Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T05:12:04.065Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The mechanism and boundary conditions of the relationship between customer incivility and service quality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2019

Cheng-Chen Lin
Affiliation:
Department of Business Administration, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology, Neipu, Pingtung, Taiwan
Fong-Yi Lai*
Affiliation:
Department of Business Administration, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology, Neipu, Pingtung, Taiwan
*
*Corresponding author: fylai@mail.npust.edu.tw
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study addresses the causal linkage between customer incivility and service quality through the lens of self-determination theory, according to which need satisfaction as a potential mechanism mediates this relationship. Additionally, it examines the moderating role of surface acting in the relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction. Dyadic questionnaires were collected from restaurant employees and their customers in Taiwan. A total of 190 employees and 645 customers participated in this study. Results found that need satisfaction mediates the negative relationship between customer incivility and service quality. Surface acting moderates the relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction as well as the mediation effect of customer incivility on service quality through need satisfaction. Specifically, the indirect effect of need satisfaction on the relation between customer incivility and service quality creativity was more significantly negative at a high level of surface acting than the effect at a low level.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press and Australian and New Zealand Academy of Management 2019

The global economy has gradually shifted its focus from manufacturing to the service industry. Outstanding service quality has become one of the critical determinants of organizational performance and success (Lytle & Timmerman, Reference Lytle and Timmerman2006; Sliter, Jex, Wolford, & McInnerney, Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010). Under this trend, almost all organizations view ‘service quality or service excellence as a strategic imperative or, at a minimum, a strategic opportunity’ (Schneider, Reference Schneider1990: 399). One of the most common ways to pursue outstanding service quality is through high-quality service interactions between front-line employees and their direct customers (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983; Bitner, Booms, & Tetreault, Reference Bitner, Booms and Tetreault1990). However, regular work interactions with customers might cause service employees to experience stress (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983; Brotheridge & Grandey, Reference Brotheridge and Grandey2002; Dorman & Zapf, 2004; Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012), especially facing customer negative behaviors can further accelerate employees’ negative outcomes (Skarlicki, van Jaarsveld, & Walker, Reference Skarlicki, van Jaarsveld and Walker2008; Shao & Skarlicki, Reference Shao and Skarlicki2014). Customer negative behaviors have been found to have negative effects on employees’ service quality and overall organization performance (e.g., Skarlicki, van Jaarsveld, & Walker, Reference Skarlicki, van Jaarsveld and Walker2008; Wang, Liao, Zhan, & Shi, Reference Wang, Liao, Zhan and Shi2011; Shao & Skarlicki, Reference Shao and Skarlicki2014), making it one of the major concerns for academic researchers and organizational practitioners in service areas in recent years (Kern & Grandey, Reference Kern and Grandey2009; Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010; Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012).

Prior research on negative customer behaviors has largely emphasized extreme cases such as jay-customers, severe customer sabotage, or aggression (Lovelock, Reference Lovelock2001; Harris & Reynolds, Reference Harris and Reynolds2004); however, recent research has pointed out that service employees actually more often encounter low-level uncivil customer behaviors, called customer incivility (Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012). Empirical evidence shows that customer incivility is a daily occurrence (e.g., Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010; Walker, van Jaarsveld, & Skarlicki, Reference Walker, van Jaarsveld and Skarlicki2014; Baranik, Wang, Gong, & Shi, Reference Baranik, Wang, Gong and Shi2017) and occurs in many different forms ranging from dealing with ambiguous or unreasonable customer demands to minor customer verbal aggression (Pearson, Anderson, & Porath, Reference Pearson, Andersson and Porath2000; Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010). Sliter, Sliter, and Jex (Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012) indicated that, unlike overt mistreatment, a single incident of incivility ‘might not be perceived as stressful, but an accumulation of perceived incivility leads to negative outcomes’ (p. 122). Therefore, customer incivility indicates the accumulative effects of low-intensity negative customer behaviors over time, rather than the effect of a specific customer encounter.

Because customer incivility was not introduced as a construct on its own until recently (Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010), empirical findings in the customer incivility literature remain at an early stage. While previous research has reported that employees who experience customer incivility can face burnout (Kern & Grandey, Reference Kern and Grandey2009), negative customer orientation (Hur, Moon, & Han, Reference Hur, Moon and Han2015), less sales performance (Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012), and be uncivil toward other customers (van Jaarsveld, Walker, & Skarlicki, Reference van Jaarsveld, Walker and Skarlicki2010), few studies examine the mechanisms and boundary conditions between customer incivility and its negative outcomes.

The present study attempts to address the above literature gaps through applying the perspective of self-determination theory (SDT, Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000) and emotional labor. SDT is a macro theory that provides a broad framework to explain and study human motivation. It addresses the relationship between motivation, performance, and well-being that brings out employees’ volitional engagement. Satisfying psychological needs can provide support to individuals’ intrinsic tendencies to function effectively and healthily. On the other hand, thwarting psychological needs will lead to negative work outcomes (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000). One of the key arguments of SDT is that the surrounding social and cultural factors can facilitate or hinder individuals’ motivation for well-being and the quality of their performance (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000; Reference Deci and Ryan2002). This study proposes that need satisfaction is a mediating mechanism between customer incivility and negative work outcomes.

Furthermore, the work of service employees often involves a certain level of emotional labor (display rules, Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). Emotional labor is the process of managing emotions of job-related interactions or the appearance of regulating emotions at work (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983; Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1996). The literature on emotional labor distinguishes between two ways in which service employees regulate their expressed emotion: deep acting and surface acting (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). Deep acting involves attempting to change actual inner feelings to match the required display rules set by organizations. In contrast, surface acting involves attempting to change affective displays as encouraged by organizations but without altering one’s underlying inner feelings (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000). Research has consistently shown that surface acting is harmful to employees’ job performance (Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011) as assessed by customer service ratings (Grandey, Fisk, Mattila, Jansen, & Sideman, Reference Grandey, Fisk, Mattila, Jansen and Sideman2005; Wagner & Ilies, Reference Wagner and Ilies2008; Groth et al., Reference Groth, Hennig-Thurau and Walsh2009). Deep acting, on the other hand, has inconsistent results for employees’ performance (Grandey, Diefendorff, & Rupp, Reference Grandey, Diefendorff and Rupp2012). While customer incivility negatively affects employees’ psychological needs, we assert that the way employees manage their work-related emotions (i.e., surface acting) can worsen the detrimental effects of customer incivility and reduce service quality. Therefore, this study proposes that emotional labor (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983; Grandey, Reference Grandey2000) is a potential moderator in the customer incivility–need satisfaction–service quality relationship.

The present study contributes to the literature in two ways. First, while previous research suggests several mechanisms, particularly focusing on stress theory and resource perspectives, this study uses SDT to propose that the experience of prolonged incivility can harm employees’ psychological needs, reduce work motivation, and lead to other negative consequences. This study will examine whether customer incivility frustrates employees’ need satisfaction, which can in turn have an indirect effect that reduces service quality. Second, the present study examines the moderating role of surface acting in the relationship between customer incivility and service quality. This examination provides the boundary condition of SDT perspective through which we can obtain a better picture of employee–customer interactions. Based on the above, this study intends to clarify these two issues by investigating whether need satisfaction serves as a mediating mechanism between customer incivility and service quality, and whether surface acting worsens the impact of customer incivility on service quality.

The theoretical model underpinning the present study is depicted in Figure 1. The following section first reviews the literature pertaining to the central concepts in the study – customer incivility, surface acting, need satisfaction, and service quality – and then discusses the hypothesized relationships. Subsequently, we describe the design of the present study, which consisted of a self-report survey of service employees and of service quality collected from customers. Finally, we present the findings and their implications for related literature.

Figure 1. Research framework

Theoretical background and development of hypotheses

In their seminal article, Andersson and Pearson (Reference Andersson and Pearson1999) defined workplace incivility as ‘low-intensity deviant behavior with ambiguous intent to harm the target, in violation of workplace norms for mutual respect. Uncivil behaviors are characteristically rude and discourteous, displaying a lack of regard for others’ (p. 457). Sliter, Sliter, and Jex (Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012) indicate that the word ‘ambiguous intent’ distinguishes between incivility and ‘other forms of interpersonal mistreatment’ (p. 122). Examples of uncivil conduct include hostile stares, sarcasm, disparaging tones and remarks, and the ‘silent treatment.’ Workplace incivility comes from two sources, one is coworker incivility and the other is customer incivility (Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012). Coworker incivility comes from within the organization, including from leaders and colleagues (Duffy, Ganster, & Pagon, Reference Duffy, Ganster and Pagon2002; Aquino & Thau, Reference Aquino and Thau2009); customer incivility is a form of low-quality interpersonal treatment at work that employees perceive originating outside of the organization (Dorman & Zapf, 2004). It is reported that service employees experience incivility more often from customers than from their coworkers (Grandey, Kern, & Frone M, Reference Grandey, Kern and Frone2007; Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010). Customer incivility is recognized as an important and emerging independent construct that needs more research attention (Totterdell & Holman, Reference Totterdell and Holman2003; Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010; Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012).

Organizational authorities have legitimate managerial power to supervise employees via formal and informal rewards and sanctions, so that service employees are often required to follow display rules and comply with the common mantra of ‘the customer is always right’ when providing services (Grandey, Dickter, & Sin, Reference Grandey, Dickter and Sin2004). In the employee–customer relationship, the customers are often in the position of relatively higher power (Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010). Some customers even feel that they have authority over employees, treating employees as subordinates in customer–employee interactions (Harris & Ogbonna, Reference Harris and Ogbonna2002), which easily makes employees the target of incivility (Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010). Most customer–employee relationships consist of episodic and often short-term exchanges. Researchers indicate that such interactions can still induce negative and prolonged adverse consequences (e.g., Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010). For example, customer incivility is found to be associated with job stress and burnout (Spector & Jex, Reference Spector and Jex1998; Cortina, Magley, Williams, & Langhout, Reference Cortina, Magley, Williams and Langhout2001; Porath & Erez, Reference Porath and Erez2007; Kern & Grandey, Reference Kern and Grandey2009).

Service employees are required to adhere to display rules on an ongoing basis, defined as expression norms, that dictate the emotions that are to be expressed in attaining their work goals (Gosserand & Diefendorff, Reference Gosserand and Diefendorff2005). While relationships with an authority figure can involve a certain level of impression management on the employee’s part, following and maintaining display rules in interactions with customers can be an ongoing stressor (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983; Ashforth & Humphrey, Reference Ashforth and Humphrey1993). Rupp and Spencer (Reference Rupp and Spencer2006) found that when employees are not treated fairly by the customers, those who work with high emotional labor will have difficulty complying with the organization’s display rules. In short, customers provide unique challenges to employees’ workplace experience.

Need satisfaction as a mediator

SDT is one of the most detailed and well-validated theoretical perspectives of motivation and psychological needs (Greguras & Diefendorff, Reference Greguras and Diefendorff2009). Different from previous psychological needs theories that assume that individual wants and desires lead to needs, SDT assumes that individuals universally possess an innate desire for personal growth (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000), the absence of which can cause a decline in psychological function and well-being that are considered psychological needs (Ryan, Reference Ryan1995; Lian, Ferris, & Brown, Reference Lian, Ferris and Brown2012). SDT posits the existence of three human needs: the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness; these are essential for individuals’ psychological growth and well-being (Ryan, Reference Ryan1995; Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000; Reference Deci and Ryan2002). The need for competence refers to the desire to attain valued outcomes and succeed at challenging tasks and daily life (White, Reference White1959; Skinner, Reference Skinner1995); the need for relatedness refers to the desire to feel a sense of connection and mutual respect with others (Baumeister & Leary, Reference Baumeister and Leary1995); and the need for autonomy refers to the desire to initiate one’s own action and choose activities consistent with one’s integrated sense of self (Ryan & Connell, Reference Ryan and Connell1989). Satisfying these three basic psychological needs contributes the most to people’s feelings of fulfillment in various events (Sheldon et al., Reference Sheldon, Elliot, Kim and Kasser2001), which is the essential prerequisite for human survival and development (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000). The thwarting of any need will produce negative outcomes (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000). Moreover, in contrast to other need theories that focus on stable individual differences in need strengths, the main arguments in SDT emphasize different external opportunities and conditions that can satisfy individuals’ needs. The surrounding social and cultural factors can facilitate or hinder individuals’ motivation for well-being and the quality of their performance (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000; Reference Deci and Ryan2002).

SDT has been broadly applied to various disciplines, including organization studies. Much empirical evidence has demonstrated that the satisfaction of these three psychological needs is related to a wide range of positive employee work outcomes within an organization including job performance, self-esteem, and organizational commitment (Gagné & Deci, Reference Gagné and Deci2005). Deci, Olafsen, and Ryan (Reference Deci, Olafsen and Ryan2017) indicated that SDT can be applied to the work domain with two sets of independent variables: workplace context and individual differences. Workplace context includes supporting and thwarting context (Deci, Olafsen, & Ryan, Reference Deci, Olafsen and Ryan2017), such as organizational support versus need thwarting; individual differences include causality orientations of aspirations and goals. There are also two sets of applied dependent variables, the consequences of needs satisfaction (Deci, Olafsen, & Ryan, Reference Deci, Olafsen and Ryan2017): work behaviors, such as quality or quantity of work, and health and wellness, including individuals’ well-being/ill-being and vitality. The basic psychological needs in this causal relationship model serve as a mediating mechanism (Deci, Olafsen, & Ryan, Reference Deci, Olafsen and Ryan2017), which include satisfied versus frustrated basic psychological needs. Although the three inner needs are conceptually distinguishable, depending on the research, they are generally used as a composite (e.g., Gagné, Reference Gagné2003; Baard Deci, & Ryan, Reference Baard, Deci and Ryan2004; Sheldon & Niemiec, Reference Sheldon and Niemiec2006; Van den Broeck, De Witte, & Lens, Reference Van den Broeck, Vansteenkiste, De Witte and Lens2008; Uysal, Lin, & Knee, Reference Uysal, Lin and Knee2010) or separately (e.g., Richer, Blanchard, & Vallerand, Reference Richer, Blanchard and Vallerand2002).

SDT represents a comprehensive and overarching theoretical perspective of human motivation and adjustment, wherein need satisfaction leads to human thriving and success, and need frustration leads to maladjustment and impaired regulatory functioning, which undermines an individual’s performance. According to the theoretical model, interactions during service encounter may foster or thwart one’s work motivation, and therefore, customer incivility is hypothesized as a need thwarting factor in the workplace context. The more frequently employees experience customer incivility, the more likely their need satisfaction is affected.

Customer incivility includes low intensity of unintentional mistreatment or hassling. Uncivil behavior by customers, such as being disrespectful or engaging in discourteous behavior (Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010), negatively affects individuals’ basic psychological needs. For example, customer incivility comprises behaviors such as belittling service employees during the process of service delivery. Being belittled or having one’s competence assailed calls into question one’s abilities and achievements, and thus can negatively affect one’s sense of competence. Meanwhile, the unequal status (i.e., employee in the relatively lower power position) in the interaction between customers and service employees can make service employees behave in line with what they believe their customers desire, to avoid being mistreated; as a result, their sense of autonomy is undermined. Finally, excluding, belittling, and rude behaviors communicate to an employee that he or she is not well-respected by customers, reducing one’s sense of belonging and relatedness (Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, Reference Tyler, Degoey and Smith1996; Ferris, Brown, Berry, & Lian, Reference Ferris, Brown, Berry and Lian2008). Based on this, customer incivility negatively impacts the overall basic need satisfaction of service employees.

By regarding these needs as inherent to human nature, SDT focuses on examining satisfactions of these needs in specific situations, with past research outlining the negative consequences associated with thwarted need satisfaction (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000). More specifically, SDT suggests that need satisfaction facilitates self-regulatory processes and adjustment (Kuhl, Reference Kuhl2000), while thwarted need satisfaction is experienced as aversive, undermines self-regulation, and causes poor performance (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000; Reference Deci and Ryan2002). In line with this proposition, need satisfaction is associated with better job performance (Greguras & Diefendorff, Reference Greguras and Diefendorff2009), being more engaged at work, and better psychological adjustment (Deci, Ryan, Gagné, Leone, Usunov, & Kornazheva, Reference Deci, Ryan, Gagné, Leone, Usunov and Kornazheva2001). In contrast, thwarted need satisfaction is related to failure of self-regulation and aggression (Shields, Ryan, & Cicchetti, Reference Shields, Ryan and Cicchetti2001) and health-undermining behaviors such as drug use (Williams, Cox, Hedberg, & Deci, Reference Williams, Cox, Hedberg and Deci2000). Building on this theoretical perspective, it has been suggested that one of the reasons individuals may react and perform negatively in response to uncivil behaviors at work is due to the impact of such behaviors on individuals’ psychological needs (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan1985; Aquino & Thau, Reference Aquino and Thau2009). More specifically, being the target of customer incivility may thwart feelings of belongingness with customers, worthiness in their role of service delivery, and ability to predict and control their task environment, which ultimately harm employees’ service quality. Deci, Olafsen, and Ryan (Reference Deci, Olafsen and Ryan2017) indicated that the workplace context can affect individuals’ psychological needs, which can further harm the quality of their work. Hur, Moon, and Han (Reference Hur, Moon and Han2015) found that the experience of customer incivility by employees can lead to negative customer orientation. Sliter et al. (Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010) reported that customer incivility has a direct negative impact on service quality to other customers. Previous research has also suggested that customer incivility can affect employees’ customer orientation, eventually damaging their service quality. As such, basic needs satisfaction is therefore proposed to mediate the relationship between customer incivility and service quality. Based on the above argument, this study proposes the following hypothesis.

Hypothesis 1: Need satisfaction mediates the negative relationship between customer incivility and service quality.

Surface acting as an exacerbator of customer incivility effects

Although customer incivility negatively impacts employees’ service quality, which is indirectly caused by need satisfaction, this study further suggests surface acting as the boundary condition under which the negative influence of customer incivility may be magnified. Specifically, the more surface acting is adopted, the more negative is the relationship between customer incivility and service quality, through the mediating effect of need satisfaction.

As previously mentioned, surface acting is defined as a type of restricted emotion regulation in which affective expressions are purposively manipulated (i.e., by suppressing or faking) to follow display rules when interacting with the focal customer. In other words, surface acting refers to modifying facial expressions (Grandey, Reference Grandey2003). On the contrary, deep acting is a promoted form of emotion regulation, whereby the undesired affective expression is genuinely replaced by the emotional state that is consistent with organizational display rules (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Gross and John, Reference Gross and John2003). Deep acting refers to modifying one’s inner feelings (Grandey, Reference Grandey2003).

Despite both surface acting and deep acting having a common intended goal that is in line with organizational display rules, Grandey (Reference Grandey2000) noted that with surface acting, an employee attempts to suppress true inner feelings and simply fake an appropriate expression set by display rules, whereas with deep acting an individual actually generates desired affective states via strategies such as attentional deployment and cognitive reappraisal of the situation (Gross, Reference Gross1998). Hence, deep acting aligns experienced affect with display rules, whereas surface acting aligns displayed affect with display rules, even if doing so increases the discrepancy between the experienced affective state and the displayed expression (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003; Scott & Barnes, Reference Scott and Barnes2011). Regarding consequences, surface acting is related to a greater number of negative work-related outcomes than deep acting (Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Mesmer-Magnus, DeChurch, & Wax, Reference Mesmer-Magnus, DeChurch and Wax2012). For instance, researchers have associated surface acting with various negative outcomes such as psychological strain (Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011), emotional exhaustion (Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011), physical illnesses (Schaubroeck & Jones, Reference Schaubroeck and Jones2000), poor job performance (Grandey, Reference Grandey2003), and counterproductive behaviors (Groth & Grandey, Reference Groth and Grandey2012). Based on the more detrimental impacts of surface acting on organizational life than of deep acting, the present study focuses on the role of surface acting.

Despite growing evidence that surface acting is detrimental for employees at work, it is surprising that there have been very few empirical studies examining the moderating role of surface acting in the process of service delivery (Baranik et al., Reference Baranik, Wang, Gong and Shi2017). As Hypothesis 1 stated, when encountering uncivil treatment from customers, an employee’s need satisfaction is thwarted. In this situation, to avoid or to exit this unfavorable situation is not feasible because employee–customer interaction is an in-role requirement in the service industry (Diefendorff, Richard, & Croyle, Reference Diefendorff, Richard and Croyle2006). Furthermore, because of the display rules of ‘service with smile’ (Brotheridge & Grandey, Reference Brotheridge and Grandey2002) and ‘the show must go on’ (Grandy, 2003), employees encountering uncivil customers still have to keep or fake a positive expression to deal with them. This strategy will decrease the feeling of autonomy because service employees have to follow display rules even when they do not experience a positive inner affective state. As a result, their volition and agency are threatened (less satisfied need for autonomy). Therefore, the negative relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction will be exacerbated.

Meanwhile, because surface acting is a discrepancy reducing approach, by which an employee suppresses felt inner emotion while faking unfelt but required emotion at the same time, an employee requires more cognitive resources and emotional effort to regulate his or her inner affective state. This depletes an employee’s cognitive and emotional resources (Rupp & Spencer, Reference Rupp and Spencer2006; Goldberg & Grandey, Reference Goldberg and Grandey2007). Consequently, an employee’s job performance will decrease. As a result, they will receive more negative reactions from customers, resulting in lower self-evaluation of competence in the long run. Under these conditions, the need to feel effective in-service contexts and capable of achieving valued outcomes will not be satisfied (less satisfaction of need for competence).

Finally, because surface acting is an inauthentic way to interact with uncivil customers (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000), it will distance the psychological connection with customers (i.e., psychological distance), which refers to the subjective distance between an actor and other people in temporal (short vs. long), spatial (close vs. distant), and social (in-group vs. other-group) dimensions (Trope & Liberman, Reference Trope and Liberman2003). According to Trope, Liberman, and Wakslak (Reference Trope, Liberman and Wakslak2007), psychological distance is detrimental for interpersonal relationships. Furthermore, as suggested by Zhan, Wang, and Shi (Reference Zhan, Wang and Shi2016), emotional expression and regulation may also elicit an interpersonal process; that is, the way people regulate their emotions may influence their interaction partners’ feelings, perceptions, and behaviors. When employees use surface acting to interact with customers, they put on a mask to treat customers in a desired way, regardless of their true inner feelings (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Zhan et al., Reference Zhan, Wang and Shi2016). Therefore, surface acting is usually perceived to be inauthentic by both service employees themselves (Brotheridge & Lee, Reference Brotheridge and Lee2002) and customers (Grandey, Reference Grandey2003), and the significant inconsistency between felt and expressed emotions is particularly considered to reduce one’s sense of self (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). From these perspectives, surface acting reveals one’s lack of interest in interaction, indicating a sense of relationship avoidance, and consequently, undermining satisfaction of the need for relatedness. When employees perceive customer incivility, the more they use surface acting, the more the negative relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction is magnified. Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed.

Hypothesis 2: Surface acting moderates the relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction such that the relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction is more strongly negative when surface acting is high as opposed to low.

Furthermore, based on Hypotheses 1 and 2, the indirect relation between customer incivility, need satisfaction, and service quality will be moderated by surface acting. Specifically, the indirect effect will be strongly negative when surface acting is high. In contrast, the indirect relationship will be weaker when surface acting is low.

Hypothesis 3: Surface acting moderates the indirect effect of customer incivility on service quality through need satisfaction such that the indirect effect is more strongly negative when surface acting is high as opposed to low.

Method

Participants and procedure

Participants in this study were restaurant front-line employees. Restaurant employees were selected because their work is considered one of the most emotionally stressful and because they frequently encounter customer incivility in their work environment (Han, Bonn, & Cho, Reference Han, Bonn and Cho2016). High-quality or chain restaurants in Taiwan usually require their employees to comply with organizational display rules when providing services. Data were collected from employees and their customers from 21 restaurants in Taiwan. All employees were administered a survey questionnaire containing measures of customer incivility, need satisfaction, surface acting, and control variables. In the customer survey, questionnaires obtained service quality ratings for the focal service employee from customers after they finished dining and before leaving the restaurant. When administering the survey, participants were assured of confidentiality, and the fact that the data were collected for research purposes was emphasized. Among the 190 customer service employees, 129 completed the survey, resulting in a response rate of 67.89%. There were 645 customers who participated in the survey. Each employee was rated by three to five customers. The final employee sample consisted of 73 women and 56 men. The employees’ average age was 26.44 years, with 3.81 years of job tenure. Most respondents were educated above college level (72.90%), and 79.8% were unmarried. Of 645 customers who participated in the present survey, 45.6% were men, 61.2% were unmarried, 76.9% were aged below 40 years, and 64.9% had an above college level of education.

Measures

Customer incivility

A scale from Sliter, Sliter, and Jex (Reference Sliter, Sliter and Jex2012) was used to measure customer incivility. The scale consists of 11 items (e.g., ‘Customers show that they are irritated or impatient’), and all items were rated on a 5-point scale (from 1=‘strongly disagree’ to 5=‘strongly agree’). Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.93.

Need satisfaction

Need satisfaction uses the basic needs satisfaction in relationship scale by La Guardia, Ryan, Couchman, and Deci (Reference La Guardia, Ryan, Couchman and Deci2000). This scale was chosen because this study is interested in the effects of need satisfaction derived from the customer rather than in the effects of need fulfillment derived from the broader working context. The scale measures each basic need satisfaction with three items (response scale: 1=totally disagree to 5=totally agree). The sample items are as follows: ‘In the service relationship with my customer, I have a say in what happens, and I can voice my opinion (i.e., autonomy),’ ‘In the service relationship with my customer, I feel like a competent person (i.e., competence),’ and ‘In the service relationship with my customer, I often feel a large personal distance (i.e., relatedness, reversely coded).’ Cronbach’s α is 0.76. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed to test whether the three-factor model and an overall second-order factor fitted our data. The results showed that the fit indexes fell within an acceptable range (χ2(24)=42.78, root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA]=0.08; comparative fit index [CFI]=0.94; Tucker–Lewis index [TLI]=0.89, standard root mean residual [SRMR]=0.05), suggesting that the model fitted the data reasonably well. Following conventional practice (e.g., La Guardia et al., Reference La Guardia, Ryan, Couchman and Deci2000; Patrick, Knee, Canevello, & Lonsbary, Reference Patrick, Knee, Canevello and Lonsbary2007; Weinstein & Ryan, Reference Weinstein and Ryan2010; Lanaj, Johnson, & Lee, Reference Lanaj, Johnson and Lee2016), all items were collapsed into an overall index of need satisfaction.

Surface acting

Daily surface acting was measured with the five items developed by Brotheridge and Lee (Reference Brotheridge and Lee2003) and Grandey (Reference Grandey2003). Instructions for the measure asked participants to indicate the extent to which each of the five statements described their work experience in the present position, with sample items including ‘I put on an act in order to deal with customers in an appropriate way’ and ‘I just pretended to have the emotions I needed to display on the job.’ Responses were rated from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (very much). Cronbach’s α was 0.95.

Service quality

Service quality was rated with a measure developed from Borucki and Burke (Reference Borucki and Burke1999). To adapt the measure, it is determined that 11 of the original 13 items could adequately capture the nature of restaurant service performance (e.g., being able to help customers when needed), and all items were rated on a 5-point scale (from 1=‘strongly disagree’ to 5=‘strongly agree’). Cronbach’s α was 0.93.

To assess the validity of aggregating customer-level data to the employee level, it was necessary to demonstrate both between-employee variability and within-employee agreement (Hofmann & Stetzer, Reference Hofmann and Stetzer1996; Hofmann, Reference Hofmann1997; Bliese, Reference Bliese2000). To check interrater agreement among customers for a focal employee, we calculated within-group agreement (rwgj) values using uniform null distribution and obtained median values of 0.89 for service quality. Additionally, the assessment between-employee variance in the employee-level service quality construct uses a one-way analysis of variance. The analysis of variance indicated significant between-employee variance in service quality (F=2.38, p<.001). The ICC (1) and ICC (2) calculated from the analysis of variance were 0.22 and 0.58, respectively. This value is at the high end of what can be expected in applied research settings (Bliese, Reference Bliese2000). Together, these statistics show acceptable levels of within-employee agreement and between-employee variability in the service quality as an employee variable.

Control variables

Based on prior customer incivility research, this study controlled for gender (1 for male, 2 for female), education (1: below college, 2: college, and 3: above college), and tenure for the present position measured in years (Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010; van Jaarsveld, Walker, & Skarlicki, Reference van Jaarsveld, Walker and Skarlicki2010).

Results

To examine the distinctiveness of the measured constructs from service employees, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted with maximum likelihood estimation in LISREL 8.8 (Joreskog & Sorbom, Reference Joreskog and Sorbom2006). Given the limited sample size relative to the large number of parameters estimated in the measurement model, this study used item-parceling as indicators to maintain a proper indicator-to-sample-size ratio (Bentler & Chou, Reference Bentler and Chou1987) and randomly created three parcels for all scales. The measurement model (theoretical model) consisted of three factors: surface acting, need satisfaction, and customer incivility. Results showed that the hypothesized three-factor model fit the data well (χ2(24)=48.60, p<.001; CFI=0.97; non-normed fit index [NNFI]=0.95; RMSEA=0.08; SRMR=0.06). Furthermore, we compared the three-factor model with the two-factor model (i.e., combining customer incivility and surface acting; χ2(26)=144.72, p<.001; CFI=0.85; NNFI=0.79; RMSEA=0.17; SRMR=0.13), and the one-factor model (i.e., combining customer incivility, surface acting, and need satisfaction; χ2(27)=512.60, p<.001; CFI=0.38; NNFI=0.17; RMSEA=0.34; SRMR=0.21). A change in the χ2-test suggested the three-factor model produced a significant improvement over the two-factor model (Δχ2(1)=96.12, p<.001) and the one-factor model (Δχ2(3)=162.03, p<.001). Despite its advantages, however, using parceled scales can mask problems with individual items (Bandalos & Finney, Reference Bandalos and Finney2001). To strengthen confidence in the factor structure of our measures, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis using all items. All items were loaded on the intended constructs, though factor loadings for two items from the need satisfaction scale were smaller than 0.40. Based on the above, these results support the discriminant validity of the constructs.

Because some of the present constructs were collected from the same source (i.e., service employees), there was a concern about common method variance, which threatens internal validity (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff2003). Harman’s one-factor test was used to address this issue of same source bias. To run this test, all the measured variables were entered into an exploratory factor analysis and the unrotated factor solution was examined to determine the number of factors necessary to account for the variance in the variables. If a problematic amount of same source bias was present, either a single factor would emerge from the exploratory factor analysis or one factor would account for the majority of the covariance among the variables. The results of the analysis revealed the presence of three distinct factors with eigenvalues >1.0 and that one factor explained 31.23% of the total variance. Therefore, because the Harman test revealed the presence of more than one distinct factor and that the majority of the total variance was not explained by one factor, the results provide empirical evidence that common source bias is not an issue. Thus, the authors are confident that it is unlikely that same source bias confounded our results.

A summary of the descriptive statistics (means, SDs, and reliabilities) and correlations among all variables is presented in Table 1. The correlations among customer incivility, need satisfaction, and service quality were in the expected direction.

Table 1. Correlations, means, and SDs for all study variables

Note: N=129; Values in parentheses on the diagonal are the Cronbach’s α coefficients.

*p=.05, **p=.01, ***p=.001.

Hypothesis 1 predicts that need satisfaction mediates the negative relationship between customer incivility and service quality evaluated by the direct customers. As shown in Table 2, customer incivility was negatively related to need satisfaction (B=–0.27, SE=0.05, p<.001, Model 1). Moreover, need satisfaction was positively related to service quality (B=0.26, SE=0.07, p<.001, Model 2). The mediation effect was significantly negative (Sobel test: B=−0.07, p<.01, the confidence interval ranged from −0.12 to −0.03 from 95% bootstrapping). Hypothesis 1 was supported.

Table 2. Results for mediation effect

Note: N=129. Unstandardized regression coefficients are reported. Bootstrap sample size=5,000.

Hypothesis 2 proposes that surface acting moderates the relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction. The results are shown in Table 3 (Model 1). The interaction between customer incivility and surface acting was negatively related to need satisfaction (B=−0.10, SE=0.04, p<.01). Following the procedure suggested by Aiken and West (Reference Aiken and West1991), we plotted the interaction effects shown in Figure 2. To further probe the relationship between customer incivility and need satisfaction at the different levels of surface acting, we conducted a series of simple slope tests at five levels of points (i.e., 1: very low=−2 SD; 2: low=−1 SD; 3: medium=0; 4: high=+1 SD; and 5: very high=+2 SD). The simple slope tests showed that customer incivility had no significant relationship with need satisfaction when surface acting was very low (B=−0.02, SE=0.12, p>.05) and low (B=−0.10, SE=0.08, p>.05), but was significantly negatively related to need satisfaction when surface acting was medium (B=−0.21, SE=0.05, p<.001), high (B=−0.33, SE=0.06, p<.001), and very high (B=−0.44, SE=0.09, p<.001). Hypothesis 2 was supported.

Table 3. Results for conditional indirect effect

Note: N=129. Unstandardized regression coefficients are reported. Bootstrap sample size=5,000.

Figure 2. Interaction between customer incivility and surface acting on need satisfaction

Hypothesis 3 predicts that surface acting moderates the mediation effect of customer incivility on service quality through need satisfaction. To test this hypothesis, we followed the moderated path analysis approach developed by Edwards and Lambert (Reference Edwards and Lambert2007) to test the overall moderated mediation effects. This approach produces a group of models that indicate how the first-stage effect (from the independent variable to the mediator), second-stage effect (from the mediator to the dependent variable), direct effect (from the independent variable to the dependent variable), and indirect effect (from the independent variable to the dependent variable through the mediator) vary at the different levels of the moderator. We also followed the suggestion of Edwards and Lambert (Reference Edwards and Lambert2007) to bootstrap 1,000 samples to construct bias-corrected confidence intervals for the significance tests of the effects. The results, as summarized in the conditional indirect effect model in Table 3, show that the size of the difference in the indirect effect of customer incivility on service quality was 0.07 (SE=0.03, p<.05), with the 95% confidence intervals computed using bootstrap estimates excluding zero. Specifically, the indirect effect of need satisfaction on the relation between customer incivility and service quality creativity was more significantly negative at a high level of surface acting (B=−0.09, SE=0.03, p<.001) than the effect at a low level of surface acting (B=−0.03, SE=0.02, p>.05). The interaction pattern is shown in Figure 3. Hypothesis 3 was supported.

Figure 3. Moderating effect of surface acting on indirecting effect of customer incivility on service quality

In spite of some researchers (e.g., Weinstein & Ryan, Reference Weinstein and Ryan2010; Lanaj, Johnson, & Lee, Reference Lanaj, Johnson and Lee2016) having suggested that needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness can be aggregated to an overall construct, there is still a possibility of different relationships among variables between the levels of construct and its dimensions. To address this concern, we added the analysis at each need dimension to probe for possible different effects of customer incivility on the three distinct psychological needs. Results showed that customer incivility was significantly negatively related to autonomy (B=−0.23, SE=0.05, p<.001), and autonomy mediated the relationship between customer incivility and service quality (mediation effect=−0.09, SE=0.02, p<.001). The same significant patterns can also be found in the direct and mediation effects of competence (B=−0.30, SE=0.05, p<.001; mediation effect=−0.17, SE=0.03, p<.001) and relatedness (B=−0.30, SE=0.04, p<.001; mediation effect=−0.15, SE=0.03, p<.001). Results of tests assessing the moderating role of surface acting on the relationships between customer incivility and three psychological needs showed that surface acting moderated the relationship between customer incivility and relatedness (interaction effect=−0.10, SE=0.03, p>.01), but not for autonomy and competence (interaction effect=−0.02, SE=0.04, p>.05; interaction effect=−0.02, SE=0.04, p>.05, respectively). With these findings, implications will be offered in the discussion section.

Discussion

From a theoretical standpoint, the current study contributes to the literature in two important ways. First, the present study extended previous empirical efforts through investigating the mediating role of need satisfaction in the relationship between customer incivility and service quality. Previous researchers have conceptualized customer incivility from the perspectives of interactional justice (e.g., Rupp, McCance, Spencer, & Sonntag, Reference Rupp, McCance, Spencer and Sonntag2008) and job demands (e.g., Dormann & Zapf, Reference Dormann and Zapf2004; Grandey, Dickter, & Sin, Reference Grandey, Dickter and Sin2004; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Liao, Zhan and Shi2011). Our results lend support to the growing body of research that suggests a new mechanism may be at play, and this theoretical angle views customer mistreatment from the SDT (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan2000).

Second, this study contributes to the existing literature by exploring whether surface acting moderates the relationship between customer incivility and employee need satisfaction. Grounded in emotional labor perspective (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000), this finding provides insight into conditions when surface acting is likely to produce a more adverse effect of customer incivility on service quality.

Theoretical and practical implications

The primary contribution of the present study is to further our understanding of the causal linkage between customer incivility and two key outcomes: need satisfaction and customer service performance. Namely, it is believed that need satisfaction – satisfaction with autonomy, relatedness, and competency – was the composite driving force behind the negative effects of customer incivility.

Drawing upon a SDT framework, the present study examined the mediating role that need satisfaction plays in the relation between customer incivility and service quality. Compared to previous research that has primarily theoretically focused on emotional resources and justice perception to account for the relation between customer behavior and service performance, the present findings, by including basic psychological needs, highlight the important internal driving force of motivation, offering a new lens for future research.

The findings also suggest that this relationship becomes more adverse when the employee uses surface acting to cope with customer incivility. In other words, the experience of customer incivility is not the only source of threat to need satisfaction. Research results suggest that if the customer service representative cannot express his or her feelings, but fakes or suppresses his or her inner emotion instead, the negative consequences of customer incivility will worsen.

Regarding customer service quality, the present results suggest that faking positive emotions or suppressing negative emotions (a common type of emotional labor) likely plays a moderating role in the relationship between customer incivility and customer service performance. The results of this study build on these findings by adding the knowledge that customer identification of inauthentic emotion can actually decrease customer ratings in the service encounter. Service providers who fake/suppress emotions when dealing with an uncivil customer may be perceived as patronizing or as minimizing the customer’s concerns. In the long run, using surface acting with an uncivil customer may lead to what Andersson and Pearson (Reference Andersson and Pearson1999) termed a spiral of incivility and, ultimately, a more dissatisfied customer.

Furthermore, the analysis results of dimensional level of need satisfaction (as the last part of results section) revealed that autonomy, competence, and relatedness individually mediate relationship between customer incivility and service quality, which are similar to the mediating role of construct level of need satisfaction. However, the moderating role of surface acting in the mediation relationship mentioned above shows interesting differences in dimensional level analyses: surface acting only exacerbates the negative consequence of customer incivility on the satisfaction of relatedness need, but not for autonomy and competence. The findings, on the one hand, imply that customers’ uncivil behaviors impair a service employee’s satisfaction of social interaction and, on the other hand, that unauthentic interaction (e.g., surface acting) will worsen actors’ (i.e., service employees) social need fulfillment. To the best of our knowledge, such a relationship perspective has received very few, if any, attention in the extant literature of customer incivility. We believe this unexplored lens will offer researchers a more sophisticated picture of the present field.

Customer incivility is a pervasive phenomenon in service settings. To cope with negative consequences (e.g., emotional exhaustion and negative emotional experience) stemming from customer negative behaviors, service employees may adopt dysfunctional coping strategies (i.e., surface acting). In doing so, however, this coping method may strengthen a cycle of negativity that results in further problems for service employees and for the organization indirectly. Based on the above finding, it suggests that managers and organizations may adopt the following ways to avert negative effects. First, suppressed negative emotions, but not faked positive emotions, have a discernible impact on customer outcomes. Therefore, employees can be trained to differentiate between these two acting processes and focus more of their attention on the former rather than the latter. Second, the display of genuine negative emotions will lower customer satisfaction. Thus, organizations may be advised to focus on mitigating negative experiences to help employees not to express genuine negative emotions or to invest cognitive resources to suppress those negative emotions. Third, given that surface acting depletes resources, the present study suggests that organizations can develop fairly standardized service processes that may divert resources otherwise used to control emotions to other tasks, without necessarily jeopardizing service satisfaction. Finally, because strong relationships with customers may buffer the negative effects of suppressed negative emotions, organizations can provide interpersonal training to strengthen customer bonds, which may mitigate the negative impact of suppressing negative emotions.

Strengths and limitations

One strength of the present study is that it used a multisource research design. In particular, it assessed customer incivility, need satisfaction, and surface acting from service employees, and service quality from multiple customers. Despite these strengths, the present work is not without methodological limitations. First, customer incivility, surface acting, and need satisfaction measures were collected from the same source (i.e., service employees). While the above three constructs are perceptual variables and should be assessed using self-reports, other reports (e.g., significant others) may have been useful to assess these three constructs. However, Harman’s one-factor test indicated that the same source might not be a major threat to the present study findings. Furthermore, Evans (Reference Evans1985) suggested that the presence of interaction effects argues against method effects. Although it cannot conclusively rule out any concern with common method variance as an explanation for the results, the precautions taken make such an explanation unlikely. Second, a potentially more serious limitation lies in the correlational nature of this study; the causal ordering of variables remains uncertain, and it is possible that poor service quality itself causes one to be treated uncivilly by customers through the effect on need satisfaction.

Acknowledgments

This paper is based on a research grant project (106-2410-H-020-016) funded by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan. The authors would like to thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments and suggestions.

About the Authors

Cheng-Chen Lin is a professor in the Department of Business Administration, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology, Taiwan. His teaching areas include research method, organizational behavior, and human resource management. He completed his PhD in the Department of Business Management, National Sun Yat-Sen University, Taiwan. His research areas include leadership, organizational citizenship behavior, and other related topics.

Fong-Yi Lai is a professor in the Department of Business Administration, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology, Taiwan. Her teaching areas include marketing management, consumer culture, and event management. She completed her PhD in the School of Marketing and Management, Griffith University, Australia. Her research areas include service behavior, sports marketing, and other related topics.

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Andersson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). ‘Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace’. Academy of Management Review, 24(3), 452471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aquino, K., & Thau, S. (2009). ‘Workplace victimization: Aggression from the target’s perspective’. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). ‘Emotional labor in service roles – the influence of identity’. Academy of Management Review, 18(1), 88115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). ‘The relation of intrinsic need satisfaction to performance and wellbeing in two work settings’. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(10), 20452068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandalos, D. L., & Finney, S. J. (2001). ‘Item parceling issues in structural equation modeling’. In Marcoulides G. A., & Schumacker R. E. (Eds.), New developments and techniques in structural equation modeling, 269296. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baranik, L. E., Wang, M., Gong, Y., & Shi, J. (2017). ‘Customer mistreatment, employee health, and job performance: Cognitive rumination and social sharing as mediating mechanisms’. Journal of Management, 43, 12611282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R., & Leary, M. R. (1995). ‘The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation’. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. P. (1987). ‘Practical issues in structural equation modeling’. Sociological Methods and Research, 16(1), 78117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bitner, M. J., Booms, B. H., & Tetreault, M. S. (1990). ‘The service encounter: Diagnosing favorable and unfavorable incidents’. Journal of Marketing, 54(1), 7184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliese, P. D. (2000). ‘Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis’. In Klein K. J., & Kozlowski S. W. J. (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations, 349381. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Borucki, C. C., & Burke, M. J. (1999). ‘An examination of service-related antecedents to retail store performance’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20(6), 943962.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Grandey, A. A (2002). ‘Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing two perspectives of people work’. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 1739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R. T. (2002). ‘Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional labor’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7(1), 5767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R. T (2003). ‘Development and validation of the emotional labour scale’. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76(3), 365379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001). Incivility at the workplace: Incidence and impact. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6(1), 6480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., Olafsen, A. H., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). ‘Self-determination theory in work organizations: The state of a science’. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 4, 1943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). ‘The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior’. Psychological Inquiry, 4(4), 227268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.) 2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). ‘Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern Bloc country’. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(8), 930942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., & Croyle, M. H. (2006). ‘Are emotional display rules formal job requirements? Examination of employee and supervisor perceptions’. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79(2), 273298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dormann, C., & Zapf, D. (2004). ‘Customer-related social stressors and burnout’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9(1), 6182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D. C., & Pagon, M. (2002). ‘Social undermining in the workplace’. Academy of Management Journal, 45(2), 331352.Google Scholar
Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). ‘Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: A general analytical framework using moderated path analysis’. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. G. (1985). ‘A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in moderated multiple regression analysis’. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36(3), 305323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferris, D. L., Brown, D. J., Berry, J., & Lian, H. (2008). ‘The development and validation of the Workplace Ostracism Scale’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 13481366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, M. (2003). ‘The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in the engagement of prosocial behavior’. Motivation and Emotion, 27(3), 199223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L (2005). ‘Self-determination theory and work motivation’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 331362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, L. S., & Grandey, A. A. (2007). ‘Display rules versus display autonomy: Emotion regulation, emotional exhaustion, and task performance in a call center simulation’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(3), 301318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosserand, R. H., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2005). ‘Emotional display rules and emotional labor: The moderating role of commitment’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6), 12561264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2000). ‘Emotional regulation in the work place: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 95110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2003). ‘When ‘the show must go on’: Surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery’. Academy of Management Journal, 46(1), 8696.Google Scholar
Grandey, A., Dickter, D., & Sin, H.-P (2004). ‘The customer is not always right: Customer verbal aggression toward service employees’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(3), 397418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Diefendorff, J. M., & Rupp, D. E. (2012). Emotional labor in the 21st century: Diverse perspectives on emotion regulation at work (Organization and Management Series). Oxford: Routledge.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Fisk, G. M., Mattila, A. S., Jansen, K. J., & Sideman, L. A. (2005). ‘Is ‘service with a smile’ enough? Authenticity of positive displays during service encounters’. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96(1), 3855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Kern, J., & Frone, M. (2007). ‘Verbal Abuse from outsiders versus insiders: Comparing frequency, impact on emotional exhaustion, and the role of emotional labor’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(1), 6379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greguras, G. J., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2009). ‘Different fits satisfy different needs: Linking person-environment fit to employee commitment and performance using self-determination theory’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(2), 465477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998). ‘The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review’. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). ‘Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groth, M., & Grandey, A. A (2012). ‘From bad to worse: negative exchange spirals in employee-customer service interactions’. Organizational Psychology Review, 2(3), 208233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groth, M., Hennig-Thurau, T., & Walsh, G. (2009). ‘Customer reactions to emotional labor: The roles of employee acting strategies and customer detection accuracy’. Academy of Management Journal, 52(5), 958974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, S. J., Bonn, M. A., & Cho, M. (2016). ‘The relationship between customer incivility, restaurant frontline service employee burnout and turnover intention’. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 52, 97106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, L. C., & Ogbonna, E. (2002). ‘Exploring service sabotage: The antecedents, types and consequences of frontline, deviant, antiservice behaviors’. Journal of Service Research, 4, 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, L. C., & Reynolds, K. L. (2004). ‘Jaycustomer behavior: An exploration of types and motives in the hospitality industry’. Journal of Services Marketing, 18(5), 339357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: commercialization of human feeling. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hofmann, D. A. (1997). ‘An overview of the logic and rationale of hierarchical linear models’. Journal of Management, 23(6), 723744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, D. A., & Stetzer, A. (1996). ‘A cross-level investigation of factors influencing unsafe behaviors and accidents’. Personnel Psychology, 49(2), 307339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hülsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F. (2011). ‘On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16(3), 361389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hur, W. M., Moon, T. W., & Han, S. J. (2015). ‘The effect of customer incivility on service employees’ customer orientation through double-mediation of surface acting and emotional exhaustion’. Journal of Service Theory and Practice, 25(4), 394413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (2006). LISREL for Windows. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.Google Scholar
Kern, J. H., & Grandey, A. A. (2009). ‘Customer incivility as a social stressor: The role of race and racial identity for service employees’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14(1), 4657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000). The volitional basis of personality systems interaction theory: Applications in learning and treatment contexts’. International Journal of Educational Research, 33(7–8), 665703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). ‘Within-person variation in security of attachment: A self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfillment, and well-being’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 367384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanaj, K., Johnson, R. E., & Lee, S. M (2016). ‘Benefits of transformational behaviors for leaders: A daily investigation of leader behaviors and need fulfillment’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 237251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lian, H., Ferris, D. L., & Brown, D. J. (2012). ‘Does taking the good with the bad make things worse? How abusive supervision and leader–member exchange interact to impact need satisfaction and organizational deviance’. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 4152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LovelockChristopher, H. Christopher, H. (2001). Services marketing: people, technology, strategy (4th ed.), Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Lytle, R. S., & Timmerman, J. E. (2006). ‘Service orientation and performance: An organizational perspective’. Journal of Services Marketing, 20(2), 136147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., DeChurch, L. A., & Wax, A. (2012). ‘Moving emotional labor beyond surface and deep acting: A discordance-congruence perspective’. Organizational Psychology Review, 2(1), 653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). ‘The dimensions, antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor’. Academy of Management Review, 21(4), 9861010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, H., Knee, C. R., Canevello, A., & Lonsbary, C. (2007). ‘The role of need fulfillment in relationship functioning and well-being: A self-determination theory perspective’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 434457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, C., Andersson, L., & Porath, C. (2000). Assessing and attacking workplace incivility. Organizational Dynamics, 29(2), 123137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). ‘Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2007). ‘Does rudeness really matter? The effects of rudeness on task performance and helpfulness’. Academy of Management Journal, 50(5), 11811197.Google Scholar
Richer, S. F., Blanchard, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (2002). ‘A motivational model of work turnover’. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 20892113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rupp, D. E., McCance, A. S., Spencer, S., & Sonntag, K. (2008). ‘Customer (in)justice and emotional labor: The role of perspective taking, anger, and emotional regulation’. Journal of Management, 34(5), 903924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rupp, D. E., & Spencer, S. (2006). ‘When customers lash out: The effects of customer interactional injustice on emotional labor and the mediating role of discrete emotions’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 971978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M. (1995). ‘Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes’. Journal of Personality, 63(3), 297427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). ‘Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(5), 749761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaubroeck, J., & Jones, J. R. (2000). ‘Antecedents of workplace emotional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21(2), 163183.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, B. (1990). ‘The climate for service: An application of the climate construct’. In Schneider B. (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture, 383412). San Francisco: Jossey- Bass.Google Scholar
Scott, B. A., & Barnes, C. M. (2011). ‘A multilevel investigation of emotional labor, affect, withdrawal, and gender’. Academy of Management Journal, 54(1), 116136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shao, R., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2014). ‘Service employees’ reactions to mistreatment by customers: A comparison between North America and East Asia’. Personnel Psychology, 67(1), 2359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). ‘What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological needs’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 325339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., & Niemiec, C. P. (2006). ‘It’s not just the amount that counts: Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(2), 331341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, A., Ryan, R. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). ‘Narrative representations of caregivers and emotion dysregulation as predictors of maltreated children’s rejection by peers’. Developmental Psychology, 37(3), 321337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skarlicki, D. P., van Jaarsveld, D. D., & Walker, D. D. (2008). ‘Getting even for customer mistreatment: The role of moral identity in the relationship between customer interpersonal injustice and employee sabotage’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 13351347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and coping. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sliter, M. T., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J. (2010). ‘How rude! Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and employee outcomes’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15(4), 468481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sliter, M. T., Sliter, K., & Jex, S. (2012). ‘The employee as a punching bag: The effect of multiple sources of incivility on employee withdrawal behavior and sales performance’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33(1), 121139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spector, P. E., & Jex, S. M. (1998). ‘Development of four self-report measures of job stressors and strain: Interpersonal conflict at work scale, organizational constraints scale, quantitative workload inventory, and physical symptoms inventory’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 3, 356367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2003). ‘Emotion regulation in customer service roles: Testing a model of emotional labor’. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8, 5573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). ‘Temporal construal’. Psychological Review, 110, 403421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Wakslak, C. (2007). ‘Construal levels and psychological distance: Effects on representation, prediction, evaluation, and behavior’. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(2), 8395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T., Degoey, P., & Smith, H. (1996). ‘Understanding why the justice of group procedures matters: A test of the psychological dynamics of the group-value model’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 913930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uysal, A., Lin, H. L., & Knee, C. R. (2010). ‘The role of need satisfaction in self-concealment and well-being’. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(2), 187199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Witte, H., & Lens, W. (2008). ‘Explaining the relationships between job characteristics, burnout, and engagement: The role of basic psychological need satisfaction’. Work & Stress, 22(3), 277294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Jaarsveld, D. D., Walker, D. D., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2010). ‘The role of job demands and emotional exhaustion in the relationship between customer and employee incivility’. Journal of Management, 36(6), 14861504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, D. T., & Ilies, R. (2008). ‘Affective influences on employee satisfaction and performance’. In AshkanasyN.M., & CooperC.L. (Eds.), Research companion to emotion in organizations, 152169). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Walker, D. D., van Jaarsveld, D. D., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2014). ‘Exploring the effects of individual customer incivility encounters on employee incivility: The moderating roles of entity (in)civility and negative affectivity’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(1), 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Liao, H., Zhan, Y., & Shi, J. (2011). ‘Daily customer mistreatment and employee sabotage against customer: Examining emotion and resource perspectives’. Academy of Management Journal, 54(2), 312334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M (2010). ‘When helping helps: Autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its influence on well-being for the helper and recipient’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 222244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, R. W. (1959). ‘Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence’. Psychological Review, 66, 297333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, G. C., Cox, E. M., Hedberg, V., & Deci, E. L. (2000). ‘Extrinsic life goals and health risk behaviors in adolescents’. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(8), 17561771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Won-Moo, H., Tae Won, M., & Su-Jin, H. (2015). ‘The effect of customer incivility on service employees’ customer orientation through double-mediation of surface acting and emotional exhaustion’. Journal of Service Theory and Practice, 25(4), 394413.Google Scholar
Zhan, Y., Wang, M., & Shi, J. (2016). ‘Interpersonal process of emotional labor: the role of negative and positive customer treatment’. Personnel Psychology, 69(3), 525557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Research framework

Figure 1

Table 1. Correlations, means, and SDs for all study variables

Figure 2

Table 2. Results for mediation effect

Figure 3

Table 3. Results for conditional indirect effect

Figure 4

Figure 2. Interaction between customer incivility and surface acting on need satisfaction

Figure 5

Figure 3. Moderating effect of surface acting on indirecting effect of customer incivility on service quality