Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-8gtf8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-20T22:32:06.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Elevated pulse pressure is associated with age-related decline in language ability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 July 2010

D.A. NATION
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California
C.E. WIERENGA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
L. DELANO-WOOD
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
A.J. JAK
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
D.C. DELIS
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
D.P. SALMON
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California
M.W. BONDI*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Mark W Bondi, 3350 La Jolla Village Dr., San Diego, CA 92161. E-mail: mbondi@ucsd.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Recent research suggests that pulse pressure (PP), a putative marker of vascular integrity, may be associated with brain microvascular damage and age-related cognitive decline. Thus, the present study examined the relationship between PP and cognition in a sample of healthy nondemented older adults. One hundred nine participants were administered neurological and neuropsychological evaluations and determined to be nondemented. Regression analyses were used to examine the relationships among pulse pressure (PP) [systolic blood pressure (SBP) – diastolic blood pressure (DBP)], age, and cognition. PP and related measures were inversely correlated with global cognitive functioning and scores on a composite measure of language function, even after adjusting for age, education, and relevant vascular risk factors. Results indicate that increases in the pulsatile component of blood pressure may convey added risk of global cognitive decline and specific impairment in language abilities. (JINS, 2010, 16, 933–938.)

Type
Brief Communications
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

INTRODUCTION

Blood pressure is typically measured at two points within the cardiac cycle, systole and diastole, which are used to calculate the steady component of blood pressure, mean arterial pressure (MAP), or its pulsatile component, pulse pressure (PP) (Nichols & O’Rourke, Reference Nichols and O’Rourke1998). The steady component, MAP, is conceptualized as the product of cardiac output and total peripheral resistance. The pulsatile component, PP, is determined by a more complex combination of factors, including stroke volume, arterial compliance, and wave reflection (Safar, Levy, & Struijker-Boudier, Reference Safar, Levy and Struijker-Boudier2003). Extensive research has investigated the effects of systolic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) on cognitive functioning (Qiu, Winblad, & Fratiglioni, Reference Qiu, Winblad and Fratiglioni2005). However, when examined in isolation, these two points on the pressure curve do not fully capture important information about the pulsatile component of blood pressure. Indeed, comparatively few studies have examined the relationship between the pulsatile aspect of blood pressure and cognitive functioning, despite substantial evidence that it is related to vascular disease (Jankowski, Bilo, & Kawecka-Jaszcz, Reference Jankowski, Bilo and Kawecka-Jaszcz2007; Nakayama, Hayashi, Yoshimaru, Tsumura, & Ueda, Reference Nakayama, Hayashi, Yoshimaru, Tsumura and Ueda2002).

In healthy individuals, the elastic properties of the aorta, carotid, and other large arteries reduce pressure fluctuations that occur during the cardiac cycle. Arterial elasticity allows stretching to reduce pressure increases during systole and recoil to buffer decreased pressure during diastole (Safar et al., Reference Safar, Levy and Struijker-Boudier2003). These factors assist in the maintenance of relatively constant pressure at the level of distal arterioles and capillary beds (Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2008). Changes in elastin and collagen fibers within the elastic lamina of larger vessels may be responsible for stiffening of large arteries with age (Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2008). As large arteries stiffen, they lose compliance and no longer buffer fluctuations in pressure throughout the cardiac cycle. The resulting increase in pulsatile stress on the vessel wall may contribute to the development of atherosclerosis, which can further exacerbate arterial stiffening (Zuckerman, Weisman, & Yin, Reference Zuckerman, Weisman and Yin1989). These changes could expose smaller vessels to harmful pressure fluctuations during each cardiac cycle, potentially leading to microvascular damage (Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2008).

This problem may be particularly salient in highly perfused organs with a rich, autoregulated microvasculature, such as the brain. This hypothesis is supported by studies showing that measures of pressure fluctuation and arterial stiffness, such as PP and pulse wave velocity (PWV), have been associated with microvascular damage [white matter lesions (WML)] in older adults with hypertension (Kearney-Schwartz et al., Reference Kearney-Schwartz, Rossignol, Bracard, Felblinger, Fay and Boivin2009) and those with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Lee, Jeong, Choi, Sohn, & Chui, Reference Lee, Jeong, Choi, Sohn and Chui2006). Additionally, elevated PP has been associated with declines in global cognitive functioning and memory in healthy populations (Waldstein, Rice, Thayer, Najjar, Scuteri, & Zonderman, Reference Waldstein, Rice, Thayer, Najjar, Scuteri and Zonderman2008) and those with various vascular risk factors (Abbatacola et al., Reference Abbatacola, Barbieri, Rizzo, Grella, Laieta and Quaranta2008; Kearney-Schwartz et al., Reference Kearney-Schwartz, Rossignol, Bracard, Felblinger, Fay and Boivin2009). Findings have been less consistent with regard to the relationship between PP and measures of working memory, visuospatial abilities, executive functioning, and psychomotor speed (Dahle, Jacobs, & Raz, Reference Dahle, Jacobs and Raz2009; Kearney-Schwartz et al., Reference Kearney-Schwartz, Rossignol, Bracard, Felblinger, Fay and Boivin2009; Muller, Grobbee, Aleman, Bots, & van der Schouw, Reference Muller, Grobbee, Aleman, Bots and van der Schouw2007; Raz, Dahle, Rodrigue, Kennedy, & Land, Reference Raz, Dahle, Rodrigue, Kennedy and Land2009; Waldstein et al., Reference Waldstein, Rice, Thayer, Najjar, Scuteri and Zonderman2008). Inconsistencies across studies may be related to methodological differences as well as variations in sample characteristics (e.g., age, degree of cognitive impairment, and prevalence of vascular risk factors).

Studies have found that the relationship between standard blood pressure measures and cognitive functioning is complex, curvilinear, and age-dependent (for review, Qiu, Winblad, & Fratiglioni, Reference Qiu, Winblad and Fratiglioni2005). Longitudinal studies have repeatedly found that elevated blood pressure in midlife is predictive of cognitive decline in late life, but cross-sectional studies indicate that both high and low blood pressure are associated with cognitive decline in older adults. Specifically, low DBP has repeatedly been associated with cognitive decline in older adults (Kennelly, Lawlor, & Kenny, Reference Kennelly, Lawlor and Kenny2009). Although SBP increases with age, MAP remains constant and DBP typically decreases, leading to elevated PP (Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2008). Thus, increased PP may be more important in age-related cognitive decline than standard blood pressure measures. Beyond the pulsatile strain produced by increased PP, elevated PP also represents an indirect measure of the increased arterial stiffness and atherosclerosis that occurs with age. Thus, even in the context of blood pressure that is well controlled by medications, PP may remain elevated and continues to be associated with cardiovascular events (Van Bortel, Struiker-Boudier, & Safar, Reference Van Bortel, Struijker-Boudier and Safar2001). Taken together these considerations point toward PP as a particularly important hemodynamic measure in studies examining age-related cognitive decline.

Given the potential differential contribution of SBP and DBP to cognitive performance in older adults, measures of pulsatile pressure that correct for these values may be more sensitive to age-related changes in cognition. These measures, referred to as fractional systolic pressure (FSP) and diastolic pressure (FDP), have been associated with vascular disease in animal and human models (Jankowski et al., Reference Jankowski, Bilo and Kawecka-Jaszcz2007; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Hayashi, Yoshimaru, Tsumura and Ueda2002), but their relationship with cognition has not been extensively investigated. Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine nondemented healthy older adults to (1) further examine the relationship between measures of the pulsatile component of blood pressure and cognitive functioning and (2) compare these findings with those of other more commonly used hemodynamic measures (e.g., systolic and diastolic blood pressure).

METHODS

Participants

One hundred nine nondemented individuals were recruited from ongoing community-based studies of normal aging and AD, as well as through the community via word-of-mouth and flyers. All participants underwent medical, neurological, and laboratory evaluations. Individuals with a history of alcoholism, drug abuse, learning disability, neurological, or psychiatric disease were excluded. Participants also underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, including measures of global cognitive functioning, attention, language, executive functioning, visuospatial abilities, memory, and functioning in daily living. To screen for possible dementia, participants with a Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) score of ≤ 126 were excluded from analyses (see also Jak et al., Reference Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom and Salmon2009). All data were obtained in compliance with institutional guidelines.

Stroke Risk Factors and Hemodynamic Measures

Hemodynamic measures and vascular risk factors were obtained through clinical interview and physical examination. Blood pressure measures were obtained from single recordings. Participants were seated comfortably for a few minutes before taking blood pressure measures. Self-reported blood pressure was used for participants that frequently measure their own blood pressure. PP and MAP were calculated from SBP and DBP using the following equations: PP = SBP – DBP; MAP=((2 × DBP)+SBP)/3. FSP and FDP were calculated using the following equations, respectively: FSP = PP/SBP; FDP = PP/DBP (Jankowski et al., Reference Jankowski, Bilo and Kawecka-Jaszcz2007; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Hayashi, Yoshimaru, Tsumura and Ueda2002).

Neuropsychological Assessment

Cognitive testing was performed using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery described previously (Salmon & Butters, Reference Salmon, Butters, Katzman and Rowe1992). The battery included an assessment of global cognitive functioning as well as the following six neurocognitive domains: Global cognitive functioning was assessed by the DRS total score. Attention was measured by the attention subscale of the DRS and the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. Language was measured with the Boston Naming Test, category (animals) fluency, and the supermarket items fluency subtest of the DRS. Visuospatial functioning was determined by performance on the Block Design subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) and the construction subscale of the DRS. The use of the WISC block design is based on extensive use of this test in research at the UCSD Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (see Salmon & Butters, Reference Salmon, Butters, Katzman and Rowe1992) on cognitive changes with aging and dementia. Executive functioning was assessed by the modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, 48 card version (perseverative responses), Trail Making Test Part B, and the letter (FAS) fluency test. Verbal Memory was assessed by the Logical Memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised (immediate and delayed free recall) and the California Verbal Learning Test (Trials 1–5 total recall and long delay free recall). Processing Speed was measured by the Trail Making Test Part A. Raw scores were converted into Z-scores based on the mean and standard deviation of the sample of participants identified as normally aging (see criteria set forth by Jak et al., Reference Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom and Salmon2009). Cognitive domain scores represented the average Z-score for the tests within each domain.

Statistical Analyses

Non-normally distributed cognitive measures were log-transformed before z-transformation. Independent samples t tests were used to investigate differences in hemodynamic measures and cognitive domain scores between men and women, individuals on or off antihypertensive medications, and those with or without various stroke risk factors, including history of atrial fibrillation (AF), cardiovascular disease (CVD), and transient ischemic attack (TIA) or minor stroke. Pearson product-moment correlations and t tests were used to examine the relationships between hemodynamic measures and neuropsychological test scores, as well as identification of appropriate covariates from demographic and stroke risk data. Bivariate correlations between hemodynamic and neuropsychological measures were treated as omnibus tests by domain, with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons being applied for each cognitive domain (i.e., p value of .05/7 = .007). Only those hemodynamic variables found to be significantly related to cognitive domain scores after correction for multiple comparisons were retained in regression models. Multiple hierarchical regression was used to test relationships between hemodynamic parameters and cognitive functioning after controlling for relevant covariates. All significance tests were 2-tailed with a cutoff of p < .05, after correction for multiple comparisons.

RESULTS

Participant Characteristics

Forty-eight participants were men (44.0%). The average age at examination was 74.2 years (SD = 10.0), ranging from 48 to 93 years, average DRS total score was 139.9 (SD = 3.7), ranging from 127 to 144, and average education was 16.2 years (SD = 2.3). Average SBP was 125.8 (SD = 12.8), average DBP was 73.2 (SD = 9.4), 48.0% were on anti-hypertensive medications, 21.2% had evidence of CVD, 11.5% had a history of TIA or minor stroke, and 16.2% had AF. Results of independent samples t tests indicated no significant differences in any hemodynamic measures between men and women, individuals on or off antihypertensive medications, or those with or without a history of AF (p > .10). Age showed a significant positive correlation with SBP (r = 0.25; p < .01) and a nonsignificant trend toward a negative correlation with DBP (r = −0.18; p = .06). Age was strongly correlated with PP measures, including PP (r = 0.38; p < .0001), FSP (r = 0.36; p = .0001), and FDP (r = 0.36; p = .0001). The steady component of blood pressure, MAP, was not correlated with age (r = −0.01; p > .90).

Hemodynamic Measures and Cognition

Commonly used hemodynamic measures, including SBP, DBP, and MAP, showed no significant correlations with scores on composite measures of cognitive functioning, after controlling for multiple comparisons. PP was inversely correlated with language ability (p = .0001), but showed no significant relationship with other cognitive domains after correction for multiple comparisons. Fractionated measures of PP, including FSP and FDP, were also significantly correlated with language ability (p < .001). Additionally, FDP showed a significant negative correlation with global cognitive functioning (p = .003) (Table 1).

Table 1. Correlations between hemodynamic and cognitive measures

Note

p values are displayed only for relationships significant after correction for multiple comparisons. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Regression Analyses

Analysis of potential covariates for multiple regression analyses indicated a negative correlation between age and language (r = −0.32; p < .01). Education was positively correlated with language (r = 0.29; p < .01) and global cognitive functioning (r = 0.40; p < .0001). Additionally, t tests indicated that individuals on antihypertensive medications exhibited lower global cognitive functioning (p < .01) and those with AF displayed lower language ability (p < .05). Consequently, these variables were retained in regression models.

The relationship between PP and language ability remained significant after controlling for age, education, and history of AF, β = −0.26; p < .01; ΔR2 = 0.06 (Figure 1). The inverse correlation between age and language ability was no longer significant after controlling for PP and these covariates, β = −0.13, p > .10, ΔR2 = .01. The relationship between FSP and language ability remained significant using the same model, β = −0.24; p = .01; ΔR2 = 0.05, with similar results holding for FDP, β = −0.24; p < .05; ΔR2 = 0.05.

Fig. 1. Scatterplots of age and pulse pressure (PP) by language ability are displayed. Age (+, dashed line) and PP (■, solid line) exhibited significant inverse correlations with language ability: r = −0.32, p < .01 and r = −0.36, p < .001, respectively.

The relationship between FDP and global cognitive functioning remained significant after controlling for education and antihypertensive medication, β = −0.22; p < .05; ΔR2 = 0.05 (Figure 1).

DISCUSSION

Our results demonstrated that nondemented older adults with elevated measures of the pulsatile component of blood pressure performed more poorly on tests of language, as well as global cognitive functioning, but not on tasks assessing attention, executive, visuospatial, memory, and processing speed abilities. The observed relationships between PP measures and cognition were of medium effect size and were not fully accounted for by relevant covariates, including age, education, and vascular risk factors. Importantly, other more commonly used hemodynamic measures, including SBP, DBP, and MAP, showed no significant relationships with measures of cognitive function after controlling for multiple comparisons. All measures of the pulsatile component of blood pressure, including fractionated measures of PP that adjust for absolute values of SBP and DBP, showed similar negative associations with cognition, suggesting that the results were not greatly influenced by either of these blood pressure points in isolation. In fact, FDP appeared to show the most consistent relationship with cognition, displaying negative relationships with language and global cognitive functioning.

All three measures of PP showed stronger relationships with language functioning than any other cognitive ability, suggesting that this cognitive domain may be particularly impacted by elevations in the pulsatile component of blood pressure. In older adults, insidious decline in language abilities and executive processes have both been associated with functional alterations within frontal-subcortical networks, suggesting that the ability to access and retrieve semantic information may underlie these word-finding problems (Wierenga et al., Reference Wierenga, Benjamin, Gopinath, Perlstein, Leonard and Rothi2008). Increased pulsatile pressure could be impacting these functional networks through disruption of frontal-subcortical white matter tracts, as neuroimaging studies have associated increased PP with subcortical leukoaraiosis (Kearney-Schwartz et al., Reference Kearney-Schwartz, Rossignol, Bracard, Felblinger, Fay and Boivin2009; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Jeong, Choi, Sohn and Chui2006).

However, the current study did not observe a significant relationship between PP and executive functioning, suggesting that other mechanisms of injury may be involved. Beyond evidence of subcortical ischemic lesions, there may be more insidious neuropathologic changes associated with alterations in hemodynamic parameters. For example, both animal and human studies have found that blood pressure elevation can cause reductions in cerebral blood flow and increased cerebral atrophy, even in the absence of frank vascular lesions (Fujishima, Ibayashi, Fujii, & Mori, Reference Fujishima, Ibayashi, Fujii and Mori1995; Gesztelyi, Finnegan, DeMaro, Wang, Chen, & Fenstermacher, Reference Gesztelyi, Finnegan, DeMaro, Wang, Chen and Fenstermacher1993). These subtle neuropathological changes have been associated with cognitive dysfunction (Dai, Lopez, Carmichael, Becker, Kuller, & Gach, Reference Dai, Lopez, Carmichael, Becker, Kuller and Gach2008; Vlek et al., Reference Vlek, Visseren, Kappelle, Geerlings, Vincken and Mali2009), and it is possible that increased pulse pressure may also convey vulnerability through more subtle changes in neurovascular function. Another possibility is that more richly perfused areas, such as cortical gray matter, could be susceptible to damage or dysfunction from increased PP. It has been proposed that increased microvascular perfusion in the context of elevated PP may be associated with particularly high risk of damage to these brain areas (Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2008). While it is possible that increased PP is causing impairment through cerebrovascular damage, it is also possible that PP is simply serving as a marker of large artery stiffness and cerebrovascular disease. Thus, PP may represent a measure of systemic vascular disease, a causative factor in cerebrovascular dysfunction, or some combination of both. Future studies examining the relationship between PP and cerebral structure, function, and perfusion may provide more information regarding the mechanism underlying the association between PP and language function.

Although the present study did not find a significant association between executive function and measures of PP after correction for multiple comparisons, performance of category fluency tasks involves some executive processes and has been periodically included as a measure of executive function in prior studies. Given the high prevalence of age-related decline in language ability, it may be more favorable to combine category fluency measures into a language composite in studies involving older adults. These semantic abilities frequently decline with age and are among the earliest cognitive abilities to decline in the course of AD (see Bondi, Jak, Delano-Wood, Jacobson, Delis, & Salamon, Reference Bondi, Jak, Delano-Wood, Jacobson, Delis and Salmon2008 for review). Studies have found that patients with AD exhibit elevated PP relative to controls and that elevated PP is associated with increased risk of developing AD (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Jeong, Choi, Sohn and Chui2006; Qiu et al., Reference Qiu, Winblad, Viitanen and Fratiglioni2003). The current study did not examine patients with AD. Thus, the significance of the current findings remains unclear in terms of risk of AD, although we have demonstrated significant declines in semantic memory in the years preceding mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or AD diagnoses (Mickes et al., Reference Mickes, Wixted, Fennema-Notestine, Galasko, Bondi and Thal2007). Future longitudinal studies examining whether PP predicts declines in language function, or progression to MCI or dementia, will further establish the significance of this finding for dementia research.

The findings of the current study are limited by the nature of our sample, which was relatively well educated and medically healthy. The relationship between PP and cognitive functioning may be different in individuals with a greater number of risk factors for cognitive impairment.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by grant IIRG 07-59343 from the Alzheimer’s Association (M.W.B.); National Institute on Aging grants R01 AG012674 (M.W.B.), K24 AG026431 (M.W.B.), and P50 AG05131 (D.P.S.); National Institutes of Health Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award MH18399 (D.A.N.); and by the Department of Veterans Affairs (D.C.D., L.D.W., A.J.J., C.E.W.).

References

REFERENCES

Abbatacola, A.M., Barbieri, M., Rizzo, M.R., Grella, R., Laieta, M.T., Quaranta, E., et al. . (2008). Arterial stiffness and cognition in elderly persons with impaired glucose tolerance and microalbuminuria. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 63, 991996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bondi, M.W., Jak, A.J., Delano-Wood, L., Jacobson, M.W., Delis, D.C., & Salmon, D.P. (2008). Neuropsychological contributions to the early identification of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology Review, 18, 7390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahle, C.L., Jacobs, B.S., & Raz, N. (2009). Aging, vascular risk, and cognition: Blood glucose, pulse pressure, and cognitive performance in healthy adults. Psychology and Aging, 24, 154162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dai, W., Lopez, O.L., Carmichael, O.T., Becker, J.T., Kuller, H., & Gach, H.M. (2008). Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in cognitively normal elderly subjects with hypertension. Stroke, 39, 349354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujishima, M., Ibayashi, S., Fujii, K., & Mori, S. (1995). Cerebral blood flow and brain function in hypertension. Hypertension Research, 18, 111117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gesztelyi, G., Finnegan, W., DeMaro, J.A., Wang, J.Y., Chen, J.L., & Fenstermacher, J. (1993). Parenchymal microvascular systems and cerebral atrophy in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Brain Research, 611, 249257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jak, A.J., Bondi, M.W., Delano-Wood, L., Wierenga, C., Corey-Bloom, J., Salmon, D.P., et al. . (2009). Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 368375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jankowski, P., Bilo, G., & Kawecka-Jaszcz, K. (2007). The pulsatile component of blood pressure: Its role in the pathogenesis of atherosclerosis. Blood Pressure, 16, 238245.Google Scholar
Kearney-Schwartz, A., Rossignol, P., Bracard, S., Felblinger, J., Fay, R., Boivin, J.M., et al. . (2009). Vascular structure and function is correlated to cognitive performance and white matter hyperintensities in older hypertensive patients with subjective memory complaints. Stroke, 40, 12291236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennelly, S.P., Lawlor, B.A., & Kenny, R.A. (2009). Blood pressure and the risk of dementia: A double edged sword. Aging Research Reviews, 8, 6170.Google Scholar
Lee, A.Y., Jeong, S.H., Choi, B.H., Sohn, E.H., & Chui, H. (2006). Pulse pressure correlates with leukoaraiosis in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 42, 157166.Google Scholar
Mickes, L., Wixted, J.T., Fennema-Notestine, C., Galasko, D., Bondi, M.W., Thal, L.J., et al. . (2007). Progressive impairment on neuropsychological tasks in a longitudinal study of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 21, 696705.Google Scholar
Mitchell, G.F. (2008). Effects of central arterial aging on the structure and function of the peripheral vasculature: Implications for end-organ damage. Journal of Applied Physiology, 105, 16521660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, M., Grobbee, D.E., Aleman, A., Bots, M., & van der Schouw, Y.T. (2007). Cardiovascular disease and cognitive performance in middle-aged and elderly men. Atherosclerosis, 190, 143149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakayama, Y., Hayashi, T., Yoshimaru, K., Tsumura, K., & Ueda, H. (2002). Low fractional diastolic pressure in the ascending aorta increased the risk of coronary heart disease. Journal of Human Hypertension, 16, 837841.Google Scholar
Nichols, W.W., & O’Rourke, M. (1998). McDonald’s blood flow in arteries: Theoretical, experimental and clinical principles (4th ed.). London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Qiu, C., Winblad, B., Viitanen, M., & Fratiglioni, L. (2003). Pulse pressure and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in persons aged 75 years and older: A community-based, longitudinal study. Stroke, 34, 594599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qiu, C., Winblad, B., & Fratiglioni, L. (2005). The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurology, 4, 487499.Google Scholar
Raz, N., Dahle, C.L., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M., & Land, S. (2009). Effects of age, genes, and pulse pressure on executive functions in healthy adults. Neurobiology of Aging, [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Safar, M.E., Levy, B.I., & Struijker-Boudier, H. (2003). Current perspectives on arterial stiffness and pulse pressure in hypertension and cardiovascular diseases. Circulation, 107, 28642869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1992). Neuropsychological assessment of dementia in the elderly. In Katzman, R. & Rowe, J.W. (Eds.), Principles of geriatric neurology. F.A. Davis Company, London distributors: Williams & Wilkins, Ltd., Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
Van Bortel, L.M., Struijker-Boudier, H.A., & Safar, M.E. (2001). Pulse pressure, arterial stiffness, and drug treatment of hypertension. Hypertension, 38, 914921.Google Scholar
Vlek, A.L., Visseren, F.L., Kappelle, L.J., Geerlings, M.I., Vincken, K.L., Mali, W.P., et al. ; SMART Study Group. (2009). Blood pressure and progression of cerebral atrophy in patients with vascular disease. American Journal of Hypertension, 22, 11831189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldstein, S.R., Rice, S.C., Thayer, J.F., Najjar, S.S., Scuteri, A., & Zonderman, A.B. (2008). Pulse pressure and pulse wave velocity are related to cognitive decline in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Hypertension, 51, 99104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wierenga, C.E., Benjamin, M., Gopinath, K., Perlstein, W.M., Leonard, C.M., Rothi, L.J., et al. . (2008). Age-related changes in word retrieval: Role of bilateral frontal and subcortical networks. Neurobiology of Aging, 29, 436451.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, B.D., Weisman, H.F., & Yin, F.C. (1989). Arterial hemodynamics in a rabbit model of atherosclerosis. American Journal of Physiology, 257, H891H897.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Correlations between hemodynamic and cognitive measures

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Scatterplots of age and pulse pressure (PP) by language ability are displayed. Age (+, dashed line) and PP (■, solid line) exhibited significant inverse correlations with language ability: r = −0.32, p < .01 and r = −0.36, p < .001, respectively.