Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T06:19:25.110Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Input versus intake – a commentary on Ambridge, Kidd, Rowland, and Theakson's ‘The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 February 2015

MEREDITH L. ROWE*
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Graduate School of Education
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Ambridge, Kidd, Rowland, and Theakston (this issue) present a compelling argument for the ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Their primary goal is to persuade the reader that “frequency effects are … a phenomenon that any successful account [of child language acquisition] must explain”. They argue that the learning mechanism is frequency sensitive, that frequency effects exist at different levels, that there are several kinds of frequency effects, and that frequent forms will be acquired before less-frequent forms (all other things being equal). I agree with the authors on these points, and would like to augment their claims by focusing my comments in two areas. First, I address the ceteris paribus condition and how it obscures many factors that deserve attention in their own right. Second, I complement their arguments by exploring the more practical implications for success in language and literacy development.

Ceteris paribus

The “all other things being equal” condition put forth by Ambridge and colleagues downplays the fact that all other things are likely never equal and at times cannot be separated from the input itself. That is, the authors argue that: “Our prediction … is that, in a regression analysis, input frequency will make a significant unique contribution to the variance of the outcome measure (in this case age of acquisition), even when all of these other factors are included in the model.” I think we need a lot more work to figure out whether this is actually the case. Many social–cognitive factors that we know play a role in language learning remain unacknowledged in the review. For example, input that occurs during episodes of joint attention can have larger effects than input outside of joint attention episodes (Tomasello & Farrar, Reference Tomasello and Farrar1986); input directed to the child has more of an effect than overheard speech (Schneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013); there are certain situational contexts where words are learned better than others (Medina, Snedeker, Trueswell & Gleitman, Reference Medina, Snedeker, Trueswell and Gleitman2011); input that is responsive to the child's vocalizations is particularly useful (Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko & Song, Reference Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko and Song2014); preschool children learn words better from speakers they know to be knowledgeable rather than uncertain (Sabbagh & Baldwin, Reference Sabbagh and Baldwin2001); or may learn more when vocabulary is embedded in challenging talk about the non-present (Rowe, Reference Rowe2012). Language is clearly not learned in isolation, thus one cannot credibly muffle these pragmatic, social–cognitive and situational influences into “all other factors being equal”. Indeed, investigations of both input quality and frequency measures either find both to be predictive (e.g., Cartmill, Armstrong, Gleitman, Goldin-Meadow, Medina & Trueswell, Reference Cartmill, Armstrong, Gleitman, Goldin-Meadow, Medina and Trueswell2013) or find the quality measure controlling for quantity predicts outcomes best (e.g., Rowe, Reference Rowe2012). Thus, I embrace the authors' claim that frequency matters, yet emphasize that there is not a one-to-one correspondence between input and intake. The more we understand these other factors that clearly contribute to the effectiveness of the input, the more we will understand the learning mechanism and how to optimize language acquisition.

Practical implications

Not only do frequency effects constrain theories about the learning mechanism, they also lead to substantial and consequential individual differences in language development. We know that caregivers vary extensively in the quantity and quality of child-directed speech they use with young children. This variability, while striking across social classes (e.g., Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995), is also evident within fairly homogeneous samples (Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer & Lyons, Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991; Pan, Rowe, Singer & Snow, Reference Pan, Rowe, Singer and Snow2005). Critically, variability in the quantity (tokens) or diversity (types) of vocabulary in the input is consistently moderately and significantly associated with children's vocabulary size or growth, even with SES controlled (see Hoff, Reference Hoff2006, for a review); and is also associated with children's lexical processing skills (Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013). Importantly, input variation is not limited to vocabulary, as parents also vary widely in the frequency and distribution of syntactic constructions they use with children, and this variability has implications for children's grammatical development. For example, Huttenlocher, Vasilyeva, Cymerman, and Levine (Reference Huttenlocher, Vasilyeva, Cymerman and Levine2002) found wide variability in the proportion of parents' utterances that were multi-clausal or complex and that, controlling for SES, this variability related to their children's use of complex sentences at home and at school.

The larger implications of this documented variation in input frequency and the resulting widespread individual differences in child language development are long lasting. Yes, all typically developing children learn language; however, they do so at different rates and those who start behind tend to stay behind (Stanovich, Reference Stanovich1986). For example, children's early vocabulary growth rates are strongly predictive of their vocabulary size in kindergarten (Rowe, Raudenbush & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Rowe, Raudenbush and Goldin-Meadow2012) and children's oral language skills in kindergarten are a strong indicator of their later literacy and school success (Snow, Burns & Griffin, Reference Snow, Burns and Griffin1998). Oral language skills in kindergarten explain most of the SES effects on fourth-grade reading skills (Durham, Farkas, Hammer, Tomblin & Catts, Reference Durham, Farkas, Hammer, Tomblin and Catts2007; Quinn, Wagner, Petscher & Lopez, in press). Indeed, large-scale studies of American children show that the SES–achievement gap in reading is already evident when children enter school (Reardon, Reference Reardon, Murnane and Duncan2011).

In light of these findings from studies on individual differences, I return to the first thesis in the review by Ambridge et al., regarding “Levels and Kinds”. The authors argue that we can gain a greater understanding of the learning mechanism if we consider both absolute frequency effects (tokens or types) as well as relative frequency effects (frequency relative to a competitor form). This is a very important point, yet it is too limited. If we think about input effects at a broader level, averaged across individuals, and look at more global accumulated language skills, additional levels and kinds become relevant. For example, is there a minimum frequency that is necessary or a point where more is not necessarily better? Is it necessarily the relative frequency of a specific construction compared to an alternative construction that matters, or could diverse uses of constructions be beneficial at a more global level (e.g., Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea & Hedges, Reference Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea and Hedges2010)? Does the proportion of specific uses of input matter more, on average, than the raw frequency? If so, what does this mean for the learning mechanism? In many cases only having large datasets will answer these questions, but these are the kinds of questions we should be asking, especially if we are interested in figuring out how to maximize language outcomes in young children.

There has indeed been a recent surge in work aimed at increasing the quantity or quality of input in an effort to improve children's language development. If input frequency does have robust effects then manipulating the input should result in improved language learning. We are currently implementing gesture training with low-income parents to improve child vocabulary development based on our previous findings that gesture input predicts child gesture and vocabulary development (Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Rowe and Goldin-Meadow2009). Others are implementing similar interventions focused on maternal responsiveness (McGillion, Pine, Herbert & Matthews, Reference McGillion, Pine, Herbert, Matthews and Rohfling2014) and overall token and type frequency (Leffel & Suskind, Reference Leffel and Suskind2013). These studies build directly off the limited existing experimental work on input effects (Landry, Smith & Swank, Reference Landry, Smith and Swank2006), and are starting to show positive results (e.g., Leffel & Suskind, Reference Leffel and Suskind2013). Thus, given the ubiquity of frequency effects, we should acknowledge their importance to learning theories and explore their practical implications more robustly.

References

REFERENCES

Cartmill, E. A., Armstrong, B. F., Gleitman, L. R., Goldin-Meadow, S., Medina, T. N. & Trueswell, J. C. (2013). Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary three years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110(28), 11278–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durham, R. E., Farkas, G., Hammer, C. S., Tomblin, J. B. & Catts, H. W. (2007). Kindergarten oral language skill: a key variable in the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic status. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 25(4), 294305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 236–48.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E. & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology 45, 337374.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J. & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology 61, 343–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E. & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology 42(4), 349–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leffel, K. & Suskind, D. (2013). Parent-directed approaches to enrich the early language environments of children living in poverty. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34, 267277.Google Scholar
McGillion, M., Pine, J., Herbert, J. & Matthews, D. (2014). A training study to promote contingent talk and test its effect on language development. Paper presented in Rohfling, K. (Chair), Longitudinal studies on predictors of early language acquisition: methodological challenges and initial findings. Symposium at the triennial meeting of the International Association for the Study of Child Language, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Medina, T. N., Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J. C. & Gleitman, L. R. (2011). How words can and cannot be learned by observation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108(22), 9014–9.Google Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D. & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 76, 763–82.Google Scholar
Quinn, J. M., Wagner, R. K., Petscher, Y. & Lopez, D. (in press). Developmental relations between vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension: a latent change score modeling study. Child Development. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12292.Google Scholar
Reardon, S. F. (2011). The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: new evidence and possible explanations. In Murnane, R. & Duncan, G. (eds), Whither opportunity? Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income children (pp. 91116). New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development 83, 1762–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323(5916), 951–3.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., Raudenbush, S. W. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). The pace of vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Development 83(2), 508–25.Google Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A. & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development 72(4), 1054–70.Google Scholar
Shneidman, L. A. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science 15(5), 659–73.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Burns, S. & Griffin, P. (eds) (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly 21(4), 360407.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y. & Song, L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science 23(2), 121–6.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1454–63.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A. & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science 24(11), 2143–52.Google Scholar