Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-hxdxx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-16T16:18:30.277Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Self-control, commitment and peer pressure: a laboratory experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Aurélie Bonein*
Affiliation:
CREM - University Rennes 1, Rennes, France
Laurent Denant-Boèmont*
Affiliation:
CREM - University Rennes 1, Rennes, France

Abstract

This paper focuses on the relationship between individual self-control and peer pressure. To this end, we performed a laboratory experiment that proceeded in two parts. The first part involved an individual real-effort task in which subjects could commit themselves to a particular level of performance while being tempted by an alternative recreational activity. The second part consisted of bargaining in a power-to-take game in which previously earned revenues were at stake. The experimental treatments involved variations in the available information provided to peers about previous individual behavior. The results show that many subjects make a serious commitment. Further, the subsequent revelation of commitment level induces subjects to increase the credible components of their commitment decisions. Past individual behaviors also play a role in bargaining because (i) partners who have committed themselves benefit from lower rates of both take and destruction and (ii) partners who have succumbed to temptation suffer from higher rates of both take and destruction.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2015 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-014-9419-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Ajzen, I (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 665683. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb00236.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ameriks, J., Caplin, A., Leahy, J., & Tyler, T. (2007). Measuring self-control problems. American Economic Review, 97(3), 966972. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/aea/aecrev/v97y2007i3p966-972.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, D, & Wertenbroch, K (2002). Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: Self-control by precommitment. Psychological Science, 13(3), 219224. 10.1111/1467-9280.00441 12009041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashraf, N., Karlan, D., & Yin, W. (2006). Tying odysseus to the mast: Evidence from a commitment savings product in the Philippines. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(2), 635672. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/tpr/qjecon/v121y2006i2p635-672.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Battaglini, M, Benabou, R, & Tirole, J (2005). Self-control in peer groups. Journal of Economic Theory, 123(2), 105134 1138.91390 2150409 10.1016/j.jet.2005.04.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanco, M, Engelmann, D, & Normann, HT (2011). A within-subject analysis of other-regarding preferences. Games and Economic Behavior, 72(2), 321338 1217.91030 2840823 10.1016/j.geb.2010.09.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosman, R., & van Winden, F. (2002). Emotional hazard in a power-to-take experiment. Economic Journal, 112(476), 147169. Retrieved from http://ideas.repec.org/a/ecj/econjl/v112y2002i476p147-169.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosman, R., Sutter, M., & van Winden, F. (2005). The impact of real effort and emotions in the power-to-take game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(3), 407429. Retrieved from http://ideas.repec.org/a/eee/joepsy/v26y2005i3p407-429.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradfield, M, & Aquino, K (1999). The effects of blame attribution and offender likableness on forgiveness and revenge in the workplace. Journal of Management, 25(5), 607631. 10.1177/014920639902500501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J, & Rachlin, H (1999). Self-control and social cooperation. Behavioral Process, 47(2), 6572 1:STN:280:DC%2BC2cjpslaqsQ%3D%3D 10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00054-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucciol, A, Houser, D, & Piovesan, M (2011). Temptation and productivity: A field experiment with children. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 78(1–2), 126136. 10.1016/j.jebo.2010.12.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buechel, B., Mechtenberg, L., & Petersen, J. (2014). Peer effects and students’ self-control. MPRA Paper 53658, University Library of Munich, Germany. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/p/pra/mprapa/53658.html.Google Scholar
Burger, N., Charness, G., & Lynham, J. (2008). Three field experiments on procrastination and willpower. Technical Report 122247000000002399, Levine’s Working Paper Archive. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/p/cla/levarc/122247000000002399.html.Google Scholar
Camerer, CF, Malmendier, U Diamond, Peter, & Vartiainen, Hannu (2007). Behavioral organizational economics. Behavioral economics and its applications, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Charness, G, & Levine, D (2010). When is employee retaliation acceptable at work? evidence from quasi-experiments. Industrial Relations, 49(4), 499523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corgnet, B., Hernán-González, R., & Rassenti, S. (2013). Peer pressure and moral hazard in teams: Experimental evidence. Working Papers 13–01, Economic Science Institute, Chapman University. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/p/chu/wpaper/13-01.html.Google Scholar
Corgnet, B., Hernán-González, R., & Schniter, E. (2014). Why real leisure really matters: Incentive effects on real effort in the laboratory. Experimental Economics, doi:10.1007/s10683-014-9401-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortina, L, & Magley, V (2003). Raising voice, risking retaliation: Events following mistreatment in the workplace. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8(4), 247265. 10.1037/1076-8998.8.4.247 14570522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, P., & Vartiainen, H. (2007). Introduction to behavioral economics and its applications. In Diamond, P., Vartiainen, H. (Eds.), Behavioral economics and its applications, Introductory Chapters, Princeton: PrincetonUniversity Press. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/h/pup/chapts/8437-1.html.Google Scholar
Falk, A, & Ichino, A (2006). Clean evidence on peer effects. Journal of Labor Economics, 24(1), 3958. 10.1086/497818CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E, & Schmidt, K (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868 0944.91022 10.1162/003355399556151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U (2007). Z-tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171178. 10.1007/s10683-006-9159-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, D., & Prowse, V. (2012). A structural analysis of disappointment aversion in a real effort competition. American Economic Review. 102(1):469503, doi:10.1257/aer.102.1.469. Retrieved from http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/aea/aer/2012/00000102/00000001/art00015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gintis, H (2005). Behavioral game theory and contemporary economic theory. Analyse and Kritik, 27(1), 647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiner, B. (2004). The online recruitment system orsee: A guide for the organization of experiments in economics. Papers on Strategic Interaction 2003–10, Max Planck Institute of Economics, Strategic Interaction Group.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica. 47(1), 153–61. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/ecm/emetrp/v47y1979i1p153-61.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, G, & Makin, D (2004). Self-control, deviant peers, and software piracy. Psychological Reports, 95(3), 921931. 10.2466/pr0.95.3.921-931 15666930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houser, D., Schunk, D., Winter, J., & Xiao, E. (2010). Temptation, commitment and self-control in the laboratory. Technical Representative 488, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/p/zur/iewwpx/488.html.Google Scholar
Houser, D., Montinari, N., & Piovesan, M. (2012). Private and public decisions in social dilemmas: Evidence from children’s behavior. PLoS One. 7(8), e41,568. Retrieved from http://ideas.repec.org/p/gms/wpaper/1034.html.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, E., & Lazear, E.P. (1992). Peer pressure and partnerships. Journal of Political Economy. 100(4), 801817. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/ucp/jpolec/v100y1992i4p801-17.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, U., Downar, J., & Montague, P.R. (2011). Interoception drives increased rational decision-making in meditators playing the ultimatum game. Frontiers in neuroscience 5, DOI10.3389/fnins.2011.00049. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21559066.Google Scholar
Knoch, D, Pascual-Leoner, A, Meyer, K, Treyer, V, & Fehr, E (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314(5800), 829832 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD28XhtFKit73P 10.1126/science.1129156 17023614 2006Sci...314..829KCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, MD Fiske, ST, & Gilbert, GLDT (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. Handbook of social psychology, 5New York: McGraw-Hill 143193.Google Scholar
Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. The American Psychologist. 57(9), 705717. Retrieved from http://www.view.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12237980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 65(3), 272292, http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/eee/jobhdp/v65y1996i3p272-292.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G, & O’Donoghue, T (2007). The heat of the moment: Modeling interactions between affect and deliberation, New York: Mimeo.Google Scholar
Masclet, D., Noussair, C., Tucker, S., & Villeval, M.C. (2003). Monetary and non monetary punishment in the voluntary contributions mechanism. American Economic Review. 93(1), 366380. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/aea/aecrev/v93y2003i1p366-380.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olfeo. (2014). La réalité de l’utilisation d’internet au bureau, Etude Olfeo 2014.Google Scholar
Rachlin, H (2000). The science of self-control, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reuben, E., & van Winden, F. (2010). Fairness perceptions and prosocial emotions in the power-to-take. Journal of Economic Psychology. 31(6), 908922. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/eee/joepsy/v31y2010i6p908-922.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanfey, AG, Rilling, JK, Aronson, JA, Nystrom, LE, & Cohen, JD (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 17551758 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3sXksVKisLs%3D 10.1126/science.1082976 12805551 2003Sci...300.1755SCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thaler, R.H., & Shefrin, H.M. (1981). An economic theory of self-control. Journal of Political Economy. 89(2), 392406. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/ucp/jpolec/v89y1981i2p392-406.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y, & Fishbach, A (2000). Counteractive self-control in overcoming temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 493506 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3crgtVemtQ%3D%3D 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.493 11045735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, W.K. (2008). How much time-inconsistency is there and does it matter? evidence on self-awareness, size, and effects. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 68(3–4), 645656. Retrieved from http://www.ideas.repec.org/a/eee/jeborg/v68y2008i3-4p645-656.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Bonein and Denant-Boèmont supplementary material

Appendix
Download Bonein and Denant-Boèmont supplementary material(File)
File 740.1 KB