Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T10:23:32.457Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can Light Contact with the Police Motivate Political Participation? Evidence from Traffic Stops

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 August 2022

Leah Christiani*
Affiliation:
University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996, USA
Kelsey Shoub
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: christiani@utk.edu

Abstract

Harsh, highly intrusive, personal contact with the criminal justice system has been shown to politically demobilize, but it is unclear whether less intrusive forms of police contact have any political effects. As the modal type of involuntary police–citizen contact is less invasive and more routine (e.g., a traffic stop), it is critical to understand the ramifications of lighter forms of contact. We argue that, unlike harsh police contact, light, personal, police contact can mobilize individuals, under certain circumstances. When a negative encounter with the police—even if it is minor—runs counter to prior expectations, people experiencing the contact are mobilized to take political action. Using 3 years of observational data and an original survey experiment, we demonstrate that individuals who receive tickets or are stopped by the police are more likely to participate in politics. These effects are most pronounced for individuals with positive evaluations of the police, often White respondents.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Race, Ethnicity, and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

Interactions with the police shape political participation. Recent work has uncovered the way that harsh, personal contact with the criminal justice system is often linked with demobilization, while indirect, proximal contact tends to mobilize political participation (for an overview, see White Reference White2022). While most work has centered on the effects of arrests or incarceration on political participation, the most common form of (involuntary) police contact comes from traffic and pedestrian stops, a much lighter form of contact: In 2015, almost half of all police-initiated contact with citizens could be attributed to routine traffic or pedestrian stops (Davis et al. Reference Davis, Whyde and Langton2018). “Light” police contact is characterized by ending in either no or a minor penalty, like a traffic ticket, minimally stripping individuals of resources, and requiring no or minimal follow-up contact with the criminal justice system. In contrast, being arrested or incarcerated comes with higher penalties, strips resources and freedoms (at least temporarily), and requires ongoing or significant follow-up contact with the criminal justice system.

While we know that harsh interactions with the police and carceral state are deeply political, less is known about how these light interactions may shape participation, despite their greater prevalence. Here, we specifically focus on nonvoting forms of political participation, as political participation can and often does take forms outside the voting booth (Owens and Walker Reference Owens and Walker2018) and less is known about the way contact with the carceral state shapes political participation outside voting (White Reference White2022). To better understand the political consequences of light police contact, we ask: Do routine encounters with the police, such as traffic stops, have consequences for nonvoting forms of political participation? Further, if there are effects, do they vary by the individuals’ perceptions of the police—or by race?

There are three possible answers to this question. First, light contact may produce similar, if more muted, responses by individuals as harsh contact—that is, those experiencing negative contact may be less likely to participate. After all, it is still involuntary contact intended to correct a behavior via punishment. Second, these encounters may be so minor that they do not affect political participation. They are less all-encompassing and less likely to result in a long-term stripping of resources than harsher forms of contact.

Lastly, and as we argue, light contact may increase the likelihood of political participation, especially for those with more positive perceptions of the police—often, White people. Considering that many people never directly interact with any form of government, even these milder interactions with law enforcement, an arm of government, could affect citizens’ willingness to participate politically. Additionally, unlike harsh contact, light contact typically does not substantially strip an individual of resources—thus allowing them a means to participate if they so choose. Further, this negative interaction may upset them in a way that mobilizes political participation. This is especially true for those who may have positive affect toward the police, as this experience runs counter to their prior evaluations. For those with more negative evaluations of the police or those viewing “light” contact as the product of a systematic pattern, light contact may not have any effect on the individual, as it only confirms what they already expect.

To test these expectations, we adopt two approaches. First, we turn to survey data to determine the generalizability of this pattern by analyzing the 2016 and 2018 Cooperative Election Studies (CES) and the 2016 American National Election Study (ANES) pilot study. Each asks about light contact with the police and political participation. We find that respondents who recently experienced light contact were more likely to engage in political action than those who did not.

To supplement this analysis, we fielded an original survey experiment that varies the level of light police contact that a respondent reads about experiencing and then measures their plans for future participation. We find that respondents stopped by the police report higher levels of intended participation. Further, we find that these effects are most pronounced among respondents who reported pretreatment favorable attitudes toward the police—and that they hold for White respondents, but not for Black respondents. In other words, those that are newly exposed to a more punitive side of law enforcement see a positive shift in projected participation—while those respondents who do not expect positive interactions are unfazed.

On the whole, these findings have important implications for our understanding of the political consequences of policing and, more broadly, the implications of government–citizen interactions. We show when and how light contact with the police in the United States meaningfully shapes political participation outside the realm of voting. Further, as the police are an arm of government, these findings have implications for our understanding of the ways in which citizen–government bureaucracies inform participation.

Police contact and political participation

Contact with the criminal justice system and the carceral state can vary widely: it can be personal (i.e., happen to the individual) or proximal (i.e., happen to a loved one) (Walker 2014; Reference Walker2019), and it can range in severity. The form and severity of contact inform the effects on political and civic participation. We first review the literature on harsh contact to develop a framework concerning light contact.

Harsh personal and proximal contact

Recent work on the link between harsh contact with the carceral state and political participation has exploded (for an overview, see: White Reference White2022). Most studies have focused on political participation as voting behavior, with the majority of studies finding descriptively that harsh contact with the criminal justice system is linked with decreased voting (Burch Reference Burch2011; Hjalmarsson and Lopez Reference Hjalmarsson and Lopez2010; Lerman and Weaver Reference Lerman and Weaver2014a; Owens and Walker Reference Owens and Walker2018; Uggen and Manza Reference Uggen and Manza2002; Weaver and Lerman Reference Weaver and Lerman2010; White Reference White2022) and is linked causally with declines in voting in the short term (White Reference White2019b), though others find that contact either has no causal effect on voting (Gerber et al. Reference Gerber, Huber, Meredith, Biggers and Hendry2017) or that there are mixed effects (Burch Reference Burch2011; Laniyonu Reference Laniyonu2019).

Less work has examined nonvoting forms of participation (White Reference White2022), despite the importance of understanding such participation, especially for subjugated groups (Owens and Walker Reference Owens and Walker2018; Soss and Weaver Reference Soss and Weaver2017). Weaver and Lerman (Reference Weaver and Lerman2010) and Lerman and Weaver (Reference Lerman and Weaver2014a) find that harsh, personal contact with the carceral state, such as arrests and incarceration, are linked with reductions in political participation beyond voting. Individuals experiencing harsh forms of contact such as arrest, convictions, or jail time are less likely to participate in political and civic life, and these demobilizing effects are exacerbated as the contact becomes more intrusive (Weaver and Lerman Reference Weaver and Lerman2010). This form of contact with the carceral state results in political alienation, mistrust, stigmatization, and a decline in social, political, and financial resources, which serve as additional barriers to participation (Frymer Reference Frymer2005; Gecas Reference Gecas1982; Lerman and Weaver Reference Lerman and Weaver2014a; Pager Reference Pager2003; Prowse et al. Reference Prowse, Weaver and Meares2020; Uggen et al. Reference Uggen, Manza and Thompson2006; Weaver and Lerman Reference Weaver and Lerman2010; Western Reference Western2006). However, Owens and Walker (Reference Owens and Walker2018) find that this connection can be interrupted when the individual with carceral state contact is part of civil society organizations. Further, Lerman and Weaver (Reference Lerman and Weaver2014b) find that police stops that feature searches or a high degree of force (harsh forms of contact) decrease neighborhood 311 calls (a form of civic engagement), while stops that do not have such features can increase such calls. Other work has found mixed evidence of the effect of policing on a willingness to call 911 (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Gunderson, Jackson, Zachary, Clark, Glynn and Owens2019; Desmond et al. Reference Desmond, Papachristos and Kirk2016; Zoorob Reference Zoorob2020).

Not only does harsh, personal contact with the police or carceral state shape political and civic participation, but so too does proximal contact. Proximal contact is contact with the police or carceral state that occurs to a loved one (e.g., a family member) (Walker 2014; Reference Walker2019). While personal, harsh contact leads to a direct stripping of resources and stigmatization, proximal contact may not. Nevertheless, watching a loved one experience such contact is a political learning moment (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Porter and Comfort2014). When people from minoritized groups see proximal contact as part of a larger system of oppression, they are mobilized to action (Walker 2014, 2019, Reference Walker2020). Individuals with proximal contact receive many of the same devaluing messages that those with direct contact do but lack many or all of the material constraints accompanying incarceration.

As such, proximal contact with the carceral state affects an individual’s willingness to participate in politics and the type of political engagement in which they take part. While proximal contact leads to (short term) declines in voter turnout (White Reference White2019a), it leads to increases in other forms of political participation (Anoll and Israel-Trummel Reference Anoll and Israel-Trummel2019; Walker 2014, Reference Walker2019). Further, when individuals with proximal contact view these encounters as a result of systematic discrimination (like racism), they can be politically mobilized in activities outside of voting (Gilmore Reference Gilmore2007; Miller Reference Miller2008; Walker 2014, Reference Walker2019). Walker (2014, 2019, Reference Walker2020) finds that non-Whites in particular can be motivated by proximal contact, as they know that their communities are targeted by police unjustly. Similarly, at the community level, Miller (Reference Miller2008) finds that poorer communities that experience crime (and policing) tend to vote at lower rates but participate more in ways that directly engage their local government. These patterns of nonvoting participation have been more generally documented: marginalized populations do not typically begin participating within the system that subjugates them, but instead, outside of it with protesting and other actions (Gillion Reference Gillion2013).

Light contact

We define “light contact” as interactions with the police that begin with a stop and end with a minor penalty, such as a citation. With this definition, light contact with the police shares elements of both personal and proximal contact. It is personal, in that it is occurring to the individual rather than to someone they know. However, because it is a minor interaction, it shares elements of proximal contact in that it does not strip the individual of significant resources: after receiving a traffic ticket, peoples’ lives are not necessarily upended, as they are when they are incarcerated. Even though police stops too often escalate and become violent or deadly, especially for Black people, our analysis centers on when they do not take this turn.

While much of the research on personal contact and political participation has understandably focused on harsher forms of contact like arrests, some studies incorporate lighter forms of contact in their analyses (Anoll et al. Reference Anoll, Epp and Israel-Trummel2022; Drakulich et al. Reference Drakulich, Hagan, Johnson and Wozniak2017; Walker Reference Walker2014; Weaver and Lerman Reference Weaver and Lerman2010). Weaver and Lerman (Reference Weaver and Lerman2010) find that being questioned by the police (which is the lightest form of contact measured) actually has a positive effect on other forms of political participation and a negative effect on trust in government. This finding runs counter to their theory, and they note that it could be due to a small sample size. Similarly, in a portion of her analysis concerning personal police contact, Walker (Reference Walker2014) uncovers an unexpected mobilizing effect for political participation outside the realm of voting. She suspects that this mobilizing effect could be due to the specific measure of contact that she employs—as it includes lighter contact like being questioned or stopped by the police. Most recently, Anoll et al. (Reference Anoll, Epp and Israel-Trummel2022) find a positive relationship between stop history and political participation, which they also characterize as surprising and counter to their expectations. It is possible that all three of these studies run into issues of measurement and sample size. However, it is also possible that these findings point to the notion that light contact is a substantively different, yet politically important, form of contact with the police. Perhaps at lower levels of contact, a negative interaction with the police, even when it is a personal interaction, can mobilize political participation. This is supported by Drakulich et al. (Reference Drakulich, Hagan, Johnson and Wozniak2017), who find that being stopped by the police can motivate political participation, as a consequence of a broader increase in political engagement, while experiencing incarceration demobilizes participation.

Less intrusive kinds of contact with the police, like receiving a traffic ticket, differ from harsher forms of contact. If we conceive of light contact as a brief, one-time, common interaction, it would likely neither result in the kind of deep political alienation that occurs after harsh contact with the police nor carry the social stigma that harsh contact does. Further, while light contact may have short-term effects on resources (e.g., a fine), it does not strip people of political, social, and financial resources in the same ways that harsh contact does. Ultimately, after receiving something like a traffic ticket, the individual is still free, can still vote, and often only has to pay a one-time fine.

However, contact with the police tends to be more memorable when it is negative (Skogan Reference Skogan2006), which is often the case (Brunson and Weitzer Reference Brunson and Weitzer2009; Voigt et al. Reference Voigt, Camp, Prabhakaran, Hamilton, Hetey, Griffiths, Jurgens, Jurafsky and Eberhardt2017; Weitzer and Tuch Reference Weitzer and Tuch2006; White et al. Reference White, Cox and Basehart1991). Skogan (Reference Skogan2006) finds that police interactions seen as fair often do not affect police evaluations, while those deemed unfair significantly depress evaluations of the police—which is in line with the procedural justice literature that finds the legitimacy of the police is heavily dependent on perceptions of fairness (Belvedere et al. Reference Belvedere, Worrall and Tibbetts2005; Justice and Meares Reference Justice and Meares2014; Meares Reference Meares2015, Reference Meares2016; Snow Reference Snow2019; Sunshine and Tyler Reference Sunshine and Tyler2003; Tyler Reference Tyler2006; Reference Tyler2011; Tyler and Fagan Reference Tyler and Fagan2008; Tyler and Huo Reference Tyler and Huo2002). During focus groups, Snow (Reference Snow2019) found that participants who recently received traffic tickets were unlikely to see their own behavior as problematic—and instead, very likely to see the behavior of the agency issuing the penalty as unjust. Even when participants recognized that they did break the law, it was often considered a “technicality” that did not undermine their law-abiding identity (Snow Reference Snow2019, pp.148–9). A minor, negative interaction with the police, then, may upset individuals without inducing deep alienation or a substantial reduction in resources—thus mobilizing individuals to political action. This leads to the first hypothesis:

Hypothesis 1: People who receive a traffic ticket will participate in more political action.

Sometimes “light” contact may not be seen as unobtrusive, though. When people see this light form of contact as connected to a broader system of oppression that marginalizes them and their racial group, it could produce or reinforce political alienation. Thus, an individual’s reaction to light contact with the police may depend on their previous contact with police, perceptions of the police, and/or expectations for such an encounter.

While encountering information that contradicts prior beliefs is often disregarded, rated as lower quality, or elicits motivated reasoning that results in reinforcing rather than updating prior beliefs (Lodge and Taber Reference Lodge and Taber2013; Lord et al. Reference Lord, Ross and Lepper1979; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006), there are some circumstances in which individuals can and do update their beliefs—and, more often, their behavior. Circumstances where individuals live through an encounter—or are forced imagine themselves living through an encounter—that runs counter to their expectations may be particularly influential. A key example of this is in reducing prejudice (for an overview, see Paluck et al. Reference Paluck, Porat, Clark and Green2020). In this context, people who have direct interactions with (or who are asked to imagine such interactions with) someone previously seen as “other” (e.g., someone of a different race) come to engage in less prejudiced behavior. In the face of repeated or particularly strong evidence, people can come to update their beliefs and behavior (Garrett Reference Garrett2017; Redlawsk et al. Reference Redlawsk, Civettini and Emmerson2010; Steffens et al. Reference Steffens, Weeks, Davidsson and Isaak2014).

In the context of policing, this means that experiencing an interaction with the police that runs counter to expectations will bring new, experiential information which could lead to behavioral shifts. For people who believe that the police serve and protect them, having a negative interaction, even if minor, may be particularly upsetting. Dissatisfaction from the mismatch between expectations and reality may then mobilize them to take political action. However, for people who do not expect positive interactions with the police, a minor (negative) interaction with the police may be either in line with their expectations or, at times, even a better outcome than they may have expected. Thus, these individuals will not be particularly upset or mobilized. This expectation leads to the next hypothesis:

Hypothesis 2: Individuals with positive perceptions of the police who receive a traffic ticket will participate in more political action than those with negative perceptions of the police.

As the police in the U.S. target Black and racially minoritized populations more than their White counterparts (Gelman et al. Reference Gelman, Fagan and Kiss2007; Mummolo Reference Mummolo2018), including during traffic stops (Baumgartner et al. Reference Baumgartner, Epp and Shoub2018; Christiani Reference Christiani2021; Fagan and Davies Reference Fagan and Davies2000; Fagan et al. Reference Fagan, Geller, Davies and West2010), public perceptions of the police understandably differ wildly by race (Hurwitz and Peffley Reference Hurwitz and Peffley2005; Jefferson et al. Reference Jefferson, Neuner and Pasek2020; Weitzer and Tuch Reference Weitzer and Tuch2004). White people tend to expect that the police will serve and protect, but Black people do not (Brunson Reference Brunson2007; Hurwitz and Peffley Reference Hurwitz and Peffley2005; Jones Reference Jones2014; Weitzer and Tuch Reference Weitzer and Tuch2005). Thus, the fact that these light interactions have political effects that depend on the individual’s perception of the police imply that they will tend to differ by race as well. White people, who tend to expect positive police interactions, may find even a minor negative police interaction bothersome, upsetting, or angering, as it runs counter to their expectations for such interactions (Suhay and Erisen Reference Suhay and Erisen2018) and consequently be mobilized. Black people, who do not have such hopeful expectations, may not feel as conflicted as Whites after experiencing a negative encounter with the police, as the experience does not run counter to their prior expectations. This leads to the final hypothesis:

Hypothesis 3: White people with positive perceptions of the police who receive a traffic ticket will participate in more political action than Black people.

Analytic strategy

To test these hypotheses, we proceed in three steps. First, we test whether people who receive a traffic ticket (a light form of police contact) participate in politics more often using three nationally representative surveys conducted in two different years (Study 1). While this provides generalizability for the first hypothesis, the design of these surveys is insufficient to test the sequencing implied by the second and third hypotheses, centering on evaluations prior to a police interaction. For this, we turn to a unique survey experiment, where respondents read a vignette in second person about a police interaction (Study 2). With this, we test whether light contact is mobilizing (hypothesis 1), whether prior perceptions of the police moderate this effect (hypothesis 2), and for heterogeneity by race (hypothesis 3).

Study 1: Evidence from the CCES and ANES

Few surveys ask respondents about their interactions with the police and the criminal justice system—especially minimally intrusive ones—alongside their civic participation. Three exceptions to this are: the 2016 and 2018 CCES, Footnote 1 which asks respondents whether they recently received a traffic ticket, and the 2016 ANES pilot, Footnote 2 which asks respondents whether they were stopped or questioned by the police. Footnote 3 Note that we follow Drakulich et al. (Reference Drakulich, Hagan, Johnson and Wozniak2017) in using the 2016 ANES pilot, but we expand the types of political participation that we analyze beyond voting. Using these surveys, we can investigate whether there is a statistical link between light contact with the police and political participation in the general population.

The key independent variables are whether a respondent experienced a small, negative interaction with the law. In the CCES, this is measured as receiving a traffic ticket. In both 2016 and 2018, the CCES asked respondents whether they had recently received a traffic ticket or citation. Footnote 4 In the 2016 ANES pilot, half of the respondents were asked whether they had been stopped or questioned by the police in the past 12 months. While there are key differences in these questions—the ANES specifies an individual interacted with an officer and spans a broader range of outcomes—each captures whether individuals had some kind of light police contact.

To measure political and civic participation, we examine whether respondents participated in a range of activities. In the CCES, respondents are asked whether they participated in these activities in the preceding year, which is in line with previous studies (e.g., Walker 2014; Reference Walker2019). Footnote 5 Both the 2016 and 2018 CCES asked whether respondents: (1) attended a local political meeting, (2) put up a political sign, (3) worked for a candidate or campaign, or (4) donated money to a candidate or campaign or political organization. Footnote 6 From these responses, we construct a measure of political participation by counting how many activities the respondent had engaged in: in 2016, there is a mean response of .57, while in 2018, there is a mean response of .60. In the 2016 ANES pilot, respondents were asked about whether they participated in a range of activities over varying timelines, including past and future (planned) participation. Because the independent variable asks about whether respondents were stopped or questioned by the police in the past 12 months, and we expect that police contact precedes political participation, we take advantage of the fact that the pilot asks about plans for future participation. Respondents are asked whether they plan to (1) attend a meeting to talk about political or social concerns, (2) give money to an organization, or (3) distribute information related to a political/social interest group. We combine these items into a scale of political participation by counting the number of activities a respondent is “very likely” or “extremely likely” to participate in, which aligns with our CCES measures.

Analysis

Using these data, we specify models to estimate (1) whether the individual engaged in any action using a logistic regression, and (2) the number of activities in which an individual participated using a Poisson regression. In the CCES models, the data from both surveys is pooled, and a fixed effect for year is included. Table 1 presents the results of these models with key variables; the full models with controls are presented in the appendix.

Table 1. Explaining political and civic participation

$^*p \lt 0.05$ . Controls include race, gender, education level, income, party affiliation, political interest, religiosity, and age.

Across all models, there is a positive and statistically significant link between light police contact and political participation. Respondents who received traffic tickets are also more likely to have engaged in activities in the past 12 months (CCES models) and respondents who were stopped or questioned by the police are more likely to engage in activities in the future (ANES models). This relationship holds when the models are disaggregated by year (for the CCES), when individual forms of participation are examined separately, and when the dependent variables for the ANES are left as scales—all analyses are reported in the online appendix.

Additionally, we estimate and examine models with an interaction between light contact and race of the respondent, which are presented in the appendix. On the whole, they do not provide evidence for the hypothesis that light contact mobilizes White people more than Black people, as the interaction term is statistically insignificant. As we theorize that it is the intersection of prior positive evaluations, race, and contact that may produce heterogeneous differences, this does not directly contradict our hypotheses, but it does raise interesting questions for future studies.

These tests reveal a statistical link between light police contact and political mobilization across multiple years. However, there remain questions as to whether this relationship is causal and about the potential mechanism producing this effect. The ANES analysis gets closer to modeling the directional nature of the first hypothesis, as police contact is measured within the last year but political participation is measured as plans for the future, but the CCES measures these simultaneously. Further, these data do not allow for a test of the causal mechanism, as prior evaluations of the police would need to be measured before interaction with the police. To examine these questions, we turn to an original survey experiment.

Study 2: Experimental evidence

To better unpack the relationship between light police contact and participation, we fielded a survey experiment in June 2020 with the survey firm Lucid. Lucid is an online survey aggregator, and its samples have been demonstrated to track well with U.S. national benchmarks and suitable for experimental research (Coppock and McClellan Reference Coppock and McClellan2019). Respondents answered pretreatment items, then read a short vignette, and finally answered dependent variables about their plans to participate. The experiment randomly assigned respondents to one of three conditions. They were told: “Now, we’re going to ask you to participate in an exercise. Imagine you’re experiencing the following scenario. Then, let us know what you think about what happened to you.” The conditions were as follows:

  • Control Condition: You are driving to the grocery store. You look down and notice that you are almost out of gas. The light for “empty” just came on. So, you decide to stop at the gas station. When you get there, you get out of the car and pay to fill up your gas tank. Several minutes pass as you pump the gas. Eventually, when your tank is full, you get your receipt and get back into your car. You turn your car on and notice that the “empty” light is off and now your gas tank is full. You continue driving to the store, where you find most of the things on your list. You check out and head home.

  • Ticket Treatment: You are driving to the grocery store. Behind you, you see flashing red and blue lights. A police officer is pulling you over, so you pull to the side of the road. The police officer in uniform comes up to your car window and asks for your driver’s license and vehicle registration without telling you anything else. You give these to him, and then he returns to his vehicle. Several minutes pass as you wait in your car. Eventually, the officer approaches your car again. He tells you that you were driving 10 miles over the speed limit. Then, he returns your license and registration—and he writes you a $150 ticket for speeding. You continue driving to the store, where you find most of the things on your list. You check out and head home.

  • Search Treatment: You are driving to the grocery store. Behind you, you see flashing red and blue lights. A police officer is pulling you over, so you pull to the side of the road. The police officer in uniform comes up to your car window and asks for your driver’s license and vehicle registration without telling you anything else. You give these to him, and then he returns to his vehicle. Several minutes pass as you wait in your car. Eventually, the officer approaches your car again. He asks you to step out of the car and you agree, getting out of your vehicle. While you are waiting on the side of the road, the officer searches your car—the interior and the trunk. He does not find anything in his search. He tells you that you were driving 10 miles over the speed limit. Then, he returns your license and registration—and he writes you a $150 ticket for speeding. You continue driving to the store, where you find most of the things on your list. You check out and head home.

Both treatments are considered light contact by our technical definition, as the interaction with the police officer ends with a ticket. However, it is clear that the search condition is considerably harsher than the ticket condition. In the search condition, the individual is subject to a search (which could be construed as illegal and/or unfair since we do not specify that the driver gave their consent). Such a procedure is invasive and, especially if it is part of a pattern of police targeting based on the individual’s identity, is likely to be a traumatic experience. Nevertheless, when compared with previous research, this condition is not as harsh as being arrested, spending time in jail, being convicted, or being incarcerated—as a search ending in a ticket does not significantly strip individuals of resources, convey a criminal record, or carry the same level of stigma as harsher forms of contact. So, when compared to types of contact with the carceral state that have often been previously examined, it is not quite as harsh, while we recognize that it may not be uniformly “light,” especially for Black people and others whose communities experience regular police targeting.

Thus, we have three conditions. The ticket condition contains the lightest amount of police contact. The search condition contains harsher contact than the ticket condition, but lighter contact than previous work has typically examined, and a control condition, which mirrors the treatment closely, as the respondent still stops on their way to the store (i.e., an unplanned interruption) but does not include any police contact. Note that both treatments are relatively gentle: they do not use emotional language, the interaction itself is basic and quick, and the officer is not explicitly rude. As a result, detecting a statistically significant relationship may be difficult.

Before the vignette, respondents relayed information on how warmly (or coldly) they felt toward various groups, institutions, and professions, including the police, which allows us to capture the respondent’s general affect toward the police and test whether it conditions the treatment effects. Their rating of the police was randomized among 14 total groups to limit the extent to which respondents were primed to think about the police prior to the experiment. We rescaled this thermometer to run from 0 (very cold) to 1 (very warm). In the sample, the median rating for the police was .61 with a standard deviation of .32.

Following the experimental vignette, respondents indicated how likely (or unlikely) they are to participate in a range of political activities in the future—in other words, their intention to participate, which is part of the behavioral chain (Bagozzi et al. Reference Bagozzi, Baumgartner and Yi1989; Kim and Hunter Reference Kim and Hunter1993a, Reference Kim and Hunter1993b; Randall and Wolff Reference Randall and Wolff1994). We count the number of political activities the respondent says they are likely or very likely to do, following Walker (2014, Reference Walker2019). To make this study comparable to the CCES analysis, we focus on the same four activities: sign a petition online, donate money, attend a (virtual) town hall or meeting, and work for a candidate or campaign. Footnote 7 Five hundred and fifty-three respondents (about 56%) said that they intended to participate in at least one activity.

At its close, 1,058 participants fully completed the survey. Of those, 984 participants completed the survey within a reasonable time span and provided quality responses. Footnote 8 Because Black people are treated more harshly by the police than White people on average, we designed the sample to be approximately evenly split between Black and White respondents (441 and 543, respectively) and—within each group—to match national distributions on age and gender. The full set of descriptive statistics can be found in the appendix.

Analysis

We again specify two models: (1) a logistic regression predicting any form of intended participation and (2) a Poisson regression predicting the number of intended participatory activities. These models are specified for the whole sample and by the respondent’s evaluation of the police. Respondents with evaluations of the police of .5 (on the 0–1 thermometer scale) or lower are coded as having negative views, while those with evaluations of .51 or higher are coded as rating the police positively. The results are reported in Table 2.

Table 2. Average treatment effects on intended political participation

$^*p \lt 0.05$ . Activities include sign a petition, work for candidate, donate to a campaign, and attend a meeting.

Models 1 and 4 in Table 2 report the effects of the treatment on intended participation in the entire sample. Those exposed to the search condition were more likely to report planning to participate ( $\beta $ = .33 and $\beta $ = .20, respectively). Model 4 shows that people in the ticket condition have higher levels of participation as well ( $\beta $ = .15), lending support for the first hypothesis. The ticket condition is not statistically significant in the logistic regression predicting whether respondents will participate, though the coefficient is in the expected direction.

The other models split the sample by pretreatment evaluations of the police. Respondents with positive prior evaluations of the police are clearly driving the overall effects detected in Models 1 and 4. In the logistic regression, the coefficient for the search condition is larger in magnitude for those who had positive evaluations of the police ( $\beta $ = .47) than in the overall models, while the relationship is statistically insignificant for those with negative prior evaluations of the police. In the Poisson regressions, the effects are again isolated to those respondents with positive prior evaluations of the police, and, again, the effect for those with positive prior evaluations is larger than the effect detected in the overall sample ( $\beta $ = .35 for the search condition and $\beta $ = .26 for the ticket condition), which is evidence for the second hypothesis.

The findings from the Lucid sample corroborate those detected in the CCES and ANES samples. Further, they provide support for our second hypothesis that those who have positive evaluations of the police likely do not expect negative interactions, like those with more negative evaluations do. This disconnect from their prior evaluations appears to be one mechanism by which light contact mobilizes political participation for some.

The third hypothesis posed that prior evaluations of the police will have stronger effects on motivating political participation among White respondents, compared with Black respondents—as we expect that White and Black respondents have different prior experiences with and expectations for the police. Before testing for heterogeneity in treatment effects, we descriptively explore differences in attitudes about the police, rates of police contact, and concerns following the hypothetical police interaction by respondent race. Figure 1(a) plots the distribution of feeling thermometer scores toward the police among White and Black respondents. On average, White respondents rate the police much more warmly (mean = .66) than Black respondents (mean = .45), but there is significant within-race variation, especially among Black respondents, in evaluations of the police.

Figure 1. Differences in attitudes about the police, rates of police contact, and concerns during a (hypothetical) police interaction by respondent race

Note: Subfigure a is a density plot of responses to the police feeling thermometer by respondent race. Subfigure b shows the proportion White and Black respondents that have ever had direct contact with the police or prison system by the most intensive form of contact reported. Maximum contact is defined as being searched, arrested, or incarcerated. Minimal contact is defined as being pulled over (but not ticketed) or questioned by the police. Subgfiure c shows the proportion of White and Black respondents from either treatment group that said they were “concerned about during [their] interaction with the officer.”

Further, there are differences in actual police contact and concerns about the police interaction from the vignette (Figures 1(b) and (c) respectively). Prior research shows that Black people tend to have more negative experiences with the police than White people—and this is evident in the Lucid survey as well. We asked respondents if they had ever had direct contact with the police or prison system, and about their most recent form of contact. Using information from both responses, we calculate the proportion of respondents who came into contact with the police in the last year conditioned by their most intense form of police contact had, shown in Figure 1(b). White respondents only report greater contact with respect to ticketing done by police officers, while Black respondents report greater rates of police contact of all other forms, such as being arrested (i.e., maximally intensive contact)—corroborating previous findings (Baumgartner et al. Reference Baumgartner, Epp and Shoub2018; Epp et al. Reference Epp, Maynard-Moody and Haider-Markel2014). This shows that Black respondents in our sample have had more negative past experiences with the police.

Finally, we have suggested that White and Black people hold drastically different expectations for interactions with the police. To examine this, we asked respondents what they were concerned about during their interaction with the officer, if anything: contracting COVID-19 (the corona virus); receiving a ticket; experiencing violence from the police officer; getting yelled at or belittled by the police officer; or nothing. Figure 1(c) plots the differences in proportion, by race, for each concern. Again, we observe expected differences in respondent concerns: White respondents were more concerned about receiving a ticket, while Black respondents were concerned about much more dire outcomes—getting yelled at, belittled, or experiencing violence. These descriptive patterns match prior research and the what we have contended in this article: White and Black people have vastly different outlooks on and experiences with the police.

To formally test the third hypothesis, we interact the treatment condition indicators with feelings about the police and fit separate regressions by respondent race. As the key relationship of interest is an interaction, we present our findings as a series of figures rather than as a regression table (Figure 2; see the online appendix for the regression tables). We plot the marginal effect of the treatment, across the full range of pretreatment feeling thermometer scores toward the police, on the top row (Figure 2(a), (b), and (c)). The bottom row plots the predicted number of activities a respondent planned to engage in, by treatment condition and police feeling thermometer (Figure 2(d), (e), and (f)).

Figure 2. Examining the moderation of treatment effects by warmth of feelings toward the police and by race

Note: Results in each subfigure come from three Poisson regression models, each explaining the number of activities a respondent intends to engage in in the coming year and includes an interaction between a feelings toward the police (0—cool to 1—warm) and treatment. The first model includes all respondents, the second includes White respondents only, and the third includes Black respondents only. These regressions are shown in the appendix. Subfigures a, b, and c show the marginal effect of the given treatment (compared with the control condition), with 95% confidence intervals represented in gray, on the number of activities. Subfigures d, e, and f show the predicted number of activities on average based on the treatment or control condition and across the range of the police feeling thermometer.

Figure 2 shows that, while feelings about the police moderate the effect of light police contact, this relationship is actually contained to White respondents—lending evidence for the third hypothesis. When decomposed by race, this effect is not statistically significantly different from zero at the 95% confidence level for Black respondents. For White respondents, prior positive affect toward the police sees light contact inducing intention to participate.

Further, subfigures 2(e) and (f) show that the treatment reverses the relationship between police affect and political participation for White—but not Black—respondents. For White respondents in the control condition, more positive feelings toward the police are linked with less political participation. When these White respondents hypothetically experience light, negative contact from the police, positive feelings about the police are linked to a modest but detectable increase in political participation. For Black respondents, affect toward the police is always positively linked with participation, regardless of the treatment condition.

One concern with this experiment—especially with respect to reactions by White respondents—is that the timing of the survey may have biased our results, as it was fielded in June 2020, during the racial justice protests in response to the police killing of George Floyd. To this end, we test whether attitudes about other prominent figures (i.e., then-President Donald Trump), societal groups (i.e., Black Americans), or the George Floyd protests moderate the observed relationship, as one might suspect that feelings toward the police are simply part of a broader change in political attitudes rather than specific attitudes truly about the police. We find no evidence for the effect of these moderators, which indicates that we are detecting something distinct (see online appendix for analysis).

Further, we consider whether if one interacts with a group that they have a positive affinity toward, here the police, which are an arm of government, then they are primed to think better of or produce a positive affect toward the government more broadly. In turn, this may motivate greater participation—especially during a period where they may feel that an institution or group they approve of is under attack as they may have during the period this survey was fielded. This stands in contrast to our proposed pathway linking light contact with political participation as a result of becoming upset (but not entirely alienated). We find no evidence that the treatment affected attitudes about trust in government more broadly. All additional analyses are reported in the appendix. Together, these results provide additional evidence that our main results are linked to feelings about the police rather than motivated by key pieces of the surrounding context (e.g., BLM, Trump, etc.), and that our experiment did not appear to activate a broader rallying effect for government.

In sum, perceptions of the police can moderate the effect of light contact on political participation—especially, for White respondents. White people do not experience police targeting based on their race and have more positive affect toward the police. Usually, this leads to less participation—but in the face of light, negative, police encounters, it can mobilize action.

Discussion

Light contact is different from harsh personal contact as it does not significantly strip the individual of social or political resources. However, that does not mean that light contact with the police does not have important implications for political behavior outside the realm of voting. Instead, these light encounters can have effects on nonvoting forms of participation. The way that those effects are born out, especially by race, illuminates the role and expectations that different groups of people have for the police.

From three sets of data, we see evidence that light contact with the police does have political consequences. Analyses of observational survey data form the CCES (2016 and 2018) and ANES (2016) showed that light contact with the police is linked with increased political participation. A survey experiment replicated these findings and demonstrated one mechanism at work: prior evaluations of the police. Reading about receiving a ticket has mobilizing effects on intended participation, especially for those who previously felt warmly toward the police. It also demonstrated that there was no equivalent effect for respondents with a more negative affect toward police. These effects were most pronounced for White respondents than Black respondents. Finally, it demonstrated that the form of contact mattered in that the search condition tended to produce stronger increases in participation than the ticket condition. This could be due to the nature of the experimental context, where the search vignette was likely stronger in grabbing respondents’ attention and producing differences in their planned behavior—but it may point to participatory differences that emerge more strongly for fruitless searches than for tickets. Future work should continue to examine these differences.

While this analysis uncovered one mechanism driving these effects, more work could be done in the future to unpack other potential drivers of this relationship. For example, it could also be that when people feel that their government will be responsive to them, they are more likely to participate following a negative encounter with one arm of government (law enforcement). This mechanism is not divorced from the one we test here, but this kind of political efficacy may be an alternate route through which light police contact can also mobilize action.

Our findings have implications for a number of literatures. First, this study contributes to the growing literature on policing and politics in the United States. Previously, this work has often focused on the harshest forms of contact—for understandable reasons, as these forms of contact are the most troubling and influential (Anoll and Israel-Trummel Reference Anoll and Israel-Trummel2019; Lerman and Weaver Reference Lerman and Weaver2014a; Walker 2014, Reference Walker2019; Weaver and Lerman Reference Weaver and Lerman2010; White Reference White2019a). Here, we look at lighter, more common experiences to explore the way that everyday interactions with the police may affect individuals. In doing so, we speak to the political consequences of everyday interactions and uncover the ways that such effects depend on prior evaluations of the police. However, these results also raise a number questions for this space, such as at what point and under what conditions does contact shift from mobilizing to demobilizing (i.e., what is the “tipping point”?), and how different forms of contact relate to each other and participation. That is, it could be that there are different consequences on participation that result from contact depending on the entity (e.g., police, court system, type of court, etc.).

Similarly, as the police are one type of bureaucracy that regularly interacts with the public, these findings contribute to our broader understanding of how interaction with government shapes future political and civic participation. Individuals take lessons from their interactions with the government. As most people do not have regular contact with the federal government, they learn about government more broadly from the local functions, policies, and agencies that they do interact with (Lawless and Fox Reference Lawless and Fox2001; Mettler et al. Reference Mettler2005; Schneider and Ingram Reference Schneider and Ingram1997; Schneider and Ingram Reference Schneider and Ingram1993; Soss Reference Soss1999a; Watson Reference Watson2015). Certain policies send the message to citizens that they are deserving and that their voice is valued, while others reinforce marginality (Mettler et al. Reference Mettler2005; Schneider and Ingram Reference Schneider and Ingram1997; Schneider and Ingram Reference Schneider and Ingram1993; Soss Reference Soss1999a, Reference Soss1999b)—and such messages have implications for political efficacy and participation (Mettler and Stonecash Reference Mettler and Stonecash2008; Meyer Reference Meyer1996; Orloff Reference Orloff1993; Skocpol Reference Skocpol1991; Watson Reference Watson2015). While this study clarifies one way in which bureaucratic contact can mobilize participation, it raises new questions for this space, such whether the entity initiating contact (i.e., the citizen or bureaucrat) alters the relationship between contact and participation and engagement.

Finally, this piece contributes to the robust literature on race, ethnicity, and politics by adding to our understanding of how racial hierarchies and individuals’ own positionalities within that hierarchy shapes perceptions, interactions, and participation in fundamental ways (e.g., Bonilla-Silva Reference Bonilla-Silva1997; Crenshaw Reference Crenshaw1989; Kim Reference Kim2000; Omi and Winant Reference Omi and Winant2014). Interactions with the police do not occur in a vacuum, divorced from these forces (Epp et al. Reference Epp, Maynard-Moody and Haider-Markel2014; Lerman and Weaver Reference Lerman and Weaver2014a; Weaver and Lerman Reference Weaver and Lerman2010). Instead, the social construction of race, racialized experiences, power, and privilege shape every interaction. These findings dovetail with recent work that has illustrated, in part, the way that anger can mobilize individuals in privileged positions to a larger degree than those in marginalized groups (Phoenix Reference Phoenix2019; Phoenix and Arora Reference Phoenix and Arora2018), and underscores the need for additional work to be done to understand what else may moderate the connection between contact and participation, such as one’s perceptions of their own law abidance and their previous contacts with the criminal justice system.

Routine but involuntary interactions with the police occur daily with about one-fifth of the U.S. population over the age of 16 having received at least 1 traffic ticket in the last 5 years. Footnote 9 Understanding the political implications of these common forms of interaction with the government is crucial. As a final note: while traffic stops are often routine and relatively unobtrusive interactions, we also must acknowledge and hold space for when minor stops turn deadly. Sandra Bland, Philando Castile, and Walter Scott are three prominent examples of Black people who died as a result of police interactions that began with a minor traffic stop. While these encounters are often routine and unobtrusive, there are times, for Black people, when they are not.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2022.18

Footnotes

The authors contributed equally. The authors would like to thank Dave Attewell, Frank Baumgartner, Lucy Britt, Derek Epp, Eroll Kuhn, Andreas Jozwiak, Tim Ryan, Katelyn Stauffer, Emily Wager, Hannah Walker, and the reviewers and editor for their thoughtful feedback on this paper.

1 The CCES is fielded by YouGov; see Ansolabehere, Schaffner, and Luks (2017, 2019).

2 The American National Election Studies is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under grant numbers SES 1444721, 2014-2017, the University of Michigan, and Stanford University.

3 In the full 2016 ANES (i.e., not the pilot), respondents were asked whether they “or a family member” were stopped or questioned by the police. The inclusion of family members in this question means that it jointly measures personal and proximal contact. As such, we restrict our analysis to the pilot.

4 The question wording in each year differs slightly. In 2016, the question reads: “Over the past FOUR YEARS, have you been issued a traffic ticket?”(8,182 of 64,600 respondents had, 12.67%). In 2018, the question reads: “Over the past year, have you been issued a traffic ticket?” (3,698 of 60,000 respondents had, 6.16%)

5 In both 2016 and 2018, the question stem reads: “During the past year did you…”

6 The 2018—but not 2016—CCES additionally asked whether respondents: (1) attended a political protest or march or demonstration or (2) contacted a public official. To make the results comparable, we only use those activities asked about in both surveys.

7 Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, the stem of these questions was: “If it is safe to do so in the coming weeks, how likely are you to…?” and many of the questions involve virtual participation.

8 We define an appropriate time span as one-third of the mean of the middle 50% of respondents (2.88 min) or three times that number (25.96 min). A non-quality response is one where on all groups or entities are rated exactly the same.

9 Princeton Survey Research Associates International, 2015. Retrieved from news report: https://www.insurancequotes.com/auto/traffic-tickets-insurance-rate-increase

References

Anoll, AP, Epp, DA and Israel-Trummel, M (2022) Contact and context: How municipal traffic stops shape citizen character. The Journal of Politics 84, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anoll, A and Israel-Trummel, M (2019) Do felony disenfranchisement laws (de) mobilize? A case of surrogate participation. The Journal of Politics 81, 15231527.10.1086/704783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagozzi, RP, Baumgartner, J and Yi, Y (1989) An investigation into the role of intentions as mediators of the attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Economic Psychology 10, 3562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, FR, Epp, DA and Shoub, K (2018) Suspect Citizens: What 20 Million Traffic Stops Tell Us About Policing & Race. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108553599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belvedere, K,Worrall, JL and Tibbetts, SG (2005) Explaining suspect resistance in police-citizen encounters. Criminal Justice Review 30, 3044.10.1177/0734016805275675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonilla-Silva, E (1997) Rethinking racism: Toward a structural interpretation. American Sociological Review, 62, 465480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunson, RK (2007) Police don’t like black people”: African-American young men’s accumulated police experiences. Criminology & Public Policy 6, 71101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunson, RK and Weitzer, R (2009) Police relations with black and white youths in different urban neighborhoods. Urban Affairs Review 44, 858885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burch, T (2011) Turnout & party registration among criminal offenders in the 2008 general election. Law & Society Review 45, 699730.10.1111/j.1540-5893.2011.00448.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christiani, L (2021) Intersectional stereotyping in policing: An analysis of traffic stop outcomes. Politics, Groups, and Identities 9, 893915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, E, Gunderson, A, Jackson, K, Zachary, P, Clark, TS, Glynn, AN and Owens, ML (2019) Do officer-involved shootings reduce citizen contact with government? The Journal of Politics 81, 11111123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, A and McClellan, OA (2019) Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents. Research & Politics 6, 2053168018822174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crenshaw, K (1989) Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. University of Chicago Legal Forum 1989, 139.Google Scholar
Davis, E, Whyde, A and Langton, L (2018) Contacts between Police & The Public, 2015. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Desmond, M, Papachristos, AV and Kirk, DS (2016) Police violence and citizen crime reporting in the black community. American Sociological Review 81, 857876.10.1177/0003122416663494CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drakulich, K, Hagan, J, Johnson, D and Wozniak, KH (2017) Race, justice, policing, and the 2016 American presidential election. Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race 14, 733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epp, CR, Maynard-Moody, S and Haider-Markel, DP (2014) Pulled Over: How Police Stops Define Race & Citizenship. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagan, J and Davies, G (2000) Street stops & broken windows: Terry, race, & disorder in New York City. Fordham Urban Law Journal, 28, 457.Google Scholar
Fagan, J, Geller, A, Davies, G and West, V (2010) Street stops and broken windows revisited. Race, Ethnicity, and Policing, 309348.Google Scholar
Frymer, P (2005) Racism revised: Courts, labor law, and the institutional construction of racial animus. American Political Science Review 99, 373387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, RK (2017) Strategies for countering false information and beliefs about climate change. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gecas, V (1982) The self-concept. Annual Review of Sociology 8, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A, Fagan, J and Kiss, A (2007) An analysis of the New York City police department’s “stop-and-frisk” policy in the context of claims of racial bias. Journal of the American Statistical Association 102, 813823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, AS, Huber, GA, Meredith, M, Biggers, DR and Hendry, DJ (2017) Does incarceration reduce voting? Evidence about the political consequences of spending time in prison. The Journal of Politics 79, 11301146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillion, DQ (2013) The Political Power of Protest: Minority Activism and Shifts in Public Policy. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139381277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, RW (2007) Golden Gulag: Prisons, Surplus, Crisis, and Opposition in Globalizing California. Vol. 21. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hjalmarsson, R and Lopez, M (2010) The voting behavior of young disenfranchised felons: Would they vote if they could? American Law and Economics Review 12, 356393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurwitz, J and Peffley, M (2005) Explaining the great racial divide: Perceptions of fairness in the US criminal justice system. The Journal of Politics 67, 762783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jefferson, H, Neuner, FG and Pasek, J (2020) Seeing blue in black and white: Race and perceptions of officer-involved shootings. Perspectives on Politics 19, 11651183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, N (2014) “The regular routine”: Proactive policing and adolescent development among young, poor black men. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 2014, 3354.10.1002/cad.20053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Justice, B and Meares, TL (2014) How the criminal justice system educates citizens. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 651, 159177.10.1177/0002716213502929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, CJ (2000) Bitter Fruit: The Politics of Black-Korean Conflict in New York City. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, M-S and Hunter, JE (1993a). Attitude-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of attitudinal relevance and topic. Journal of Communication 43, 101142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, M-S and Hunter, JE (1993b). Relationships among attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior: A meta-analysis of past research, part 2. Communication Research 20, 331364.10.1177/009365093020003001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laniyonu, A (2019) The political consequences of policing: Evidence from New York City. Political Behavior 41, 527558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawless, JL and Fox, RL (2001) Political participation of the urban poor. Social Problems 48, 362385.10.1525/sp.2001.48.3.362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, H, Porter, LC and Comfort, M (2014) Consequences of family member incarceration: Impacts on civic participation and perceptions of the legitimacy and fairness of government. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 651, 4473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerman, A and Weaver, V (2014a). Arresting Citizenship: The Democratic Consequences of American Crime Control. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerman, A and Weaver, V (2014b). Staying out of sight? Concentrated policing and local political action. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 651, 202219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lodge, M and Taber, CS (2013) The Rationalizing Voter. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, CG, Ross, L and Lepper, MR (1979) Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37, 2098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meares, T (2015) Broken windows, neighborhoods, and the legitimacy of law enforcement or why i fell in and out of love with Zimbardo. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 52, 609625.10.1177/0022427815583911CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meares, T (2016) Policing and procedural justice: Shaping citizens’ identities to increase democratic participation. Northwestern University Law Review 111, 1525.Google Scholar
Mettler, S et al. (2005) Soldiers to Citizens: The Gi Bill and the Making of the Greatest Generation. Oxford University Press on Demand.Google Scholar
Mettler, S and Stonecash, JM (2008) Government program usage and political voice. Social Science Quarterly 89, 273293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, MH (1996) Making claims as workers or wives: The distribution of social security benefits. American Sociological Review, 449465.10.2307/2096358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, LL (2008) The Perils of Federalism: Race, Poverty, & The Politics of Crime Control. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mummolo, J (2018) Militarization fails to enhance police safety or reduce crime but may harm police reputation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 91819186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omi, M and Winant, H (2014) Racial Formation in the United States. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orloff, AS, 1993. Gender and the social rights of citizenship: The comparative analysis of gender relations and welfare states. American Sociological Review, 303328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, ML and Walker, HL (2018) The civic voluntarism of “custodial citizens”: Involuntary criminal justice contact, associational life, and political participation. Perspectives on Politics 16, 9901013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pager, D (2003) The mark of a criminal record. American Journal of Sociology 108, 937975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paluck, EL, Porat, R, Clark, CS and Green, DP (2020) Prejudice reduction: Progress and challenges. Annual Review of Psychology 72.Google ScholarPubMed
Phoenix, DL (2019) The Anger Gap: How Race Shapes Emotion in Politics. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108641906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phoenix, DL and Arora, M (2018) From emotion to action among Asian Americans: Assessing the roles of threat and identity in the age of trump. Politics, Groups, and Identities 6, 357372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prowse, G, Weaver, V and Meares, TL (2020) The state from below: Distorted responsiveness in policed communities. Urban Affairs Review 56, 14231471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randall, DM and Wolff, JA (1994) The time interval in the intention-behaviour relationship: meta-analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology 33, 405418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlawsk, DP, Civettini, AJ and Emmerson, KM (2010) The affective tipping point: Do motivated reasoners ever “get it”? Political Psychology 31, 563593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, AL and Ingram, HM (1997) Policy Design for Democracy. University Press of Kansas.Google Scholar
Schneider, A and Ingram, H (1993) Social construction of target populations: Implications for politics and policy. American Political Science Review 87, 334347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skocpol, T (1991) Targeting within universalism: Politically viable policies to combat poverty in the United States. The Urban Underclass 411, 437459.Google Scholar
Skogan, WG (2006) Asymmetry in the impact of encounters with police. Policing & Society 16, 99126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, A (2019) Receiving an on the spot penalty: A tale of morality, common sense and law-abidance. Criminology & Criminal Justice 19, 141159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soss, J (1999a). Lessons of welfare: Policy design, political learning, and political action. American Political Science Review, 363380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soss, J (1999b). Spoiled identity and collective action: Political consequences of welfare stigma. Presentation at the Annual Conference of the International Society for Political Psychology. Amsterdam, The Netherlands. July.Google Scholar
Soss, J and Weaver, V (2017) Police are our government: Politics, political science, and the policing of race–class subjugated communities. Annual Review of Political Science 20, 565591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffens, PR, Weeks, CS, Davidsson, P and Isaak, L (2014) Shouting from the ivory tower: A marketing approach to improve communication of academic research to entrepreneurs. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice 38, 399426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suhay, E and Erisen, C (2018) The role of anger in the biased assimilation of political information. Political Psychology 39, 793810.10.1111/pops.12463CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunshine, J and Tyler, TR (2003) The role of procedural justice and legitimacy in shaping public support for policing. Law & Society Review 37, 513548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taber, CS and Lodge, M (2006) Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science 50, 755769.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, TR (2006) Why People Obey the Law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, TR (2011) Trust and legitimacy: Policing in the USA and Europe. European Journal of Criminology 8, 254266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, TR and Fagan, J (2008) Legitimacy and cooperation: Why do people help the police fight crime in their communities. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law 6, 231.Google Scholar
Tyler, TR and Huo, YJ (2002) Trust in the Law: Encouraging Public Cooperation with the Police and Courts. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Uggen, C and Manza, J (2002) Democratic contraction? Political consequences of felon disenfranchisement in the United States. American Sociological Review, 777803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uggen, C, Manza, J and Thompson, M (2006) Citizenship, democracy, and the civic reintegration of criminal offenders. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 605, 281310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voigt, R, Camp, NP, Prabhakaran, V, Hamilton, WL, Hetey, RC, Griffiths, CM, Jurgens, D, Jurafsky, D and Eberhardt, JL (2017) Language from police body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, 65216526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, HL (2014) Extending the effects of the Carceral state: Proximal contact, political participation, and race. Political Research Quarterly 67, 809822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, HL (2019) Targeted: The mobilizing effect of perceptions of unfair policing practices. The Journal of Politics 82, 119134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, HL (2020) Mobilized by Injustice: Criminal Justice Contact, Political Participation, and Race. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, S (2015) Does welfare conditionality reduce democratic participation? Comparative Political Studies 48, 645686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, V and Lerman, A (2010) Political consequences of the Carceral state. American Political Science Review 104, 817833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weitzer, R and Tuch, SA (2004) Race and perceptions of police misconduct. Social Problems 51, 305325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weitzer, R and Tuch, SA (2005) Racially biased policing: Determinants of citizen perceptions. Social Forces 83, 10091030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weitzer, R and Tuch, SA (2006) Race and Policing in America: Conflict and Reform. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Western, B (2006) Punishment and Inequality in America. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
White, A (2019a). Family matters? Voting behavior in households with criminal justice contact. American Political Science Review 113, 607613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, A (2019b). Misdemeanor disenfranchisement? The demobilizing effects of brief jail spells on potential voters. American Political Science Review 113, 311324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, A (2022) Political participation amid mass incarceration. Annual Review of Political Science, 25.Google Scholar
White, M, Cox, T and Basehart, J (1991) Theoretical considerations of officer profanity and obscenity in formal contacts with citizens (from police deviance, pp 275–297, 1991, Barker T and Carter DL (eds), see ncj-128045).Google Scholar
Zoorob, M (2020) Do police brutality stories reduce 911 calls? Reassessing an important criminological finding. American Sociological Review 85, 176183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Explaining political and civic participation

Figure 1

Table 2. Average treatment effects on intended political participation

Figure 2

Figure 1. Differences in attitudes about the police, rates of police contact, and concerns during a (hypothetical) police interaction by respondent raceNote: Subfigure a is a density plot of responses to the police feeling thermometer by respondent race. Subfigure b shows the proportion White and Black respondents that have ever had direct contact with the police or prison system by the most intensive form of contact reported. Maximum contact is defined as being searched, arrested, or incarcerated. Minimal contact is defined as being pulled over (but not ticketed) or questioned by the police. Subgfiure c shows the proportion of White and Black respondents from either treatment group that said they were “concerned about during [their] interaction with the officer.”

Figure 3

Figure 2. Examining the moderation of treatment effects by warmth of feelings toward the police and by raceNote: Results in each subfigure come from three Poisson regression models, each explaining the number of activities a respondent intends to engage in in the coming year and includes an interaction between a feelings toward the police (0—cool to 1—warm) and treatment. The first model includes all respondents, the second includes White respondents only, and the third includes Black respondents only. These regressions are shown in the appendix. Subfigures a, b, and c show the marginal effect of the given treatment (compared with the control condition), with 95% confidence intervals represented in gray, on the number of activities. Subfigures d, e, and f show the predicted number of activities on average based on the treatment or control condition and across the range of the police feeling thermometer.

Supplementary material: PDF

Christiani and Shoub supplementary material

Online Appendix

Download Christiani and Shoub supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 246.3 KB