Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:42:29.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Getting the Message? Choice, Self-Selection, and the Efficacy of Social Movement Arguments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2020

Paul F. Testa*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Brown University, 111 Thayer St, Providence, RI02912, USA, Twitter: @ProfPaulTesta
Tarah Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Allegheny College, Allegheny, PA, USA
Kylee Britzman
Affiliation:
Social Sciences Program, Lewis-Clark State College, Lewiston, ID, USA, Twitter: @kyleebritzman
Matthew V. Hibbing
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, UC Merced, Merced, CA, USA, Twitter: @matthibbing
*
*Corresponding author. Email: paul_testa@brown.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The dynamics of choice and self-selection are central features of politics but absent from most experimental designs. We show how designs that incorporate choice, by allowing some subjects the option to receive or avoid treatment, can be extended by randomizing conditional on subjects’ treatment choices to answer further questions of interest while preserving statistical power. We apply this design to study how the gender of messengers for the #MeToo social movement conditions who receives the movement’s message and how they respond. Our results, from both convenience and nationally representative samples, suggest that #MeToo movement’s message reaches a wide audience with the intended effect. The potential for backlash in response to the message appears limited but more likely when this message is delivered by a woman.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Experimental Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Choice and experiments

Social movements build power through coalitions. Their messages and messengers must mobilize supporters and persuade the unconvinced while also trying to minimize opposition to their cause (Benford and Snow, Reference Benford and Snow2000). A one-size-fits-all approach may not accomplish both of these goals, because the people social movements hope to reach often differ from those who actually receive their message.

Similarly, a one-size-fits-all experimental approach fails to capture the real world choices that determine who receives a social movement message. While standard survey experiments provide unbiased estimates of average treatment effects (ATE), scholars and practitioners often want to understand the effect of treatment on the people who will actually receive it (Gaines and Kuklinski, Reference Gaines and Kuklinski2011). Experimental designs that allow some subjects to choose to receive a treatment provide a means of estimating these effects (Knox et al., Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019). Despite the appeal of enhanced ecological validity, such designs remain rare in political science.

By incorporating choice, this study makes contributions to both experimental political science research and to research on social movement messaging. First, like previous studies incorporating choice, our design estimates the effects of treatment on those likely and unlikely to receive it. The former speaks to the potential real-world impact of a social movement’s message, while the latter offers insights into the persuasive potential of a message on a hard-to-reach and potentially resistant audience. Second, we show how randomizing conditional on subjects’ choices can allow researchers to address additional questions of interest while preserving statistical power. Specifically, we use our design to assess whether different messengers are more or less persuasive for these hard-to-reach subjects, but we believe the benefits of randomizing conditional on choice are broader. Experiments incorporating choice are costly, and this innovation offers researchers a way to get more bang for their experimental buck.

Using data from both convenience and nationally representative quota-based samples, we examine how the gender of messengers advocating for the #MeToo movement shapes who receives this message and how they process it. Support for gender equality and the #MeToo movement varies by partisanship and gender (Barnes and Cassese, Reference Barnes and Cassese2017; Deckman, Reference Deckman2018). But, polling on these issues also reveals that experience matters. Those who talk about sexual harassment with women and those with prior experiences with sexual assault and harassment are more supportive of the #MeToo movement. Footnote 1 Women have been the face of the #MeToo movement, and the gender of the movement’s messengers likely shapes attitudes in two ways. First, research on source cues clearly demonstrates that the source of a message matters, and that the effects of source cues are often heterogenous (Arceneaux and Kolodny, Reference Arceneaux and Kolodny2009; Goren, Federico and Kittilson, Reference Goren, Federico and Kittilson2009; Kam, Reference Kam2005; Nicholson, Reference Nicholson2012). Gender, in particular, has been found to shape perceptions of a speaker’s credibility, knowledge, and partisanship (Mendez and Osborn, Reference Mendez and Osborn2010; McDermott, Reference McDermott1998; Winter, Reference Winter2010). But, the gender of a message’s source can also matter in a second, more subtle way. Patterns of political discussion differ markedly by gender (Atkeson and Rapoport, Reference Atkeson and Rapoport2003; Hansen, Reference Hansen1997; Huckfeldt and Sprague, Reference Huckfeldt and Sprague1995). Men are less likely to encounter women in political conversation and vice versa (Karpowitz and Mendelberg, Reference Karpowitz and Mendelberg2014; Mendez and Osborn, Reference Mendez and Osborn2010), and individuals may be more likely to seek out information that conforms to their prior beliefs (Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Hahn, Krosnick and Walker2008; Stroud, Reference Stroud2008, Reference Stroud2010). More concretely, someone may find a female speaker less persuasive for the same reasons they might avoid hearing the woman’s views. By allowing some subjects a choice in whether to encounter our treatment, we provide a more complete picture of who the #MeToo movement is likely to reach, how these individuals will respond, and how to persuade those who are less likely to encounter these messages. The results from this choice-based experimental approach can inform both the current efforts of social movements and expand the scope of future studies of framing, persuasion, and political communication.

Research design

To assess the effects of the #MeToo movement’s message, we build on experimental designs that provide some subjects with the opportunity to choose whether they receive the treatment – in this case, a persuasive argument about the importance of the #MeToo movement and the need for gender equality. Such designs are commonly known in public health as “patient preference trials” (Long, Little and Lin, Reference Long, Little and Lin2008; Rücker, Reference Rücker1989; Torgerson, Klaber-Moffett and Russell, Reference Torgerson, Klaber-Moffett and Russell1996). Similar designs are less common in political science but have been used to assess the effects of negative campaigns and partisan media (Arceneaux, Johnson and Murphy, Reference Arceneaux, Johnson and Murphy2012; de Benedictis-Kessner et al., Reference de Benedictis-Kessner, Baum, Berinsky and Yamamoto2019; Gaines and Kuklinski, Reference Gaines and Kuklinski2011) as well as preferences about policies (Leeper, Reference Leeper2017).

The stages of our design are presented in Figure 1. First, we told subjects that we were interested in their opinions about the #MeToo movement. After reporting their familiarity with the movement, subjects were randomly assigned to one of two design conditions (Stage 1). Approximately 40% of subjects were assigned to the experimental design in the top branch of Figure 1 where they were then randomly assigned to treatment or control (Stage 2a). Treated subjects read a brief persuasive argument attributed to the pictured “Joan” designed to convey the importance of the #MeToo movement and the broader need for gender equality in society. Footnote 2 Control subjects received no additional information.

Figure 1 Triply Randomized Parallel Design.

NOTES: It outlines the three stages of our design. In the first stage, subjects are randomly assigned to either an experimental condition, in which treatment is then randomly assigned (Stage 2a), or a choice condition in which subjects have the opportunity to select into or out of treatment (Stage 2b). Subjects opting to avoid treatment are then randomly assigned either to receive no further information or to receive the same information attributed to either a different woman or a man (Stage 3).

Subjects randomly assigned to the bottom branch of Figure 1 (Stage 2b) were first asked: before providing your opinions, would you like to hear what “Joan” (pictured) had to say on this issue? Subjects who said yes received the same information described above. Subjects who said no were randomly assigned to one of three conditions (Stage 3): they could receive no further information and proceed directly to answering our outcome measures, or they could be randomly assigned to receive the same information attributed to a different woman (“Jane”) or a different man (“John”). The images associated with Joan, Jane, and John come from the Chicago Face Database (Ma, Correll and Wittenbrink, Reference Ma, Joshua and Wittenbrink2015) and were chosen to have similar facial features.

We adapt the notation of Knox et al. (Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019) to describe our design: Let ${D_i} \in \{ 0,1\} $ denote whether a subject is assigned to the experiment ( ${D_i}=0$ ) or choice conditions ( ${D_i}=1$ ). Let ${C_i} \in \{ 0,1\} $ denote whether subjects in the choice condition choose to receive the treatment ( ${C_i}=1$ ) or avoid it ( ${C_i}=0$ ). Finally, let ${T_i} \in \{ {T_{{\rm{Joan}}}},{T_{{\rm{Jane}}}},{T_{{\rm{John}}}},{T_{{\rm{Control}}}}\} $ denote the treatment a subject actually receives, and let ${Y_i}(t)$ correspond to subjects’ potential outcomes.

Following Knox et al. (Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019), the random assignment of design conditions ( ${Y_i}(t),{C_i}, \Bot {D_i}$ ) and treatment conditions in the experiment ( $Yi(t),{C_i} \Bot {T_i}|Di=0$ ) and the third stage of our design ( $Yi(t), \Bot {T_i}|Di=1,{C_i}=0$ ) allow us to identify five causal estimands. Footnote 3 First, from Stage 2a of Figure 1, we estimate the average treatment effect of a traditional experiment ( $ATE = E(Y|D=0,T = {T_{{\rm{Joan}}}}) - E(Y|D=0,T = {T_{{\rm{Control}}}})$ ). Second, as Gaines and Kuklinski (Reference Gaines and Kuklinski2011) show, the ATE reflects the effect of treatment were everyone to receive it and can be thought of as a weighted average of the treatment effects among the proportion, $\alpha $ , of respondents likely to seek out treatment and those likely to avoid it ( $1 - \alpha $ ). Random assignment of design conditions coupled with the random assignment of treatments allows us to estimate what Knox et al. (Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019) refer to as Average Choice-Specific Treatment Effects ( $ACTE$ ). Specifically, by taking the difference between a weighted average of the outcome in the selection condition ( $E(Y|D=1)$ ) Footnote 4 and the average of those in the experimental control ( $E(Y|D=0,T = {T_{{\rm{Control}}}})$ ), and dividing this estimate by the proportion of people selecting treatment ( $\alpha $ ), we can isolate the effect of the treatment on those likely to seek it out ( $ACT{E_{{\rm{Select}}}}$ ).

$$ACT{E_{{\rm{Select}}}} = {{\overbrace {E[Y|D = 1]}^{{\rm{Average:\;Choice}}} - \overbrace {E[Y|D = 0,T = {T_{{\rm{Control}}}}]}^{{\rm{Average:\;Control}}}} \over {\underbrace \alpha _{{\rm{Proportion}}\,{\rm{Selecting}}\,{\rm{Treatment}}}}}$$

Similarly, as Gaines and Kuklinski (Reference Gaines and Kuklinski2011) show, we can estimate the treatment’s effect on those likely to avoid it ( $ACT{E_{{\rm{Avoid}}}}$ ) as follows:

$$ACT{E_{{\rm{Avoid}}}} = {{\overbrace {E[Y|D = 0,T = {T_{{\rm{Joan}}}}]}^{{\rm{Average:\;Treatment}}} - \overbrace {E[Y|D = 1]}^{{\rm{Average:\;Choice}}}} \over {\underbrace {(1 - \alpha )}_{{\rm{Proportion}}\,{\rm{Avoiding}}\,{\rm{Treatment}}}}}$$

Standard errors for a ratio of estimates are constructed via the delta method (Cameron and Trivedi, Reference Cameron and Trivedi2005).

Finally, using only the responses from Stage 3 of Figure 1, we can estimate two conditional ACTEs ( $CACTE$ ) by simply taking the difference of means between subjects assigned to receive the message from either Jane or John and subjects assigned to receive no additional information. The CACTE for the treatment attributed to Jane ( $CACT{E_{{\rm{Female}}}} = E[Y|D=1,C=0,T = {T_{{\rm{Jane}}}}] - E[Y|D=1,C=0,T = {T_{{\rm{Control}}}}]$ ) offers a related but distinct estimate to the $ACT{E_{{\rm{Avoid}}}}$ . It provides insights into the effects of encountering the messages of #MeToo movement from a typical source after explicitly trying to avoid this information. Similarly, the CACTE for the treatment attributed to John ( $CACT{E_{{\rm{Male}}}} = E[Y|D=1,C=0,T = {T_{{\rm{John}}}}] - E[Y|D=1,C=0,T = {T_{{\rm{Control}}}}]$ ) assesses the effect of encountering the same message but from an unexpected source, in this case, a man. As we discuss below, the $CACT{E_{{\rm{Female}}}}$ provides a stronger test of the possibility that the messages of #MeToo could backfire, while the $CACT{E_{{\rm{Male}}}}$ offers insights into whether an unexpected messenger might be more effective for this particular audience. Footnote 5

While the added insights of these $CACTE$ s are appealing, researchers might balk at adding another layer of randomization to an already complicated design. Figure 2 illustrates some of the statistical benefits of randomizing conditional on choice using a series of power simulations discussed in further detail in the appendix. The simulations presented here assume that the effects of treatment are of equal size but opposite signs for those likely and unlikely to receive the treatment, that more people choose to receive the treatment than avoid it, and, as seems likely, that this choice is at least somewhat correlated with attitudes about the movement. While the $ACTE$ among those avoiding treatment is underpowered (because fewer people in this scenario avoid treatment), the $CACTE$ s have similar statistical power to the $ACTE$ among those selecting treatment. While the $CACTE$ s are estimated off of smaller samples, they gain precision from not having to estimate the variance of a ratio and decreased variance in the outcome due to the correlation between treatment choices and attitudes about #MeToo. In essence, randomizing conditional on choice provides similar benefits to conditioning on covariates or matching on a propensity score, but does so non-parametrically. By leveraging subjects’ observed choices, our design avoids issues that may arise from misspecification and omitted variable bias in more parametric approaches.

Figure 2 Statistical Power with More Selectors than Avoiders, Equal, and Offsetting Effects.

NOTES: It shows the probability of correctly rejecting a null hypothesis at p<0.05 for each of our estimands, assuming there are more people selecting treatment than avoiding it and that the effects of treatment (τ) are equal in size and opposite in direction.

Expectations

We consider three possible patterns of results. Footnote 6 First, the baseline scenario is one of the homogeneous effects where the $ATE$ , $ACTE$ s, and $CACTE$ s are all similar. In short, a standard survey experiment would have sufficed. Second, we consider patterns of heterogeneous and offsetting effects. Here, treatment effects are conditional on a subject’s willingness to receive the message. A positive $ACTE_{} {_{{\rm{Select}}}} $ suggests that the message has its intended effect on its likely audience. Similarly, we interpret a negative or null $ACT{E_{{\rm{Avoid}}}}$ as evidence of counter-arguing or resistance among those who would avoid #MeToo’s message if given the chance. Depending on the distribution and the size of the treatment effects for these two “types,” the overall ATE may be positive, negative, or non-significant. We expect the $ACT{E_{{\rm{Avoid}}}}$ to be similar in sign to the $CACT{E_{{\rm{Female}}}}$ since both receive the same message attributed to a woman, although it is possible that receiving this message after trying to avoid it may produce more backlash or resistance. For the $CACT{E_{{\rm{Male}}}}$ , it is possible that hearing the same argument from an unexpected source has a more positive (or at least less negative) effect (Berinsky, Reference Berinsky2017). Finally, we assess whether these heterogeneous results depend on characteristics of the subjects themselves. Specifically, we examine whether the $ATE$ s, $ACTE$ s, and $CACTE$ s vary by gender and partisanship. Footnote 7

Data and measurement

The data for our study come from two samples. First, we conducted a study in September 2018 using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) to recruit 1,137 respondents. Berinsky, Huber and Lenz (Reference Berinsky, Huber and Lenz2012) note that MTurk samples tend to be less representative than national probability samples (though more representative than other convenience samples), because they tend to be younger and more liberal (Huff and Tingley, Reference Huff and Tingley2015). To address these limitations, we fielded a second study in January 2019, with 1,000 respondents from Qualtrics’s online panel recruited via quota-based sampling to be nationally representative by gender, age, education, and race. Footnote 8 Overall, the MTurk sample tends to be younger, more liberal, more Democratic, and more likely to be familiar with the #MeToo movement. The Qualtrics sample is more racially and economically diverse and has more respondents who, when given the choice, opted out of receiving treatment. The primary outcomes for our analysis consisted of a scale constructed from a principal components analysis of three items measured on a 100-point scale tapping specific support for the #MeToo movement. Footnote 9

Study 1: MTurk sample

Given a choice, 81% (N = 573) of our respondents chose to hear what “Joan” had to say about the #MeToo movement while 19% (N = 131) did not. Figure 3 shows how those who selected treatment differ from those who avoided it for the full sample and by gender and partisanship. As we will see below, gender strongly conditions the sign and size of our effects, but is unrelated to exposure. Other factors like partisanship and prior familiarity with #MeToo predict exposure, and, as we show in the appendix, condition the responses of men and women differently. In short, many factors may contribute to heterogeneous patterns of exposure and response. Rather than trying to detect individual sources of heterogeneity from post-hoc subgroup analysis, experiments with choice reveal potential heterogeneity through subjects’ observed choices.

Figure 3 Who is Likely to Seek Out or Avoid the Message of the #MeToo Movement?.

NOTES: It shows the difference in means between subjects who opted into and out of receiving treatment overall (left panel) and separately (right panel) for men (triangles) and women (squares).

Figure 4 presents the results from our design with 90% and 95% confidence intervals from the full sample and then separately for men and women and Republicans and Democrats. Point estimates and confidence intervals are provided in Table 1. Looking at the full sample, the $ATE$ of reading Joan’s account appears to increase support for the #MeToo movement by 0.22 points (p < 0.05). Among those who would choose to receive this information when given the chance, the effect is also positive (0.19 points) and marginally significant (p < 0.10). Among those who would likely avoid this information, the $ACTE$ is also positive but with a wide 95% confidence interval that spans a range of values from −0.47 to 1.20. Similarly, the effects of hearing this information from a different woman or a man are also positive, but not statistically significant, although the standard errors of these $CACTE$ s are about two-thirds the size of the standard error for the $ACTE$ for those avoiding treatment.

Figure 4 Heterogeneous Effects in the #MeToo MTurk Study.

NOTES: It compares the ATE of our message to the ACTEs of those likely and unlikely to receive it and the CACTEs of avoiding the treatment and then receiving the same information attributed to a different woman or a man.

Table 1 Treatment Effect Estimates on Specific Support for #MeToo (MTurk Sample)

Note: The table provides point estimates and 95% confidence intervals for treatment effect estimated from the full sample and separately by gender and partisanship.

Turning to the results by gender in the center panel of Figure 4, we see that the effects are more evident among women. The $ATE$ among women is 0.41 (p < 0.05), while among men it is 0.03 (p = 0.83). Among those women who would choose to hear another woman’s opinions, the $ACTE$ is 0.33 (p < 0.05), while among men, the effect is 0.02 (p = 0.92). The $ACTE$ among women who would opt out of receiving the message is substantively large (0.83 points) but imprecisely estimated (p = 0.11). Among those who tried to avoid the treatment but received an alternative version, neither male nor female versions of this alternative appear to have much of an effect for men or women. However, when we turn to the $CACTE$ s estimated separately by partisanship, we see large, positive effects among Republicans who initially opted to avoid the treatment but subsequently received the same information from a man or a different woman. The $ACTE$ among Republicans avoiding the treatment is similar in sign and magnitude, but less precisely estimated.

Had we only conducted a standard survey experiment, we would conclude that the treatment increased support for #MeToo and that these effects were most evident in the responses of women. Including choice in our design, we learn that most respondents are open to hearing a woman’s perspective on #MeToo when given the choice, and this information appears more resonant for women than men. Randomizing conditional on choice, we see that our treatment appears effective among Republicans who might, otherwise, avoid this information.

Study 2: Qualtrics sample

In our Qualtrics sample, two-thirds of respondents (N = 397) opted to hear what “Joan” had to say, while the other third chose to avoid the message (N = 197). Overall, people open to hearing a woman’s view about the #MeToo movement in this sample had higher levels of income, education, and familiarity with the movement, particularly among men. As with our first study, the results in Figure 5 suggest no single factor explains who is likely to encounter this message and how they will respond.

Figure 5 Who Seeks Out or Avoids the Message of the #MeToo Movement in a More Nationally Representative Sample?.

NOTES: It shows the difference in means between subjects who opted to receive treatment (N = 397) or chose to avoid this information (N = 197) overall (top panel) and for men and women separately (bottom panel).

Figure 6 and Table 2 presents the $ATE$ , $ACTE$ s, and $CACTE$ s of our treatment on specific support for the #MeToo movement again for the full sample and then by gender and partisanship. The $ATE$ is positive (0.29, p < 0.05). The $ACTE$ s suggest this effect is most evident for those likely to encounter the treatment (0.24, p < 0.10). The estimates for those unlikely to receive the treatment are similar in sign and magnitude but estimated with less precision. The $CACTE$ s for the full sample are also non-significant, but suggestive of polarizing heterogeneity. The effect of receiving the treatment after initially opting out has a positive effect when that message is attributed to a man (0.32, p < 0.10), but a negative, non-significant effect when the message is attributed to a different woman (−0.23, p = 0.22). This polarized response becomes more evident once we estimate the separate effects by gender. Again, looking just at the responses of those individuals who opted to avoid treatment but received a similar version, we see that men appear to respond favorably to this information when it is delivered by “John” (0.62, p < 0.05) but have roughly the opposite response when that information comes from “Jane” (−0.48, p < 0.10). We see a similar but more muted pattern of responses in the $CACTE$ s for Republicans and show in the appendix that this polarized pattern of response is most evident among Republican men.

Figure 6 Heterogeneous Effects in the #MeToo Qualtrics Study.

NOTES: It provides the ATE, ACTEs, and CACTEs overall and separately for men and women and Republicans and Democrats in our national sample.

Table 2 Treatment Effect Estimates on Specific Support for #MeToo (Qualtrics Sample)

Note: The table provides point estimates and 95% confidence intervals for treatment effect estimated from the full sample and separately by gender and partisanship.

Discussion

Are movements like #MeToo preaching to the choir or changing hearts and minds? The answer depends both on who receives the message and how they respond to it. Our results suggest that these movements may reach a relatively large audience: when given a choice, two-thirds to four-fifths of subjects chose to receive our treatment. How such audiences respond to a movement’s messages is a more complicated question. Our two studies provide somewhat conflicting results. In our convenience sample, we see evidence of persuasion, particularly among women likely to encounter this message, but also among Republicans who initially avoided the treatment but were subsequently exposed to the same information attributed to a different source. In our more nationally representative sample, the treatment again appears to have the expected effect on its likely audience. However, for men in this sample who chose to avoid our treatment but received a similar message from a different source, the effects appear to depend on the gender of the source. A male increases support for #MeToo while a female source decreases support. Differences in the composition of each sample likely account for some of these divergent results: Subjects in our MTurk study were younger, more educated, and more familiar with the #MeToo movement than respondents to our Qualtrics study, and these differences were larger among men than women in the two studies. Another possible interpretation is that the treatment effects depend on people’s prior information about the #MeToo movement, which varies within groups and across samples. In the appendix, we find that the effects tend to be larger for those who report lower levels of familiarity with the movement.

The takeaway for #MeToo proponents is that both the content and source of their message are likely to matter. While our design focused on the role of source cues in this process, how social movements frame their messages clearly shapes their success (Johnston and Noakes, Reference Johnston and Noakes2005). Our study framed its #MeToo message around facts and statistics related to sexual harassment. Undoubtedly other frames, such as a message about personal experiences with sexual harassment, could have different effects and would likely reach different audiences. While this is a limitation of our experiment, it is an opportunity for future research to incorporate multiple messages into this expanded choice framework.

More broadly, we hope this paper encourages scholars to bring choice and self-selection into the design and analysis of their experiments. As the opportunity to choose what political messages or information we receive increases, researchers must account for this process. While fielding experiments incorporating choice can seem daunting, these designs are more powerful and readily adaptable to the questions scholars care about. Knox et al. (Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019), for example, show how scholars can estimate effects when subjects are presented with multiple treatment options. Our own work highlights the potential of randomizing conditional on subjects’ choices. Doing so allows scholars to answer more nuanced questions while preserving statistical power. Our design is but one of many ways scholars could accomplish this, and we urge future work to focus on relative tradeoffs between different approaches. Choice is an inherent feature of politics. We hope it becomes a prominent feature of more experimental designs.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2020.24

Footnotes

Support for this research was provided by Brown University and the Carrie Chapman Catt Prize for Research on Women and Politics from Iowa State University. The authors report no conflict of interests associated with this project. No outside funding was used to conduct the study, and the authors hold no additional positions outside their academic posts. The authors thank the editor, Kaye Usry, Andy Bloeser, and three anonymous reviewers for helpful feedback. The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article are available at the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network, at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/CWEHBA under Testa et al. (2020)

* Support for this research was provided by Brown University and the Carrie Chapman Catt Prize for Research on Women and Politics from Iowa State University. The authors thank the editor, Kaye Usry, Andy Bloeser, and three anonymous reviewers for helpful feedback. The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article are available at the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network, at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/CWEHBA

1 See, for example, Kurtzleben, Danielle. “Poll: Americans Overwhelmingly Support ‘Zero Tolerance’ On Sexual Harassment.” NPR.org. 14 December, 2017. Retrieved March 1, 2019 from: https://www.npr.org/2017/12/14/570601136/poll-sexual-harassment-ipsos

2 Full text of the treatment is provided in the appendix.

3 Like Knox et al. (Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019), we also assume SUTVA (Imbens and Rubin, Reference Imbens and Rubin2015). Knox et al. (Reference Knox, Yamamoto, Baum and Berinsky2019) include a further assumption, mean ignorability of measurement error, a stricter assumption not required for designs with a binary choice of treatments but necessary for identifying effects with more than two treatment conditions (see pp. 5–6).

4 Weighted to reflect the fact that only a portion of those opting out of treatment receive no information.

5 Our design, however, does not address how a male source might change who receives the message, which would require replacing Joan with John in Stage 2b of our design or following the Multi-Choice approach of de Benedictis-Kessner et al. (Reference de Benedictis-Kessner, Baum, Berinsky and Yamamoto2019). Our goal here is to illustrate many possibilities available to researchers once they incorporate choice into their designs. We thank two anonymous reviewers for this important clarification.

6 We Pre-Registered our design with EGAP (ID: 20180910AB).

7 We examine the intersection of gender and partisanship in the appendix while acknowledging that our approach may miss some aspects of gender gaps like those Bittner and Goodyear-Grant (Reference Bittner and Goodyear-Grant2017) describe with the more nuanced measures of gender identity.

8 Following Miratrix et al. (Reference Miratrix, Sekhon, Theodoridis and Campos2018), we report the sample estimates for our study rather than population weighted estimates.

9 Full question wordings, descriptive statistics, and additional analyses are provided in the appendix.

References

Arceneaux, Kevin, Johnson, Martin and Murphy, Chad 2012. Polarized Political Communication, Oppositional Media Hostility, and Selective Exposure. The Journal of Politics 74(01): 174–86. http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/pdfplus/10.1017/S002238161100123X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, Kevin and Kolodny, Robin 2009. Educating the Least Informed: Group Endorsements in a Grassroots Campaign. American Journal of Political Science 53(4): 755–70. http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1540–5907.2009.00399.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkeson, Lonna Rae and Rapoport, Ronald B 2003. The More Things Change the More They Stay the Same: Examining Gender Differences in Political Attitude Expression, 1952–2000. Public Opinion Quarterly 67(4):495521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, Tiffany D. and Cassese, Erin C. 2017. American Party Women: A Look at the Gender Gap within Parties. Political Research Quarterly 70(1): 127–41. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1065912916675738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benford, Robert D. and Snow, David A. 2000. Framing Processes and Social Movements: An Overview and Assessment. Annual Review of Sociology 26(1): 611–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J. 2017. Rumors and Health Care Reform: Experiments in Political Misinformation. British Journal of Political Science 47(2): 241–62. http://people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/537.pdf CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J, Huber, Gregory A and Lenz, Gabriel S 2012. Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon.com”s Mechanical Turk . Political Analysis 20(3): 351–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bittner, Amanda and Goodyear-Grant, Elizabeth 2017. Sex isn’t Gender: Reforming Concepts and Measurements in the Study of Public Opinion. Political Behavior 39(4): 1019–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, A Colin and Trivedi, Pravin K 2005. Microeconometrics: Methods and applications. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Benedictis-Kessner, Justin, Baum, Matthew A., Berinsky, Adam J. and Yamamoto, Teppei 2019. Persuading the Enemy: Estimating the Persuasive Effects of Partisan Media with the Preference-Incorporating Choice and Assignment Design. American Political Science Review 113(4): 902–16. https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0003055419000418/type/journal_article CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deckman, Melissa 2018. “#MeToo and the Midterms - Gender Watch 2018.” http://www.genderwatch2018.org/metoo-and-the-midterms/ Google Scholar
Gaines, Brian J and Kuklinski, James H 2011. Experimental Estimation of Heterogeneous Treatment Effects Related to Self-Selection. American Journal of Political Science 55(3): 724–36. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23024947 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goren, P, Federico, C M and Kittilson, M C 2009. Source Cues, Partisan Identities, and Political Value Expression. American Journal of Political Science 53(4): 805–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, Susan B. 1997. Talking About Politics: Gender and Contextual Effects on Political Proselytizing. The Journal of Politics 59(01): 73103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huckfeldt, Robert R and Sprague, John 1995. Citizens, Politics and Social Communication: Information and Influence in an Election Campaign. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huff, Connor and Tingley, Dustin 2015. “Who are these people?” Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of MTurk survey respondents.Research & Politics 2(3): 2053168015604648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imbens, Guido W. and Rubin, Donald B. 2015. Causal Inference in Statistics, Social, and Biomedical Sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, S, Hahn, K. S., Krosnick, J. A. and Walker, J. 2008. Selective Exposure to Campaign Communication: The Role of Anticipated Agreement and Issue Public Membership. Journal of Politics 70(1): 186200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, Hank and Noakes, John A. 2005. Frames of Protest: Social Movements and the Framing Perspective. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
Kam, C. D. 2005. Who Toes the Party Line? Cues, Values, and Individual Differences. Political Behavior 27(2): 163–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpowitz, Christopher F. and Mendelberg, Tali 2014. The Silent Sex: Gender, Deliberation, and Institutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Knox, Dean, Yamamoto, Teppei, Baum, Matthew A. and Berinsky, Adam J. 2019. Design, Identification, and Sensitivity Analysis for Patient Preference Trials. Journal of the American Statistical Association. https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uasa20 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeper, Thomas J. 2017. How Does Treatment Self-Selection Affect Inferences About Political Communication? Journal of Experimental Political Science 4(1): 2133. https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S205226301700001X/type/journal_article CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, Qi, Little, Roderick J. and Lin, Xihong 2008. “Causal Inference in Hybrid Intervention Trials Involving Treatment Choice. Journal of the American Statistical Association 103(482):474–84. http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uasa20 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ma, Debbie S., Joshua, Correll and Wittenbrink, Bernd 2015. The Chicago Face Database: A Free Stimulus Set of Faces and Norming Data. Behavior Research Methods 47(4): 1122–35. http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428–014–0532–5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, Monika L. 1998. Race and Gender Cues in Low-Information Elections. Political Research Quarterly 51(4): 895918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendez, Jeanette Morehouse and Osborn, Tracy 2010. Gender and the Perception of Knowledge in Political Discussion. Political Research Quarterly 63(2): 269–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miratrix, Luke W., Sekhon, Jasjeet S., Theodoridis, Alexander G. and Campos, Luis F. 2018. Worth Weighting? How to Think About and Use Weights in Survey Experiments. Political Analysis 26(3): 275–91. https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S1047198718000013/type/journal_article CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, Stephen P. 2012. Polarizing Cues. American Journal of Political Science 56(1): 5266. http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1540–5907.2011.00541.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rücker, Gerta 1989. A Two-Stage Trial Design for Testing Treatment, Self-Selection and Treatment Preference Effects. Statistics in Medicine 8(4):477–85. http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/sim.4780080411 Google Scholar
Stroud, N. J. 2008. “Media Use and Political Predispositions: Revisiting the Concept of Selective Exposure.Political Behavior 30(3): 341–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroud, N. J. 2010. “Polarization and Partisan Selective Exposure.Journal of Communication 60(3): 556–76.Google Scholar
Testa, Paul, Williams, Tarah, Britzman, Kylee and Hibbing, Matthew 2020. Replication Data for: “Getting the Message? Choice, Self-Selection, and the Efficacy of Social Movement Arguments, Harvard Dataverse, V1. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/CWEHBA CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgerson, David J., Klaber-Moffett, Jennifer and Russell, Ian T. 1996. Patient Preferences in Randomised Trials: Threat or Opportunity? Journal of Health Services Research & Policy 1(4):194–7. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/135581969600100403 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, Nicholas J. G. 2010. Masculine Republicans and Feminine Democrats: Gender and Americans’ Explicit and Implicit Images of the Political Parties. Political Behavior 32(4): 587618. https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007%2Fs11109–010–9131-z.pdf CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Triply Randomized Parallel Design.NOTES: It outlines the three stages of our design. In the first stage, subjects are randomly assigned to either an experimental condition, in which treatment is then randomly assigned (Stage 2a), or a choice condition in which subjects have the opportunity to select into or out of treatment (Stage 2b). Subjects opting to avoid treatment are then randomly assigned either to receive no further information or to receive the same information attributed to either a different woman or a man (Stage 3).

Figure 1

Figure 2 Statistical Power with More Selectors than Avoiders, Equal, and Offsetting Effects.NOTES: It shows the probability of correctly rejecting a null hypothesis at p<0.05 for each of our estimands, assuming there are more people selecting treatment than avoiding it and that the effects of treatment (τ) are equal in size and opposite in direction.

Figure 2

Figure 3 Who is Likely to Seek Out or Avoid the Message of the #MeToo Movement?.NOTES: It shows the difference in means between subjects who opted into and out of receiving treatment overall (left panel) and separately (right panel) for men (triangles) and women (squares).

Figure 3

Figure 4 Heterogeneous Effects in the #MeToo MTurk Study.NOTES: It compares the ATE of our message to the ACTEs of those likely and unlikely to receive it and the CACTEs of avoiding the treatment and then receiving the same information attributed to a different woman or a man.

Figure 4

Table 1 Treatment Effect Estimates on Specific Support for #MeToo (MTurk Sample)

Figure 5

Figure 5 Who Seeks Out or Avoids the Message of the #MeToo Movement in a More Nationally Representative Sample?.NOTES: It shows the difference in means between subjects who opted to receive treatment (N = 397) or chose to avoid this information (N = 197) overall (top panel) and for men and women separately (bottom panel).

Figure 6

Figure 6 Heterogeneous Effects in the #MeToo Qualtrics Study.NOTES: It provides the ATE, ACTEs, and CACTEs overall and separately for men and women and Republicans and Democrats in our national sample.

Figure 7

Table 2 Treatment Effect Estimates on Specific Support for #MeToo (Qualtrics Sample)

Supplementary material: Link

Testa et al. Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Testa et al. supplementary material

Testa et al. supplementary material

Download Testa et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 808.3 KB