Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T22:24:54.206Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy is associated with blood pressure at age 5–6

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 February 2018

L. J. C. A. Smarius*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Academic Center for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry de Bascule, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, VU University Medical Center, AmsterdamThe Netherlands
T. G. A. Strieder
Affiliation:
Arkin Institute for Mental Health, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
T. A. H. Doreleijers
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, VU University Medical Center, AmsterdamThe Netherlands
T. G. M. Vrijkotte
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
S. R. de Rooij
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Bio-informatics, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: L. J. C. A. Smarius, Department of Public Health, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Postbus 22660, 1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail: laetitiasmarius@online.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Early life stress has been shown to contribute to alterations in biobehavioral regulation. Whereas many different forms of childhood adversities have been studied in relation to cardiovascular outcomes, very little is known about potential associations between caregivers’ verbally aggressive behavior and heart rate and blood pressure in the child. This prospective study examined whether maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy is associated with heart rate or blood pressure at age 5–6. In the Amsterdam Born Children and their Development study, a large prospective, population-based birth cohort, maternal verbally aggressive behavior was assessed by questionnaire in the 13th week after birth. The child’s blood pressure and heart rate were measured during rest at age 5–6 (n=2553 included). Maternal verbally aggressive behavior in infancy was associated with a higher systolic blood pressure (SBP) both in supine and sitting position after adjustment for sex, height and age (SBP supine B=1.01 mmHg; 95% CI [0.06; 1.95] and SPB sitting B=1.29 mmHg; 95% CI [0.12; 2.46]). Adjustment for potential confounding variables, such as other mother–infant dyad aspects, family hypertension and child’s BMI, only slightly attenuated the associations (SBP supine B=0.99 mmHg; 95% CI [0.06; 1.93] and SPB sitting B=1.11 mmHg; 95% CI [−0.06; 2.27]). Maternal verbally aggressive behavior was not associated with diastolic blood pressure or heart rate at age 5–6. Maternal verbally aggressive behavior might be an important early life stressor with negative impact on blood pressure later in life, which should be further investigated. Possible underlying mechanisms are discussed.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press and the International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 2018 

Introduction

Increasing evidence indicates that early life stress such as adverse childhood experiences may have long-term consequences for cardiovascular health, possibly by altering the sensitivity of stress-related physiological systems predisposing to adverse cardiovascular outcomes.Reference Loria, Ho and Pollock 1 , Reference Murphy, Cohn and Loria 2 Exposure to parental aggression in childhood is sadly common.Reference Theodore, Chang and Runyan 3 Various kinds of aggression such as child physical abuse and family violence have been shown to be associated with altered biobehavioral regulation in adults, and blood pressure (BP) or hypertension in particular.Reference Parrish, Surkan and Martins 4 , Reference Slopen, Non, Williams, Roberts and Albert 5 Adults who experienced multiple adverse childhood experiences, including abuse, neglect and household dysfunction, showed a faster rise in BP levels after 30 years of age, than those without those experiences.Reference Su, Wang and Pollock 6 In addition, accumulation of four or more childhood adversities was positively associated with resting heart rate (HR) in adolescents.Reference Pretty, O’Leary, Cairney and Wade 7 Consequences of infant’s exposure to parental aggressive behavior for later cardiovascular outcomes are largely unknown. Also, very little is known about potential associations between verbally aggressive behavior to the young infant and BP and HR in the child.

The prevalence of maternal verbally aggressive behavior is unknown. Interestingly, high ‘expressed emotion’ (criticism, hostility and emotional over involvement) has been shown to appear in 6% of pregnant women.Reference Lambregtse-van den Berg, Lucassen and Kuipers-Nap 8 Maternal verbally aggressive behavior to the very young infant can be considered a profound early life stressor, as soothing maternal behavior would be more appropriate at this young age. Vocalizations have been shown to be highly important in neuroendocrine regulation of bonding, equally as important as touch.Reference Seltzer, Ziegler and Pollak 9 The quality of mother–infant interaction influences the development of the child’s self-regulation capacitiesReference MacLean, Rynes and Aragón 10 and disturbances might affect biobehavioral pathways.

Indeed, maternal sensitivity or attunement is vital for mother–infant communication, and the quality of the mother–infant dyad can be a source of stress reduction or distress to the infant. Maternal verbally aggressive behavior has been regarded as a kind of emotional abuse. Maternal–child interactions of emotionally abusing mothers are characterized by poor sensitivity, hostility and criticism or disinterest.Reference Naughton, Maguire and Mann 11 Other factors that may influence the quality of the mother–infant dyad are maternal depression and low levels of pleasure in taking care of the infant. Depressed mothers often show less sensitivity and more harsh or disrupted parenting behaviors.Reference Brummelte and Galea 12 Most mothers, both depressed and not, enjoy taking care of their infant, but some do not. Mothers experiencing low levels of pleasure in infant care could be less sensitive to the infant’s needs,Reference Dollberg, Feldman and Keren 13 possibly enabling less positive interactions similar to depressed mothers.

Based on the evidence demonstrating adverse cardiovascular outcomes in people who had been exposed to parental physically aggressive behavior in childhood, we hypothesized that children exposed to maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy would have a higher resting BP and HR already at age 5–6, compared with children not exposed to maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy, regardless of other important mother–infant dyad aspects.

Method

Participants and research design

The study sample is part of a large prospective population-based multiethnic birth cohort, the Amsterdam Born Children and their Development (ABCD) study, which started in 2003. Extensive information about the cohort and procedures regarding data collection has been published elsewhere.Reference Van Eijsden, Vrijkotte, Gemke and van der Wal 14 Approval of the ABCD study was obtained from local ethical committee (Institution name XXX, approval number MEC 02/39#02.17.392). All participating mothers gave written informed consent for themselves and their children.

The flow chart of participants included is presented in Fig. 1. Between January 2003 and March 2004, all pregnant women living in Amsterdam were asked to participate in the ABCD study during their first prenatal visit to an obstetric care provider. Of the 12,373 women approached, 8266 women filled out the pregnancy questionnaire [mean gestational age: 15.7 weeks (SD 3.5)(response rate: 67%)]. A total of 6735 women gave permission for follow-up. Three months after birth, 5131 women filled out the infancy questionnaire. For the questionnaire at the child’s age of 5–6 years, addresses of 6161 mothers were retrieved and 4488 mothers returned the questionnaire. Data on all three measurements (pregnancy, infancy and early childhood) had to be available to be included in the present study. Children who were twins, born prematurely (gestational age <37 weeks) or had congenital diseases or cardiac diseases were excluded (n=2553 included).

Fig. 1 Flow chart of participants included for analysis. *Attrition at this stage due to withdrawal, infant or maternal death, and unknown address or emigration.

Maternal verbal aggressive behavior in infancy

Maternal verbally aggressive behavior was assessed by a maternal self-report questionnaire completed in the 13th week after birth (range 11–25 weeks, SD 2 weeks), using a question on frequency of speaking angrily to the infant on a six-point scale: ‘Have you ever spoken angrily to your baby in order to diminish the crying?.’ The score was dichotomized [speaking angrily (frequency ⩽1 or ⩾2)]. Maternal verbally aggressive behavior was considered present if speaking angrily to the infant had been present twice or more. This cut-off was chosen in order to make a distinction between speaking angrily to the infant infrequently, for example by accident, v. twice or more often.

Children’s cardiovascular outcomes at age 5–6

Children’s BP was measured twice both in supine and sitting position after a test reading and 5 min of rest with the automatic oscillometric method, using an automatic device with a small cuff (arm circumference 17–22 cm) on the non-dominant arm. When either the systolic (SBP) or diastolic pressure (DBP) differed more than 10 mmHg between the two measurements, a third measurement was taken (n=502: 17%). The average of the two supine position measurements closest together was used. HR was measured using the VU University Ambulatory Monitoring System (VU-AMS-version-5fs, TD-FPP, Amsterdam, the Netherlands). Reliability and validity aspects and recording methodology of the VU-AMS have been described previously.Reference de Geus, Willemsen, Klaver and van Doornen 15 , Reference Van Dijk, van Lien and van Eijsden 16 To obtain the mean value of HR, mean HR across all supine conditions was used, excluding the period during which BP was measured.

Potential confounders

The following child characteristics during infancy were assessed: sex, birth weight, gestational age, and excessive crying. Excessive infant crying was considered present if mothers estimated their infant to cry for three or more hours per 24 h/day on average in the past week (best approximation of the Wessel’s criteria).Reference Van der Wal, van Eijsden and Bonsel 17 , Reference Smarius, Strieder and Loomans 18 The following maternal characteristics during pregnancy were included: age, parity (0 or ⩾1), ethnicity (European origin or non-European origin), cohabitation status (single or living together with partner), and level of education (years after primary school). During infancy maternal postnatal depressive symptoms, maternal physically aggressive behavior and maternal smoking at home were included. Maternal postnatal depressive symptoms were measured using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D).Reference Knight, Williams, McGee and Olaman 19 The summed scores were dichotomized (<16 or ⩾16), using the cut-off for potential cases. Pleasure in infant care was measured using five questions on a four-point scale (very true, true, not true, not true at all): ‘Do I feel very good taking care of my baby?’; Do I feel very satisfied taking care of my baby?; Do I feel happy taking care of my baby?; Am I fed up taking care of my baby?; Do I really enjoy taking care of my baby?.’ The summed score were analyzed continuously. A higher score corresponded with lower levels of pleasure in infant care. Maternal physically aggressive behavior consisted of three behaviors, which were dichotomized [cloth on mouth (frequency 0 or ⩾1), slapping (frequency 0 or ⩾1), shaking baby (frequency 0 or ⩾1)]. These three behaviors were scored as maternal physically aggressive behavior, if one out of three was present (yes or no). Maternal smoking at home during infancy was either present or absent (yes or no).

In addition, the following characteristics were included: maternal pre-pregnancy BMI (kg/m2) and preexistent hypertension (yes or no). Pregnancy-induced hypertension (yes or no) was available by combining patient records and the Dutch Perinatal Registration and classified in accordance with the guidelines of the International Society for the Study of Hypertension in Pregnancy (www.isshp.com). Paternal hypertension and family (maternal or paternal) hypertension were derived from a questionnaire previously described elsewhere.Reference van Dijk, van Eijsden, Stronks, Gemke and Vrijkotte 20

Covariates at age 5 included the exact age of the child at the 5–6 years health check. Height and weight of the child were measured from which BMI was calculated. Authoritarian parenting style was measured by the 12-items subscale of the short version of the Parenting Styles and Dimensions QuestionnaireReference Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen and Hart 21 and analyzed continuously. Maternal parenting stress was measured by a 9-items subscale on attachment of the 123-items of the ‘Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index’Reference de Brock, Vermulst, Gerris and Abidin 22 and analyzed continuously. Maternal depressive symptoms at age 5 were measured continuously by the depression severity subscale of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS 21).Reference Henry and Crawford 23

Statistical analysis

Analyses were conducted using SPSS 19.0 (SPSS inc, Chicago, IL, USA). Descriptive statistics were used to describe maternal and child characteristics. Associations between these characteristics and maternal verbally aggressive behavior were tested using analysis of variance for continuous variables and χ2 tests for categorical variables. The associations of maternal verbally aggressive behavior and BP and HR were analyzed by means of multivariable linear regression analysis. Potential confounders were selected a priori and included in the regression model by using a forced-entry method. In the first model, associations were tested in a standardized model for sex, age and height of the child. Subsequently, the potential confounders birth weight, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, pregnancy related hypertension, family hypertension, child’s BMI at age 5, maternal postnatal depressive symptoms, pleasure in infant care, and maternal physically aggressive behavior were added to a second model. In addition, in order to correct for possible stressful parenting behavior at age 5–6, authoritarian parenting style, maternal parenting stress and depressive symptoms at that age were added to a third model. The significance level we used in the study was 5%.

Results

Subjects characteristics

The characteristics of both mothers and children are presented in Table 1. Of the 2553 included children, 246 (9.6%) had been exposed to maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy. Compared with children without exposure to maternal verbally aggressive behavior, exposed children more often had been excessive crying babies, had mothers who reported more postnatal depressive symptoms, lower levels of pleasure in infant care, and more often behaved physically aggressively to the infant. At age 5–6, mothers who had been verbally aggressive in infancy more often used an authoritarian parenting style, reported more parenting stress and more depressive symptoms, compared with mothers who had not been verbally aggressive in infancy.

Table 1 Characteristics of 2553 women and their children according to maternal verbal aggressive behavior in early infancy

M, maternal; P, paternal; CES-D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; NOSI-K, ‘Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index’ for children.

Association of verbal maternal aggressive behavior with HR and BP at age 5–6

Maternal verbally aggressive behavior in infancy was associated with a higher SBP both in supine and sitting position after adjustment for sex, height and age (Model 1) (SBP supine B=1.01 mmHg; 95% CI [0.06; 1.95] and SPB sitting B=1.29 mmHg; 95% CI [0.12; 2.46]). Adjustment for potential confounding factors (Model 3) only slightly attenuated the associations between maternal verbally aggressive behavior in infancy and SBP in supine position at age 5–6 (B=0.99 mmHg; 95% CI [0.06; 1.93], R 2=0.16) and SPB sitting B=1.11 mmHg; 95% CI [−0.06; 2.27], R 2=0.14). Maternal verbally aggressive behavior was not associated with DBP nor HR (Table 2).

Table 2 Heart rate and blood pressure at age 5–6 years according to maternal verbal aggressive behavior in early infancy

SBP, systolic blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; Model 1: adjusted for sex, height and age of the child; Model 2: additionally adjusted for birth weight, pregnancy related hypertension, family hypertension, maternal BMI, child’s BMI, maternal depression in infancy, pleasure in infant care, maternal physical aggression; Model 3: additionally adjusted for authoritarian parenting style, maternal depressive symptoms and parenting stress (NOSI-K) at the child’s age of 5–6.

Discussion

Our study showed that children exposed to maternal verbally aggressive behavior during early infancy had higher SBP at the age of 5–6, compared with those unexposed to maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy. These prospective findings are in line with literature showing increased cardiovascular risk after early life stress exposure and in particular retrospective studies showing increased BP and hypertension after parental aggression.Reference Parrish, Surkan and Martins 4 , Reference Slopen, Non, Williams, Roberts and Albert 5 , Reference Gooding, Milliren, Austin, Sheridan and McLaughlin 24 Adding to this, we have now shown that infant’s exposure to maternal verbally aggressive behavior is an early life stressor which might have long-term consequences for cardiovascular health. The effects on SBP were small at age 5–6, but as childhood BP has been shown to track into adulthood,Reference Chen and Wang 25 , Reference Toschke, Kohl, Mansmann and von Kries 26 we can expect our reported increased SBP to persist and possibly become more pronounced in later life.

In contrast to our hypothesis, and contrasting with literature on physical abuse and DBP and HR in adults,Reference Slopen, Non, Williams, Roberts and Albert 5 we found no associations between maternal verbally aggressive behavior and DBP or HR at age 5–6. An association might start showing after multiple early life stressors, comparable with the study of Pretty et al.,Reference Pretty, O’Leary, Cairney and Wade 7 in which four or more ACEs were positively associated with resting HR or might only show in stressful circumstances.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this study were the large, population-based, multiethnic birth cohort with prospective design, extensive data collection from early infancy onwards and BP measurements according to a standardized protocol. Importantly, we were able to control for a large number of potentially confounding factors, for example maternal physical aggression and maternal postnatal depressive symptoms, authoritarian parenting style at age 5–6 and factors known to be related to BP, such as birth weight, pre-pregnancy BMI, family hypertension, and BMI child. The response rate of the study (initial participation, phase I) was 67%. In an era where response rates to large epidemiological studies are decreasing,Reference Galea and Tracy 27 our response rate can be considered adequate. However, selective loss to follow-up was present as in most cohort studies. Our responders were slightly healthier compared with the total study population, but our responders did not differ in the prevalence of maternal verbally aggressive behavior in infancy, compared with our non-responders at age 5–6 (9.6% v. 9.3%, P=0.335). We specifically excluded previously known physiological variables of influence on BP such as congenital disorders and preterm birth.Reference Johansson, Norman and Legnevall 28 BP was assessed by an automatic oscillometric device which may have eliminated potential inter-observer differences.

Maternal verbally aggressive behavior was considered present if angry speaking had been present more than once, at the infant’s age of 3 months, according to maternal self-report. It may be difficult to ask mothers to self-report their verbally aggressive behavior to their infant. Possible social desirability in answering this question could lead to underestimation of the true frequency of maternal verbally aggressive behavior. However, it is the only way to measure this, as it is impossible to ask the infant itself, and continuous observation of the mother–infant dyad is not feasible. Also, the definition of maternal verbally aggressive behavior seems quite narrow, but, at the same time, encloses all from only twice to frequent exposure to maternal verbally aggressive behavior in the first 3 months. The associations in our study might differ between children having experienced maternal verbally aggressive behavior only twice and children having experienced this behavior frequently during the first 3 months of life, or even ongoing until the age of 5–6. The distinction between temporary and persistent maternal verbally aggressive behavior to the infant, thus creating chronic stress, could not be made based on our data. Due to small numbers we were not able to investigate a possible dose–response relationship.

Another limitation included the inability to assess the contribution of possible paternal aggressive behavior. However, even in the absence of additional data on abuse and neglect in early childhood, maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy was associated with an increased SBP. We suspect that, in line with studies in adults,Reference Su, Wang and Pollock 6 the experience of simultaneous other adverse events will probably increase this association.

Potential underlying mechanisms

Our results suggest that presence of maternal verbal aggression is an experience of unpredictable intense or chronic stress to the very young infant. Exposure to frequent interactions with a frightening parent motivates infants’ defense system to flee, while the parent embodies the solution for infants’ needs.Reference Cassidy and Mohr 29 , Reference Liotti 30 This exposure could be a relational trauma influencing the stress-coping system.Reference Schore 31

Possible pathophysiological mechanisms explaining the positive association between maternal verbally aggressive behavior to the infant and SBP at age 5–6, could be programming effects of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA axis)Reference Pesonen, Räikkönen and Feldt 32 Reference Pervanidou and Chrousos 34 and the sympathetic nervous system (ANS). The experience of maternal verbally aggressive behavior could potentially lead to increased levels of stress in the infant with consequent programming alterations in the HPA axis, such as elevated cortisol levels and altered glucocorticoid feedback sensitivity, which, after frequent exposure, ultimately impacts BP regulation. Indeed, cortisol concentrations have been positively associated with BP in children cross-sectionally.Reference Boyne, Woollard and Phillips 35 , Reference Krishnaveni, Veena and Dhube 36 Likewise, alterations of the sympathetic nervous system, such as resetting of the baroreflex function or possibly oxidative stressReference Murphy, Cohn and Loria 2 , Reference Nuyt and Alexander 37 may mediate the effect of early life stress on dysregulation of BP. Programming effects of cerebral function have been reported recently. Emotional abuse in early life, as reported in retrospect by adults, has been shown to reduce resting state functional connectivity (rs-FC) between the amygdala and the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex in later life,Reference Fan, Herrera-Melendez and Pestke 38 affecting connectivity in brain areas related to stress responsiveness. Furthermore, stress conditions have been shown to be associated with upregulated amygdalar activityReference Shin, Wright and Cannistraro 39 , Reference Bremner, Vermetten and Schmahl 40 and resting amygdalar activity has been shown to be predictive of cardiovascular disease events.Reference Tawakol, Ishai and Takx 41 A final explanation may be a genetic profiling by maternal hypertension. Importantly, in our study maternal pre-pregnancy hypertension was not associated with maternal verbally aggressive behavior, hereby eliminating the possibility of adding profiling risk in the child.

Conclusion

Exposure to maternal verbally aggressive behavior in early infancy was associated with a higher SBP at age 5–6 compared with unexposed children. Maternal verbally aggressive behavior may be an important early life stressor with negative impact on cardiovascular health later in life, which should be further investigated.

Financial Support

Financial support for the study was granted by the Dutch Heart Foundation, grant number DHF-2007B103 and by the Dutch Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMW), grant number 2100.0076.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the large contribution and involvement of all participating mothers and children of the ABCD study.

References

1. Loria, AS, Ho, DH, Pollock, JS. A mechanistic look at the effects of adversity early in life on cardiovascular disease risk during adulthood. Acta Physiol (Oxf). 2014; 210, 277287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2. Murphy, MO, Cohn, DM, Loria, AS. Developmental origins of cardiovascular disease: Impact of early life stress in humans and rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017; 74, 453465.Google Scholar
3. Theodore, AD, Chang, JJ, Runyan, DK, et al. Epidemiologic features of the physical and sexual maltreatment of children in the Carolinas. Pediatrics. 2005; 115, e331e337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. Parrish, C, Surkan, PJ, Martins, SS, et al. Childhood adversity and adult onset of hypertension and heart disease in São Paulo, Brazil. Prev Chronic Dis. 2013; 10, E205.Google Scholar
5. Slopen, N, Non, A, Williams, DR, Roberts, AL, Albert, MA. Childhood adversity, adult neighborhood context, and cumulative biological risk for chronic diseases in adulthood. Psychosom Med. 2014; 76, 481489.Google Scholar
6. Su, S, Wang, X, Pollock, JS, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and blood pressure trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: the Georgia stress and Heart study. Circulation. 2015; 131, 16741681.Google Scholar
7. Pretty, C, O’Leary, DD, Cairney, J, Wade, TJ. Adverse childhood experiences and the cardiovascular health of children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr. 2013; 17, 208.Google Scholar
8. Lambregtse-van den Berg, MP, Lucassen, N, Kuipers-Nap, MF, et al. Assessing expressed emotion during pregnancy. Psychiatry Res. 2013; 205, 285288.Google Scholar
9. Seltzer, LJ, Ziegler, TE, Pollak, SD. Social vocalizations can release oxytocin in humans. Procedings. Biological Sciences. 2010; 277, 26612666.Google ScholarPubMed
10. MacLean, PC, Rynes, KN, Aragón, C, et al. Mother-infant mutual eye gaze supports emotion regulation in infancy during the Still-Face paradigm. Infant Behav Dev. 2014; 37, 512522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Naughton, AM, Maguire, SA, Mann, MK, et al. Emotional, behavioral, and developmental features indicative of neglect or emotional abuse in preschool children: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatr. 2013; 167, 769775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Brummelte, S, Galea, LA. Postpartum depression: etiology, treatment and consequences for maternal care. Horm Behav. 2016; 77, 153166.Google Scholar
13. Dollberg, D, Feldman, R, Keren, M. Maternal representations, infant psychiatric status, and mother-child relationship in clinic-referred and non-referred infants. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010; 19, 2536.Google Scholar
14. Van Eijsden, E, Vrijkotte, TGM, Gemke, RJBJ, van der Wal, MF. Cohort profile: The Amsterdam Born Children and their Development (ABCD) study. Int J Epidemiol. 2011; 40, 11761186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. de Geus, EJ, Willemsen, GH, Klaver, CH, van Doornen, LJ. Ambulatory measurement of respiratory sinus arrhythmia and respiration rate. Biol Psychol. 1995; 41, 205227.Google Scholar
16. Van Dijk, AE, van Lien, R, van Eijsden, M, et al. Measuring cardiac autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity in children. J Vis Exp. 2013; 74, e50073.Google Scholar
17. Van der Wal, MF, van Eijsden, M, Bonsel, GJ. Stress and emotional problems during pregnancy and excessive infant crying. J Dev Behav Paediatr. 2007; 28, 431437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Smarius, LJ, Strieder, TG, Loomans, EM, et al. Excessive infant crying doubles the risk of mood and behavioral problems at age 5: evidence for mediation by maternal characteristics. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017; 26, 293302.Google Scholar
19. Knight, RG, Williams, S, McGee, R, Olaman, S. Psychometric properties of the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in a sample of women in middle life. Behav Res Ther. 1997; 35, 373380.Google Scholar
20. van Dijk, A E, van Eijsden, M, Stronks, K, Gemke, R J, Vrijkotte, TG. The association between prenatal psychosocial stress and blood pressure in the child at age 5-7 years. PLoS One. 2012; 7, e43548.Google Scholar
21. Robinson, CC, Mandleco, B, Olsen, SF, Hart, CH. The Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ). In: Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques (eds. Perlmutter BF, Touliatos J, Holden GW), 2001; pp. 319321. Sage: Thousand Oaks.Google Scholar
22. de Brock, AJ, Vermulst, AA, Gerris, JR, Abidin, RR. NOSI, handleiding experimentele versie [NOSI, manual experimental version in Dutch]/Parenting Stress Index. 1992. Pearson: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
23. Henry, JD, Crawford, JR. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005; 44, 227239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24. Gooding, HC, Milliren, CE, Austin, SB, Sheridan, MA, McLaughlin, KA. Child abuse, resting blood pressure, and blood pressure reactivity to psychosocial stress. J Pediatr Psychol. 2016; 41, 514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Chen, X, Wang, Y. Tracking of blood pressure from childhood to adulthood: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Circulation. 2008; 117, 31713180.Google Scholar
26. Toschke, AM, Kohl, L, Mansmann, U, von Kries, R. Meta-analysis of blood pressure tracking from childhood to adulthood and implications for the design of intervention trials. Acta Paediatr. 2010; 99, 2429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Galea, S, Tracy, M. Participation rates in epidemiological studies. Ann Epidemiol. 2007; 17, 643653.Google Scholar
28. Johansson, S, Norman, M, Legnevall, L, et al. Increased catecholamines and heart rate in children with low birth weight: perinatal contributions to sympathoadrenal overactivity. J Intern Med. 2007; 261, 480487.Google Scholar
29. Cassidy, J, Mohr, JJ. Unsolvable fear, trauma and psychopathology; theory, research and clinical considerations related to disorganized attachment across the live cycle. Clin Psychol. 2001; 8, 275298.Google Scholar
30. Liotti, G. Trauma, dissociation and disorganized attachment: three stands of a single braid. Psychotherapy. 2004; 41, 472486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31. Schore, AN. Affect Dysregulation and the Disorders of the Self. 2003. Norton: New York.Google Scholar
32. Pesonen, AK, Räikkönen, K, Feldt, K, et al. Childhood separation experience predicts HPA axis hormonal responses in late adulthood: a natural experiment of World War II. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010; 35, 758767.Google Scholar
33. Danese, A, McEwen, BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiol Behav. 2012; 106, 2939.Google Scholar
34. Pervanidou, P, Chrousos, GP. Metabolic consequences of stress during childhood and adolescence. Metabolism. 2012; 61, 611619.Google Scholar
35. Boyne, MS, Woollard, A, Phillips, DI, et al. The association of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity and blood pressure in an Afro-Caribbean population. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009; 34, 736742.Google Scholar
36. Krishnaveni, GV, Veena, SR, Dhube, A, et al. Size at birth, morning cortisol and cardiometabolic risk markers in healthy Indian children. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2014; 80, 7379.Google Scholar
37. Nuyt, AM, Alexander, BT. Developmental programming and hypertension. Curr Opin Nephrol Hypertens. 2009; 18, 144152.Google Scholar
38. Fan, Y, Herrera-Melendez, AL, Pestke, K, et al. Early life stress modulates amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity: implications for oxytocin effects. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014; 35, 53285339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. Shin, LM, Wright, CI, Cannistraro, PA, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005; 62, 273281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Bremner, JD, Vermetten, E, Schmahl, C, et al. Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2005; 35, 791806.Google Scholar
41. Tawakol, A, Ishai, A, Takx, RA, et al. Relation between resting amygdalar activity and cardiovascular events: a longitudinal and cohort study. Lancet. 2017; 389, 834845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow chart of participants included for analysis. *Attrition at this stage due to withdrawal, infant or maternal death, and unknown address or emigration.

Figure 1

Table 1 Characteristics of 2553 women and their children according to maternal verbal aggressive behavior in early infancy

Figure 2

Table 2 Heart rate and blood pressure at age 5–6 years according to maternal verbal aggressive behavior in early infancy