Introduction
Worsening political inequality, reflected in unequal rates of participation and governmental responsiveness, is one of the most pressing issues facing our democracy today (Bartels Reference Bartels2008; Garcia Bedolla and Michelson Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012; Gilens Reference Gilens2012; Gilens and Page Reference Gilens and Page2014; Hacker and Pierson Reference Hacker and Pierson2010; Hill and Leighley Reference Hill and Leighley1999; Leighley and Oser Reference Leighley and Oser2018; Verba, Schlozman, and Brady Reference Verba, Schlozman and Brady1995). At the core of the debate around political inequality are persistent disparities in campaign contributions, which largely flow from the wealthiest citizens and offer them disproportionate access and influence (Kalla and Broockman Reference Kalla and Broockman2016; Schlozman, Verba, and Brady Reference Schlozman, Verba and Brady2012).Footnote 1
Of the reforms enacted around the country to address this inequality (see Malbin and Parrott Reference Malbin and Parrott2017), among the most innovative is Seattle, Washington’s democracy voucher program. Enacted via ballot initiative in 2015, this program gives each registered voter four $25 vouchers per election cycle to contribute to campaigns for city council and city attorney (Berman Reference Berman2015).Footnote 2 The idea behind democracy vouchers, first proposed in a 2011 op-ed by law professor Lawrence Lessig, is that “more money can beat big money” (Lessig Reference Lessig2011). However, prior research indicates that electoral reforms are often insufficient to engage people in politics who have rarely participated before (Gronke, Galanes-Rosenbaum, and Miller Reference Gronke, Galanes-Rosenbaum and Miller2007). We present the results from the first experiment to examine how outreach to encourage participation in Seattle’s democracy voucher program affected inequalities in political participation.
Seattle’s 2017 municipal election presented voters with their first opportunity to contribute their democracy vouchers to candidates (Berman Reference Berman2015).Footnote 3 To conduct this study, we partnered with a coalition which sought to engage voters from underrepresented populations in the program. This coalition, the Win/Win Network, brings together advocacy and community organizations in Washington state to promote civic engagement, improve representation, and work toward social justice. We set up a field experiment which randomly assigned voters to a control condition and a treatment condition involving multiple modes of outreach encouraging voters to use their vouchers.
Results from our experiment show that the mobilization treatment – which included door-to-door canvassing, texting, e-mails, and digital advertisements – significantly increased the rate of voucher use, though this effect was greater among those who were already frequent voters. Mobilization also increased voter turnout, but only among regular voters. We posit that deeper efforts to build persistent relationships with low-propensity voters will be necessary to engage people who participate infrequently in politics and ameliorate inequalities in campaign contributions.
The mobilization effort in our experiment contributed to a trend of expanding participation in Seattle’s campaign finance system. In 2017, the city’s donor base more than tripled, with 84% of voucher contributors making their first-ever political donations. Moreover, the donor base became substantially more diverse in terms of age, race, class, and gender (Falk Reference Falk2017; Friedenbach Reference Friedenbach2017). However, overall participation was low; only around 3% of Seattle voters who received vouchers contributed them to candidates (Kliff Reference Kliff2018). With more competitive races and reforms such as an online portal for voucher submission and grants to community organizations working to increase participation, voucher use more than doubled from nearly 70,000 in 2017 to over 147,000 in 2019, and candidates were unable to accept additional vouchers after reaching their spending limits (Friedenbach Reference Friedenbach2017; SEEC 2019a). These numbers represent a promising departure from the state of campaign finance in Seattle prior to the voucher program – small-dollar donors have substantially increased as a share of contributors to city council candidates over the past two election cycles, making electoral coalitions more egalitarian (Heerwig and McCabe Reference Heerwig and McCabe2019).
Electoral reform and mobilization as responses to inequality
Existing research raises questions about the ability of Seattle’s democracy voucher program to ameliorate inequality. This research shows that election reforms which make participation easier are often insufficient to engage infrequent participants.Footnote 4 While voting by mail, for instance, has been shown to modestly increase turnout, this effect is largely concentrated among regular voters in low-salience races (Gronke, Galanes-Rosenbaum, and Miller Reference Gronke, Galanes-Rosenbaum and Miller2007; Gronke and Miller Reference Gronke and Miller2012). In a mail-in voucher system such Seattle’s, then, we might expect higher participation and responsiveness from frequent voters. To address the gap between reforms’ intended impact and the actual outcome, some scholars have argued that mobilization can aid electoral reforms in democratizing the active public (Mann Reference Mann, Burden and Stewart2014; Stein, Owens, and Leighley Reference Stein, Owens and Leighley2003).
However, most experimental research examining the effects of mobilization – which largely focuses on efforts to get out the vote (GOTV) – finds that GOTV is most effective in activating those who are already likely to participate in politics (Enos, Fowler, and Vavreck Reference Enos, Anthony and Vavreck2014). Personal forms of contact such as canvassing and volunteer phone calls tend to be most effective (Green and Gerber Reference Green and Gerber2015). The effects of GOTV on low-propensity populations vary by the salience of the election (Arceneaux and Nickerson Reference Arceneaux and Nickerson2009) and people’s embeddedness within social networks (Sinclair Reference Sinclair2012). In general, however, high-propensity voters are most responsive to mobilization efforts.Footnote 5 While Seattle’s democracy voucher program was adopted to address inequality in campaign contributions, mobilization may be necessary to make it work. We ask whether mobilization can indeed reduce inequality in one of the most unequal areas of public life.
Previous work suggests that mobilization efforts which entail building an enduring relationship with low-propensity communities might stand a better chance of activating this population around election time. This research finds that GOTV efforts among members of civic organizations can significantly increase participation among low-income, minority, and low-propensity voters (Davenport Reference Davenport2010; Garcia Bedolla and Michelson Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012; Wong Reference Wong2004). Association members may be more likely to accept a mobilization treatment or more familiar with the organization delivering the treatment. Alternatively, due to stronger political interest, efficacy, civic skills, or relational ties, members may develop a stronger predisposition to comply with requests to participate in political activity (Han Reference Han2014; Verba, Schlozman, and Brady Reference Verba, Schlozman and Brady1995; Sinclair Reference Sinclair2012; Warren Reference Warren2001).Footnote 6 In other words, a higher propensity to vote could be a mechanism through which organization membership increases responsiveness to mobilization. If this were the predominant mechanism, we would find an interaction between treatment assignment and vote history, but not between treatment assignment and organization membership.
We propose the following hypotheses for our study:
-
H1: Mobilization will increase the use of democracy vouchers.
-
H2: The effect of mobilization on voucher use will be greater among high-propensity voters.
-
H3: The effect of mobilization on voucher use will be greater among members of the organizations affiliated with the Win/Win Network.
In addition to testing these hypotheses regarding voucher use, we examine whether mobilization increased voter turnout, and whether this effect was greater among high-propensity voters and Win/Win organization members.
Experimental setting and design
The November 2017 general municipal election in Seattle offered a chance to estimate the effect of mobilization on participation in the city’s new democracy voucher program. Beyond the city council and city attorney races for which voters could contribute vouchers, the mayor’s race headlined the ticket. Voting occurred largely by mail, and about 49% of Seattle voters cast their ballots by Election Day. Each registered voter in the city received a set of four democracy vouchers on January 3, 2017, along with prepaid postage to encourage voters to send the vouchers in to the city government for distribution to candidates. Of the 17 primary election candidates, six qualified to receive vouchers by meeting the program’s requirements. In the general election, all but one of the six candidates used vouchers to finance their campaigns. In a sign of the program’s viability, a candidate using vouchers won each of the three races for which vouchers could be used (Friedenbach Reference Friedenbach2017).Footnote 7
In collaboration with the Win/Win Network, we conducted a field experiment which randomly assigned a multifaceted mobilization effort providing information about the democracy vouchers. We assembled the experimental universe through a two-stage sampling procedure. First, we identified the precincts in the southern half of the city – which is more racially diverse than the northern half of the city – with at least 20% people of color. Figure 1 maps the precincts included in the sample. Second, within these precincts, we sampled people of color and people under the age of 35 from a voter file provided by Win/Win.Footnote 8 This sampling procedure facilitated Win/Win’s efforts to increase participation among underrepresented communities in the city. As the intervention was designed to encourage people to use their vouchers, we dropped from the experimental universe anyone who had used a voucher before we selected our sample on July 31, 2017.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20220218014556581-0319:S2052263020000329:S2052263020000329_fig1.png?pub-status=live)
Figure 1 Precincts in the Experimental Universe.
Notes: Pink shading indicates that a precinct was included in the experimental sample. This map was provided by the Win/Win Network.
We used block random assignment, clustered by household, to assign voters to treatment and control groups.Footnote 9 To combine clustered random assignment with blocking, we first randomly selected one individual per household to stand in for the household in the random assignment process. We then stratified this subsample into blocks based on a set of voter characteristics. To help assess our core hypotheses, the random assignment process blocked on voting history and membership in a Win/Win partner organization.Footnote 10 To improve precision, we also blocked on whether the voter shared a household with other voters in the experimental universe, reachability by cell phone, reachability by e-mail, and – among members of the Washington Community Action Network (Washington CAN) – reachability by landline or cell phone.Footnote 11 In total, voters in the subsample were sorted into 124 separate strata representing distinct combinations of voter characteristics, and random assignment to the treatment and control groups occurred separately within each stratum. Following random assignment, we conducted balance checks on age, race, sex, income, and education level, re-randomizing if imbalance reaching at least borderline levels of significance (p < 0.1) was found. Next, we assigned each individual sharing a household with a subject in the subsample to the same experimental group as their selected housemate. After removing households with eight or more voters, we were left with a sample of 41,414 voters. Our final sample is 30.88% Asian/Pacific Islander, 21.63% Black, 8.24% Latinx, and 34.18% White; 59.69% of our sample is under 35.Footnote 12
Win/Win coordinated several organizations to deliver four modes of outreach to voters in the treatment group.Footnote 13 The voter contact methods included door-to-door canvassing, text messages, e-mail messages, and digital advertisements delivered through Facebook.Footnote 14 Each of these methods provided information about the democracy vouchers and reminded voters to contribute their vouchers to municipal campaigns. The door-to-door canvassing script also included a message intended to frame the voucher program as a means of empowering ordinary voters:
“The idea is that people in our community, no matter how big their wallets are, should be able to contribute to campaigns, and that elected officials should be accountable to us—not just wealthy special interests that make big donations.”
As is increasingly common in mobilization efforts, the canvassing script incorporated questions intended to prompt the voter to make a plan to send in their vouchers (Nickerson and Rogers Reference Nickerson and Rogers2010). Canvassers also delivered literature to each household they visited regardless of whether a voter answered the door. Win/Win followed up with voters who responded to the text messages to provide information about how to use the vouchers.Footnote 15
Win/Win provided us with data on covariatesFootnote 16 and contact rates.Footnote 17 Fourteen percent of voters had a conversation with a canvasser, 25% received texts, and 1% received e-mails, while the contact rates for the digital ads ranged from 23% to 37%.Footnote 18 After the election, Win/Win obtained data on voucher use and voter turnout from the Public Disclosure Commission and the Washington Secretary of State, respectively (Henderson and Han Reference Henderson and Han2020).
Using publicly available data, we construct four categories of voting history. We tally how often each person voted over the four previous general elections (2013, 2014, 2015, and 2016) and divide this figure by four. We code voters with perfect voting records or who missed one election as high-propensity voters, while those who voted in two of four elections are medium–high-propensity voters. Those who voted just once are medium–low-propensity voters, while those who did not vote at all are low-propensity voters.Footnote 19 We drop voters ages 21 and younger from the analyses involving voting history, as these voters were ineligible for at least one of the past four elections.
Analysis
Our analysis estimates the degree to which mobilization increased participation in the voucher program in the aggregate. The purpose of this research is not to examine the effects of particular kinds of treatments (e.g., door-to-door canvassing versus texting), but rather to understand whether mobilization in general can significantly increase participation in the program. Consistent with similarly designed mobilization studies, our estimand is the intent-to-treat effect (ITT) on voucher use (see e.g., Garcia Bedolla and Michelson Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012; Green and Gerber Reference Green and Gerber2015).Footnote 20 Rather than the effect of receiving the treatment – known as the treatment-on-treated effect (TOT) – the ITT represents the effect of being assigned to receive the treatment. An advantage of estimating the ITT is that it incorporates noncompliers into the estimate, thus accounting for the challenges that mobilization efforts face in reaching voters. A TOT estimate would indicate the intervention’s effect only on compliers, which is less informative from the perspective of evaluating the intervention’s effect on the composition of the active public.
First, we estimate the mobilization campaign’s effect on voucher use. To test H1, we fit a linear probability model with standard errors clustered by household and block fixed effects. Model 1 takes voucher use as the dependent variable and treatment assignment as the explanatory variable, and Model 2 adds a series of demographic covariates. These covariates include a discrete variable for age (A), an indicator for female (F), ordinal variables for education (E) and income bracket (I), and indicators for Asian American/Pacific Islander (P), Black (B), and Latinx (L).
We test H2 and H3 by adding interactions between treatment assignment and an indicator of high-propensity voter, and between treatment assignment and an indicator of Win/Win partner organization membership, to the two previous models (see Model 3). Model 4 adds demographic covariates; the full model is shown below:
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20220218014556581-0319:S2052263020000329:S2052263020000329_eqnu1.png?pub-status=live)
In the above model, Di is a binary indicator of treatment assignment, Hi is a binary indicator of high-propensity voter, and Wi is a binary indicator of Win/Win organization membership. The ITT estimate is provided by β1, β4 provides an estimate of the interactive effect of treatment assignment and high-propensity voter on voucher use, and β5 provides an estimate of the interactive effect of treatment assignment and organizational membership. Block fixed effects are represented by γi and εi represents the error term.
For each hypothesis test, we conduct randomization inference as a robustness check to account for the effect of re-randomization on the sampling distribution from which our permutation of the random assignment was drawn. As further robustness checks, we estimate heterogeneous treatment effects by voting history using both OLS regression and randomization inference. Finally, we replicate our analyses with voter turnout as the dependent variable.
Mobilization increases voucher use largely among frequent voters
The rate of participation in the voucher program among voters in our experimental universe was 1.45%, well below the city’s overall rate of voucher use, 3.3% (Kliff Reference Kliff2018). However, voucher use appears to increase with past voting. Among high-propensity voters in our sample, the rate of voucher use was 3.18%, more than twice the rate among the experimental universe as a whole. Medium–high-propensity voters contributed vouchers at a rate of 2.31%, compared to 1.23% among medium–low-propensity voters and 0.52% among low-propensity voters. While 1.83% of newly registered voters contributed their vouchers, none of the more than 3,000 unregistered voters in the experimental universe contributed a voucher. The rate of voucher use was also higher among members of the organizations comprising the Win/Win Network – 2.8% of members used their vouchers, more than twice the rate (1.2%) among non-members (Table 1).
Table 1 ITT Effects of Voter Mobilization on Voucher Use
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20220218014556581-0319:S2052263020000329:S2052263020000329_tab1.png?pub-status=live)
NOTES: We fit four linear probability models to estimate intent-to-treat (ITT) effects – the effects of treatment assignment (rather than receipt of the treatment) on the likelihood of voucher use. We cluster standard errors by household and add block fixed effects to account for the treatment assignment process, which randomized households within blocks. Models 3 and 4 exclude voters below the age of 22, as these voters were ineligible to vote in at least one of the elections that we use to operationalize the high-propensity voter variable. ***p < 0.01; **p < 0.05; *p < 0.1, two-tailed test.
Our analysis estimates the effect of mobilization on voucher use and how this effect varied by voting history and organizational membership. First, we find that assignment to the mobilization effort significantly increased voucher use. Model 1, without covariates, shows an ITT of 0.37 percentage points. Given that the baseline rate of voucher use in the control group was 1.26%, this effect represents a substantial 29.57% increase in voucher use. Controlling for age, gender, race, education, and income (Model 2), assignment to treatment increased voucher use by 0.35 percentage points. We thus find support for H1. As a robustness check, we test H1 using randomization inference. This procedure adjusts the standard errors on our point estimates by accounting for the permutations of the random assignment procedure which were precluded by our design. Replicating the test of H1 using this procedure does not substantially alter our results.
Second, the effect of mobilization was 1.11 percentage points greater for high-propensity voters. Consistent with H2, we find a positive interaction between treatment assignment and high-propensity voter (Model 3). This effect is significant at conventional levels using a one-sided test, which is appropriate given that we hypothesized a positive relationship. Adding demographic covariates (Model 4) does not substantially alter the results. As a robustness check, we estimate the interaction between treatment assignment and high-propensity voter with regression-adjusted randomization inference, using the specification from Model 3. Using this approach, the interaction of treatment assignment and high-propensity voter becomes significant at conventional levels using a two-sided test.
Our analysis of heterogeneous treatment effects offers further support for our hypothesis that mobilization is more effective among more frequent voters.Footnote 21 We find that mobilization increased voucher use by 1.51 percentage points among high-propensity voters, roughly four times the effect among the whole experimental universe. While treatment assignment may have increased voucher use among medium–high-propensity voters, we lack the precision to be certain of this result. However, perhaps because there are more medium–low-propensity voters in the dataset, we find a significant treatment effect for this subgroup. Among these voters, mobilization increased voucher use by 0.35 percentage points, comparable to the effect among the broader experimental universe. Finally, we do not find a significant effect among low-propensity voters (Figure 2).Footnote 22
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20220218014556581-0319:S2052263020000329:S2052263020000329_fig2.png?pub-status=live)
Figure 2 ITT Effects by Voting History.
Notes: This figure illustrates heterogeneous treatment effects by voting history category. Each model includes block fixed effects and clusters standard errors at the household level. The dots in the figure represent point estimates. The thick error bars span one standard error above and below the point estimate, while the thin error bars represent the 95% confidence interval estimated using a two-tailed test.
Third, we examine whether treatment effects varied by organizational membership. Contrary to H3, we do not find an interaction between treatment assignment and membership in a Win/Win partner organization (see Model 3). Adding controls to the interactive model does not substantially alter the results (see Model 4).
Finally, our analysis of voter turnout finds a pattern resembling the results for voucher use. While we do not find an effect of the mobilization effort on turnout overall, mobilization was significantly more effective among high-propensity voters, by nearly 4 percentage points (Table AJ1). Mobilization significantly increased voter turnout only among high-propensity voters; those assigned to treatment were 3.68 percentage points more likely to vote (Table AK1). We again fail to reject the null hypothesis that organization membership increased responsiveness to mobilization.
Discussion
The democracy voucher program has expanded and diversified participation in Seattle’s campaign finance system. Yet our data suggest that transactional approaches to mobilization for voucher contribution will activate those who regularly vote, rather than engaging new participants in the political system. While the mobilization effort increased voucher use in underrepresented communities, this increase was significantly greater among frequent voters than among the rest of the electorate. In the same vein, mobilization increased voter turnout only among regular voters.
Among the barriers to broadening political participation is a widespread feeling of political inefficacy. As one disaffected Seattle resident put it, “maybe [vouchers] would do some good, but politicians don’t want to listen to us” (Cohen Reference Cohen2017). Another voter added that “[o]ur little voucher would be so small compared to corporate America’s donations.” The canvassing message in this study similarly emphasized how vouchers could counter the influence of “wealthy special interests that make big donations”; perhaps by reminding voters of powerful impediments to democracy, the message dampened the effect of mobilization among those who were less predisposed to participate (Levine and Kline Reference Levine and Kline2019). Further, many Seattle residents had simply misplaced or forgotten about their vouchers by the time of the election (Kliff Reference Kliff2018). This challenge speaks to a deeper problem – relatively low levels of political engagement outside of election season.
On the other side of the coin, voters with a strong voting record across election cycles may be those who have an interest in local government. These voters may have been more likely to retain their vouchers when they received them in the mail and to have them ready to use after receiving a blandishment to participate. While the 2017 Seattle election experienced high turnout for a local contest held off-cycle (Anzia Reference Anzia2013), it is still worth noting that this study was conducted in a relatively low-salience election. As mobilization has been shown to activate lower-propensity voters in higher-salience elections (Arceneaux and Nickerson Reference Arceneaux and Nickerson2009), it seems plausible that if a similar voucher program were adopted for state or federal elections, a mobilization effort such as the one reported here could significantly increase participation beyond the most politically engaged voters.
We posit that efforts to engage infrequent political participants in the voucher program will need to go beyond traditional voter mobilization tactics. As the language barrier could help explain the lower treatment effects among lower-propensity voters (Garcia Bedolla and Michelson Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012), outreach in multiple languages could help address disparities in participation. Further, a large body of research documents how community organizations have fostered high levels of political engagement among low-income, majority–minority communities (Christens, Peterson, and Speer Reference Christens, Peterson and Speer2011; Christens and Speer Reference Christens and Speer2011; Osterman Reference Osterman2006; Speer et al Reference Speer, Peterson, Allison, Christens, Roberts-DeGenaro and Fogel2010; Tesdahl and Speer Reference Tesdahl and Speer2015; Warren Reference Warren2001). Fortunately, Seattle has launched a program to provide grants to community organizations in underrepresented areas of the city working to increase enrollment and participation in the democracy voucher system, including by “distributing translated program materials” for non-native English speakers (SEEC 2019b). A complete vision of a democratic polity involves a robust civil society mediating between the people and their representatives, yet we are in the midst of a decades-long decline in civic organizations and communal activities (Putnam Reference Putnam2000; Schlozman et al Reference Schlozman, Jones, You, Burch, Verba and Brady2015; Skocpol Reference Skocpol2003). These collective contexts, if revitalized, may hold the key to unlocking the potential of Seattle’s democracy voucher program, and other reforms like it, to reshape representation in American democracy.
Supplementary Material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2020.32.