Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-6tpvb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-14T18:40:22.972Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Value Orientations, Income and Displacement Effects, and Voluntary Contributions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Neil Buckley
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4M4
James Chowhan
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4M4
Mohamed Shehata
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4M4
Kenneth S. Chan
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4M4
Stuart Mestelman*
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4M4

Abstract

Identifying the value orientations of subjects participating in market or non-market decisions by having them participate in a ring game may be helpful in understanding the behaviour of these subjects. This experiment presents the results of changes in the centre and the radius of a value orientations ring in an attempt to discover if the measured value orientations exhibit income or displacement effects. Neither significant income effects nor displacement effects are identified. An external validity check with a voluntary contribution game provides evidence that value orientations from rings centred around the origin of the decision-space explain significant portions of voluntary contributions while value orientations from displaced rings do not.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2001 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brown, P.M., Cameron, L.D., and Chapman, J.G. (1996). “Understanding Compliance with an Environmental Regulation.” Manuscript.Google Scholar
Buckley, N., Mestelman, S., and Shehata, M. (2001). “Subsidizing Public Inputs.” Journal of Public Economics. forthcoming.Google Scholar
Cameron, L.D., Brown, P.M., and Chapman, J.G. (1998). “Social Value Orientations and Decisions to Take Proenvironmental Action.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 28, 675697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehue, F.M., McClintock, C.G., and Liebrand, W.B.G. (1993). “Social Value Related Response Latencies: Unobtrusive Evidence for Individual Differences in Information Processing.” European Journal of Social Psychology. 23, 273293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griesinger, D.W. and Livingston, J.W. Jr. (1973). “Toward a Model of Interpersonal Motivation in Experimental Games.” Behavioral Science. 18, 173188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhlman, D.M. and Marchello, A. (1975). “Individual Differences in the Game Motives of Own, Relative, and Joint Gain.” Journal of Research in Personality. 9, 240251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebrand, W.B.G. (1984). “The Effect of Social Motives, Communication and Group Size on Behaviour in an n-Person Multi-Stage Mixed-Motive Game.” European Journal of Social Psychology. 14, 239264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebrand, W.B.G. and van Run, G.J. (1985). “The Effects of Social Motives on Behaviour in Social Dilemmas in Two Cultures.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 21, 86102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Offerman, T. (1997). Beliefs and Decision Rules: Theory and Experiments. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dortrecht, The Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Offerman, T., Sonnemans, J., and Schram, A. (1996). “Value Orientations, Expectations and Voluntary Contributions in Public Goods.” Economic Journal. 106, 817845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusbult, Caryl E. and Van Lange, Paul A.M. (1996). “Interdependence Processes.” In Higgins, E. Tory and Kruglanski, Arie W. (eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. The Guilford Press, New York, pp. 564596.Google Scholar