Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T12:08:42.230Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Just DO(HaD) It! Testing the clinical potential of the DOHaD hypothesis to prevent mental disorders using experimental study designs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2016

R. J. Van Lieshout
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
J. E. Krzeczkowski*
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
*
*Address for correspondence: J. E. Krzeczkowski, Department of Health Sciences, 1280 Main Street West, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada. (Email krzeczkj@mcmaster.ca)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Optimal early cognitive and emotional development are vital to reaching one’s full potential and represent our best chance to improve the mental health of the population. The developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) hypothesis posits that adverse perinatal exposures can alter physiology and increase disease risk. As physiological plasticity decreases with age, interventions applied during gestation may hold the most promise for reducing the impact of mental disorders across the lifespan. However, this vast clinical potential remains largely unrealized as the majority of human DOHaD research is observational in nature. The application of more rigorous experimental designs [e.g. Randomized Controlled Trials (RCTs)] not only represents a major step toward unlocking this potential, but are required to fully test the scientific validity of the DOHaD hypothesis as it pertains to mental illness. Here, we argue that the optimization of maternal diet and exercise during pregnancy represents our best chance to improve offspring neurodevelopment and reduce the burden of mental disorders. Follow-up studies of the offspring of pregnant women enrolled in new and existing RCTs of maternal gestational nutrition+exercise interventions are required to determine if acting during pregnancy can prevent and/or meaningfully reduce the prevalence and severity of mental disorders in the population.

Type
Review
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press and the International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 2016 

Introduction

The burden of mental disorders

It is estimated that approximately half of all Americans meet diagnostic criteria for a psychiatric disorder at some point in their lives.Reference Kessler, Berglund and Demler 1 Nearly one in five children suffer from a mental disorderReference Costello, Foley and Angold 2 , Reference Offord, Boyle and Szatmari 3 and up to 50% of adult psychiatric patients met diagnostic criteria for these disorders in childhood.Reference Kim-Cohen, Caspi and Moffitt 4 The experience of mental illness can result in great suffering for the individual as well as enormous costs for healthcare systems and society as a whole. The World Health Organization reports that mental health services account for up to 23% of all healthcare costs in developed nations. 5 Despite these staggering personal and societal costs, just one in five adults and one in six children receive treatment.Reference Gandhi, Chiu and Lam 6 , Reference Wang, Lane and Olfson 7

Developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) and mental illness

The DOHaD hypothesis posits that adverse pre and early postnatal exposures can alter an organism’s physiology and increase the risk of disease across the lifespan.Reference Gluckman, Hanson, Cooper and Thornburg 8 , 9 The process by which physiological alterations result from exposure to intrauterine conditions is referred to as prenatal programming.Reference Gluckman, Hanson, Cooper and Thornburg 8 , 9 As most of the neuroanatomical and neurochemical mediators of cognition and emotion develop in utero, it is reasonable to expect that maternal exposure to physiological and/or psychological stresses during gestation could increase the risk for psychopathology in postnatal life. Given the ubiquity of these stresses in today’s world, the potential impact of intrauterine adversities on neurodevelopment and psychiatric risk is significant.

Optimal infant and child cognitive and emotional development are precursors to good mental health across the lifespan, integral to the attainment of one’s full academic, social and vocational potential, and represent society’s best chance to boost the mental health of the population.Reference Heckman and Masterov 10 Indeed, landmark early postnatal intervention studies like the Elmira Nurse Home Visiting Program, and the Abecedarian and Perry Preschool Projects have shown that interventions undertaken in early life can have significant, positive long-term effects on the mental health of children.Reference Mervis 11 , Reference Muennig, Robertson and Johnson 12 In fact, follow-up studies of these children have shown that for every dollar invested in these programs, society reaps an average return of $8 over the life of the individual, an annual return rate of 17%.Reference Rolnick and Grunewald 13 As brain plasticity decreases over timeReference Levitt 14 , prenatal interventions may be poised to produce similar or even more impressive gains.

However, not all early intervention programs have yielded positive results. For example, a recent study of the Family Nurse Partnership intervention in the UK reported no benefits on primary maternal and infant health outcomesReference Robling, Bekkers and Bell 15 during gestation and at 2 years of age. Important lessons can be learned from less successful early postnatal intervention studies, including the importance of longitudinal follow-up, minimizing attrition, collecting data from multiple informants, and properly standardizing interventions.Reference Mendelson and Tandon 16 Reference Wachs, Georgieff, Cusick and Mcewen 20 Despite the null results seen in some studies, early interventions have generally been shown to be much more effective and far less expensive than corrective measures applied later on.Reference Campbell, Ramey, Pubgello, Sparling and Miller-Johnson 21

Despite the accumulation of evidence supporting the usefulness of early intervention, the massive clinical and public health potential of intervening during gestation remains unrealized as nearly all human DOHaD research into mental illness is observational in nature. In this review, we argue that more rigorous experimental study designs are needed to determine the clinical applicability and preventive potential of the DOHaD hypotheses to mental disorders.

The preventive potential of intervening during pregnancy

Interventions undertaken during the prenatal period may have multiple advantages over those applied in postnatal life for preventing or reducing the prevalence and/or severity of mental disorders. First, women appear to be more motivated during pregnancy to make healthy changes than at any other time in life.Reference Olson 22 Second, as two of the three biggest concerns women have about their children are their educational attainment and alcohol and substance problemsReference Erickson and Aird 23 , if they are made aware that changes made during pregnancy can reduce these difficulties, they may be especially motivated to improve their health. Third, pregnancy is a time when women regularly interact with healthcare providers (e.g. physicians, nurses, midwives), increasing the likelihood that they will be exposed to positive messages about their health and potentially comply with interventions. In addition to motivation, access and compliance, women may also be more able to fully engage in interventions prenatally than after delivery.

In the early postnatal period many demands are put on women by their infants, their other children, and their partners, and some may even have to return to work outside of the home. When these realities are combined with sleep deprivation, compliance with even the simplest interventions may become quite difficult. Finally, after delivery, transactional processes extant between women, their children and their families become more complex. Indeed, the postnatal environment may pose additional challenges, particularly with respect to the number of unpredictable factors that can affect intervention effectiveness. Therefore, prenatal interventions may have the potential to yield greater effects than postnatal interventions in-part, due to the nature of earlier intervention and the number of factors present in pregnancy that can increase compliance. Moreover, while compliance is not likely to be perfect postnatally, the development of healthy behaviors in pregnancy will increase the likelihood that lifestyle improvements will continue into postnatal life.

Prenatal programming and mental health

A large body of evidence has linked prenatal environmental adversity to mental health and cognitive problems later in life. As reliable measurements of the intrauterine environment can be difficult to obtain, the role of prenatal programming of offspring outcomes had, until recently, largely relied on proxies of sub-optimal intrauterine exposures such as birth weight and preterm birth.Reference Schlotz and Phillips 24 Reference Van Lieshout and Boyle 26 This research has shown that variation in birth weight and gestational age across their spectrums have been linked to decreased intelligence quotient (IQ), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and mood, anxiety, psychotic, and personality disordersReference Schlotz and Phillips 24 , Reference Kim, Bale and Epperson 27 Reference Colman, Ploubidis, Wadsworth, Jones and Croudace 29 throughout life.

As an alternative to investigating proxies of sub-optimal intrauterine environments, more recent research has considered the effects of specific pregnancy exposures and mechanisms that are associated with an increased risk of offspring psychiatric and cognitive problems. Intrauterine conditions including sub-optimal maternal nutritional intake, physical inactivity, obesity, excessive gestational weight gain (GWG), elevated levels of psychological stress, environmental toxins, and smoking, alcohol, and recreational substance use have all been linked to impaired neurodevelopment and later psychopathology in offspring.Reference Bale, Baram and Brown 30 Reference Keyes, Davey Smith and Susser 33 Evidence from animal models has shown that these exposures share a number of common physiological pathways including increased inflammation, oxidative stress and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and neurotransmitter dysregulation.Reference Rivera, Christiansen and Sullivan 31 , Reference Golan, Lev, Hallak, Sorokin and Huleihel 34 , Reference Ramsay, Ferrell and Crawford 35 Modifications to these pathways are thought to occur via epigenetic mechanisms. For example, the offspring of rat dams exposed to high-fat diets have been observed to have significant epigenetic alterations in genes regulating dopamine and serotonin.Reference Vucetic, Kimmel, Totoki, Hollenbeck and Reyes 36 , Reference Sullivan, Grayson and Takahashi 37

Despite the accumulation of this impressive body of evidence, the vast majority of research to date has utilized observational study designs. The use of this type of study methodology limits causal inference and so the true clinical utility of intervening on these exposures is still not known.

In the absence of causal associations and an unawareness of the true effect size of interventions undertaken to optimize offspring mental health, it is difficult for funders to justify expenditures aimed at supporting interventions aimed at reducing the prevalence and severity of mental disorders. Likewise, it is difficult for clinicians to confidently recommend treatments to women, and for women to invest their time, energy and money to follow them.

Limitations of observational designs in establishing the clinical utility of perinatal interventions

The most significant limitation of observational study designs involves confounding variables. Confounders are the most significant threats to establishing causal associations between maternal pregnancy exposures and offspring mental health. Observational studies must attempt to adjust for all confounding variables, however, as these variables must be accounted for ahead of time and measured without error, adjustment is usually incomplete.Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith 38

Newer observational designs have been utilized to attempt to address some of these methodological limitations and increase our confidence in their findings. These include the use of negative controls, twin and family studies and Mendelian randomization.Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith 38 For example, Keyes et al.Reference Keyes, Davey Smith and Susser 33 investigated the association between maternal smoking in utero and offspring ADHD utilizing paternal smoking as a negative control. Following adjustment, symptoms of ADHD were more strongly associated with maternal smoking, increasing confidence that intrauterine exposure to smoking is linked to ADHD.

Twin and family studies have also been used to attempt to control for genetic and shared environmental confounders.Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith 38 , Reference D’Onofrio, Class, Lahey and Larsson 39 In one twin study, maternal adiposity was associated with aggression in offspring even when heritability factors were accounted for.Reference Antoniou, Fowler and Reed 40 However, in a sibling design, associations between maternal obesity and offspring hyperactivity were no longer statistically significant in full sibling comparisons.Reference Chen, Sjolander and Langstrom 41 In another study utilizing a sibling design, a link between autism spectrum disorders and low birth weight within sibling pairs was observed, however, links between birth weight and substance abuse were fully attenuated following sibling comparisons.Reference Class, Rickert, Larsson, Lichtenstein and D’Onofrio 42 Although there are many advantages to using these designs to infer causal associations, both twin and sibling designs cannot rule out potential non-shared environmental influences.Reference Chen, Sjolander and Langstrom 41

Finally, Mendelian randomization has also been used to attempt to address potential genetic confounding of associations between offspring mental health and maternal adiposity, smoking and alcohol use.Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith 38 , Reference Smith 43 Despite their usefulness in addressing the limitations of more classical observational designs, these studies are still significantly limited in their ability to predict the clinical utility of intervening on particular maternal exposures for reducing mental disorders.

It’s time: using experimental designs to determine preventive and therapeutic potential

Experimental designs provide the best evidence for the causal effects of early life exposures on offspring outcomes (Fig. 1).Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith 38 The utilization of experimental studies like randomized controlled trials (RCTs) in humans therefore provides us with the best chance to determine if (a) a perinatal intervention can positively impact offspring mental health and (b) determine if the magnitude of these changes is clinically meaningful. Currently, there is very limited experimental evidence of the effectiveness of perinatal interventions on the health of offspring, let alone their mental health.

Fig. 1 Limitations of observational designs and the need for randomized controlled trials to determine the clinical applicability of developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD).

RCTs in pregnancy

The use of experimental designs in high-risk pregnancies

The majority of RCTs that have been conducted with pregnant women to determine the impact of interventions on offspring health have been conducted in those with specific, known disease conditions. Although these are important to demonstrate the preventive potential of intervening during pregnancy, they may be limited in terms of their generalizability.

Interventions that have attempted to address maternal nutritional deficiencies in order to improve offspring anthropometric outcomes are the most common. Indeed, RCTs conducted in low- and middle-income countries found that nutritional interventions for malnourished women can reduce fetal growth restriction, and the prevalence of low birth weight and stillbirth.Reference Christian, Mullany, Hurley, Katz and Black 44 , Reference Macleod, Tang and Hobbs 45 In addition, in women with a previous macrosomic birth, central adiposity in offspring was optimized by providing low glycemic index dietary advice in pregnancy.Reference Horan, McGowan, Gibney, Donnelly and McAuliffe 46 Another trial designed to help women abstain from smoking during pregnancy, the prevalence of low birth weight (<2500 g) was significantly reduced in offspring born to women receiving its smoking cessation intervention.Reference Higgins, Bernstein and Washio 47

RCTs have also reported offspring outcomes following interventions for various problems like prenatal atopy, where omega-3 fatty acid supplementation led to improvements in offspring respiratory functioningReference Escamilla-Nuñez, Barraza-Villarreal and Hernández-Cadena 48 , and mood disorders where women randomized to a cognitive behavioral stress management intervention had offspring with improved 1 and 5 min Apgar scores.Reference Karamoozian and Askarizadeh 49 However, not all of these trials have shown beneficial effects. For example, one recent RCT investigating the efficacy of a lifestyle intervention in obese women did not find beneficial changes in metabolic risk factors in 3 year-old offspring.Reference Tanvig, Vinter and Jørgensen 50 Although data from these studies appears promising, it is important that future RCTs measure clinically relevant outcome variables and follow-up participants into early and later childhood (and beyond) in order to extend upon these findings.

RCTs of interventions applied to general population samples

Even though pregnancy interventions applied to the general population have the most potential to improve the mental health of offspring across the lifespan, RCTs of this nature are very limited (and completely lacking when it comes to mental health as an outcome). A recent RCT examining the impact of a lifestyle intervention aimed at optimizing metabolic health failed to show significant benefit for offspring outcomes at birth even though it did improve maternal diet.Reference Sagedal, Øverby and Bere 51 However, while a number of RCTs of similar interventions aimed at optimizing maternal GWG have been successful at improving outcomes in women, none have measured offspring outcomes past birth.Reference Thangaratinam, Rogozinska and Jolly 52 This lack of offspring follow-up, despite promising evidence of maternal benefit, thwarts our ability to determine if exposing women in the general population to interventions designed to optimize the intrauterine environment can improve child health beyond delivery. This is important as these lifestyle interventions are efficacious in addressing risk factors for mental disorders like maternal pre-pregnancy and pregnancy adiposity and excess GWG.Reference Van Lieshout 53 , Reference Van Lieshout, Taylor and Boyle 54

The case for RCTs of comprehensive nutrition+exercise interventions to optimize offspring neurodevelopment and mental health

Maternal gestational health, nutrition and physical activity are among the most potent and modifiable environmental determinants of offspring intelligence, emotion regulation and behavior,Reference Monk, Georgieff and Osterholm 55 , Reference Nyaradi, Li, Hickling, Foster and Oddy 56 and are important predictors of mental health across the lifespan. Sub-optimal nutrition in pregnancy appears to significantly affect the trajectory of offspring neurodevelopment, impacting the emerging neural circuitry required for optimal cognitive and behavioral outcomes.Reference Georgieff 57 For example, three large cohort studies reported associations between an unhealthy prenatal diet and offspring externalizing, and emotion regulation problems in children up to age 7 years.Reference Jacka, Ystrom and Brantsaeter 32 , Reference Steenweg-de Graaff, Tiemeier and Steegers-Theunissen 58 , Reference Pina-Camacho, Jensen, Gaysina and Barker 59 In addition, exercise in pregnancy is associated with optimal neurobehavioral outcomes in offspring, potentially through reduced inflammation, upregulation of neurotrophic factors and increased maternal psychological well-being. This evidence is significant as a sizable proportion of pregnant women in developed countries still consume unhealthy diets, are sedentary, overweight and/or manifest nutrient deficiencies.

Excess GWG in pregnancy is also a major clinical challenge, affecting up to 50% of women.Reference Bartholomew 60 As both excess GWG and maternal adiposity have been linked to mental disorders in children, a state-of-the-art maternal nutrition+exercise program that optimizes maternal weight and GWG in all pregnant women may be the most effective means by which offspring neurodevelopment can be improved and the impact of future mental disorders reduced.

Maternal nutrition

Much of the evidence that supports links between poor nutrition during pregnancy and offspring cognitive and emotional problems in humans comes from observational studies of deficiencies of specific nutrients (e.g. protein, iodine, zinc, folate, iron).Reference Georgieff 57 , Reference Grantham-McGregor and Baker-Henningham 61 , Reference Tamura, Goldenberg and Hou 62 Unfortunately, despite the simplicity and ease of compliance with single nutrient supplements, RCTs in humans have failed to find conclusive evidence of positive effects of single nutrient supplementation on offspring cognition and behavior.Reference Hamadani, Fuchs, Osendarp, Huda and Grantham-McGregor 63 , Reference Murcia, Rebagliato and Iinguez 64

That more favorable outcomes have been seen in observational studies of diets high in fish compared with omega-3 fatty acid supplementation alone, and that RCTs that supplement women with multiple nutrients have had more positive effectsReference Nyaradi, Li, Hickling, Foster and Oddy 56 , Reference Leung, Wiens and Kaplan 65 on offspring cognition and psychopathology, suggest that complete diets may be more effective at optimizing offspring brain development than single nutrients by themselves.Reference Leung, Wiens and Kaplan 65 As nutrient deficiencies rarely occur in isolation, and as combinations of foods may contain other compounds that could act synergistically to promote neurodevelopment, it is important to consider overall maternal diet as a potential point of intervention. Indeed, observational studies in developed nations have also shown that infants and children who consume healthier diets have IQs ⩾5 points higher and fewer emotion regulatory and behavioral problems.Reference Jacka, Ystrom and Brantsaeter 32 , Reference Smithers, Golley and Mittinty 66 , Reference Theodore, Thompson and Waldie 67 However, the observational nature of these studies limit causal inference.

Experimental research in animal models has shown that maternal consumption of a ‘western’ diet (i.e. high in fats and sugars) during pregnancy adversely affects offspring cognition, emotion regulation and behavior. Indeed, the offspring of these animals manifest more problems with learning, memory, anxiety, aggressionReference Sullivan, Grayson and Takahashi 37 , Reference Ong and Muhlhausler 68 as well as altered reward pathwaysReference Vucetic, Kimmel, Totoki, Hollenbeck and Reyes 36 , and abnormalities in brain regions implicated in emotion regulation.Reference Oades 69

Taken together, this work suggests that nutritional interventions undertaken during pregnancy have the potential to enhance neurodevelopment in offspring, but should focus on the promotion of overall diet quality rather than single micronutrients or specific dietary components in isolation.

Maternal exercise

The American College of Obstetrics and Gynaecology (2015) and exercise guidelines based on the PARmed-X studyReference Davies, Wolfe and Mottola 70 recommend that pregnant women engage in regular moderate-intensity aerobic activity (such as brisk walking) weekly. In some cases, women that regularly engaged in more vigorous physical activity before pregnancy may be able to continue these activities, following consultation with their obstetrical care provider. 71 However, contact sports and activities with a high risk of falling are potentially harmful and are not recommended. Overall, current evidence suggests that the benefits of moderate exercise in uncomplicated pregnancies greatly outweigh the risks.

Optimizing maternal physical activity during pregnancy has also been linked in observational studies to optimal offspring neurodevelopment. In three separate case-control studies, Clapp et al. compared the offspring of women who had been active before pregnancy but then (a) reduced their level of exercise during gestation or (b) remained just as active.Reference Clapp, Simonian, Lopez, Appleby-Wineberg and Harcar-Sevcik 72 Reference Clapp, Lopez and Harcar-Sevcik 74 Women who continued to exercise had infants who had superior emotion regulation at 5 days of age, and higher cognitive scores (up to 8 IQ points) at 1 and 5 years of age.Reference Clapp, Simonian, Lopez, Appleby-Wineberg and Harcar-Sevcik 72 More recently, the 1-month-old offspring of exercising women were shown to have more adaptive emotion regulation than infants born to non-exercisers.Reference May, Scholtz, Suminski and Gustafson 75

Interestingly, in a single small RCT, Ellemberg et al. recently showed that women randomized to an exercise intervention during the second trimester of pregnancy had infants with superior auditory memory at 8–12 days of age compared with the offspring of women who were not.Reference Labonte-Lemoyne, Curnier and Ellemberg 76 This evidence provides support for the beneficial effects of exercise in pregnancy on offspring neurodevelopment. However, the single outcome measure limits its predictive ability of offspring cognitive and mental health outcomes.

Despite the fact that studies that have intervened on overall maternal diet and exercise separately provide tantalizing evidence of effectiveness on early neurodevelopment, previous RCTs of perinatal interventions suggest that integrated treatments may be more effective than simpler ones, despite their complexity. As a result, a state-of-the-art nutrition plus exercise intervention that optimizes overall maternal diet and physical activity during pregnancy may represent our best chance to reduce the prevalence and severity of the precursors of mental disorders in children and perhaps mental illness across the lifespan (Fig. 2). Indeed, RCTs of diet+exercise interventions for pregnant women already underway further support this approach to determining the full clinical applicability of the DOHaD hypotheses.

Fig. 2 Design of a complete diet+exercise intervention to optimize mental health across the lifespan.

Clinical applicability of the nutrition+exercise RCT for optimal neurodevelopment

Although there are potential challenges to improving diet and increasing physical activity at any time in life, women are more highly motivated to improve their health during pregnancy, receive high levels of support from the healthcare system during this time, and studies have shown that such lifestyle interventions are not only well-accepted by women, but are likely to positively impact multiple developing physiological systems in offspring. Indeed, both healthy maternal diet and exercise represent proven means of ameliorating a number of the biological mechanisms shown to link adverse intrauterine exposures and offspring mental health including inflammation, oxidative stress and HPA axis dysregulation.

However, in order to fully harness the clinical and public health potential of these RCTs, a number of challenges must be addressed. Although women are more likely to be compliant with interventions during pregnancy than at any other time in their lives, compliance and participant attrition could limit the size of the effects of these intervention, and therefore investigators must focus on monitoring and optimizing compliance, and recruit samples that take attrition into account. Ecologically valid ambulatory assessment methods should be considered to assess and monitor diet and exercise throughout the intervention.Reference Trull and Ebner-Priemer 77 The use of pedometers, actigraphs and daily exercise logs would increase intervention adherence and provide objective measures of exercise behavior. In addition, food diaries completed using ecological momentary assessment methods collected through smartphone applications could be used to quantify nutrition intake and to monitor intervention adherence to the dietary component.Reference Dunton, Liao and Dzubur 78 These methods are also advantageous in that they reduce potential for retrospective bias.

Currently, the gold standard for assessing and monitoring intake involves the use of diet weighed food records.Reference Flynn, Dalrymple and Barr 79 In addition, quality measures of diet such as the Diet Quality Index for pregnancy, or the Healthy Eating Index (2010) could be used to objectively monitor diet and allow researchers to tease apart specific effects of dietary components on relevant outcomes.Reference Martin, Zhou and Flynn 80 Two systematic reviews of diet interventions in pregnancy recommend that the diet component be comprised of (i) individualized nutrition plans catered to estimated energy needs, (ii) provision of certain foods that are essential to the intervention and (iii) information sessions with a dietician to provide dietary advice.Reference Flynn, Dalrymple and Barr 79 , Reference Kuhlmann, Dietz, Galavotti and England 81 Overall, investigators must determine which type of interventions are likely to be the most effective, the doses required and the women and children who will benefit most.

Finally, valid and reliable measurements of clinically relevant outcomes are important to establish the preventative applicability of these interventions. This is a particularly perplexing issue in studies of mental disorders given our imperfect diagnostic methods, reliance on self-reports, evolving classification and nosological systems, issues of clinical heterogeneity and comorbidity, and the presence of heterotypic continuity in youth mental disorders. Researchers should consider the use of more proximal phenotypic markers of behavioral systems and multiple informant reports help address these limitations and complement traditional measures.

Perhaps the most efficient way to determine if nutrition+exercise lifestyle interventions have the potential to optimize mental health is to follow the offspring of women already enrolled in existing trials into childhood and beyond. The Maternal Obesity Management (MOMs) trial,Reference Adamo, Ferraro and Goldfield 82 a structured physical activity and nutrition program designed to optimize GWG, and other prenatal lifestyle interventions such as the Fit for Delivery trial,Reference Sagedal, Øverby and Lohne-Seiler 83 and the UK Pregnancy better eating and activity (UPBEAT trial), the Be Healthy in Pregnancy (BHIP), and the Lifestyle in Pregnancy (LiPO) trialsReference Tanvig, Vinter and Jørgensen 50 , Reference Poston, Briley and Barr 84 are examples of existing RCTs that could be utilized. Finally, as more benefits of specific diet+exercise interventions accrue, it will be important to consider the ethical implications of randomization during pregnancy. In the meantime, control group participants should receive prenatal care as usual from healthcare providers.

Conclusions

The impact that mental illness can have on individuals, families and society, and difficulties individuals have in accessing treatments highlight the need to make the prevention of mental disorders a primary clinical and public health priority.Reference Whiteford, Degenhardt and Rehm 85 Despite consistently noted associations in observational studies between sub-optimal intrauterine conditions and offspring mental health problems, no experimental studies in humans have attempted to intervene on these adversities and assess their clinical impact. However, maternal nutrition+exercise interventions are acceptable to women and may have the potential to truly test the clinical utility of acting during pregnancy to optimize the brain development and mental health of offspring across the lifespan. Within the next 20 years, the global effect of mental disorders could amount to US $16 trillion.Reference Bloom, Cafiero and Jané-Llopis 86 Combined with the impact these disorders have on human capital and lost potential, we believe that the time is now to act on sub-optimal maternal diet and physical inactivity in an attempt to elucidate the clinical potential of the DOHaD hypothesis. If prenatal nutrition+exercise interventions leads to improved neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants, it could provide the scope for the primary prevention or amelioration of mental disorders very early in life with significant benefits for offspring and society.

Acknowledgments

None.

Financial Support

Dr Van Lieshout is the Albert Einstein/Irving Zucker Chair in Neuroscience at McMaster University.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

1. Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, et al. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005; 62, 593602.Google Scholar
2. Costello, EJ, Foley, DL, Angold, A. 10-year research update review: the epidemiology of child and adolescent psychiatric disorders: II. developmental epidemiology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005; 45, 825.Google Scholar
3. Offord, DR, Boyle, MH, Szatmari, P, et al. Ontario child health study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987; 44, 832836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. Kim-Cohen, J, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, et al. Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003; 60, 709717.Google Scholar
5. WHO. The world health report 2001: Mental Health: New understanding new hope, 2001. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
6. Gandhi, S, Chiu, M, Lam, K, et al. Mental health service use among children and youth in Ontario: population-based trends over time. Can J Psychiatry. 2016; 61, 510.Google Scholar
7. Wang, PS, Lane, M, Olfson, M, et al. Twelve-month use of mental health services in the United States. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005; 62, 629640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Gluckman, PD, Hanson, MA, Cooper, C, Thornburg, KL. Effect of in utero and early-life conditions on adult health and disease. N Engl J Med. 2014; 359, 6173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9. Van Den Bergh BRH. Developmental programming of early brain and behaviour development and mental health: a conceptual framework. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2011; 53, 1923.Google Scholar
10. Heckman, JJ, Masterov, DV. The productivity argument for investing in young children. App Econ Perspect and Policy. 2007; 29, 446493.Google Scholar
11. Mervis, J. Past successes shape effort to expand early intervention. Science. 2011; 333, 952956.Google Scholar
12. Muennig, P, Robertson, D, Johnson, G, et al. The effect of an early education program on adult health: The Carolina abecedarian project randomized controlled trial. Am J Public Health. 2011; 101, 512516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Rolnick, A, Grunewald, R. Early childhood development: economic development with a high public return. The Region. 2003; 17, 612.Google Scholar
14. Levitt, P. Structural and functional maturation of the developing primate brain. J Pediatr. 2003; 143(Suppl. 4), S35S45.Google Scholar
15. Robling, M, Bekkers, MJ, Bell, K, et al. Effectiveness of a nurse-led intensive home-visitation programme for first-time teenage mothers (building blocks): a pragmatic randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016; 387, 146155.Google Scholar
16. Mendelson, T, Tandon, SD. Prevention of depression in childhood and adolescence. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2016; 25, 201218.Google Scholar
17. Burger, K. How does early childhood care and education affect cognitive development? An international review of the effects of early interventions for children from different social backgrounds. Early Child Res Q. 2010; 25, 140165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18. Cina, A, Röösli, M, Schmid, H, et al. Enhancing positive development of children: effects of a multilevel randomized controlled intervention on parenting and child problem behavior. Fam Sci. 2011; 2, 4357.Google Scholar
19. Anderson, LM, Shinn, C, Fullilove, MT, et al. The effectiveness of early childhood development programs: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2003; 24(Suppl. 3), 3246.Google Scholar
20. Wachs, TD, Georgieff, M, Cusick, S, Mcewen, BS. Issues in the timing of integrated early interventions: contributions from nutrition, neuroscience, and psychological research. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014; 1308, 89106.Google Scholar
21. Campbell, FA, Ramey, CT, Pubgello, E, Sparling, J, Miller-Johnson, S. Early childhood education: young adult outcomes from the abecedarian project. Appl Dev Sci. 2002; 6, 4257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22. Olson, CM. Tracking of food choices across the transition to motherhood. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005; 37, 129136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Erickson, MF, Aird, EG. The motherhood study: fresh insights on mothers’ attitudes and concerns. Children, Youth and Family Consortium, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN. 2005.Google Scholar
24. Schlotz, W, Phillips, DIW. Fetal origins of mental health: evidence and mechanisms. Brain Behav Immun. 2009; 23, 905916.Google Scholar
25. Räikkönen, K, Pesonen, AK, Roseboom, TJ, Eriksson, JG. Early determinants of mental health. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012; 26, 599611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Van Lieshout, RJ, Boyle, MH. Is bigger better? Macrosomia and psychopathology later in life. Obes Rev. 2011; 12, 405411.Google Scholar
27. Kim, DR, Bale, TL, Epperson, CN. Prenatal programming of mental illness: current understanding of relationship and mechanisms. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2015; 17, 19.Google Scholar
28. Colman, I, Ataullahjan, A, Naicker, K, Van Lieshout, RJ. Birth weight, stress, and symptoms of depression in adolescence: evidence of fetal programming in a National Canadian Cohort. Can J Psychiatry. 2012; 57, 422428.Google Scholar
29. Colman, I, Ploubidis, GB, Wadsworth, MEJ, Jones, PB, Croudace, TJ. A longitudinal typology of symptoms of depression and anxiety over the life course. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 62, 12651271.Google Scholar
30. Bale, TL, Baram, TZ, Brown, AS, et al. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 68, 314319.Google Scholar
31. Rivera, HM, Christiansen, KJ, Sullivan, EL. The role of maternal obesity in the risk of neuropsychiatric disorders. Front Neurosci. 2015; 9, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Jacka, FN, Ystrom, E, Brantsaeter, AL, et al. Maternal and early postnatal nutrition and mental health of offspring by age 5 years: a prospective cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013; 52, 10381047.Google Scholar
33. Keyes, KM, Davey Smith, G, Susser, E. Associations of prenatal maternal smoking with offspring hyperactivity: causal or confounded? Psychol Med. 2013; 44, 857867.Google Scholar
34. Golan, HM, Lev, V, Hallak, M, Sorokin, Y, Huleihel, M. Specific neurodevelopmental damage in mice offspring following maternal inflammation during pregnancy. Neuropharmacology. 2005; 48, 903917.Google Scholar
35. Ramsay, JE, Ferrell, WR, Crawford, L, et al. Maternal obesity is associated with dysregulation of metabolic, vascular, and inflammatory pathways. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2002; 87, 42314237.Google Scholar
36. Vucetic, Z, Kimmel, J, Totoki, K, Hollenbeck, E, Reyes, TM. Maternal high-fat diet alters methylation and gene expression of dopamine and opioid-related genes. Endocrinology. 2010; 151, 47564764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Sullivan, EL, Grayson, B, Takahashi, D, et al. Chronic consumption of a high-fat diet during pregnancy causes perturbations in the serotonergic system and increased anxiety-like behavior in nonhuman primate offspring. J Neurosci. 2010; 30, 38263830.Google Scholar
38. Gage, SH, Munafò, MR, Davey Smith, G. Causal inference in developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) research. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016; 67, 567585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. D’Onofrio, BM, Class, QA, Lahey, BB, Larsson, H. Testing the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis for psychopathology using family-based quasi- experimental designs. Child Dev Perspect. 2014; 8, 151157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Antoniou, EE, Fowler, T, Reed, K, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy weight and externalising behaviour problems in preschool children: a UK-based twin study. BMJ Open. 2014; 4, e005974.Google Scholar
41. Chen, Q, Sjolander, A, Langstrom, N, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index and offspring attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a population-based cohort study using a sibling-comparison design. Int J Epidemiol. 2014; 43, 8390.Google Scholar
42. Class, QA, Rickert, ME, Larsson, H, Lichtenstein, P, D’Onofrio, BM. Fetal growth and psychiatric and socioeconomic problems: Population-based sibling comparison. Br J Psychiatry. 2014; 205, 355361.Google Scholar
43. Smith, GD. Assessing intrauterine influences on offspring health outcomes: can epidemiological studies yield robust findings? Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol. 2008; 102, 245256.Google Scholar
44. Christian, P, Mullany, LC, Hurley, KM, Katz, J, Black, RE. Nutrition and maternal, neonatal, and child health. Semin Perinatol. 2015; 39, 361372.Google Scholar
45. Macleod, J, Tang, L, Hobbs, FDR, et al. Effects of nutritional supplementation during pregnancy on early adult disease risk : follow up of offspring of participants in a randomised controlled trial investigating effects of supplementation on infant birth weight. PloS one. 2013; 8, 115.Google Scholar
46. Horan, MK, McGowan, CA, Gibney, ER, Donnelly, JM, McAuliffe, FM. Maternal low glycaemic index diet, fat intake and postprandial glucose influences neonatal adiposity – secondary analysis from the ROLO study. Nutr J. 2014; 13, 1.Google Scholar
47. Higgins, ST, Bernstein, IM, Washio, Y, et al. Effects of smoking cessation with voucher-based contingency management on birth outcomes. Addiction. 2010; 105, 20232030.Google Scholar
48. Escamilla-Nuñez, MC, Barraza-Villarreal, A, Hernández-Cadena, L, et al. Omega-3 fatty acid supplementation during pregnancy and respiratory symptoms in children. Chest. 2014; 146, 373382.Google Scholar
49. Karamoozian, M, Askarizadeh, G. Impact of prenatal cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention on maternal anxiety and depression and newborns’ Apgar scores. Iran J Neonatol. 2015; 6, 1423.Google Scholar
50. Tanvig, M, Vinter, CA, Jørgensen, JS, et al. Effects of lifestyle intervention in pregnancy and anthropometrics at birth on offspring metabolic profile at 2.8 years: results from the Lifestyle in Pregnancy and Offspring (LiPO) study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015; 100, 175183.Google Scholar
51. Sagedal, L, Øverby, N, Bere, E, et al. Lifestyle intervention to limit gestational weight gain: the Norwegian Fit for Delivery randomised controlled trial. BJOG. 2016; doi:10.1111/1471-0528.13862.Google Scholar
52. Thangaratinam, S, Rogozinska, E, Jolly, K, et al. Effects of interventions in pregnancy on maternal weight and obstetric outcomes: meta-analysis of randomised evidence. BMJ. 2012; 344, e2088e2088.Google Scholar
53. Van Lieshout, RJ. Role of maternal adiposity prior to and during pregnancy in cognitive and psychiatric problems in offspring. Nutr Rev. 2013; 71(Suppl. 1), 95101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54. Van Lieshout, RJ, Taylor, VH, Boyle, MH. Pre-pregnancy and pregnancy obesity and neurodevelopmental outcomes in offspring: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2011; 12, 548559.Google Scholar
55. Monk, C, Georgieff, MK, Osterholm, EA. Research review: maternal prenatal distress and poor nutrition – Mutually influencing risk factors affecting infant neurocognitive development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013; 54, 115130.Google Scholar
56. Nyaradi, A, Li, J, Hickling, S, Foster, J, Oddy, WH. The role of nutrition in children’s neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013; 7, 97.Google Scholar
57. Georgieff, MK. Nutrition and the developing brain. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007; 85(Suppl), 614S619S.Google Scholar
58. Steenweg-de Graaff, J, Tiemeier, H, Steegers-Theunissen, RPM, et al. Maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and child internalising and externalising problems. the Generation R study. Clin Nutr. 2014; 33, 115121.Google Scholar
59. Pina-Camacho, L, Jensen, SK, Gaysina, D, Barker, ED. Maternal depression symptoms, unhealthy diet and child emotional – behavioural dysregulation. Psychol Med. 2015; 45, 18511860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60. Bartholomew, S. What mothers say: The Canadian Maternity Experiences Survey. Public Health Agency of Canada, Winnipeg, Manitoba. 2009.Google Scholar
61. Grantham-McGregor, S, Baker-Henningham, H. Review of the evidence linking protein and energy to mental development. Public Health Nutr. 2005; 8, 11911201.Google Scholar
62. Tamura, T, Goldenberg, RL, Hou, J, et al. Cord serum ferritin concentrations and mental and psychomotor development of children at five years of age. J Pediatr. 2002; 140, 165170.Google Scholar
63. Hamadani, JD, Fuchs, GJ, Osendarp, SJM, Huda, SN, Grantham-McGregor, SM. Zinc supplementation during pregnancy and effects on mental development and behaviour of infants: a follow-up study. Lancet. 2002; 360, 290294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64. Murcia, M, Rebagliato, M, Iinguez, C, et al. Effect of iodine supplementation during pregnancy on infant neurodevelopment at 1 year of age. Am J Epidemiol. 2011; 173, 804812.Google Scholar
65. Leung, BMY, Wiens, KP, Kaplan, BJ. Does prenatal micronutrient supplementation improve children’s mental development? A systematic review. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2011; 11, 1.Google Scholar
66. Smithers, LG, Golley, RK, Mittinty, MN, et al. Dietary patterns at 6, 15 and 24 months of age are associated with IQ at 8 years of age. Eur J Epidemiol. 2012; 27, 525535.Google Scholar
67. Theodore, RF, Thompson, JMD, Waldie, KE, et al. Dietary patterns and intelligence in early and middle childhood. Intelligence. 2009; 37, 506513.Google Scholar
68. Ong, ZY, Muhlhausler, BS. Maternal ‘junk-food’ feeding of rat dams alters food choices and development of the mesolimbic reward pathway in the offspring. FASEB J. 2011; 25, 21672179.Google Scholar
69. Oades, RD. Dopamine-serotonin interactions in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Prog Brain Res. 2008; 172, 543565.Google Scholar
70. Davies, GA, Wolfe, LA, Mottola, MF, C. M. Joint SOGC/CSEP clinical practice guideline: exercise in pregnancy and the postpartum period. Canadain J Appl Physiol. 2003; 28, 330441.Google Scholar
71. American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. Committee opinion: physical activity and exercise during pregnancy and the postpartum period. Obstet Gynecol. 2015; 26, 135142.Google Scholar
72. Clapp, JF, Simonian, S, Lopez, B, Appleby-Wineberg, S, Harcar-Sevcik, R. The one-year morphometric and neurodevelopmental outcome of the offspring of women who continued to exercise regularly throughout pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1998; 178, 594599.Google Scholar
73. Clapp, JF. Morphometric and neurodevelopmental outcome at age five years of the offspring of women who continued to exercise regularly throughout pregnancy. J Pediatr. 1996; 129, 856863.Google Scholar
74. Clapp, JF, Lopez, B, Harcar-Sevcik, R. Neonatal behavioral profile of the offspring of women who continued to exercise regularly throughout pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1999; 180, 9194.Google Scholar
75. May, LE, Scholtz, SA, Suminski, R, Gustafson, KM. Aerobic exercise during pregnancy influences infant heart rate variability at one month of age. Early Hum Dev. 2014; 90, 3338.Google Scholar
76. Labonte-Lemoyne, E, Curnier, D, Ellemberg, D. Foetal brain development is influenced by maternal exercise during pregnancy (poster). Presented at Neuroscience 2013, San Diego, CA, November 10, 2013.Google Scholar
77. Trull, TJ, Ebner-Priemer, UW. Ambulatory assessment. Eur Psychol. 2009; 14, 9597.Google Scholar
78. Dunton, GF, Liao, Y, Dzubur, E, et al. Investigating within-day and longitudinal effects of maternal stress on children’s physical activity, dietary intake, and body composition: protocol for the MATCH study. Contemp Clin Trials. 2015; 43, 142154.Google Scholar
79. Flynn, AC, Dalrymple, K, Barr, S, et al. Dietary interventions in overweight and obese pregnant women: a systematic review of the content, delivery, and outcomes of randomized controlled trials. Nutr Rev. 2016; 74, 312328.Google Scholar
80. Martin, JC, Zhou, SJ, Flynn, AC, et al. The assessment of diet quality and its effects on health outcomes pre-pregnancy and during pregnancy. Semin Reprod Med. 2016; 34, 8392.Google Scholar
81. Kuhlmann, AKS, Dietz, PM, Galavotti, C, England, LJ. Weight-management interventions for pregnant or postpartum women. Am J Prev Med. 2008; 34, 523528.Google Scholar
82. Adamo, KB, Ferraro, ZM, Goldfield, G, et al. The Maternal Obesity Management (MOM) Trial Protocol: a lifestyle intervention during pregnancy to minimize downstream obesity. Contemp Clin Trials. 2013; 35, 8796.Google Scholar
83. Sagedal, LR, Øverby, NC, Lohne-Seiler, H, et al. Study protocol: fit for delivery – can a lifestyle intervention in pregnancy result in measurable health benefits for mothers and newborns? A randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2013; 13, 132.Google Scholar
84. Poston, L, Briley, AL, Barr, S, et al. Developing a complex intervention for diet and activity behaviour change in obese pregnant women (the UPBEAT trial); assessment of behavioural change and process evaluation in a pilot randomised controlled trial. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2013; 13, 116.Google Scholar
85. Whiteford, HA, Degenhardt, L, Rehm, J, et al. Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. 2013; 382, 15751586.Google Scholar
86. Bloom, DE, Cafiero, E, Jané-Llopis, E, et al. The Global Economic Burden of Noncommunicable Diseases. World Economic Forum: Geneva, 2011; pp. 1–46.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Limitations of observational designs and the need for randomized controlled trials to determine the clinical applicability of developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD).

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Design of a complete diet+exercise intervention to optimize mental health across the lifespan.