Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T07:50:14.997Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adolescent Heavy Episodic Drinking: Neurocognitive Functioning during Early Abstinence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2014

Jennifer L. Winward
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Karen L. Hanson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California VA Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, San Diego, California
Nicole M. Bekman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Susan F. Tapert
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
Sandra A. Brown*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Sandra A. Brown, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive (MC 0043), La Jolla, CA 92093-0043. E-mail: sandrabrown@ucsd.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The present study investigated the rate and pattern of neuropsychological recovery in heavy episodic drinking teens during the initial days to weeks of abstinence from alcohol. Adolescents (ages, 16–18 years) with histories of heavy episodic drinking (HED; N = 39) and socio-demographically similar control teens (CON; N = 26) were recruited from San Diego area schools. HED and CON were comparable on 5th grade standardized math and language arts test performance to ensure similar functioning before onset of substance use. Participants were administered three neuropsychological test batteries with 2-week intervals during a 4-week monitored abstinence period. HED teens performed worse overall than CON on tests of prospective memory (p = .005), cognitive switching (p = .039), inhibition task accuracy (p = .001), verbal memory (p's < .045), visuospatial construction (p's < .043), and language and achievement (p's < .008). The statistically significant group × time interaction for block design demonstrated normalization within the 4 weeks of abstinence for the HED (p = .009). This study identified cognitive performance deficits associated with heavy episodic drinking in adolescence during early abstinence and with sustained 4-week abstention. These findings suggest alcohol-related influences on several underlying brain systems that may predate the onset of alcohol abuse or dependence or take longer than 4 weeks to recover. (JINS, 2014, 20, 218–229)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

INTRODUCTION

Alcohol is the most commonly used intoxicant during adolescence. By their senior year of high school, 71% of students have consumed alcohol and 54% have been drunk (Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman and Schulenberg2012). According to national data, 41% of high school seniors drank alcohol in the past month, and 23% of seniors reported heavy episodic drinking (≥5 drinks in males, ≥4 drinks in females within a 2-hr period) in the prior 2 weeks (Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman and Schulenberg2012). Compared to adults, adolescents drink alcohol less frequently but in higher doses, and such heavy episodic drinking among adolescents may be more harmful than consuming alcohol in moderation (1 or 2 drinks) every day (Tapert & Schweinsburg, Reference Tapert and Schweinsburg2005). Consuming greater quantities of alcohol in one sitting is concerning because heavy alcohol consumption associates with high risk, life-threatening outcomes including motor vehicle accidents, alcohol poisoning, illegal activities, school failure, and risky sexual behavior (Hingson, Heeren, Winter, & Wechsler, Reference Hingson, Heeren, Winter and Wechsler2005). A growing number of animal and human studies also suggests that heavy episodic drinking appears to alter developmental trajectories and to interfere with normal neuroanatomical and neurocognitive development (Brown et al., Reference Brown, McGue, Maggs, Schulenberg, Hingson, Swartzwelder and Murphy2008; Brown & Tapert, Reference Brown and Tapert2004; Crews, Braun, Hoplight, Switzer, & Knapp, Reference Crews, Braun, Hoplight, Switzer and Knapp2000; Hommer et al., Reference Hommer, Momenan, Rawlings, Ragan, Williams, Rio and Eckardt1996; Nixon, Tivis, Ceballos, Varner, & Rohrbaugh, Reference Nixon, Tivis, Ceballos, Varner and Rohrbaugh2002; Spear & Varlinskaya, Reference Spear and Varlinskaya2005).

Animal research suggests that adolescents are more vulnerable than adults to ethanol-induced decrements in functioning, especially following chronic, intermittent exposure to high levels of ethanol, which is considered the analog of ‘heavy episodic drinking’ in humans (White, Ghia, Levin, & Swartzwelder, Reference White, Ghia, Levin and Swartzwelder2000). Adolescent rats show more susceptibility to hippocampal injury (Nixon et al., Reference Nixon, Tivis, Ceballos, Varner and Rohrbaugh2002; Slawecki, Betancourt, Cole, & Ehlers, Reference Slawecki, Betancourt, Cole and Ehlers2001; Ward et al., Reference Ward, Colivicchi, Allen, Schol, Lallemand, de Witte and Dexter2009) and to frontal-anterior cortical damage (Crews et al., Reference Crews, Braun, Hoplight, Switzer and Knapp2000), and adolescent rats exposed to ethanol continue to show structural and functional abnormalities into adulthood (Slawecki, Reference Slawecki2002; Slawecki & Roth, Reference Slawecki and Roth2004; White et al., Reference White, Ghia, Levin and Swartzwelder2000). In particular, adolescent rats seem to experience (1) lower initial brain sensitivity to ethanol (Roehrs, Beare, Zorick, & Roth, Reference Roehrs, Beare, Zorick and Roth1994; Silveri & Spear, Reference Silveri and Spear1998), (2) abnormal development of sensitivity to alcohol-induced motor impairments (White et al., Reference White, Bae, Truesdale, Ahmad, Wilson and Swartzwelder2002), and (3) slower onset and magnitude of sedation following alcohol exposure (Little, Kuhn, Wilson, & Swartzwelder, Reference Little, Kuhn, Wilson and Swartzwelder1996; Silveri & Spear, Reference Silveri and Spear1998; Swartzwelder, Richardson, Markwiese-Foerch, Wilson, & Little, Reference Swartzwelder, Richardson, Markwiese-Foerch, Wilson and Little1998). That adolescents have reduced sensitivity to ethanol-induced motor impairing and sedative effects may theoretically allow youth to drink greater quantities of alcohol and attain higher blood alcohol concentrations with less sedation than would be expected in adulthood. The concurrence of reduced susceptibility to the sedating and motor impairing effects of alcohol with an enhanced vulnerability to alcohol-induced neuroanatomical and neurocognitive deficits presents a concerning effect during adolescence.

The extant human literature is consistent with animal research and suggests that heavy and recent alcohol exposure in adolescence is associated with poorer neuropsychological outcomes relative to those of non-drinkers. Studies of adolescents with alcohol use disorders (AUD) and of nonclinical populations of heavy episodic drinkers (HED) have consistently found deficits on executive function measures of planning, decision-making, verbal working memory, and inhibition (Giancola & Mezzich, Reference Giancola and Mezzich2000; Giancola & Moss, Reference Giancola and Moss1998; Giancola, Shoal, & Mezzich, Reference Giancola, Shoal and Mezzich2001; Goudriaan, Grekin, & Sher, Reference Goudriaan, Grekin and Sher2007; Moss, Kirisci, Gordon, & Tarter, Reference Moss, Kirisci, Gordon and Tarter1994). Adolescents with AUDs also demonstrate deficits in verbal learning and recognition discriminability (Brown, Tapert, Granholm, & Delis, Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Hanson, Medina, Padula, Tapert, & Brown, Reference Hanson, Medina, Padula, Tapert and Brown2011; Tapert et al., Reference Tapert, Brown, Kindermann, Cheung, Frank and Brown2001), and they have shown mild decrements in visuospatial memory (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000) such as delayed recall of a complex figure (Squeglia, Spadoni, Infante, Myers, & Tapert, Reference Squeglia, Spadoni, Infante, Myers and Tapert2009).

Adolescent and young adult heavy drinkers commonly show decrements in aspects of visuospatial function including block constructions, spatial working memory, and pattern recognition (e.g., Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Sher, Martin, Wood, & Rutledge, Reference Sher, Martin, Wood and Rutledge1997; Tapert & Brown, Reference Tapert and Brown1999; Tapert, Granholm, Leedy, & Brown, Reference Tapert, Granholm, Leedy and Brown2002; Tapert et al., Reference Tapert, Schweinsburg, Barlett, Brown, Frank, Brown and Meloy2004; Weissenborn & Duka, Reference Weissenborn and Duka2003). Studies also suggest higher error rates among AUD youth (Tarter, Mezzich, Hsieh, & Parks, Reference Tarter, Mezzich, Hsieh and Parks1995; Tapert et al., Reference Tapert, Schweinsburg, Barlett, Brown, Frank, Brown and Meloy2004) and deficits in processing speed, motor speed, and attention (Medina, Schweinsburg, Cohen-Zion, Nagel, & Tapert, Reference Medina, Schweinsburg, Cohen-Zion, Nagel and Tapert2007; Sher et al., Reference Sher, Martin, Wood and Rutledge1997). Finally, alcohol abusing adolescents have been shown to have significantly lower verbal and full scale IQ scores (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Giancola et al., Reference Giancola, Shoal and Mezzich2001) and lower academic achievement in math, reading, and spelling (Moss et al., Reference Moss, Kirisci, Gordon and Tarter1994; Tarter et al., Reference Tarter, Mezzich, Hsieh and Parks1995) than their nondrinking peers.

While many of these studies report deficits across several neurocognitive domains, to date no study has investigated the rate and pattern of neuropsychological recovery in heavy episodic drinking teens throughout the initial days to weeks of abstinence from alcohol (Brown et al., Reference Brown, McGue, Maggs, Schulenberg, Hingson, Swartzwelder and Murphy2008). Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, no existing study has ensured groups’ comparable academic functioning that predates initiation of substance use (e.g., standardized academic test scores), which limits the ability to make generalizations about the impact of alcohol as compared to preexisting differences. By ensuring comparable, premorbid academic functioning and by following adolescents over several weeks of abstinence, this study aimed to elucidate the pattern of neurocognitive recovery during early abstinence from heavy alcohol use. Such knowledge may have important implications for clinical intervention and for strategies to improve academic functioning and reduce relapse risk.

The present study examined cognitive performance of youth engaged in heavy episodic drinking during adolescence, a critical time of brain development. Drinking and nondrinking participants completed a neuropsychological battery three times at 2-week intervals over 4 weeks of monitored abstinence. We aimed to (1) identify neuropsychological deficits associated with recent heavy episodic drinking during adolescence, and (2) determine whether alcohol-induced neurocognitive deficits improve with abstinence. Based on prior research, we hypothesized that (1) recent heavy episodic drinking youth would display neuropsychological deficits during early abstinence relative to similar nondrinking peers in the domains of executive functioning, learning and memory, visuospatial construction, working memory, attention, processing speed, and learning and achievement, and (2) abstaining heavy episodic drinkers would demonstrate improvements in these cognitive domains over a 4-week abstinence period when compared to nondrinking teens studied at comparable timepoints. In other words, we expected that prolonged abstinence would be linked to normalization of functions previously shown to be affected by alcohol.

METHOD

Participants

In accordance with the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) Human Research Protections Program and high school district policies, written informed assent (adolescent participant) and consent (parent/legal guardian) were obtained before participation. The current study examined 65 adolescents (ages, 16–18 years) who were classified into two groups: heavy episodic drinkers (N = 39) and nondrinking controls (N = 26). Classification criteria for the heavy episodic drinkers (HED) included ≥50 lifetime drinking episodes, ≥1 past month heavy episodic drinking episodes, ≥1 alcohol withdrawal symptom in the prior 2 weeks, and limited experience with marijuana and other drugs. Nondrinking controls (CON) had fewer than 10 lifetime experiences with alcohol; had no history of heavy episodic drinking, alcohol withdrawal symptoms, or drug use; and did not meet criteria for heavy drinking status. HED and CON were drawn from the same schools and similar on socio-demographics including age, gender (46% female), ethnic composition (75% Caucasian), grades completed, socioeconomic status (Hollingshead, 1965), and recent grade point average. Fifth grade standardized tests (i.e., California Standards Test) of math and language arts were also comparable between HED and CON (Table 1).

Table 1 Demographic and substance use characteristics for control (CON) and heavy episodic drinking (HED) adolescents (ages 16–18)

aA first-degree biological relative with alcohol or drug related dependence.

bHollingshead (1965) SES (socioeconomic status): Higher scores = lower SES.

cCBCL: Child Behavior Checklist.

dScaled score on California Standards Test (CST).

Participants were recruited from high schools and colleges throughout the San Diego area via mailings and flier distribution (Tapert et al., Reference Tapert, Schweinsburg, Barlett, Brown, Frank, Brown and Meloy2004). The fliers advertised an “Adolescent Development Project,” and no information regarding substance use criteria was described in the flier or discussed before screening. Participants responding by phone were informed of the study protocol and assessment schedule (see below), potential risks and benefits, and the confidentiality of their participation. All interested teens and their guardians underwent an extensive screening process to determine eligibility, and those potentially eligible (i.e., recent heavy episodic drinkers or non-drinkers) were mailed consent packets. After completing the assents/consents, teens and their guardians participated separately in more detailed, structured clinical interviews performed by a different interviewer for each family member.

To minimize confounds, exclusionary criteria included history of a DSM-IV Axis I disorder other than alcohol abuse, extensive other drug use, head trauma, a learning disorder, neurological dysfunction, or serious medical illness; family history of bipolar I or psychotic disorder; significant prenatal alcohol or drug exposure; sensory problems; use of psychoactive medications; and substance use during the abstinence protocol. Overall, 3% of the 2300 teens who responded to the recruitment fliers (approximately 15,000 were distributed) initiated the study. Others did not enroll because they were non-users who were not similar to heavy episodic drinkers (46%), had a history of a psychiatric disorder or psychotropic medication use (25%), used marijuana or other drugs extensively (22%), or were eligible but not interested in the abstinence protocol (7%).

Measures

Structured clinical interview

After providing their assent/consent, adolescent participants and their parents were separately administered confidential structured clinical interviews assessing demographics, social and academic functioning (Brown, Vik, & Creamer, Reference Brown, Vik and Creamer1989), family history of psychiatric disorders using the structured clinical interview of Family History Assessment Module Screener (Rice et al., Reference Rice, Reich, Bucholz, Neuman, Fishman, Rochberg and Beleiter1995), and personal history of Axis I psychiatric disorders using the Computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC; Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, Reference Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan and Schwab-Stone2000). Parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Ruffle, Reference Achenbach and Ruffle2000) and teens completed the Youth Self Report (YSR; Achenbach & Ruffle, Reference Achenbach and Ruffle2000) to assess levels of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Teen substance use history was documented using the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Myers, Lippke, Tapert, Stewart and Vik1998), which assessed lifetime and recent tobacco, alcohol, and drug use (12 classes), withdrawal symptoms, DSM-IV abuse and dependence criteria, and other negative consequences associated with heavy drinking. Good inter-rater reliability, internal consistency, and test-retest ability have been demonstrated with the CDDR among adolescent participants (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Myers, Lippke, Tapert, Stewart and Vik1998; Stewart & Brown, Reference Stewart and Brown1995). The Timeline Followback (TLFB; Sobell & Sobell, Reference Sobell and Sobell1992) modified to include other drugs was used to collect frequency and quantity of alcohol, marijuana, and other drug use for the 6 weeks before initiating the protocol and for the 4-week duration in the study.

Neuropsychological test battery

The thrice-repeated neuropsychological (NP) battery assessed five key domains: (1) executive functioning, (2) learning and memory, (3) visuospatial construction, (4) working memory, attention, processing efficiency, and psychomotor speed, and (5) language and achievement. Standardized neuropsychological tests included the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) Vocabulary and Block Design; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997): Arithmetic, Digit Span, and Digit Symbol; California Verbal Learning Test - Second Edition (CVLT-II; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000); Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures copy and 30-min delayed recall (Osterrieth, Reference Osterrieth1944; Strauss & Spreen, Reference Strauss and Spreen1990; Taylor, Reference Taylor1989); Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System (D-KEFS; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001) Trail Making and Color-Word Interference; a modified version of the Memory for Intentions Test (MIST; Raskin & Buckheit, Reference Raskin and Buckheit1999) to examine prospective memory; and the Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT-4; Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2006) Reading subtest. Alternate forms were used when possible to reduce practice effects (i.e., Rey-O figure at times 1 and 3, and Taylor figure at time 2; alternation of CVLT-II lists also).

State measures

At each testing session, teens completed the Hamilton Depression and Anxiety Rating Scales (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1996) and the state scale of the Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene1970). These measures have well-established psychometric properties (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1996; Spielberger et al., Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene1970). The Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988) assessed baseline mood before testing.

Procedures

All eligible CON and HED participants who initiated study protocol were monitored for abstinence (see below) and assessed using neuropsychological tests at three timepoints over their 4-week participation. At each timepoint, a 150-min NP battery was administered by a trained neuropsychometrist. Before each session, all participants provided a urine sample, submitted a breathalyzer reading (Intoximeter, St. Louis, MO), and completed affective state and personality questionnaires. Upon completion of the NP battery, the participants rated their level of focus, how hard they tried, how seriously they took the session, and level of effort on a scale of 1 (low) to 10 (high). Participants were compensated for their participation and abstention throughout the protocol with the largest payment at the third assessment wave to encourage study completion.

At all appointments, adolescents participated in a toxicology screening protocol to minimize the possibility of their substance use. HED were first studied within 14 days of heavy episodic drinking and subsequently at two 2-week intervals over 4 subsequent weeks of monitored abstinence (1st testing session: M = 5.56 days since last HED episode, SE = 0.60; 2nd testing session: M = 19.52 days since last HED episode, SE = 0.70; 3rd testing session: M = 32.81 days since last HED episode, SE = 0.70). CON teens followed the same abstinence monitoring and neuropsychological testing protocol at the same time intervals. Abstinence was monitored and facilitated through behavioral and biochemical procedures including 10-panel drug urine testing and breathalyzer. Supervised urine and breath samples were collected three times per week to assess for recent use of alcohol with ethyl glucoronide (EtG) and ethyl sulfate (EtS) metabolites and use of methamphetamines, cocaine, THC (cannabis), benzodiazepines, methadone, barbiturates, ecstasy, opiates, PCP, and oxycodone. We used an observed sample collection procedure to minimize the likelihood of participant tampering. Samples were analyzed by Redwood Toxicology (Santa Rosa, CA) using cloned enzyme donor immunoassay (CEDIA) kits. If abstinence maintenance was confirmed via subject self-report, breathalyzer, and quantitative toxicology results, participants continued to be scheduled for neuropsychological assessments. Abstinence was also facilitated using a standardized Motivational Interviewing protocol (Miller & Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick1991) demonstrated to encourage the maintenance of abstinence for adolescents in prior research (Brown, Anderson, Schulte, Sintov, & Frissell, Reference Brown, Anderson, Schulte, Sintov and Frissell2005; Schweinsburg et al., Reference Schweinsburg, Schweinsburg, Cheung, Brown, Brown and Tapert2005). Eleven HED teens drank alcohol during the abstinence period (detected via positive ETG toxicology screen and then confirmed with self-report) and data collected after their alcohol use were excluded from the analyses.

Data Analyses

Chi-square tests (for categorical variables) and t-tests (for continuous variables) compared socio-demographic characteristics between groups. To test for HED-CON differences and changes over time, we used linear mixed effects models to look for group effects at time 1 and time 3, time effects, and group by time interactions. In the mixed model analyses, the fixed variables were timepoint and group, the random variable was the individual subject, and the dependent variable was the standard, age-scaled, or raw score of the NP domain in question. These analyses modeled error structures among repeated dependent variables by using fixed effects, specifying a covariance structure for both between and within subjects, and fitting the means model accounting for specific covariates (Gelman & Hill, Reference Gelman and Hill2007; Rabe-Hesketh & Skrondal, Reference Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal2005; Singer & Willett, Reference Singer and Willett2003). Because groups differed in their CBCL externalizing behavior (p = .002), family history of alcohol dependence (p = .004), and lifetime marijuana use (p < .001), the mixed model analyses controlled for these variables.

Results

Demographics, Substance Use, and Mood

As mentioned previously, the groups were similar on socio-demographic characteristics (Table 1). To ensure CON and HED were comparable on pre-drinking academic performance, 5th grade California Standards Test (CST) scores in language arts and mathematics were examined, and the groups did not differ statistically (p's > .05). While standardized test scores of CON were slightly higher than those of HED (Language Arts: 370.27 ± 11.06 and 354.80 ± 11.48, respectively; p = .141; Math: 394.73 ± 21.59 and 352.30 ± 14.60, respectively; p = .064), both groups ranged from the “basic” to “proficient” level. Participants were typically from lower-middle to upper-middle class families and of average to above-average intelligence. Heavy episodic drinkers self-reported slightly higher CBCL externalizing behavior (p = .002), although still within normal range on average.

Lifetime and recent (i.e., days/month in the 3 months before initiating study) heavy episodic drinking episodes were, as designed, greater in the HED sample (p's < .001). The lifetime marijuana episodes in HED youth were modest for a population with such high levels of drinking experience (average alcohol exposure of 230.50 ± 27.50 vs. average marijuana exposure of 57.62 ± 11.46). HED youth had limited lifetime episodes with other drugs (Table 1).

STAI anxiety and Hamilton depression ratings were similar and within the normal range at all assessments, and both groups had similar PANAS pre-testing mood states (p's > .05). Additionally, groups did not differ in their effort ratings following each NP session with both groups indicating moderately high levels of focus, effort, and seriousness.

Neuropsychological Performance

Neuropsychological test scores at each of the three test sessions are presented in Table 2. The following analyses investigated neurocognitive differences and changes in adolescent heavy episodic drinkers compared to nondrinking teens. A False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons was used to recalculate p-values from the mixed models (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). All reported p-values were generated from the FDR correction.

Table 2 Marginal means (SE) of control (CON) and heavy episodic drinking (HED) adolescents (age 16–18) demonstrate differences and changes in neuropsychological performance with extended abstinence

Note. SS = scaled score; Complex Figure = Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures copy and 30-minute delayed recall (Osterrieth, Reference Osterrieth1944; Strauss & Spreen, Reference Strauss and Spreen1990; Taylor, Reference Taylor1989); D-KEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001); CVLT-II = California Verbal Learning Test - Second Edition (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000); MIST = Memory for Intentions Test (Raskin & Buckheit, Reference Raskin and Buckheit1999); WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999); WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997; WRAT-4 = Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2006).

aInitial group difference (at Time 1) but no group × time interaction.

bInitial group difference (at Time 1) and group × time interaction at 3.

cGroup difference.

Executive functioning

The MIST examined prospective memory: abilities to monitor time, maintain a planned activity in mind, and initiate appropriate action. HED youth performed significantly worse on the MIST at the first timepoint (b (SE) = −1.09 (0.30); z = −3.58; p = .005) and did not improve to levels of CON over repeated testing (p's > .106; Figure 1). CON performed consistently across time (p's > .483) with an overall 2% increase in performance, on average, from Time 1 to Time 3. HED showed most improvements from the first to second timepoint (i.e., between weeks 1 and 3 of abstinence, on average). HED showed a 6% increase in performance from Time 1 to Time 3, but this improvement still left their performance 11% lower than that of CON. HED youth performed worse on the D-KEFS Trail Making Number-Letter Switching at the first timepoint (b (SE) = −1.15 (0.46); z = −2.49; p = .039) and did not improve to levels of CON (p's > .238; Figure 1). Both CON and HED showed a 17% score increase from Times 1 to 3, with HED consistently performing 8–10% lower. On D-KEFS Color-Word Interference, HED made 50–100% more errors than CON at Time 1 (b (SE) = 2.56 (0.79); z = 3.24; p = .001) and across time (p's > .382; Figure 1). While HED accuracy improved with time, they still made nearly double the errors as CON by the third timepoint. No initial differences or group × time interactions were identified on D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Inhibition Switching trial (p's > .090).

Fig. 1 Executive functioning tasks of Memory for Intentions Test (MIST), D-KEFS Trail Making Number-Letter Switching, and D-KEFS Color Word Interference by heavy episodic drinking youth (HED) and controls (CON). From linear mixed effects models with standard error bars, controlling for externalizing behavior, family history of alcohol or drug related dependence, and lifetime marijuana use at three assessments over 4 weeks of abstinence. Average number of days since last heavy episodic drinking episode in HED youth was 5.56 days at Time 1, 19.52 days at Time 2, and 32.81 days at Time 3. At the first timepoint, HED youth performed worse on MIST (p = .005) and Trail Making Switching standard score (SS; p = .039) and committed more errors than CON on Color Word Interference Inhibition (p = .001). Performance did not improve with time on all three tasks (p's > .238). MIST = Memory for Intentions Test.

Learning and memory

On tests of verbal memory, HED youth showed poorer performance at Time 1 on short delay cued recall (b (SE) =−0.60 (0.27); z = −2.26; p = .044), long delay cued recall (b (SE) = −0.76 (0.24); z = −3.19; p = .005), and long delay free recall (b (SE) = −0.70 (0.23); z = −2.98; p = .010). HED verbal memory did not improve to levels of CON, performing 0.36–0.48 standard deviations below CON across time (p's > .292; Figure 2). Although HED scores were consistently lower, they did not differ statistically at the first timepoint or across time on verbal learning (CVLT-II total words recalled trials 1–5; p's > .288) or visuospatial memory (Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures 30-min delayed recall; p's > .280).

Fig. 2 CVLT-II short and long delay verbal memory z-scores by heavy episodic drinking youth (HED) and controls (CON). From linear mixed effects models with standard error bars, controlling for externalizing behavior, family history of alcohol or drug related dependence, and lifetime marijuana use at three assessments over 4 weeks of abstinence. Average number of days since last heavy episodic drinking episode in HED youth was 5.56 days at Time 1, 19.52 days at Time 2, and 32.81 days at Time 3. HED evidenced poorer short delay cued recall (p = .044), long delay cued recall (p = .005), and long delay free recall (p = .010) than CON at the initial testing. Poorer verbal memory continued across time (p's > .292).

Visuospatial construction

On WASI Block Design, HED performed approximately 9% worse than CON at the initial testing (b (SE) = −5.22 (2.15); z = −2.43; p = .039). A group × time interaction was a trend at the second testing (b (SE) = 2.33 (1.24); z = 1.88; p = .059) and statistically significant at the third testing (b (SE) = 3.63 (1.38); z = 2.63; p = .009) with HED improving their performance to that of CON (Figure 3). From Time 1 to Time 3, CON showed a 7% improvement in Block Design performance, while HED scores improved 9% by Time 2 and another 6% from Time 2 to Time 3, showing the bigger percent change from weeks 1 to 3 of abstinence, on average, and continued improvement from weeks 3 to 5 of abstinence, on average. On direct copy of the Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures, HED performed more poorly than CON at the initial testing (b (SE) = −2.18 (.99); z = −2.21; p = .043) and did not improve with time (p's > .585), with HED 6–8% worse than CON across assessments (Figure 3).

Fig. 3 Visuospatial construction tasks of Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures (CF) and Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) Block Design by heavy episodic drinking youth (HED) and controls (CON). From linear mixed effects models with standard error bars, controlling for externalizing behavior, family history of alcohol or drug related dependence, and lifetime marijuana use at three assessments over 4 weeks of abstinence. Average number of days since last heavy episodic drinking episode in HED youth was 5.56 days at Time 1, 19.52 days at Time 2, and 32.81 days at Time 3. HED performed more poorly than CON at the initial testing (p = .043) and across time (p's > .585) on two-dimensional CF copying. On three-dimensional block construction, HED performed worse than CON at the initial testing (p = .039) but improved their performance to that of CON by the third testing (p = .009), showing the biggest improvement between, on average, weeks 1–3 of abstinence.

Working memory, attention, processing efficiency, and psychomotor speed

Groups performed similarly and did not differ statistically in their performance at the first testing or across time on all measures of verbal working memory [WAIS-III Digit Span backward (p's > .288) or Arithmetic (p's > .290)], attention and processing efficiency [D-KEFS Trail Making Visual Scanning task (p's > .288); WAIS-III Digit Span forward (p's > .634); D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Color Naming (p's > .291) and Word Reading (p's > .796)], and psychomotor speed [WAIS-III Digit Symbol (p's > .232); D-KEFS Trail Making Number Sequencing (p's > .474) and Letter Sequencing (p's > .568)].

Language and achievement

HED performed, on average, 12% worse than CON on WASI Vocabulary (average versus high average range; p = .005) and, on average, 7% worse than CON on WRAT-4 Reading (both groups in average range; p = .008). Given the statistically (although not clinically) significant difference in WASI Vocabulary scores between groups, we also conducted the NP analyses covarying for Vocabulary T-score. All results remained consistent, except the finding for the initial difference on the MIST was reduced to a trend (p = .058). Of note, covarying for 5th grade math and language arts standardized test scores did not alter findings.

DISCUSSION

This study examined neurocognitive differences and patterns of recovery in abstinent, adolescent heavy episodic drinkers compared to nondrinking peers. Importantly, groups had comparable California Standards Test (CST) math and language performance on standardized tests that pre-date initiation of drinking in the heavy episodic drinking group, suggesting similar functioning before alcohol use. We found that adolescents with histories of an average of over 200 lifetime drinking episodes who initiated heavy episodic drinking at an average age of 15.33 differed from socio-demographically similar nondrinkers across several neuropsychological domains both during the early stages of abstinence and with continued abstention. The findings are consistent with prior results in youth with much greater alcohol use histories (e.g., Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Giancola & Moss, Reference Giancola and Moss1998). Heavy episodic drinking adolescents performed worse on prospective memory, cognitive switching, inhibition task accuracy, verbal memory, visuospatial abilities, and language and achievement.

Studies on adolescents with alcohol use disorders have consistently found deficits in executive functioning, and the current study, which focused on a nonclinical population of heavy episodic drinkers, also identified deficiencies in prospective memory, cognitive switching, and inhibition task accuracy. Prospective memory requires multiple skills: monitoring time, remembering the task to be performed, and self-initiating the task at the appropriate time. Poorer performance in prospective memory, cognitive switching, and response inhibition may apply to academic and professional settings, as goal-oriented behavior and cognitive flexibility are essential to stay on task, quickly shift mental modes, and respond accurately.

Longer lasting and heavier drinking patterns among adolescents have been linked to disruptions in the hippocampus, a brain structure critical for learning and memory, with adolescent drinkers showing smaller hippocampal volumes and disturbed hippocampal white matter integrity (De Bellis et al., Reference De Bellis, Clark, Beers, Soloff, Boring, Hall and Keshavan2000; Medina et al., Reference Medina, Schweinsburg, Cohen-Zion, Nagel and Tapert2007; Nagel, Schweinsburg, Phan, & Tapert, Reference Nagel, Schweinsburg, Phan and Tapert2005). Our study involved youth earlier in their drinking careers and identified poorer performance in both short delay and long delay verbal memory that did not resolve within 5 weeks of abstinence, on average. Poorer verbal memory is likely to have a significant influence on daily functioning as recall of verbal information occurs when following instructions, remembering lists, taking exams, and other daily activities.

Our finding of poorer visuospatial abilities among heavy episodic drinking adolescents is consistent with many adolescent studies reporting an association between visuospatial impairments and frequency of alcohol use (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Hanson, Cummins, Tapert, & Brown, Reference Hanson, Cummins, Tapert and Brown2011; Squeglia et al., Reference Squeglia, Spadoni, Infante, Myers and Tapert2009) and withdrawal symptoms (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Tapert et al., Reference Tapert, Brown, Kindermann, Cheung, Frank and Brown2001, Reference Tapert, Granholm, Leedy and Brown2002; Tapert and Brown, Reference Tapert and Brown1999). We found initial differences on two- and three-dimensional constructions (i.e., complex figure drawing, block design); however, only performance on the block constructions showed improvements to levels of nondrinking peers, while complex figure reproduction remained poorer across time. This finding could suggest more recovery of mental rotation and spatial navigation functions, whereas spatial organization and fine motor skills may take longer to recover.

Alcohol dependent adolescents have frequently demonstrated significantly lower verbal IQ and reading achievement scores (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tapert, Granholm and Delis2000; Giancola et al., Reference Giancola, Shoal and Mezzich2001; Moss et al., Reference Moss, Kirisci, Gordon and Tarter1994; Tarter et al., Reference Tarter, Mezzich, Hsieh and Parks1995). The present study's finding of poorer vocabulary and reading scores in nonclinical, heavy episodic drinking youth is consistent with such prior research. Given that the drinkers and nondrinkers had comparable math and language scores in 5th grade, it is possible that the poorer vocabulary and reading skills observed in adolescence may be at least partially due to related environment, brain, or behavior changes occurring after the onset of heavy drinking.

Unlike prior research, our study did not identify statistically significant deficits in verbal learning, visuospatial memory, working memory, attention, or psychomotor speed when comparing heavy episodic drinking youth, who have not yet experienced substantial alcohol related problems, to nondrinking youth. Intensity of alcohol use may not yet be severe enough to manifest in differences. Alternatively, methodological differences (e.g., variations in abstention protocol or drug use eligibility criteria; sample size) may also have contributed to incongruent findings.

The present design allowed us to identify significant improvements across time, suggesting that both groups improved with repeated testing and that the 2-week interval between neuropsychological assessments is short enough to evidence practice effects. The improvement seen across time supports the importance of including controls to compare to heavy episodic drinkers. As expected, tasks on which both groups improved with repeated testing showed greater performance increases in the heavy drinkers. Despite greater improvement (i.e., steeper slope) from their Time 1 to Time 3 assessment, heavy drinkers did not perform to levels of nondrinkers on prospective memory, cognitive switching, inhibition task accuracy, verbal memory, or two-dimensional visuospatial construction, performing approximately 5–10% lower and committing 50–100% more errors across time. Assuming adolescents respond to initial abstinence in a similar pattern as adults, they would show an initial improvement in attention, memory, and visuospatial skills within the first 2 weeks of abstinence, with gradual recovery thereafter (Bates, Voelbel, & Buckman, Reference Bates, Voelbel and Buckman2005; Fein, Bachman, Fisher, & Davenport, Reference Fein, Bachman, Fisher and Davenport1990; Reed, Grant, & Rourke, Reference Reed, Grant and Rourke1992; Sullivan, Rosenbloom, & Pfefferbaum, Reference Sullivan, Rosenbloom and Pfefferbaum2000). Our study detected significant improvements in prospective memory, cognitive switching, inhibition task accuracy, and visuospatial abilities from weeks 1 to 3 of abstinence, on average. However, we only identified an improvement significant enough to bring drinkers’ performance to that of nondrinkers on the three-dimensional visuospatial construction task. It may be that a longer period of recovery is needed before the expected improvements seen in adult populations become evident among adolescent drinkers.

We demonstrated that we can repeatedly and intensively assess functioning of 16- to 18-year-olds with and without histories of heavy episodic drinking. Our efforts to measure sustained abstinence were sufficient for this length of time. We went to great lengths to measure continuous abstinence, including the collection of urine samples from all participants on Sunday mornings. These procedures were necessary to detect alcohol exposure in 11 heavy episodic drinking participants (who were discontinued from the study) and to report confidently abstinence in the other drinkers. The study also provides preliminary evidence to support the success of the motivational interviewing protocol to sustain abstinence in a population of heavy episodic drinking adolescents (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Anderson, Schulte, Sintov and Frissell2005; Miller & Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick1991; Schweinsburg et al., Reference Schweinsburg, Schweinsburg, Cheung, Brown, Brown and Tapert2005).

This study featured many design strengths but has several limitations. First, the sample was carefully selected yet modest in size, which limited our statistical power and prevented further exploration of associations between neuropsychological performance and gender, family history, or alcohol use characteristics. Second, as expected, heavy episodic drinkers had some exposure to marijuana or other drugs. While we did covary for marijuana exposure in the mixed models, it is possible that other substance use, although limited, may have contributed to group differences. Of note, the heavy episodic drinkers consumed alcohol four times more than marijuana in their lifetimes, and had an average of approximately 10 lifetime experiences with other drugs. Third, while the study established a relationship between heavy episodic drinking and neurocognitive impairments among adolescents, the directionality and causality can only be determined by longitudinal studies that examine adolescents before any substance involvement.

In summary, consistent with our hypotheses and with previous studies, 16- to 18-year-old heavy episodic drinking adolescents with recent, frequent drinking and limited other drug exposure exhibit modest but significant neurocognitive functioning differences during early abstinence and, in many cases, with sustained abstinence. Although requiring replication, we found decrements in prospective memory, cognitive switching, inhibition task accuracy, verbal memory, visuospatial abilities, and language at the first testing that improve but not to levels of nondrinking peers even after 4 weeks of sustained abstinence (with the exception of three-dimensional block construction). In the present sample, scores on tasks of verbal learning, working memory, attention, and psychomotor speed did not differ as a function of youth drinking experience. Our findings, coupled with extant literature in this field, suggest that deficient neuropsychological functioning is present among adolescents with recent histories of heavy episodic drinking relative to their nondrinking peers. These cognitive differences persist across 4 to 6 weeks of abstinence, suggesting a possible alcohol-induced impact to underlying brain systems, particularly given that groups were comparable on pre-drinking academic test performance. This possibility coincides with the animal literature's finding that adolescence is a time of enhanced sensitivity to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol.

This study has the potential to contribute to improved methods for (1) measuring changes on important neurocognitive, affective, and behavioral domains associated with heavy episodic drinking in adolescents, and (2) monitoring and facilitating real life behavioral improvements associated with abstinence from alcohol. Poorer performance in prospective memory, cognitive switching, inhibition accuracy, verbal memory, visuospatial ability, and language may affect adolescents’ daily experiences in academic or occupational settings. With this knowledge, educators may be able to improve outcomes for these teens by considering their cognitive abilities during instruction and using strategies of repetition and active learning to more effectively engage and instruct a population of heavy episodic drinking youth (Myers, Brown, & Mott, Reference Myers, Brown and Mott1993; Roehrich & Goldman, Reference Roehrich and Goldman1993).

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the NIAAA R21 AA017321 (PI: S. Brown) award. There is no conflict of interest for any author involved in this work. The authors would like to thank Chase Wagner, Rachel Carter, Stephan Jordan, Sidney Bennett, Michael Zamost, and the undergraduate lab volunteers for their assistance with data collection and management. We also sincerely thank the participants and their families for their time and efforts during the study.

References

Achenbach, T.M., Ruffle, T.M. (2000). The Child Behavior Checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatric Review, 21(8), 265271.Google Scholar
Bates, M.E., Voelbel, G.T., Buckman, J.F. (2005). Short term neuropsychological recovery in clients with substance use disorders. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(3), 367377.Google Scholar
Benjamini, Y., Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.Google Scholar
Brown, S., Anderson, K., Schulte, M., Sintov, N., Frissell, K. (2005). Facilitating youth self change through school-based intervention. Addictive Behaviors, 30, 17971810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S.A., McGue, M., Maggs, J., Schulenberg, J., Hingson, R., Swartzwelder, S., Murphy, S. (2008). A developmental perspective on alcohol and youths 16 to 20 years of age. Pediatrics, 121(Suppl. 4), S290S310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S.A., Myers, M.G., Lippke, L., Tapert, S.F., Stewart, D.G., Vik, P.W. (1998). Psychometric evaluation of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR): A measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 59(4), 427438.Google Scholar
Brown, S.A., Tapert, S.F. (2004). Adolescence and the trajectory of alcohol use: Basic to clinical studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 234244.Google Scholar
Brown, S.A., Tapert, S.F., Granholm, E., Delis, D.C. (2000). Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: Effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 164171.Google Scholar
Brown, S.A., Vik, P.W., Creamer, V.A. (1989). Characteristics of relapse following adolescent substance abuse treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 291300.Google Scholar
Crews, F.T., Braun, C.J., Hoplight, B., Switzer, R.C. III, Knapp, D.J. (2000). Binge ethanol consumption causes differential brain damage in young adolescent rats compared with adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(11), 17121723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bellis, M.D., Clark, D.B., Beers, S.R., Soloff, P.H., Boring, A.M., Hall, J., Keshavan, M.S. (2000). Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(5), 737744.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H. (2001). Manual for the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., Ober, B.A. (2000). Manual for the California Verbal Learning Test, 2nd edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Fein, G., Bachman, L., Fisher, S., Davenport, L. (1990). Cognitive impairments in abstinent alcoholics. Addiction Medicine [Special Issue], 152, 531537.Google Scholar
Gelman, A., Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giancola, P.R., Mezzich, A.C. (2000). Neuropsychological deficits in female adolescents with a substance use disorder: Better accounted for by conduct disorder? Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61(6), 809817.Google Scholar
Giancola, P.R., Moss, H.B. (1998). Executive cognitive functioning in alcohol use disorders. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 14, 227251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giancola, P.R., Shoal, G.D., Mezzich, A.C. (2001). Constructive thinking, executive functioning, antisocial behavior, and drug use involvement in adolescent females with a substance use disorder. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 9(2), 215227.Google Scholar
Goudriaan, A.E., Grekin, E.R., Sher, K.J. (2007). Decision making and binge drinking: A longitudinal study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31(6), 928938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1996). The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Hanson, K.L., Cummins, K., Tapert, S.F., Brown, S.A. (2011). Changes in neuropsychological functioning over 10 years following adolescent substance abuse treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25, 127142.Google Scholar
Hanson, K.L., Medina, K.L., Padula, C.B., Tapert, S.F., Brown, S.A. (2011). Impact of Adolescent Alcohol and Drug Use on Neuropsychological Functioning in Young Adulthood: 10-Year Outcomes. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 20(2), 135154.Google Scholar
Hingson, R., Heeren, T., Winter, M., Wechsler, H. (2005). Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public Health, 26, 259279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A.B. (1965). Two-factor index of social position. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hommer, D., Momenan, R., Rawlings, R., Ragan, P., Williams, W., Rio, D., Eckardt, M. (1996). Decreased corpus callosum size among alcoholic women. Archives of Neurology, 43, 359363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, L.D., O'Malley, P.M., Bachman, J.G., Schulenberg, J.E. (2012). Monitoring the future national results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2011. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Little, P.J., Kuhn, C.M., Wilson, W.A., Swartzwelder, H.S. (1996). Differential effects of ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 20(8), 13461351.Google Scholar
Medina, K.L., Schweinsburg, A.D., Cohen-Zion, M., Nagel, B.J., Tapert, S.F. (2007). Effects of alcohol and combined marijuana and alcohol use during adolescence on hippocampal volume and asymmetry. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 29, 141152.Google Scholar
Miller, W., Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people to change addictive behavior. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Moss, H.B., Kirisci, L., Gordon, H.W., Tarter, R.E. (1994). A neuropsychologic profile of adolescent alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 18, 159163.Google Scholar
Myers, M.G., Brown, S.A., Mott, M.A. (1993). Coping as a predictor of adolescent substance abuse treatment outcome. Journal of Substance Abuse, 5, 1529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagel, B.J., Schweinsburg, A.D., Phan, V., Tapert, S.F. (2005). Reduced hippocampal volume among adolescents with alcohol use disorders without psychiatric comorbidity. Psychiatry Research, 139(3), 181190.Google Scholar
Nixon, S.J., Tivis, R., Ceballos, N., Varner, J.L., Rohrbaugh, J. (2002). Neurophysiological efficiency in male and female alcoholics. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 26(5), 919927.Google Scholar
Osterrieth, P.A. (1944). Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Archives of Psychology, 30, 206356.Google Scholar
Rabe-Hesketh, S., Skrondal, A. (2005). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling using stata. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Raskin, S.A., Buckheit, C.A. (1999). Examination of prospective memory. Washington, DC: Cognitive Neuroscience Society.Google Scholar
Reed, R.J., Grant, I., Rourke, S.B. (1992). Long-term abstinent alcoholics have normal memory. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 16, 677683.Google Scholar
Rice, J.P., Reich, T., Bucholz, K.K., Neuman, R.J., Fishman, R., Rochberg, N., Beleiter, H. (1995). Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 19(4), 10181023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roehrich, L., Goldman, M.S. (1993). Experience-dependent neuropsychological recovery and the treatment of alcoholism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 812821.Google Scholar
Roehrs, T., Beare, D., Zorick, F., Roth, T. (1994). Sleepiness and ethanol effects on simulated driving. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 18, 154158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweinsburg, A.D., Schweinsburg, B.C., Cheung, E.H., Brown, G.G., Brown, S.A., Tapert, S.F. (2005). fMRI response to spatial working memory in adolescents with comorbid marijuana and alcohol use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 79(2), 201210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C.P., Dulcan, M.K., Schwab-Stone, M.E. (2000). NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 2838.Google Scholar
Sher, K.J., Martin, E.D., Wood, P.K., Rutledge, P.C. (1997). Alcohol use disorders and neuropsychological functioning in first-year undergraduates. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(3), 304315.Google Scholar
Silveri, M.M., Spear, L.P. (1998). Decreased sensitivity to the hypnotic effects of ethanol early in ontogeny. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 22(3), 670676.Google Scholar
Singer, J.D., Willett, J.B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slawecki, C.J. (2002). Altered EEG responses to ethanol in adult rats exposed to ethanol during adolescence. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 26(2), 246254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slawecki, C.J., Betancourt, M., Cole, M., Ehlers, C.L. (2001). Periadolescent alcohol exposure has lasting effects on adult neurophysiological function in rats. Brain Research, Developmental Brain Research, 128(1), 6372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slawecki, C.J., Roth, J. (2004). Comparison of the onset of hypoactivity and anxiety-like behavior during alcohol withdrawal in adolescent and adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 28(4), 598607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobell, L.C., Sobell, M.B. (1992). Timeline follow-back: A technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption. In R. Z. Litten & J. P. Allen (Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biochemical methods (pp. 4172). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.Google Scholar
Spear, L.P., Varlinskaya, E.I. (2005). Adolescence. Alcohol sensitivity, tolerance, and intake. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 17, 143159.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., Lushene, R.E. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Squeglia, L.M., Spadoni, A.D., Infante, M.A., Myers, M.G., Tapert, S.F. (2009). Initiating moderate to heavy alcohol use predicts changes in neuropsychological functioning for adolescent girls and boys. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23(4), 715722.Google Scholar
Stewart, D.G., Brown, S.A. (1995). Withdrawal and dependency symptoms among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers. Addiction, 90, 627635.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Spreen, O. (1990). A comparison of the Rey and Taylor figures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5(4), 417420.Google Scholar
Sullivan, E.V., Rosenbloom, M.J., Pfefferbaum, A. (2000). Pattern of motor and cognitive deficits in detoxified alcoholic men. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(5), 611621.Google Scholar
Swartzwelder, H.S., Richardson, R.C., Markwiese-Foerch, B., Wilson, W.A., Little, P.J. (1998). Developmental differences in the acquisition of tolerance to ethanol. Alcohol, 15(4), 311314.Google Scholar
Tapert, S.F., Brown, S.A. (1999). Neuropsychological correlates of adolescent substance abuse: Four year outcomes. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(6), 481493.Google Scholar
Tapert, S.F., Brown, G.G., Kindermann, S., Cheung, E.H., Frank, L.R., Brown, S.A. (2001). fMRI measurement of brain dysfunction in alcohol-dependent young women. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 25, 236245.Google ScholarPubMed
Tapert, S.F., Granholm, E., Leedy, N.G., Brown, S.A. (2002). Substance use and withdrawal: Neuropsychological functioning over 8 years in youth. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(7), 873883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tapert, S.F., Schweinsburg, A.D. (2005). The human adolescent brain and alcohol use disorders. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 17, 177197.Google Scholar
Tapert, S.F., Schweinsburg, A.D., Barlett, V.C., Brown, S.A., Frank, L.R., Brown, G.G., Meloy, M.J. (2004). Blood oxygen level dependent response and spatial working memory in adolescents with alcohol use disorders. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 28(10), 15771586.Google Scholar
Tarter, R.E., Mezzich, A.C., Hsieh, Y.C., Parks, S.M. (1995). Cognitive capacity in female adolescent substance abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 39(1), 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, D.C. (1989). Affective disorders in epilepsies: A neuropsychiatric review. Behavioral Neurology, 2, 4968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, R.J., Colivicchi, M.A., Allen, R., Schol, F., Lallemand, F., de Witte, P., Dexter, D. (2009). Neuro-inflammation induced in the hippocampus of ‘binge drinking’ rats may be mediated by elevated extracellular glutamate content. Journal of Neurochemistry, 111(5), 11191128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 10631070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissenborn, R., Duka, T. (2003). Acute alcohol effects on cognitive function in social drinkers: Their relationship to drinking habits. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 165, 306312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
White, A.M., Bae, J.G., Truesdale, M.C., Ahmad, S., Wilson, W.A., Swartzwelder, H.S. (2002). Chronic-intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence prevents normal developmental changes in sensitivity to ethanol-induced motor impairments. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 26(7), 960968.Google Scholar
White, A.M., Ghia, A.J., Levin, E.D., Swartzwelder, H.S. (2000). Binge pattern ethanol exposure in adolescent and adult rats: Differential impact on subsequent responsiveness to ethanol. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(8), 12511256.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G.S., Robertson, G.J. (2006). The Wide Range Achievement Test-4 Administration Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic and substance use characteristics for control (CON) and heavy episodic drinking (HED) adolescents (ages 16–18)

Figure 1

Table 2 Marginal means (SE) of control (CON) and heavy episodic drinking (HED) adolescents (age 16–18) demonstrate differences and changes in neuropsychological performance with extended abstinence

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Executive functioning tasks of Memory for Intentions Test (MIST), D-KEFS Trail Making Number-Letter Switching, and D-KEFS Color Word Interference by heavy episodic drinking youth (HED) and controls (CON). From linear mixed effects models with standard error bars, controlling for externalizing behavior, family history of alcohol or drug related dependence, and lifetime marijuana use at three assessments over 4 weeks of abstinence. Average number of days since last heavy episodic drinking episode in HED youth was 5.56 days at Time 1, 19.52 days at Time 2, and 32.81 days at Time 3. At the first timepoint, HED youth performed worse on MIST (p = .005) and Trail Making Switching standard score (SS; p = .039) and committed more errors than CON on Color Word Interference Inhibition (p = .001). Performance did not improve with time on all three tasks (p's > .238). MIST = Memory for Intentions Test.

Figure 3

Fig. 2 CVLT-II short and long delay verbal memory z-scores by heavy episodic drinking youth (HED) and controls (CON). From linear mixed effects models with standard error bars, controlling for externalizing behavior, family history of alcohol or drug related dependence, and lifetime marijuana use at three assessments over 4 weeks of abstinence. Average number of days since last heavy episodic drinking episode in HED youth was 5.56 days at Time 1, 19.52 days at Time 2, and 32.81 days at Time 3. HED evidenced poorer short delay cued recall (p = .044), long delay cued recall (p = .005), and long delay free recall (p = .010) than CON at the initial testing. Poorer verbal memory continued across time (p's > .292).

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Visuospatial construction tasks of Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures (CF) and Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) Block Design by heavy episodic drinking youth (HED) and controls (CON). From linear mixed effects models with standard error bars, controlling for externalizing behavior, family history of alcohol or drug related dependence, and lifetime marijuana use at three assessments over 4 weeks of abstinence. Average number of days since last heavy episodic drinking episode in HED youth was 5.56 days at Time 1, 19.52 days at Time 2, and 32.81 days at Time 3. HED performed more poorly than CON at the initial testing (p = .043) and across time (p's > .585) on two-dimensional CF copying. On three-dimensional block construction, HED performed worse than CON at the initial testing (p = .039) but improved their performance to that of CON by the third testing (p = .009), showing the biggest improvement between, on average, weeks 1–3 of abstinence.