Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T07:24:37.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Minimalism and French /ʀ/: Phonological representations in phonetically based phonology*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2009

ERIC RUSSELL WEBB*
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
*
Address for correspondence: Eric Russell Webb, 108 Sproul Hall, University of California, Davis, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616, USA e-mail: erussell@ucdavis.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper examines the phonological structure of French rhotics and their treatment in a production grammar. Assuming emergent featural specification, it is argued that the underlying representation of /ʀ/ contains only values for sonorance, continuance and place of articulation. Grammatical analysis is undertaken in an Optimality Theoretic framework, where evaluation highlights the effect of effort reduction and perceptual augmentation on /ʀ/, also demonstrating that more richly specified segments are unaffected by these constraints. The proposal is shown to be compatible with antecedent analyses and data from regional forms and registers.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

INTRODUCTION

This article examines the phonological representation of French /ʀ/.Footnote 1 Based on the premise that form emerges from language use and that abstract structure is acquired, rather than innate, it is argued that /ʀ/ is structurally minimal, including only values for continuance and dorsality in most varieties of French. Variable surface forms are shown to be the product of phonetically based constraints, the interaction of which is formalised in a broadly Optimality Theoretic (OT; Prince & Smolensky Reference Prince and Smolensky1993) grammatical model. It is noted that the unique representational minimalism of /ʀ/ renders this segment particularly sensitive to phonetically based constraints.

The article proceeds as follows. The first section examines the phonetic and phonological characteristics of French /ʀ/, surveys previous accounts, and addresses larger issues associated with rhotic sounds. An alternative specification of /ʀ/ is advanced in the second section, drawing upon non innatist and emergent featural theories. The third section focuses on grammatical treatment of minimal underlying representations. It is shown that constraints targeting positional augmentation, interacting with those promoting effort reduction are crucially ranked in the production grammar; this successfully predicts surface forms and provides a template for the analysis of data from regional forms and registers. A discussion section underscores the theoretical and methodological advantages of this approach, also highlighting related questions and paths of future research.

1. BACKGROUND

The sound represented by the grapheme r is one of the most variable segments in French. This section provides a short background to /ʀ/, focusing on its phonetic characteristics, distributional regularities and association with phonological classes.

1.1. The phonetics of French R

In most varieties of convergent French, /ʀ/ is articulated as a fricative or approximant, having a uvulo-velar or velar place of articulation, and may be voiced or voiceless, as in (1) (Tranel, Reference Tranel1987, forthcoming; Fougeron and Smith, Reference Fougeron and Smith1993; Walker, Reference Walker2001; Russell Webb, Reference Russell Webb2002, Reference Russell Webb2004a, Reference Russell Webb2004a; Rose Reference Rose2003).Footnote 2 In (1a) and throughout this article, the diacritics [˔] and [˕] indicate a relatively narrower and wider aperture at the point of oral constriction, corresponding to fricative and approximant articulations, respectively. In (1b), [˯] and [˳] indicate voiced and voiceless surface forms, respectively.

  1. (1) Surface forms of /ʀ/: Convergent French

    1. a. aperture

    2. b. voicing

Most speakers produce surface forms of /ʀ/ as fricative [] in word initial onsets and as approximant [] in codas, as in (1a). In intervocalic environments a good deal more variability is observed. In a phonetic study involving both spontaneous and careful speech tokens, Russell Webb (Reference Russell Webb2002) notes that speakers produce more approximant like segments in these contexts, an observation also echoed in Tranel (Reference Tranel1987, forthcoming) and O'Shaughnessy (1982), among others. At the same time, it is noted that fricative [] in intervocalic positions is far from extraordinary, especially in emphatic or careful speech styles.Footnote 3 /ʀ/ aperture also varies in hyper- or hypoarticulate speech styles, e.g. final /ʀ/ may be fricated for emphasis, whereas initial /ʀ/ may be lenited due to enchainment, i.e. the sequential resyllabification of speech. As noted in (1b), /ʀ/ is also subject to patterns of passive voicing (Tranel, Reference Tranel1987: 21; Walker, Reference Walker2001: 137–139; Russell Webb, Reference Russell Webb2004b).

Regional forms of French testify to additional surface forms and distributions. For many Brussels French (BF) speakers, for instance, aperture variation is largely absent (Grootaerts, Reference Grootaerts1953; Baetens Beardsmore, Reference Baetens Beardsmore1971: 84–85, 437–439); /ʀ/ almost always surfaces as a fricative, regardless of position or context, as in (2). Trilled [R] is also noted, although with far less regularity.

  1. (2) Brussels French

Tranel (PC) observes that trilled [R] is marginal in modern French (see also Delattre, Reference Delattre and Delattre1966: 206), although it does occur in a number of highly marked speech styles, e.g. singing. Other examples of dialectal particularity are noted in Southwestern France, where /ʀ/ is often realised as a voiceless fricative (Tranel, Reference Tranel1987: 143); in Montréal, where /ʀ/ was until recently produced as [R] or [r], although fricatives are now most common (Tousignant, Sankoff and Santerre, Reference Tousignant, Sankoff, Santerre, Fasold and Schiffrin1989; cf. Sankoff and Blondeau, Reference Sankoff and Blondeau2007); and in Francophone Africa, where /ʀ/ is most often produced as an apical vibrant [ɾ], although velar articulations are not unknown (Dumont, Reference Dumont and Valdman1979; Duponchel, Reference Duponchel and Valdman1979; Manessy, Reference Manessy1984; Renaud, Reference Renaud and Valdman1979; see also Walter, Reference Walter1977).

Coda /ʀ/ vocalisation is noted in a handful of dialects, e.g. Quebecois (QF) and St. Thomas French (STF). In QF, /ʀ/ can surface as a homorganic vowel in syllabic codas.Footnote 4 In STF (3b), /ʀ/ vocalisation only occurs in post-vocalic clusters (i.e. complex codas); in simple codas, /ʀ/ is elided.

  1. (3) /ʀ/ vocalisation

    1. a. Quebecois French (Côté, Reference Côté2004: 169-170)

    2. b. St. Thomas French (Highfield, Reference Highfield1979: 45)Footnote 5

Similar to STF data in (3b), coda /ʀ/ vocalization or erasure is common in regional French, as well as in non-standard registers and fast speech. This phenomenon is especially frequent when /ʀ/ is found in final, post-obstruent /ʀ/ position, e.g. cadre ‘frame’ [kad].

1.2. R distribution

A crucial issue with regard to the phonological status of /ʀ/ is the participation of this segment in phonetic and phonological classes, i.e. groups of sounds which behave in similar manners or take part in common processes. Like voiced fricatives, but distinct from /l/ and glides, /ʀ/ triggers phonetic vowel lengthening, as in (4).

  1. (4) Vowel lengthening (Colantoni and Steele, Reference Colantoni and Steele2005; Laeufer, Reference Laeufer1992; Walker, Reference Walker2001: 42-44)

The question of whether vowel lengthening is best attributed to phonetic implementation or the phonological grammar is not taken up here (see e.g. Lindblom, Reference Lindblom2006 for discussion of the phonetics-phonology divide). Rather, what can be noted here is that /ʀ/ is associable with voiced fricatives and not /l/ and glides.

Despite the above, /ʀ/ distribution is most closely aligned with that of /l/, as exemplified in (5). Both segments frequently occur as the rightmost constituent in complex onsets; both also precede obstruents in complex codas or occur in final, post-obstruent codas.Footnote 6

  1. (5) Liquid Distribution

    1. a. complex onset

    2. b. coda

    3. c. final post-obstruent

It should be noted that in complex codas such as (5b), /ʀ/ and /l/ also pattern with fricative /s/, e.g. vaste ‘vast’ [vast].Footnote 7

Other continuants surface as the rightmost element of onsets following schwa elision, e.g. s'maine (semaine ‘week’), d'sous (dessous ‘under’). Likewise, /ʀ/ may appear as the leftmost element in a surface onset, e.g. r'venir (revenir ‘return’). Onset tokens such as these do not obviate the regularities noted in (5), however, as similar processes result in clusters of any consonant types, including plosives (e.g. d'venir/devenir ‘become’) and nasals (e.g. n'veu/neveu ‘nephew’, v'nu/venu ‘come-PART’). With the exception of certain loans and scholarly words (e.g. pseudonyme, tsar), which may be analysed as affricates, fricatives are only the rightmost member of surface branching onsets resulting from schwa elision.

Similarities seen in (5) notwithstanding, /ʀ/ and /l/ distributions are not identical. /ʀ/ occurs in complex onsets following any plosive, regardless of the latter's place specification, while /l/ does not occur following coronal plosives. Conversely, /l/ may follow coronal fricative /s/ in a complex onset (e.g. slave, [slav] ‘Slavic’). /ʀ/ only appears in these positions due to schwa elision (e.g. s'ra/sera ‘be-3Sing’), as well as in a handful of loanwords (e.g. Srebrenica). While many /sl/ tokens are indeed loanwords, these are arguably more frequent and better integrated in the contemporary lexicon than are /sʀ/ tokens. Tellingly, data from Russell Webb (Reference Russell Webb2002) indicates that approximant [] is heavily favoured in both isolated tokens and sequential speech when such onsets arise from schwa elision.

Additional phonological data regarding possible statuses of /ʀ/ are observed in several regional varieties. Côté (Reference Côté1997, Reference Côté2000, Reference Côté2004) presents expanded analyses of QF final cluster simplification, as in (6), which is blocked by /ʀ/.

  1. (6) QF Cluster Simplification (Côté, Reference Côté2004: 154-155, 168-169)

In coda clusters the final consonant is not elided when preceded by /ʀ/; according to her analysis, /ʀ/ patterns with /j/ and is distinct vis-à-vis /l/, nasals and fricatives.Footnote 8

/ʀ/ is an oddity in the phonological inventory of French, highlighting a central question of the present work. This is the only segment whose surface forms alternate along a continuum from fricative to approximant, also inheriting voice quality from the phonotactic environment. /ʀ/ is historically one of the few segments which have undergone a radical shift in place of articulation (i.e. from coronal to dorsal) and which is phonetically and phonologically variable in regional forms and registers. This is not to say that other French consonants are invariable. Liquids and nasals are also subject to voice assimilation, for example, but do not show fortis-lenis variability like /ʀ/. By the same token, intervocalic voiceless obstruents are often partially voiced (e.g. été [et ˯ e] ‘summer’) and final voiced obstruents may be partially devoiced (e.g. cède [sɛd ˳] ‘cede-3Sing’) (Russell Webb, Reference Russell Webb2002, Reference Russell Webb2004a). These data are, however, phonologically distinct from the above, as obstruents are contrastive for voicing (viz. aider [ede] ‘aid-INF’; cette [sɛt] ‘demonstrative-FEM’), whereas liquid voicing and /ʀ/ aperture are never contrastive.

Systemic associations become more complicated when similarities between /ʀ/ and glides are taken into account. Approximant [] and glides share similar phonetic profiles (vowel like formant structure with the highest resonance peaks below 3000 Hz), but, unlike both stable and derived glides, /ʀ/ is never a constituent of the syllable nucleus. And whereas glide formation involves changes to an underlying vowel, /ʀ/ lenition results in the partial vocalisation of a consonant (and complete vocalisation in certain regional forms and registers). It is also worth noting that [j] (the only glide licensed in codas) blocks vowel lengthening, whereas the opposite obtains in the case of coda [], as in (4).Footnote 9

1.3. Accounting for R: featural theory

It emerges from the above discussion that /ʀ/ is somewhat of a chameleon. Phonetically, it resembles either an obstruent or a sonorant, depending on its position and (for some speakers at least) speech style, while phonologically it participates in natural classes with fricatives and liquids, as well as with glides in some regional forms and registers. At the same time, /ʀ/ is unique vis-à-vis both obstruents and all other sonorants, which are resistant to aperture variation. The variability characterised by French /ʀ/ is hardly unique and, perhaps for that reason, interest in rhotics is recurrent within the phonological community. This section provides a brief overview of selected works which account for behaviour of /ʀ/ and the cross-linguistic grouping of rhotics in different languages. Those which do not address representational and/or featural issues (e.g. Lindau, Reference Lindau and Fromkin1985; Widdison, Reference Widdison1997) are not reviewed, although their consultation may be useful to readers unfamiliar with questions of rhotic classhood.

Most phonological accounts of French /ʀ/ surface patterns are founded on abstract representations constructed from a relatively small set of distinctive features, e.g. Chomsky and Halle (Reference Chomsky and Halle1968; see also Jakobson, Reference Jakobson1939; Jakobson, Fant and Halle, Reference Jakobson, Fant and Halle1952). A survey of phonological literature from the past three decades reveals that the putative structure assigned to /ʀ/ is far from uniform. Three examples, among the dozens of possible accounts, are provided in (7).

  1. (7) Featural Specifications of /ʀ/

Each underlying representation calls upon different units in its account of /ʀ/. Bibeau (Reference Bibeau1975) groups /ʀ/ with non obstruents by virtue of the feature and value [+vocalic]. On the other hand, Dell (Reference Dell1980) employs more traditional featural notation ([+sonorant]) to facilitate this classhood, whereas Brousseau and Nikiema (Reference Brousseau and Nikiema2001) provide that /ʀ/ is [+syllabic], alluding to autosegmental issues. Little discussion of /ʀ/ surface variability is undertaken by these authors, despite acknowledgement of the intermediate nature of /ʀ/. The exception to this is voice assimilation, attributed to sonorant underspecification for laryngeal features by Dell (Reference Dell1980: 57–58).

A handful of recent works address the phonological status of /ʀ/ with more limited focus. Côté is primarily interested in cluster simplification in QF, ignoring both distribution and representation (Reference Côté2004: 159). Her classification of /ʀ/ as a glide considers only final and pre-final positions, rather than onsets (in which she acknowledges /ʀ/ to pattern with fricatives). Colantoni and Steele (Reference Colantoni and Steele2005) also question the grouping of /ʀ/ and /l/ among liquids. Given historical asymmetries between the two sounds, as well as frequent vowel intrusion in QF complex onsets, they conclude that /ʀ/ is a fricative (2005). Russell Webb presents a distinct approach to French /ʀ/ representation, taking into account voice (Reference Russell Webb2004b) and aperture (Reference Russell Webb2004a), as well as the systemic relations between phonemes and their input (2002). He assumes that underling representations may be minimal, but proposes no formal structure to this end.

Looking beyond the question of French /ʀ/, a review of works focusing on the cross linguistic status of rhotics underscores the difficulties associated with the phonological status and content of these segments. Walsh Dickey (Reference Walsh Dickey1997) argues for an abstract class [rhotic] as a structural byproduct. According to her analysis, formalised along the lines of Clements’ (Reference Clements1985) feature geometric model, different rhotics share a secondary laminal node under the coronal sub-node. The relatively rich featural content of rhotics explains their cross-linguistic patterning, most notably resistance to palatalisation. In her analysis, dorsal rhotics are specified as having both coronal and dorsal place specification, as well as pharyngeal specification, as in Figure 1. Under such an interpretation, all rhotics contain a secondary laminal node and are structurally both coronal and dorsal.

Figure 1. Dorsal Rhotic Featural Representation (Walsh Dickey, Reference Walsh Dickey1997: 138, her 3.18, modified).

A distinct approach is taken by Wiese (Reference Wiese and Hall2001), who accounts for cross-linguistic patterning as the result of rhotics’ unique position on a sonority scale, derived from Clements (Reference Clements, Kingston and Beckman1990), as in (8).

  1. (8) Rhotic Sonority (Wiese, Reference Wiese and Hall2001: 355, emphasis of the original)

  2. obstruent < nasal < lateral < /ʀ/ < glide < vowel

Following his formalisation, rhotic patterning is attributable to this segment's situation intermediate to /l/ and glides. As a logical extension, distinct sonorities might be combined or interleaved if a given rhotic patterns with another segment (e.g. /l/) in a given language. It is, however, unclear what precise representational structure should be viewed as the foundation of rhotic sonority.

Van Oostendorp (Reference Oostendorp, van de Velde and van Hout2001) presents the case for the Dutch rhotic as a placeless, empty consonant in Brabant and Limburg dialects, also reviewing varieties of English. He argues that rhotics do not occupy a fixed point on the sonority scale, but are chameleonic with regard to consonantality, i.e. may or may not be a consonant and thus may pattern with sonorants. Variability is attributed to the relative ranking of the markedness constraint Final-C (‘a phrase (word) should end in a consonant’), and proposes that the Dutch rhotic is ‘particularly sensitive to this because /r/ is not predetermined for place’ (2001: 116, 114). In essence, van Oostendorp posits that rhotics lack featural content and that their surface forms are determined by the grammar. While he alludes to representational emptiness or minimalism in other languages, he makes no formal proposal to this end. A similar proposal is made by Rose (Reference Rose2003) concerning QF /ʀ/ based on child acquisition data, although it is unclear as to whether the placelessness of a child's representation changes over time, eventually including specification for dorsality or the like. It should be noted that, upon grammatical maturation, the vast majority of French speakers do not show place variation similar to that seen among Dutch speakers.

2. RETHINKING THE PHONOLOGY OF /ʀ/

The preceding section raises the question of the abstract representational structure of /ʀ/, as well as the implications of such structure for phonological classes and formal grammars. On the one hand, there is a good deal of phonological evidence (not to mention disciplinary tradition) associating /ʀ/ and /l/ under the umbrella of liquids. On the other hand, /ʀ/ surface forms are relatively dissimilar to those of /l/, more closely resembling fricatives or glides. This section focuses how to best conceive of the psychological reality of /ʀ/ in a formal model built around distinctive features.

One plausible approach to the structural question establishes distinct /ʀ/ phonemes, each of which includes a set of features motivating its grouping with /l/, glides, or fricatives. The assumption of multiple /ʀ/ phonemes is descriptively infelicitous, however, as it ignores complementary distribution and stylistic variation, not to mention derivational and historical associations, as well as evidence from dialects and regional forms. The presumption of distinct underlying representations (i.e. / / and / /) furthermore approaches the sharp edge of Occam's Razor, requiring both a heavier phonemic inventory and a more complicated grammar.

A second possibility assumes a unique underlying representation and accounts for input-output transformations within a formal grammar. Accordingly, /ʀ/ would be associable with either liquids or fricatives by virtue of its representation and grammatical processing would be responsible for the selection of surface forms. The question remains as to whether /ʀ/ is fundamentally an obstruent, i.e. a fricative which undergoes lenition, or a sonorant, i.e. an approximant which undergoes fortition. This approach results in a dilemma of circularity similar to the well known ‘chicken and egg’ problem, at least for speakers of convergent French. While eluding Occam's Razor – regardless of the selection of underlying representation – the choice between an underlying fricative or approximant relies on prioristic reasoning.Footnote 10

A final option, pursued here, establishes a single /ʀ/ representation absent of particular values relating to surface aperture and voicing. This approach is founded on emergent features and values and assumes that post lexical representations contain only those features which promote systemic contrast, i.e. the psychological reality of a segment includes only information pertinent to its distinction from other segments within the phonological system. A test of this approach is the ability of a grammar to select attested outputs without either over-predicting, i.e. providing outputs which do not occur, or under-predicting, i.e. failing to account for a surface true output. The remainder of this section is given to discussion of non innate featural theory and the promotion of representational minimalism. A formal grammatical account, in which surface forms are predicted by constraint interaction, is proposed in a subsequent section.

2.1. Theoretical Background

The traditional and nearly unanimous assumption (see e.g. Clements and Hume, Reference Clements, Hume and Goldsmith1995: 245) that features are universal has come under increasing scrutiny in recent years. Boersma, for example, acknowledges the quasi-universal nature of articulatory and perceptual space, but denies the universality of featural values. Accordingly, the phonological building blocks of a language are defined by use and segmental representations include only relevant values (Reference Boersma1998: 172). A similar approach is taken by Mielke, who argues against the primacy of phonetic universals in the establishment of features, positing the latter as ‘abstract categories based on generalisations that emerge from phonological patterns’ (Reference Mielke2004: 7). According to EFT, features are born of generalisations made during acquisition, based on phonological evidence, and are only indirectly grounded in diachronic change or phonetic form (2004: 118–121, 126–127). The apparent universality of certain featural distinctions such as place (e.g. [labial]) or manner (e.g. [continuant]) is motivated by segmental participation in phonologically active classes, and putative cross linguistic universals are considered epiphenomenal, rather than innate (2004: 141–142).

Focusing on the question of rhotic cross linguistic patterning, Russell Webb (Reference Russell Webb2002) begins with the presupposition that features are not universal, based in large part on Boersma (Reference Boersma1998). Similar to EFT, he proposes that features derive from learned patterns of similarity and difference within a system, essentially from the holistic productive and receptive input made available to language users. It is hypothesised that a given segment may be defined according to intrinsic features (e.g. backness or dorsality), as well as the absence of features (e.g. the lack of high frequency noise), both of which provide for systemic contrast. Following the stated denial of featural universality, the rhotic of one language cannot be directly defined in reference to another or its form motivated by cross linguistic patterns. Cross linguistic comparison may be made between inventories and phonological dynamics (i.e. systems of similarity and distinction), rather than unique segments, but this only indirectly establishes rhotic classhood (2002: 163–175).

The implications of abandoning innate features are far reaching, especially in cases such as the one at hand. Rather than depend upon a fixed set of universal phonological units, requiring that a segment be fit to the set, it is possible to construct a phonological representation dependent on surface data, essentially deriving the feature set from surface forms and their observable phonetic and phonological characteristics. Unlike approaches founded on innate features and values, where function is induced from form, an emergent approach provides that form is deduced from function (function being allegorically understood as linguistic performance). From this perspective, it is perfectly feasible that one speaker's /ʀ/ is distinct from that of another, even if both are speakers of a common language. Similarly, the features and values assigned to the rhotic of one language or regional variety of a language need not be assumed of others. In the following section, it is argued that the chameleonic nature of /ʀ/ in convergent French leads to the establishment of a minimally specified abstract representation, i.e. one which contains little featural information. It is further proposed that different surface forms and distributions seen in regional forms of French may lead to the emergence of variant representations in some cases.

Before examining emergent features and their application to French, brief mention should be made of the repercussions of this approach for contemporary phonological analysis. The presumption that features are not innate poses a distinct problem for OT, especially given its principle of Richness of the Base (ROTB; Prince and Smolensky, Reference Prince and Smolensky1993: 207). This provides that no constraint holds at the level of input and implies that all grammars should contend with all possible input. At the heart of this matter are representational underspecification and the status of redundant phonological features in the input. Itô, Mester and Padgett (Reference Itô, Mester and Padgett1995) demonstrate that OT's non-derivational framework denies any serial ordering or minimalism, providing that underspecification is an output – rather than an input – property. This approach to indeterminate representations is distinct from that taken by Kirchner (Reference Kirchner1998) and Russell Webb (Reference Russell Webb2004a), who argue that input may be assumed to lack structural information as a means of avoiding circuitousness (see also Boersma Reference Boersma1998). This implies a weak understanding of ROTB. At a lexical level, input is unconstrained, but productive input, such as that under discussion here, is finite. The present approach assumes full specification at a lexical evaluative level, which is filtered by declarative constraints (see Myers, Reference Myers1999; Gess, Reference Gess and Holt2003, Reference Gess2004). While full treatment of this question far surpasses the scope of the present work, it should be noted that emergent features are compatible with OT and the proposal made here does not, a priori, invalidate ROTB, although this is necessarily considered a theorem, rather than a principle of linguistic production (see e.g. Hale & Reiss, Reference Hale and Reiss1998; Reiss, Reference Reiss2000; see also McMahon, Reference McMahon2000, Reference McMahon2007).

2.2. Emergent R

In order to contextualise discussion of /ʀ/ and related segments, the consonantal phoneme inventory of French is provided in Table 1, noting place and manner characteristics. A first step in accounting for /ʀ/ particularity is the establishment of features and featural values promoting its distinction and similarity vis-à-vis other segments. Given the phonological and phonetic evidence confronting language users and focusing for the moment on convergent French, a series of unary place and binary manner and major class features is employed. Foremost among these is [vowel], accounting for segments which may feature in syllabic nuclei (note that glides are specified as [+vowel] and [+sonorant]).

Table 1. French Consonant Inventory: adapted from Walker (Reference Walker2001: ix)

Place (e.g. [coronal]) and most manner features (e.g. [nasal]) are straightforwardly posited from the assumptions outlined above, i.e. that language users posit features and the values assigned to them based on their acquisition and use of the language. For example, a user is attentive to the contrast between poux [pu] ‘lice’, tout [tu] ‘all’ and cou [ku] ‘neck,’ leading to distinct place values. Likewise, the feature [vowel], is assigned to segments which appear in syllabic nuclei (i.e. vowels and semi-vowels) and which thus constitute the necessary element of a rhyme. Voicing is assumed to be specified only for those segments that are contrastive in this regard, i.e. fricatives and plosives. This is presented as [±voice], although emergent values might also be subsumed under privative headings such as [voice] and [spreadglottis] (see e.g. Jessen and Ringen, Reference Jessen and Ringen2002).

The manner feature [sonorant] is posited based on phonotactic evidence available to users, notably the ability of some segments to take part in complex onsets and codas. From this it is assumed that a language user distinguishes between nasals and liquids on the one hand and fricatives and plosives on the other. The former are frequently in complex syllabic structures and are relatively unrestricted in this regard; by contrast, the latter are far more restricted in their distribution (the question of /s/ not withstanding). This represents a departure from traditional definitions of sonorance, which derive from notions of spontaneous voicing (e.g. Chomsky and Halle, Reference Chomsky and Halle1968: 302) or aperture (e.g. Clements, Reference Clements, Kingston and Beckman1990). The major manner feature [obstruent] is associated with the class of segments whose surface forms require constriction in the oral cavity. Complementary to the definition of [sonorant], [obstruent] is further assumed to emerge from the relative restriction placed on a sound's distribution and potential to constitute complex syllabic structures. Finally, [continuant] is assumed to emerge from the distributional, articulatory and perceptual properties of sounds that do not require an oral cavity constriction. This is similar to Chomsky and Halle, who ascribe [continuant] to segments having an unimpeded oral airstream, providing uninterrupted source noise (1968: 317–318), although considerations outlined below add precision as to the nature of constriction in question.

A preliminary featural specification of French continuants is given in Table 2. In this matrix, values are assigned following Radical Underspecification Theory (Archangeli, Reference Archangeli1988; Stemberger, Reference Stemberger1991; Cohn, Reference Cohn1995), according to which an underlying representation contains only those specifications promoting distinction, with surface values being filled in by the grammar. As noted by question marks in Table 2, the status of /ʀ/ and /l/ with regard to [continuant] requires additional discussion. In the case of convergent French /ʀ/, language users are confronted with a sound which can never be in a syllable nucleus, i.e. a sound which does not contain the value [+vowel], but which differs from all other non-vowel segments as it does not involve specific constriction or closure target and is contextually variable for both manner and voice quality. Importantly, only /ʀ/ implies surface forms whose aperture or major manner characteristics track closely to context. Distinction of /ʀ/ from fricatives and /l/ derives from the latter sounds’ relatively stable articulatory targets and contextual aperture invariability.

Table 2. French Consonant Inventory: features and values

The status of /ʀ/ and, especially, /l/ with regard to [continuant] has long been a subject of debate in the phonological community (see e.g. van de Weijer, Reference Weijer1995) and serves as an important illustration of EFT (Mielke, Reference Mielke2004: 241–246, 267–270). In the present work, [continuant] is posited based on vowel lengthening and cluster restrictions, indirectly reflecting phonetic considerations. In the first instance, as in (4a), it is noted that vowels are lengthened before voiced fricatives and /ʀ/, but not before voiceless fricatives and /l/. Assuming that differential patterning involving fricatives is due to anticipatory laryngeal constriction impeding vowel lengthening, the crucial distinction lies between /ʀ/ and /l/. /ʀ/ surface forms do not result in complete contact between the articulator and articulatory target, allowing a central airstream channel, similar to that of vowels. In contrast, surface forms of /l/ (as well as /j/) involve mid-sagittal closure and a lateral air channel, more closely resembling that of plosives (Hirose, Reference Hirose, Hardcastle and Laver1997; Stevens, Reference Stevens1998: 543–555). Additional phonological evidence for /l/ specification as [−continuant] is gleaned from its distribution within onset and coda clusters, as in (5). /ʀ/ is more widely licensed in triple branching onset clusters as compared to /l/. Confronted with phonological input similar to this, it is plausible that learners posit /ʀ/ and fricatives as [+continuant] and /l/ and [−continuant]. These considerations lead to the representational values given in Table 3, where /l/ is specified as [−continuant, +sonorant] and /ʀ/ as [+continuant, +sonorant].

Table 3. /ʀ/ and /l/ specification

A visual model accounting for phonological classes is provided in Figure 2. Using only the values [+sonorant], [+continuant], [+obstruent] and [+vowel], it is possible to account for the phonetic and phonological patterns discussed in §1. Consistent with Table 3, a liquid is any non-nasal, non-vowel segment containing the value [+sonorant], whereas a fricative is any non-sonorant specified as [+obstruent, +continuant]. /ʀ/ patterns with fricatives, as both are specified as [+continuant], while the conjunction of /ʀ/, /l/ and glides (exemplified by /j/) is attributed to the value [+sonorant]. /ʀ/ may also be represented in a feature geometric model, following Clements (Reference Clements1985), as in Figure 3. It is worth noting that minimalism in this instance should not be conflated with emptiness, akin to van Oostendorp (Reference Oostendorp, van de Velde and van Hout2001) or Rose (Reference Rose2003), as /ʀ/ is specified for a major root, as well as place and manner features. Furthermore, /ʀ/ does not demonstrate the phonological behavior characteristic of so called empty consonants, e.g. epenthetic status or major place variation, as in Dutch, where rhotics can be coronal or dorsal.

Figure 2. Phonological classes.

Figure 3. Feature geometric representation of /ʀ/.

3. CONSTRAINTS AND THE PRODUCTION GRAMMAR

This section tests the minimal representations articulated above and develops a production grammar in which /ʀ/ surface forms are successfully predicted, with particular attention given to convergent French data in (1); additional surface variation (e.g. coda elision) is not covered in any detail, although the proposal may be readily adapted to these ends (see e.g. Russell Webb, Reference Russell Webb2004a).

The grammatical model outlined here captures linguistic competence involved in speech production. Like all OT grammars, the proposal makes use of two constraints types: faithfulness, statements governing the formation relationship between the input and output; and markedness, statements concerning output form. Here, constraints target the adjudication of abstract underlying structure, i.e. the correspondences between underlying representations and surface structural descriptions (faithfulness), and the productive implications of surface structural descriptions, themselves (markedness). The proposal combines constraints militating for the preservation of underlying phonological information alongside constraints promoting modifications to features and feature values in response to the tension between articulatory economy and perceptual salience. This formalisation of productive competence results in a significant blurring of the phonetics – phonology divide, advocating the integration into the grammar of constraints derived from phonetic factors.Footnote 11

As it concerns the questions at hand, faithfulness is captured by Pres(erve) (Kirchner Reference Kirchner1998), a cover constraint promoting the maintenance of input structural information in the output, as in (9). Although similar in its effect to correspondence constraints (e.g. Max(imality)), Pres is formally and implicationally distinct. Where correspondence refers (in most analyses) to the faithful preservation of segments, Pres refers to the faithful adjudication of segmental featural content.

  1. (9) Pres: input information is preserved in the output, i.e. features and featural values of the input should be conveyed in the output

Pres may be targeted to specific features, e.g. Pres(obstruent), which provides that underlying values for obstruence should be present in the output. It should be noted that a complementary faithfulness consideration militating against the presence of information in the output that is not contained in the underlying specification, e.g. Dep(endence), is not included in the present analysis. Given the minimal content of inputs in the case of this segment, it can be assumed that pertinent Dep constraints (e.g. Dep(obstruent)) are relatively low ranked and are therefore without effect on the selection of productive output.

As proposed below, markedness constraints are motivated by two factors: productive effort, understood as an abstract measure of muscular force required for articulator displacement over time and the relative precision required of articulators during displacement (Boersma, Reference Boersma1998 et seq.; Kirchner, Reference Kirchner1998: 39–44, 50–52, Reference Kirchner, Hayes, Steriade and Kirchner2004); and perceptual salience, implying the relative perceptibility of candidate surface structural descriptions (Côté, Reference Côté2000; Steriade, Reference Steriade2001; Wright, Reference Wright, Hume and Johnson2001, Reference Wright, Hayes, Kirchner and Steriade2004). In the present analysis, constraints referring to articulatory forces are subsumed under -Effort, adapted from Kirchner's Lazy, which states that ‘articulatory effort should be minimized’ (1998: 38, his 2–1).Footnote 12 Constraints referring to perceptual augmentation are modeled along the lines of Smith (Reference Smith2002, Reference Smith2004; see also Wright, Reference Wright, Hume and Johnson2001, Reference Wright, Hayes, Kirchner and Steriade2004), providing that productive output should be enhanced in prominent positions.

A primary markedness consideration targets the relative articulatory effort involved in the abduction or adduction of vocal cords. In a given sequence of segments X1 X2 X3, where X1 and X3 have specific glottal targets (i.e. are specified for [±voice]), positive effort reduction obtains when X2 takes on the glottal features of one or both adjacent sounds. At the same time, subglottal pressure decreases in the production of word final and, especially, utterance final obstruents. Two factors are at play in the promotion of vocal fold abduction: an increase in supraglottal pressure caused by occlusion and a corresponding decrease in sub-glottal pressure, perhaps exacerbated by the end of pulmonic egression and the corresponding requirement of pulmonic ingression. With final occlusives, unconstrained pressure differentiation provides for the passive abduction or spreading of the glottis, impeding vocal fold vibration (Westbury and Keating, Reference Westbury and Keating1986: 156–157; Kirchner, Reference Kirchner1998: 56–58). Final devoicing as a means of effort reduction is also supported by data demonstrating that post vocalic instantiations of voiced consonants are longer than those of voiceless ones (Dunn, Reference Dunn1993; Maddieson, Reference Maddieson, Hardcastle and Laver1997). In this instance, effort reduction is predicated upon the assumption that articulatory effort is greater during a relatively longer occlusion.

Taken as a cover constraint promoting passive voicing, or the lack of specific gestural control of glottal muscles, -Effort(glot) penalises active control of the glottis, as in (10).

  1. (10) Passive Voicing (Russell Webb, Reference Russell Webb2004b: 144–145)

  2. -Effort(glot): do not actively control glottal gestures (allow passive voicing and devoicing)

The ranking of -Effort(glot) above faithfulness provides that no contraction of laryngeal muscles should be made to counter the passive glottal adduction or abduction, as might obtain in intervocalic or word-final positions, respectively (Hirose, Reference Hirose, Hardcastle and Laver1997; Steriade, Reference Steriade1997; Bradley and Delforge, Reference Bradley, Delforge, Arteaga and Gess2006). For a hypothetical ranking -Effort(glot) » Pres(voice), voice assimilation obtains in all circumstances. Assuming Pres(voice) » -Effort(glot), only segments having no input value [± voice] are affected.

A second type of markedness concerns the aperture of output candidates. In this instance, perceptual considerations play a primary role, namely the availability of auditory cues and their relative robustness (see Steriade, Reference Steriade1997, Reference Steriade2001; Wright, Reference Wright, Hume and Johnson2001, Reference Wright, Hayes, Kirchner and Steriade2004; Smith Reference Smith2002, Reference Smith2004). Here, it is important to contrast the cues contained in approximant [] with those of fricative [] (Rialland, Reference Rialland, Wetzels and Sezer1986; Ladefoged and Maddieson, Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996; Russell Webb, Reference Russell Webb2002: 115–122, Reference Russell Webb2004a). In the case of approximant [], these include the presence of source noise and diffuse spectral peaks, resembling weak vowel formants. In the case of fricative [], cues include more intense and more distributed resonance peaks with significant high frequency noise at or above 3000 Hz.

Additional evidence for the relative output salience of different rhotic surface forms comes from studies showing an increased sensitivity to transitions of greater amplitude prior to vowels, especially in syllabic onsets (Wright, Reference Wright1996, Reference Wright, Hume and Johnson2001; Delgutte, Reference Delgutte, Hardcastle and Laver1997). These considerations do not ipso facto result in [] being perceptually better output than []; rather, the former provide more robust cues and greater perceptual salience, increasing the likelihood of cochlear stimulation, as they produce more intense noise spectra. Further cues to the contextual perceptibility of the different output candidates are found in transitions occurring either pre- or post-vocalically, and are more abundant in transitions resulting from distally greater articulatory movements (e.g. from a close aperture to a vowel). In the case of pre-vocalic /ʀ/, [] is more discriminable and less confusable than []. The putative knowledge underlying the preferential production of fortis variants in prominent positions is captured by Onset(obs), in (11).Footnote 13 This states that segments in the onset should be obstruents, i.e. they should have specific articulatory targets which result in more robust perceptual cues.

  1. (11) Onset Fortition

  2. Onset(obs): segments in the onset should be obstruents

Onset(obs) is supported by empirical data from other languages. In many dialects of Dutch, for example, rhotics may surface as a fricative, tap, trill or approximant, while more sonorant outputs (i.e. approximants) are only seen in coda (van Reenen, Reference Reenen1994; van de Velde Reference Velde1994). Likewise, the German rhotic surfaces as a low-back vowel [ɐ] in codas, but as a fricative in onsets (Hall, Reference Hall1992, Reference Hall2000; Wiese Reference Wiese and Hall2001).

Onset fortition does not apply without exception in French, as /ʀ/ often surfaces as [] in intervocalic positions. Here, articulatory considerations favour effort reduction in vowel-consonant-vowel transitions, frequent sites of consonant lenition (Bauer, Reference Bauer1988; Kirchner, Reference Kirchner1998: 180–213, Reference Kirchner, Hayes, Steriade and Kirchner2004). This is captured in -Effort(V_V), as in (10). Fricative [] is relatively more effortful than approximant [], as the former involves greater displacement from a neutral position and more precise gestural coordination (Delattre, Reference Delattre1971; Ladefoged and Maddieson, Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996: 217–230).

  1. (12) Intervocalic Effort Minimisation

  2. -Effort(V_V): minimise articulatory effort in intervocalic position

Due to its minimal representational content, /ʀ/ is effectively the only consonant of modern French sensitive to -Effort(V_V), reflected in the ranking Pres » -Effort(V_V); other consonants are predicted to remain blind to intervocalic lenition. Despite a relative reduction in acoustic intensity, intervocalic perceptual robustness is assured by formant transitions and source resonance, offering additional cues regarding segment identity (Steriade, Reference Steriade2001; Wright, Reference Wright, Hayes, Kirchner and Steriade2004). Essentially, intervocalic positions permit the conspiracy of articulatory and perceptual factors, providing that lenition may occur and that less effortful gestural sequences may be realised with little perceptual penalty. The interaction of -Effort(V_V) with Onset(obs) additionally allows the grammar to neatly capture the effects of style shifting or emphatic stress, as might obtain in a hyper (careful or emphatic) speech style.

3.1. Accounting for Surface Forms

Constraint interaction in French highlights two important points concerning the formalisation of linguistic competence in a production grammar. Firstly, the representational minimalism of /ʀ/ renders it particularly sensitive to effort reduction and positional augmentation. Secondly, other segments, whose representations are relatively richer, are unaffected by these factors. Discussion and tableaux presented in this section concern the relative ranking of productive markedness and faithfulness, highlighting the effect of these on /ʀ/ inputs and their lack of effect on other segments, notably fricatives, /l/ and glides.

The relative ranking of markedness constraints is noted in Tables 4a through d (here, voiceless /ʀ/ is represented by [χ] in the output). In Table 4a, only [gy] satisfies -Effort(glot); approximant and vocalised candidates are eliminated by Onset(obs). The converse obtains in Table 4b, where the optimal output contains a voiceless fricative, this a consequence of the relative ranking of -Effort(glot). Interaction in Table 4c establishes the crucial ordering of −Effort(V_V), −Effort(glot) » Onset(obs), predicting the attested output [aab] for input /aʀab/ and blocking onset fortition in intervocalic environments (e.g. *[aab]). Here, the grammar predicts an outcome mirroring data from Russell Webb (Reference Russell Webb2002) and Tranel (Reference Tranel1987). In syllabic codas, as in Table 4d, the grammar predicts that the minimally specified /ʀ/ will be interpolated as an approximant [].

Table 4a. /ʀ/ in complex onset following voiced obstruent, grue ‘crane’

Table 4b. /ʀ/ in complex onset following voiceless obstruent, cru ‘raw’

Table 4c. Intervocalic /ʀ/, arabe ‘arab’

Table 4d. Word-final /ʀ/, perte ‘loss’

Table 5 presents an alternative possibility involving the ranking of −Effort(V_V) vis-à-vis Onset(obs). This grammar predicts equally optimal outputs, [aab] and [aab]. The question is purposefully left unanswered as to whether these constraints should be coranked or variably ranked, following proposals concerning output variation, such as Auger (Reference Auger2001) and the possibility of floating constraints or Espinosa (Reference Espinosa2004), who follows integrates work by Boersma (Reference Boersma1997), Hayes (Reference Hayes, Dekkers, van der Leeuw and van de Weijer2000) and Boersma & Hayes (Reference Boersma and Hayes2001) by situating competing constraints within a constriction band (see also Anttila, Reference Anttila, Hinskens, van Hout and Wetzels1997, Reference Anttila, Chambers, Trudgill and Schilling-Estes2002). These sources consider style shifting in registers or regional forms of a heteronymous language, while the present work endeavours to articulate a framework within which either constriction bands or floating constraints can be accommodated.

Table 5. Intervocalic /ʀ/, arabe ‘arab’: Co-optimality

Because the /ʀ/ input representations contain no information relative to manner of articulation, Pres has little effect on the selection of productive output structural descriptions. This constraint proves crucial for prediction of attested outputs involving /l/, /j/ and fricative inputs, however, as shown in Tables 6a through e. Following Tables 2 and 3, fricatives, glides and /l/ have relatively richer phonological content. Highly ranked Pres is crucial to output selection in these instances, as this constraint militates against the non-correspondence of input features to output form. In Tables 6a and b, Pres violations eliminate candidates in which the feature [obstruent] is unfaithful to input values. Pres also rules out possible changes in voicing and militates against infidelity to input featural information, e.g. candidates having a fricated semi-vowel, as in Table 6c, or those containing a lenited fricative, as in Tables 6d and e.

Table 6a. Word-initial /l/, blanche ‘white-FEM’

Table 6b. Word-final /l/, calte ‘flee-3S’

Table 6c. /j/, paye ‘pay-3S’

Table 6d. /s/, peste ‘plague’

Table 6e. /s/, plaça ‘place-3S/PAST’

3.2. Regional Variation

This section discusses the compatibility of the proposed grammatical model with data from regional forms of French. While it is far beyond the scope of the present article to address all regional variation, it is noted that the approach to both emergent representation and grammatical formalism is amenable to different input and divergent constraint rankings.

One possible solution to the question raised by alternate surface forms is predicated on the assumption that the phonological /ʀ/ representations may be more richly specified in some instances. The lack of aperture allophony in BF, for example, might be accounted for by /ʀ/ being specified as [+obstruent], as would obtain were learners to include /ʀ/ in the category of sounds having specific articulatory targets involving oral cavity stricture. This follows from data in (2) and the discussion of the feature [obstruent] in §2. Assuming for the purpose of discussion that the underlying representation of BF /ʀ/ contains the values [dorsal], [+continuant] and [+obstruent], no changes to the grammar articulated in Tables 4 or 6 need be made; this input would not be insensitive to phonetically-based constraints such as -Effort(V_V), as these are dominated by Pres. Questions of register shifting notwithstanding, this approach requires that BF speakers always produce /ʀ/ as a fricative and that positional or stylistic variation is absent, a possibility supported by the cited sources as well as anecdotal evidence. Assumption of more richly specified representations does not account for the surface forms of /ʀ/ in QF or STF, however, indicating that the difference between these and other varieties French may be due to grammatical distinction (i.e. a constraint ranking that is different from that given in Tables 4 through 6).

In her analysis of QF cluster simplification, Côté (Reference Côté2004) refers to the relative perceptual salience of consonants in the output. She notes that consonants adjacent to a vowel or to a consonant which is vowel like are maintained. This is explicated in reference to the acoustic transition from one segment to another in output, which either provides cues to consonant identity (e.g. from vowel or approximant to stop) or does not inhibit the transmission of segment internal cues (e.g. from vowel or approximant to fricative). She also shows that elision of plosives is more frequent and is more likely in the case of input sequences having the same place of articulation (2004: 178–180). This is analysed in an OT grammar comprising constraints targeting the optimal position of consonants (2004: 185–190).

While the approach to features and featural specification in Table 3 and Figure 2 is distinct from that included in Côté (Reference Côté2004), emergent, minimal specification of the input is not incompatible with her analysis (theoretical and formal differences regarding features notwithstanding). Following the proposal, post-vocalic /ʀ/ - obstruent clusters do not violate markedness constraints promoting output well-formedness as a measure of perceptual salience. /ʀ/ minimal specification presents the added advantage of capturing both the vowel like qualities of /ʀ/ (i.e., /ʀ/ is not an obstruent, but is a continuant), which are directly called upon in her perceptual account of cluster simplification (Côté, Reference Côté2004: 176). Minimal representation also facilitates the distinction of /ʀ/ vis-à-vis /l/, accounting for the differential treatment of clusters containing /ʀ/ and /l/.

A final issue that can be addressed is the vocalization of /ʀ/ in syllabic codas in QF and STF. Côté (Reference Côté2004) analyses QF vocalisation as the effect of markedness constraints promoting featural similarity among adjacent segments, input-output correspondence targeting featural specification (e.g. stop vs. non-stop) and position (notably vowel adjacency), and output-input uniformity, militating against multiple output-input correspondents. Data such as (3a) are predicted in her analysis by the ranking of Max-C (‘input consonants should have an output correspondent’) above Uniformity-V (‘No vowel in the output corresponds to itself and another segment in the input’) and C↔V (‘a consonant is adjacent to a vowel’), the latter of which are co-ranked (2004: 187–190). While the featural terminology and formalisation are different from those advanced above, a minimally specified /ʀ/ representation is compatible with Côté’s analysis. The distinction between QF and STF vis-à-vis French is captured in the relative ranking of either Pres (as in Table 4a) or Uniformity.

4. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

The present analysis argues that /ʀ/ representation is, in many cases, minimal, i.e. lacking in abstract structural information regarding manner of articulation. In a production grammar including constraints promoting effort minimization and perceptual augmentation, alongside those advocating the preservation of input information in the output, /ʀ/ surface forms are successfully predicted (Tables 4a through 4d and 5), indicating that the abstract weight (or lack thereof) of this phoneme makes it particularly sensitive to phonetically based constraints. By contrast, phonemes having more richly specified representations are insensitive to these forces, establishing both the uniqueness of /ʀ/ and the relative stability of other consonants within the phonological inventory of French (Tables 6a through 6e). The minimal representation ascribed to /ʀ/ responds to several questions, most notably whether this segment should be considered a liquid, approximant or fricative. This approach advocates the view that, while /ʀ/ is congruous to each of these, it is fundamentally distinct. This conceptualisation of underlying phonological structure offers a compelling formalisation of the relationship of /ʀ/ to other continuant consonants, obviating the otherwise necessary choice between competing underlying statuses (i.e. between the view of /ʀ/ as either fundamentally fricative or liquid).

Beyond the immediate concern of French /ʀ/ phonological status, the present analysis also demonstrates that minimal specification can account for data in related grammars, most notably those of regional forms and dialects. While /ʀ/ may be more richly specified, e.g. in BF, it is also possible to account for regional surface particularities within the grammar. A minimally specified /ʀ/ is compatible with analysis of QF in Côté (Reference Côté2004) and other regional forms of French, where /ʀ/ distribution and phonotactics are conceived of as the output effect of constraints referring to cue preservation and output-input uniformity.

The present work suggests additional questions specific to historical change and dialectal divergence which are not addressed here. For instance, has this phoneme always been minimally specified in French and, if so, how should particular phonological evolutions (e.g. mid vowel lowering, the shift from apical to dorsal places of articulation) be described and explained? How might the proposal contend with varieties of French in which /ʀ/ is apical (e.g. rural forms) or where /ʀ/ has historically alternated between apical and dorsal sounds? And what is the form and phonological status of /ʀ/ in languages closely related to French, such as Haitian and Mauritian, in which /ʀ/ surfaces as [ʁ] and [w] in onsets and fails to surface in codas? Continued examination of these questions serves not only to enrich our understanding of French, but to nourish debate about and research into related questions in other languages.

Footnotes

*

I am grateful to three anonymous referees for suggestions in making this paper more coherent. Thanks also to B. Tranel and B. Bullock for useful comments on earlier versions of this work. It goes without saying that all remaining shortcomings are my own.

1 Throughout this paper, /ʀ/ is used in reference to the French rhotic without further place or manner specification.

2 The term convergent French is used to refer to varieties lacking in particular regional characteristics, i.e. those used in most national or international media and those which are typically the object of second language instruction. While categorical distinction between varieties is difficult, if not impossible to make, this and other terms are used for purposes of discussion. It is noted that the proposed analysis is not dependent on this classification and is, indeed, amenable to a wide range of speech data.

3 Data made available by ongoing research under the aegis of Phonologie du français contemporain (www.project-pfc.net) contains several tokens of intervocalic []. It is unclear whether these examples result from emphatic style, are lexically specified or are more idiosyncratic in nature.

4 Several instances of QF /l/ vocalization are also noted; these involve the optional lengthening of the preceding vowel and are limited to /lm/ clusters, of which there are relatively few (Côté, Reference Côté2004: 168–169, 170–171); /l/ vocalization is not noted in STF. Walker (Reference Walker1984) reports several instances of systematic /ʀ/ and /l/ elision. Similarly, in a sociolinguistic study of Franco-Ontarian, Thomas indicates that /ʀ/ erasure is linked to register and speech style, among other variables (Reference Thomas1986: 82–91). It is not clear the extent to which such erasure is lexical.

5 STF /ʀ/ vocalization is similar to that of Maurician French (Baggioni and de Robillard, Reference Baggioni and de Robillard1990: 86–87), as well as the regional French of Réunion (Ledegen and Bordal, Reference Ledegen and Bordal2005, PC).

6 Nasals are also licensed in the latter position, although this is limited to a restricted set of lexical items (e.g. hypne ‘hypnus’) and derivational suffixes (e.g. communisme ‘communism’).

7 Coda /f/ is noted in a small number of lexical items, many of which are loanwords of Germanic origin (e.g. loft).

8 While providing that cluster reduction is primarily attributable to perceptually motivated constraints, Côté (Reference Côté2004) notes the intervention of lexical factors in the case of /ld/ simplification (e.g. solde ‘sale’ cannot be simplified, whereas calme ‘calm’ may be).

9 Several studies of second language learners of French (e.g. Colantoni and Steele Reference Colantoni and Steele2007) note that speakers target the fricative like qualities of /ʀ/ in all positions. Interestingly, first language studies indicate complementary outcomes, where /ʀ/ is often elided or vocalized until a relatively late period, although this is not always the case (see e.g. Edwards, Reference Edwards1973; Kehoe and Hilaire-Debove, Reference Kehoe and Hilaire-Debove2004; Rose, Reference Rose2000, Reference Rose2003; Goad and Rose Reference Goad, Rose, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004). The extent to which preference for the fricative in second language learning is due to pedagogical practice is unclear, as is the influence of perception and habituation on child acquirers.

10 It should be noted that acquisition data are not particularly helpful in this regard, as child language indicates that /ʀ/ patterns with both fricatives and approximants (see note 9). Likewise, historical data is not particularly enlightening as it concerns the status of /ʀ/, as positional fortis-lenis variations are seen throughout the history of French (see e.g. Delattre, Reference Delattre and Delattre1966; Bourciez & Bourciez, Reference Bourciez and Bourciez1974; Giauque, Reference Giauque1976; Lozachmeur, Reference Lozachmeur1976; Straka, 1979).

11 The reader is referred to Hayes, Kirchner and Steriade (Reference Hayes and Kirchner2004), Hayes (Reference Hayes and McCarthy2004) and Lindblom (Reference Lindblom2006) for more complete discussion of the issues surrounding the phonetics – phonology divide and the place of phonetically based constraints within contemporary phonological theory.

12 Similar constraints have been formalized as *Gesture (Boersma, Reference Boersma1998) and Conserve Articulatory Effort (Gess, Reference Gess and Holt2003, Reference Gess2004), inter alia.

13 Onset(obs) is closely related to Prince and Smolensky's principle of the best onset (Reference Prince and Smolensky1993: 99–101).

References

REFERENCES

Anttila, A. (1997). Deriving Variation from Grammar. In: Hinskens, F., van Hout, R. and Wetzels, W. L. (eds.), Variation, Change and Phonological Theory. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 3568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anttila, A. (2002). Variation in Phonological Theory. In: Chambers, J. K., Trudgill, P. and Schilling-Estes, N. (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 206243.Google Scholar
Archangeli, D. (1988). Aspects of Underspecification Theory. Phonology, 5: 183207.Google Scholar
Auger, J. (2001). Phonological variation and Optimality Theory: Evidence from word-internal vowel epenthesis in Vimeu Picard. Language Variation and Change, 13: 253303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baetens Beardsmore, H. (1971). Le français régional de Bruxelles. Brussels: Presses de l'Université.Google Scholar
Baggioni, D. and de Robillard, D. (1990). Ile Maurice: Une francophonie paradoxale. Paris: L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
Bauer, L. (1988). What is lenition? Linguistics, 24: 381392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bibeau, G. (1975). Introduction à la phonologie générative du français. Montreal: Didier.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. (1997). How we learn variation, optimality, and probability. Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences, 21: 4358.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. (1998). Functional Phonology: formalizing the interactions between articulatory and perceptual drives. The Hague: LOT/Holland Academic Graphics.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. and Hayes, B. (2001). Empirical tests of the gradual learning algorithm. Linguistic Inquiry, 32.1: 4586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourciez, E. and Bourciez, P.. (1974). Phonétique française: Etude historique. Paris: Klincksieck.Google Scholar
Bradley, T. and Delforge, M. (2006). Systemic contrast and the diachrony of Spanish sibilant voicing. In: Arteaga, D. and Gess, R. (eds.), Historical Romance Linguistics: Retrospectives and Perspectives. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 1952.Google Scholar
Brousseau, A.-M. and Nikiema, E. (2001). Phonologie et morphologie du français. St. Laurent: Fides.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. (1968). The Sound Pattern of English. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1985). The geometry of phonological features. Phonology Yearbook, 2: 225252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1990). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In: Kingston, J. and Beckman, M. E. (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 283333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, G. N. and Hume, E. (1995). The internal organization of speech sounds. In: Goldsmith, J. A. (ed.), the Handbook of Phonological Theory. London: Blackwell, pp. 245306.Google Scholar
Cohn, A. (1995). Privativity, underspecification, and consequences for an adequate theory of phonetic implementation. Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory, 10: 3965.Google Scholar
Colantoni, L. and Steele, J. (2005). Liquid asymmetries in French and Spanish. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics, 24: 114.Google Scholar
Colantoni, L. and Steele, J. (2007). Acquiring /ʁ/ in context. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 29: 381406.Google Scholar
Côté, M.-H. (1997). Phonetic salience and the OCP in Coda Cluster Reduction. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society: the Main Session, 33: 5771.Google Scholar
Côté, M.-H. (2000). Consonant cluster phonotactics: a Perceptual approach. PhD Dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst.Google Scholar
Côté, M.-H. (2004). Consonant cluster simplification in Quebec French. PROBUS, 16: 151201.Google Scholar
Delattre, P. (1966). A contribution to the history of ‘R Grasseyé.’ In: Delattre, P. (ed.), Studies in French and Comparative Phonetics. The Hague: Mouton, pp. 206207.Google Scholar
Delattre, P. (1971). Pharyngeal features in the consonants of Arabic, German, Spanish, French, and American English. Phonetica, 23: 129155.Google Scholar
Delgutte, B. (1997). Auditory neural processing of speech. In: Hardcastle, W. J. and Laver, J. (eds.), The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences. London: Blackwell, 507538.Google Scholar
Dell, F. (1980). Generative phonology and French phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dumont, P. (1979). La situation du français au Sénégal. In: Valdman, A. (ed.), Le français hors de France. Paris: Champion, pp. 363381.Google Scholar
Dunn, M. (1993). The phonetics and phonology of geminate consonants: a production study. PhD Dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Duponchel, L. (1979). Le français en Côte d'Ivoire, au Dahomey et au Togo. In: Valdman, A. (ed.), Le français hors de France. Paris: Champion, pp. 383417.Google Scholar
Edwards, M. (1973). The acquisition of liquids. Ohio State University Working Papers in Linguistics, 15:154.Google Scholar
Espinosa, J. A. C. (2004). Meaningful variability: a sociolinguistically-grounded approach to variation in optimality theory. International Journal of English Studies 4.2: 165184.Google Scholar
Fougeron, C. and Smith, C. (1993). Illustrations of the IPA – French. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 23.2: 7376.Google Scholar
Gess, R. (2003). On re-ranking and explanatory adequacy in a constraint-based theory of phonological change. In: Holt, D. E. (ed.), Optimality Theory and Language Change. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 6790.Google Scholar
Gess, R. (2004). Phonetics, phonology and phonological change in Optimality Theory: another look at the reduction of three-consonant sequences in Late Latin. PROBUS, 16: 2141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giauque, G. (1976). An early example of uvular r. Beiträge zur Germanisch-Romanisch Sprachforschung, 3: 3945.Google Scholar
Goad, H. and Rose, Y. 2004. Input elaboration, head faithfulness, and evidence for representation in the acquisition of left-edge clusters in West Germanic. In: Kager, R., Pater, J. and Zonneveld, W. (eds.), Constraints in Phonological Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 109157.Google Scholar
Grootaerts, L. (1953). Het Nederlands substraat van het Brussels-Frans klanksysteem. Nieuwe Taalgids, 46: 3841.Google Scholar
Hale, M. and Reiss, C. (1998). Formal and empirical arguments concerning phonological acquisition. Linguistic Inquiry, 29.4: 656683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, T. A. (1992). Syllable structure and syllable related processes in German. Tübingen: Max Niemayer.Google Scholar
Hall, T. A. (2000). Phonologie: eine Einführung. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (2000). Gradient well-formedness in optimality theory. In: Dekkers, J., van der Leeuw, F., and van de Weijer, J. (eds.), Optimality Theory: Phonology, Syntax, and Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 88120.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (2004). Phonetically driven phonology: The role of optimality theory and inductive grounding. In: McCarthy, J. J. (ed.), Optimality Theory in Phonology. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 343364.Google Scholar
Hayes, B, Kirchner, R. and Steriade, D. (2004). Phonetically Based Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Highfield, A. (1979). The French Dialect of St. Thomas, U.S. Virgin Islands. Ann Arbor: Karoma.Google Scholar
Hirose, H. (1997). Investigating the physiology of laryngeal structures. In: Hardcastle, W. J. and Laver, J. (eds.), The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences. London: Blackwell, pp. 116135.Google Scholar
Itô, J., Mester, A. and Padgett, J. (1995). Licensing and underspecification in optimality theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 26.4: 571613.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1939). Observations sur le classement phonologique des consonnes, Proceedings if the Third International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (Ghent) 31–41.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R., Fant, G. and Halle, M. (1952). Preliminaries to speech analysis: the distinctive features and their correlates. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jessen, M. and Ringen, C. (2002). Laryngeal features in German. Phonology, 19: 189218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M and Hilaire-Debove, G. (2004). The structure of branching onsets and rising diphthongs: Evidence from the acquisition of French. Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development, 28.1: 282–293.Google Scholar
Kirchner, R. (1998). An Effort Based Approach to Consonant Lenition. PhD Dissertation, UCLA.Google Scholar
Kirchner, R. (2004). Consonant lenition. In: Hayes, B., Steriade, D. and Kirchner, R. (eds.), Phonetically Based Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 313345.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. and Maddieson, I. (1996). The Sounds of the World's Languages. London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Laeufer, C. (1992). Patterns of voicing-conditioned vowel duration in French and English. Journal of Phonetics, 20: 411440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ledegen, G. and Bordal, G. (2005). PFC sur le terrain réunionnais: un paysage linguistique diversifié. Presentation at Phonological Variation: the Case of French, Tromso University, August 2005.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory. In: Hardcastle, W. J. and Marchal, A. (eds.), Speech Production and Speech Modelling. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 403439.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (2006). Rejecting the phonetics/phonology split. Theoretical Linguistics, 23.2: 237243.Google Scholar
Lindau, M. (1985). The story of /r/. In: Fromkin, V. A. (ed.), Phonetic Linguistics: Essays in Honor of Peter Ladefoged. Orlando: Academic Press, pp. 157168.Google Scholar
Lozachmeur, J.-C. (1976). Contribution à l'étude de l'évolution de R. Revue de linguistique romane, 40: 311.320.Google Scholar
Maddieson, I. (1997). Phonetic universals. In: Hardcastle, W. and Laver, J. (eds.), The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences. London: Blackwell, pp. 619639.Google Scholar
Manessy, G. (1984). Le français en Afrique noire: tel qu'on le parle, tel qu'on le dit. Paris: L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
McMahon, A. (2000). The emergence of the optimal? Optimality theory and sound change. The Linguistic Review, 17: 231240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahon, A. (2007). Who's afraid of the Vowel Shift Rule? Language Sciences, 29: 341359.Google Scholar
Mielke, J. (2004). The Emergence of Distinctive Features. PhD Dissertation, The Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Myers, J. (1999). Lexical Phonology and the Lexicon. MS – Rutgers Optimality Archives.Google Scholar
Oostendorp, M. van. (2001). The phonology of postvocalic /r/ in Brabant Dutch and Limburg Dutch. In: van de Velde, H. and van Hout, R. (eds.), R-actics: Sociolinguistic, phonetic and phonological characteristics of /ʀ/. Brussels: Presses de l'Université, pp. 113122.Google Scholar
Prince, A. and Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Technical Report 2 of the Rutgers Center for Cognitive Sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reenen, P. van. (1994). Driemaal /r/ in Nederlandse dialecten. Taal en Tongval, themanummer 7: 5472.Google Scholar
Reiss, C. (2000). Optimality theory from a cognitive science perspective. The Linguistic Review, 17: 191201.Google Scholar
Renaud, P. (1979). Le français au Cameroun. In: Valdman, A. (ed.), Le français hors de France. Paris: Champion, pp. 419439.Google Scholar
Rialland, A. (1986). Schwa et syllabes en français. In: Wetzels, L. and Sezer, E. (eds.), Studies in Compensatory Lengthening. Dordrecht: Foris, pp. 187226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Yvan. (2000). Headedness and Prosodic Licensing in the L1 Acquisition of Phonology. Ph.D. Dissertation, McGill University.Google Scholar
Rose, Yvan. (2003). Place specification and segmental distribution in the acquisition of word-final consonant syllabification. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 48.3–4: 409435.Google Scholar
Russell Webb, E. (2002). The Relational /ʀ/: Three Case Studies in Rhotic Integrity and Variation. PhD Dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Russell Webb, E. (2004a). Explanatory adequacy in phonology: a deductive approach to /ʀ/. Linguistica Atlantica, 25: 7794.Google Scholar
Russell Webb, E. (2004b). Voice alternation as passive lenition: /ʁ/ in French. Linguistik Online, 18.1: 127148.Google Scholar
Sankoff, G. and Blondeau, H. (2007). Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal French. Language, 83.3: 560588.Google Scholar
Smith, J. (2002). Phonological augmentation in prominent positions. PhD Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Smith, J. (2004). Making constraints positional: toward a compositional model of CON. Lingua, 114: 14331464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, K. (1991). Radical underspecification in language production. Phonology, 8: 73112.Google Scholar
Steriade, D. (1997). Phonetics in phonology: the case of laryngeal neutralization. MS, UCLA.Google Scholar
Steriade, D. (2001). The phonology of perceptibility effects: the P-map and its consequences for constraint organization. MS, UCLA.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. (1998). Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Straka, G. (1965). Contribution à l'histoire de la consonne R en français. Neuphilologische Mitteilungen, 66.3: 572606.Google Scholar
Thomas, A. (1986). La variation phonétique: cas du franco-ontarien. Montréal: Didier.Google Scholar
Tousignant, C., Sankoff, D. and Santerre, L. (1989). New results on Montreal French /r/. In: Fasold, R. and Schiffrin, D. (eds.), Language Change and Variation. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 8594.Google Scholar
Tranel, B. (1987). The Sounds of French: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tranel, B. forthcoming. Les sons du français.Google Scholar
Velde, H. van de. (1994). 60 jaar (r)evolutie in het Standaard-Nederlands. Taal en Tongval, themanummer 7: 2242.Google Scholar
Walker, D. C. (1984). The Pronunciation of Canadian French. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press.Google Scholar
Walker, D. C. (2001). French Sound Structure. Calgary: University of Calgary Press.Google Scholar
Walsh Dickey, L. (1997). The Phonology of Liquids. PhD Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Walter, H. (1977). La phonologie du français. Paris: PUF.Google Scholar
Weijer, J. van de. (1995). Continuancy in liquids and obstruents. Lingua, 96: 4561.Google Scholar
Westbury, J. and Keating, P. (1986). On the naturalness of stop consonant voicing. Journal of Linguistics 22: 145166.Google Scholar
Widdison, . (1997). Variability in lingual vibrants: changes in the story of /r/. Language and Communication, 17.3: 187193.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. (2001). The phonology of /r/. In: Hall, T. A. (ed.), Distinctive Feature Theory. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 335368.Google Scholar
Wright, R. (1996). Consonant clusters and cue preservation in Tsou. Ph.D. Dissertation, UCLA.Google Scholar
Wright, R. (2001). Perceptual cues in contrast maintenance. In: Hume, E. and Johnson, K. (eds.), The role of speech perception phenomena in phonology. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 251277.Google Scholar
Wright, R. (2004). A review of perceptual cues and cue robustness. In: Hayes, B., Kirchner, R. and Steriade, D. (eds), Phonetically Based Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3457.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Dorsal Rhotic Featural Representation (Walsh Dickey, 1997: 138, her 3.18, modified).

Figure 1

Table 1. French Consonant Inventory: adapted from Walker (2001: ix)

Figure 2

Table 2. French Consonant Inventory: features and values

Figure 3

Table 3. /ʀ/ and /l/ specification

Figure 4

Figure 2. Phonological classes.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Feature geometric representation of /ʀ/.

Figure 6

Table 4a. /ʀ/ in complex onset following voiced obstruent, grue ‘crane’

Figure 7

Table 4b. /ʀ/ in complex onset following voiceless obstruent, cru ‘raw’

Figure 8

Table 4c. Intervocalic /ʀ/, arabe ‘arab’

Figure 9

Table 4d. Word-final /ʀ/, perte ‘loss’

Figure 10

Table 5. Intervocalic /ʀ/, arabe ‘arab’: Co-optimality

Figure 11

Table 6a. Word-initial /l/, blanche ‘white-FEM’

Figure 12

Table 6b. Word-final /l/, calte ‘flee-3S’

Figure 13

Table 6c. /j/, paye ‘pay-3S’

Figure 14

Table 6d. /s/, peste ‘plague’

Figure 15

Table 6e. /s/, plaça ‘place-3S/PAST’