resources (Price & Friston, Reference Price and Friston2002; Ricker, Hillary, & DeLuca, Reference Ricker, Hillary and DeLuca2001). In the present investigation, the task accuracy did not differ between the TBI and control groups, but there were significant differences for RT when PTSD or depressive symptoms were statistically controlled or when data from both arrows conditions were combined. There is the possibility that RT may have been slower in the TBI subjects before injury and this may reflect greater vulnerability to the effects of mild TBI, but preinjury data were not available for our analysis. Cognitive slowing associated with the TBI is another possible explanation for the RT differences and this may have contributed to increased activation within the TBI group. However, these between-group RT differences were relatively small, both blue and red arrows RT were included in the primary image analysis as covariates, and the RT covariates were not significantly related to activation within that ANCOVA model. Thus, it is likely that neural injury made some contribution to increased activation that was not mediated by changes in response speed.

Relative to the control group, ratings on the GOS-E were consistent with decreased functioning within the TBI group and depressive, PTSD, and somatic symptoms were elevated. Statistically controlling for scores on the BSI Somatization Scale did not alter the results of the primary between-group comparison and it is unlikely that somatic symptoms or the subjects’ concern about their injury is responsible for the major fMRI findings. Likewise, scores from the PCL-C and BSI Depression Scale were used as covariates in the primary image analysis that revealed greater activation in the TBI subjects. The BSI Depression Scale was a significant covariate within the ANCOVA model, but it did not exhibit a significant relation with brain activation when examined alone in a simple regression analysis. This failure to confirm a strong significant relation is surprising in light of the attenuated prefrontal activation reported by Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Johnson, Petrides and Ptito2008b) for concussed athletes with depression. However, Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Johnson, Petrides and Ptito2008b) had used a different cognitive task with civilians whereas the current study examined a vastly different population that included individuals with elevated PTSD symptoms, some of which may overlap with the emotional and somatic symptoms of depression. There is the possibility that the effects of depression on brain activation were masked or altered by the presence of these other conditions.

Morey, Petty, Cooper, LaBar, and McCarthy (Reference Morey, Petty, Cooper, LaBar and McCarthy2008) examined Iraq war veterans and found that ventral frontal and limbic activation was positively correlated with PTSD symptoms during the presentation of combat-related images, while such symptoms were negatively correlated with dorsal frontal and parietal activation during a simple executive function task. Their findings were interpreted as providing support for interrelated dorsal executive and ventral emotional processing networks that are differentially affected by PTSD (Morey et al., Reference Morey, Petty, Cooper, LaBar and McCarthy2008). In addition, they proposed that individuals with PTSD may have a hyper-responsive limbic system that interferes with processing in other brain areas so that activation during executive functions is reduced. The current study also used a simple executive function task and found decreased activation in association with higher scores on the PCL-C, but the brain areas exhibiting these reductions differed some from those reported by Morey et al. (Reference Morey, Petty, Cooper, LaBar and McCarthy2008). This discrepancy may reflect differences in the cognitive task and analysis procedures, but another possibility is that other pathology present in our sample contributed to the activation pattern.

In the present investigation, the symptoms of PTSD were associated with reduced brain activation in both the ANCOVA and a simple regression analysis, but the results of the TBI within-group analysis indicated significant activation within several brain areas. Control subjects, in contrast, had elevated PTSD symptoms and did not exhibit a significant level of activation in their within-group analysis. Therefore, it appears that TBI increases brain activation during stimulus-response incompatibility to the point where task-related activation is observable, despite the presence of PTSD symptoms and any suppressive effects they might have. When considered in combination, the overall level and pattern of activation observed during this task seems to reflect the influence of both neural injury and emotional distress upon brain function. Relationships among these various conditions may be further complicated by the possibility that brain injury, in itself, may be a risk factor for the development of PTSD (Bryant, Reference Bryant2008) and depression (Jorge et al., Reference Jorge, Robinson, Moser, Tateno, Crespo-Facorro and Arndt2004). However, the finding that PTSD symptoms are associated with reduced activation, while TBI has the opposite effect, suggests that the influences of PTSD and neural injury on brain function may be partially dissociable with functional neuroimaging.

The methodology used in the present investigation, fMRI, reveals changes at the network level of analysis and it is difficult to speculate upon underlying mechanisms of neural injury using these findings, alone. The results of the current study do have some parallels with those of Scheibel et al. (Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009), including over-activation following TBI. Those previous findings were said to suggest deafferentiation due to diffuse axonal injury (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009), and this is similar to the interpretation presented by Huang et al. (Reference Huang, Theilmann, Robb, Angeles, Nichols, Drake and Lee2009), who found abnormalities on magnetoencephalography and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) in subjects with mild TBI. There is currently inconsistent evidence for white matter injury in combatants who have sustained a blast-related TBI of mild to moderate severity. Levin et al. (Reference Levin, Wilde, Troyanskaya, Petersen, Scheibel, Newsome and Li2010) reported that DTI findings in personnel imaged after an average interval exceeding 2 years after blast-related mild to moderate TBI did not differ from data obtained in a control group. In contrast, MacDonald et al. (Reference MacDonald, Johnson, Cooper, Nelson, Werner, Shimony and Brody2011) recently found DTI evidence of microstructural white matter injury in 29% of service members who had been medically evacuated from theater and initially imaged within 90 days following exposure to blast and other mechanisms of TBI. Evidence of white matter injury persisted in a subgroup with repeat DTI six to 12 months later. The role of white matter injury and other possible mechanisms of blast-related TBI is clearly an area that requires additional research.

The present study used a modified version of an fMRI paradigm that is sensitive to TBI severity in civilians (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009), but the research design has several limitations. The identification of subjects with TBI and estimates of injury severity were based entirely on self-report, often involving recollection of events that occurred in the combat theatre many months earlier, and such information may be subject to recall biases and reliability problems (Fear et al., Reference Fear, Jones, Groom, Greenberg, Hull, Hodgetts and Wesseley2009). However, the design for this project included the screening of a much larger number of subjects with possible injury, an experienced clinician performed an interview to confirm the diagnosis, and subjects were not enrolled in the TBI group unless they clearly recalled events that met Department of Defense criteria (French & Parkinson, Reference French and Parkinson2008). The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) (Blake et al., Reference Blake, Weathers, Nagy, Kaloupek, Gusman, Charney and Keane1995) is a superior instrument for measuring PTSD symptoms and for supporting a diagnosis of that disorder, but the CAPS often requires over 45 min to complete and use of the PCL-C allowed the acquisition of PTSD covariate data and completion of other study procedures during a single session. Other limitations of the current study include the use of only mild TBI subjects who had been injured many months before study (i.e., chronic injury), the presence of multiple injuries in some TBI subjects, the presence of orthopedic injury in only 5 of the 15 control subjects, no matching for combat exposure, and differences in recruitment strategy so that the control group was largely a community-based sample, while many TBI subjects were probably seeking services. Due to limited access to records there was no way to confirm how many subjects in each group had accessed services for mental health or physical complaints. Findings from this sample should not be generalized to more severe or more acute blast-related TBI and relevance for civilian, non-blast TBI populations is likely to be limited due to possible differences in the mechanism of injury, multiple injuries, and emotional factors such as PTSD. Future research on blast TBI may benefit from the use of an additional fMRI paradigm that relates more directly to emotional symptoms and the inclusion of a non-blast TBI group.

Acknowledgments

The authors report no conflicts of interest. This work was supported by a Department of Veterans Affairs Center of Excellence grant (H.S.L., grant number B6812C), Neurorehabilitation: Neurons to Networks Center for Rehabilitation Research; and by a Department of Veterans Affairs Merit Review grant (H.S.L., grant number B4596R). The Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center in Houston, TX, and the South Central Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (MIRECC) provided access to equipment and facilities used for the analysis of the image data. We thank Xiaoqi Li for assisting with statistical analysis and Drs. Helene K. Henson and David P. Graham for assisting with the screening and recruitment of subjects.

References

Armistead-Jehle, P. (2010). Symptom validity test performance in U.S. veterans referred for evaluation of mild TBI. Applied Neuropsychology, 17, 5259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barona, A., Reynolds, C.R., Chastain, R. (1984). A demographically based index of premorbid intelligence for the WAIS-R. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 885887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belanger, H.G., Kretzmer, T., Yoash-Gantz, R., Pickett, T., Tupler, L.A. (2009). Cognitive sequelae of blast-related versus other mechanisms of brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhattacharjee, Y. (2008). Shell shock revisited: Solving the puzzle of blast trauma. Science, 319, 406408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, L.M., Rohling, M.L., Larrabee, G.J. (1997). A review of mild head trauma, Part I: Meta-analytic review of neuropsychological studies. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 421431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, D.D., Weathers, F.W., Nagy, L.M., Kaloupek, D.G., Gusman, F.D., Charney, D.S., Keane, T.M. (1995). The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 7590.Google ScholarPubMed
Brenner, L.A., Vanderploeg, R.D., Terrio, H. (2009). Assessment and diagnosis of mild traumatic brain injury, posttraumatic stress disorder, and other polytrauma conditions: Burden of adversity hypothesis. Rehabilitation Psychology, 54, 239246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R.A. (2008). Disentangling mild traumatic brain injury and stress reactions. The New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 525527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cernak, I. (2005). Penetrating and blast injury. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 23, 139143.Google Scholar
Cernak, I., Noble-Haeusslein, L.J. (2010). Traumatic brain injury: An overview of pathobiology with emphasis on military populations. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 30, 255266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.K., Johnson, K.M., Collie, A., McCrory, P., Ptito, A. (2007). A validation of the post concussion symptom scale in the assessment of complex concussion using cognitive testing and functional MRI. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78, 12311238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.K., Johnson, K.M., Petrides, M., Ptito, A. (2008a). Recovery from mild head injury in sports: Evidence from serial functional magnetic resonance imaging studies in male athletes. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 18, 241247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.K., Johnson, K.M., Petrides, M., Ptito, A. (2008b). Neural substrates of symptoms of depression following concussion in male athletes with persisting postconcussive symptoms. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 8189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christodoulou, C., DeLuca, J., Ricker, J.H., Madigan, N.K., Bly, B.M., Lange, G. Ni, A.C. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 71, 161168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicerone, K.D., Kalmar, K. (1995). Persistent postconcussion syndrome: The structure of subjective complaints after mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicerone, K., Levin, H., Malec, J., Stuss, D., Whyte, J. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: Moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 12121222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, A.C., Courtney, M.W. (2009). A thoracic mechanism of mild traumatic brain injury due to blast pressure waves. Medical Hypotheses, 72, 7683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, L.R. (1975). Brief symptom inventory. Baltimore: Clinical Psychometric Research.Google Scholar
Dobie, D.J., Kivlahan, D.R., Maynard, C., Bush, K.R., McFall, M.E., Epler, A.J., Bradley, K.A. (2002). Screening for post-traumatic stress disorder in female Veteran's Affairs patients: Validation of the PTSD Checklist. General Hospital Psychiatry, 24, 367374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fear, N.T., Jones, E., Groom, M., Greenberg, N., Hull, L., Hodgetts, T.J., Wesseley, S. (2009). Symptoms of post-concussive syndrome are non-specifically related to mild traumatic brain injury in UK armed forces personnel on return from deployment in Iraq: An analysis of self-reported data. Psychological Medicine, 39, 13791387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, M.F. (2006). The neurological consequences of explosives. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 249, 6367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, L.M., Parkinson, G.W. (2008). Assessing and treating veterans with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 10041013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galati, G., Pelle, G., Berthoz, A., Committeri, G. (2010). Multiple reference frames used by the human brain for spatial perception and memory. Experimental Brain Research, 20, 109120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoge, C.W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J.L., Cox, A.L., Engel, C.C., Castro, C.A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. The New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 453463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, M.X., Theilmann, R.J., Robb, A., Angeles, A., Nichols, S., Drake, A. Lee, R.R. (2009). Integrated imaging approach with MEG and DTI to detect mild traumatic brain injury in military and civilian patients. Journal of Neurotrauma, 26, 12131226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jantzen, K.J., Anderson, B., Steinberg, F.L., Kelso, J.A. (2004). A prospective functional MR imaging study of mild traumatic brain injury in college football players. AJNR American Journal of Neuroradiology, 25, 738745.Google ScholarPubMed
Jorge, R.E., Robinson, R.G., Moser, D., Tateno, A., Crespo-Facorro, B., Arndt, S. (2004). Major depression following traumatic brain injury. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 4250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinbaum, D.G., Kupper, L.L., Muller, K.E., Nizam, A. (1998). Applied regression analysis and other multivariate methods. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Levin, H.S., Wilde, E.A., Troyanskaya, M., Petersen, N.J., Scheibel, R., Newsome, M. Li, X. (2010). Diffusion tensor imaging of mild to moderate blast-related traumatic brain injury and its sequelae. Journal of Neurotrauma, 27, 683694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ling, G., Bandak, F., Armonda, R., Grant, G., Ecklund, J. (2009). Explosive blast neurotrauma. Journal of Neurotrauma, 26, 815825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, J.B., Bentley, T.L., Wessner, K.A., Cerone, C., Sweeney, S., Bauman, R.A. (2009). Blast overpressure in rats: Recreating a battlefield injury in the laboratory. Journal of Neurotrauma, 26, 827840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, C.L., Johnson, A.M., Cooper, D., Nelson, E.C., Werner, N.J., Shimony, J.S. Brody, D.L. (2011). Detection of blast-related traumatic brain injury in U.S. military personnel. The New England Journal of Medicine, 364, 20912100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAllister, T.W., Saykin, A.J., Flashman, L.A., Sparling, M.B., Johnson, S.C., Guerin, S.J. Yanofsky, N. (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1 month after mild traumatic brain injury: A functional MRI study. Neurology, 53, 13001308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T.W., Sparling, M.B., Flashman, L.A., Guerin, S.J., Mamourian, A.C., Saykin, A.J. (2001). Differential working memory load effects after mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage, 14, 10041012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morey, R.A., Petty, C.M., Cooper, D.A., LaBar, K.S., McCarthy, G. (2008). Neural systems for executive and emotional processing are modulated by symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in Iraq War veterans. Psychiatry Research, 162, 5972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orban, G.A., Van Essen, D., Vanduffel, W. (2004). Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 315324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, B.D., Kragh, J.F., Wenke, J.C., Macaitis, J., Wade, C.E., Holcomb, J.B. (2008). Combat wounds in operation Iraqi Freedom and operation Enduring Freedom. Journal of Trauma, 64, 295299.Google ScholarPubMed
Price, C.J., Friston, K.J. (2002). Functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients: Applications and limitations. Neurocase, 8, 345354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabadi, M.H., Jordan, B.D. (2001). The cumulative effect of repetitive concussion in sports. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 11, 194198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ricker, J.H., Hillary, F.G., DeLuca, J. (2001). Functionally activated brain imaging (O-15 PET and fMRI) in the study of learning and memory after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 191205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheibel, R.S., Newsome, M.R., Steinberg, J.L., Pearson, D.A., Rauch, R.A., Mao, H. Levin, H.S. (2007). Altered brain activation during cognitive control in patients with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 21, 3645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheibel, R.S., Newsome, M.R., Troyanskaya, M., Steinberg, J.L., Goldstein, F.C., Mao, H., Levin, H.S. (2009). Effects of severity of traumatic brain injury and brain reserve on cognitive-control related brain activation. Journal of Neurotrauma, 26, 14471461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taber, K.H., Warden, D.L., Hurley, R.A. (2006). Blast-related traumatic brain injury: What is known? Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 218, 141145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, G.M., Pettigrew, L.E., Wilson, J.T., Murray, G., Jennett, B. (1998). Analyzing outcome of treatment of severe head injury: A review and update on advancing the use of the Glasgow Outcome Scale. Journal of Neurotrauma, 15, 587597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warden, D. (2006). Military TBI during the Iraq and Afghanistan wars. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 21, 398402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, J.E., Kosinski, M., Turner-Bowker, D.M., Gandek, B. (2004). How to score version 2 of the SF-12 Health Survey. Lincoln, RI: QualityMetric Incorporated.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Self-reported medication use by group

Figure 1

Table 2 Demographic characteristics, outcome measures, and cognitive task performance by group

Figure 2

Table 3 Coordinates and anatomical region definitions for the results of the image analyses.

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Brain surface images displaying cortical areas with significant t test and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) results: (A) significant activation in subjects with traumatic brain injury (TBI), (B) areas where the TBI group had greater activation than the control group, and C) areas where the TBI group had greater activation than the control group after controlling for blue arrows reaction time (RT), red arrows RT, and scores on the BSI Depression Scale and PCL-C.

Figure 4

Fig. 2 Brain structures where the traumatic brain injury (TBI) group had greater activation than the control group when the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model included the BSI Depression Scale, PCL-C total score, blue arrows reaction time (RT), and red arrows RT as covariates, as well as areas where there was a significant negative relation between brain activation and scores on the PCL-C (all subjects). The statistical parametric maps are overlaid onto high resolution T1-weighted images from a typical control subject.