Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-wdhn8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-16T16:16:18.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive bubbles

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Ciril Bosch-Rosa*
Affiliation:
Berlin University of Technology, Berlin, Germany Colegio Universitario de Estudios Financieros, Madrid, Spain
Thomas Meissner
Affiliation:
Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Antoni Bosch-Domènech
Affiliation:
Universitat Pompeu Fabra and Barcelona GSE, Barcelona, Spain

Abstract

Smith et al. (Econometrica 56(5):1119, 1988) reported large bubbles and crashes in experimental asset markets, a result that has been replicated many times. Here we test whether the occurrence of bubbles depends on the experimental subjects’ cognitive sophistication. In a two-part experiment, we first run a battery of tests to assess the subjects’ cognitive sophistication and classify them into low or high levels. We then invite them separately to two asset market experiments populated only by subjects with either low or high cognitive sophistication. We observe classic bubble and crash patterns in markets populated by subjects with low levels of cognitive sophistication. Yet, no bubbles or crashes are observed with our sophisticated subjects, indicating that cognitive sophistication of the experimental market participants has a strong impact on price efficiency.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2017 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-017-9529-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Andersson, O, Tyran, J-R, Wengström, E, & Holm, H (2015). Risk aversion relates to cognitive ability: Preferences or noise?. Journal of the European Economic Association, 14(5), 11291154. 10.1111/jeea.12179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baghestanian, S, & Walker, TB (2015). Anchoring in experimental asset markets. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 116, 1525. 10.1016/j.jebo.2015.03.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caginalp, G, Porter, D, & Smith, V (1998). Initial cash/asset ratio and asset prices: An experimental study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(2), 756761. 10.1073/pnas.95.2.756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caginalp, G, Porter, D, & Smith, VL (2000). Overreactions, momentum, liquidity, and price bubbles in laboratory and field asset markets. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 1(1), 2448. 10.1207/S15327760JPFM0101_04CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, SL, Hedegaard, M, & Palan, S (2014). To see is to believe: Common expectations in experimental asset markets. European Economic Review, 66, 8496. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2013.11.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corgnet, B, Hernán-González, R, Kujal, P, & Porter, D (2015). The effect of earned versus house money on price bubble formation in experimental asset markets. Review of Finance, 19(4), 14551488. 10.1093/rof/rfu031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa-Gomes, MA, & Crawford, VP (2006). Cognition and behavior in two-person guessing games: An experimental study. The American Economic Review, 96(5), 17371768. 10.1257/aer.96.5.1737CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cueva, C, & Rustichini, A (2015). Is financial instability male-driven? Gender and cognitive skills in experimental asset markets. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 119, 330344. 10.1016/j.jebo.2015.08.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T, Falk, A, Huffman, D, & Sunde, U (2010). Are risk aversion and impatience related to cognitive ability?. The American Economic Review, 100(3), 12381260. 10.1257/aer.100.3.1238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufwenberg, M, Lindqvist, T, & Moore, E (2005). Bubbles and experience: An experiment. American Economic Review, 95(5), 17311737. 10.1257/000282805775014362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, CC, & Füllbrunn, S (2015). Thar SHE blows? Gender, competition, and bubbles in experimental asset markets. American Economic Review, 105(2), 906920. 10.1257/aer.20130683CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171178. 10.1007/s10683-006-9159-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 2542. 10.1257/089533005775196732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gneezy, U, Rustichini, A, & Vostroknutov, A (2010). Experience and insight in the race game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 75(2), 144155. 10.1016/j.jebo.2010.04.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiner, B (2015). Subject pool recruitment procedures: organizing experiments with ORSEE. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 1, 114125. 10.1007/s40881-015-0004-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Güth, W, Kocher, M, & Sutter, M (2002). Experimental beauty contests with homogeneous and heterogeneous players and with interior and boundary equilibria. Economics Letters, 74(2), 219228. 10.1016/S0165-1765(01)00544-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanaki, N., Akiyama, E., Funaki, Y., & Ishikawa, R. (2015). Diversity in cognitive ability enlarges mispricing, mimeo.Google Scholar
Haruvy, E, Lahav, Y, & Noussair, CN (2007). Traders’ expectations in asset markets: Experimental evidence. American Economic Review, 97(5), 19011920. 10.1257/aer.97.5.1901CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haruvy, E, & Noussair, CN (2006). The effect of short selling on bubbles and crashes in experimental spot asset markets. The Journal of Finance, 61(3), 11191157. 10.1111/j.1540-6261.2006.00868.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, CA, & Laury, SK (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5), 16441655. 10.1257/000282802762024700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, J, & Kirchler, M (2012). The impact of instructions and procedure on reducing confusion and bubbles in experimental asset markets. Experimental Economics, 15(1), 89105. 10.1007/s10683-011-9290-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hussam, RN, Porter, D, & Smith, VL (2008). Thar she blows: Can bubbles be rekindled with experienced subjects?. American Economic Review, 98(3), 924937. 10.1257/aer.98.3.924CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchler, M, Bonn, C, Huber, J, & Razen, M (2015). The “inflow-effect”-Trader inflow and price efficiency. European Economic Review, 77, 119. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2015.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchler, M, Huber, J, & Stöckl, T (2012). Thar she bursts: Reducing confusion reduces bubbles. American Economic Review, 102(2), 865883. 10.1257/aer.102.2.865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knott, JH (2012). The president, congress, and the financial crisis: Ideology and moral hazard in economic governance. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 42(1), 81100. 10.1111/j.1741-5705.2012.03942.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kocher, MG, & Sutter, M (2006). Time is money—Time pressure, incentives, and the quality of decision-making. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 61(3), 375392. 10.1016/j.jebo.2004.11.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, SD, List, JA, & Sadoff, SE (2011). Checkmate: Exploring backward induction among chess players. American Economic Review, 101(2), 975990. 10.1257/aer.101.2.975CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, KA, & Smith, VL (2000). A comparison of naive and sophisticated subject behavior with game theoretic predictions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(7), 37773781. 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3777CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagel, R (1995). Unraveling in guessing games: An experimental study. American Economic Review, 85(5), 13131326.Google Scholar
Noussair, C, Robin, S, & Ruffieux, B (2001). Price bubbles in laboratory asset markets with constant fundamental values. Experimental Economics, 4(1), 87105. 10.1023/A:1011445522861CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noussair, C, & Tucker, S (2006). Futures markets and bubble formation in experimental asset markets. Pacific Economic Review, 11(2), 167184. 10.1111/j.1468-0106.2006.00308.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noussair, CN, Tucker, SJ, & Xu, Y (2014). “A futures market reduces bubbles but allows greater profit for more sophisticated traders,” SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 2490326, Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network.Google Scholar
Oechssler, J, Roider, A, & Schmitz, PW (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 147152. 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palan, S (2013). A review of bubbles and crashes in experimental asset markets. Journal of Economic Surveys, 27(3), 570588. 10.1111/joes.12023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, L, & Winn, A (2014). Does money illusion matter? Comment. American Economic Review, 104(3), 10471062. 10.1257/aer.104.3.1047CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, DP, & Smith, VL (1995). Futures contracting and dividend uncertainty in experimental asset markets. Journal of Business, 68(4), 509541. 10.1086/296675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, VL, Boening, Mv, & Wellford, CP (2000). Dividend timing and behavior in laboratory asset markets. Economic Theory, 16(3), 567583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, VL, Suchanek, GL, & Williams, AW (1988). Bubbles, crashes, and endogenous expectations in experimental spot asset markets. Econometrica, 56(5), 1119 10.2307/1911361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stöckl, T, Huber, J, & Kirchler, M (2010). Bubble measures in experimental asset markets. Experimental Economics, 13(3), 284298. 10.1007/s10683-010-9241-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stöckl, T, Huber, J, & Kirchler, M (2014). Multi-period experimental asset markets with distinct fundamental value regimes. Experimental Economics, 18(2), 314334. 10.1007/s10683-014-9404-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, AW Plott, CR, & Smith, VL (2008). Chapter 29 price bubbles in large financial asset markets. Handbook of experimental economics results, Amsterdam: Elsevier 242246. 10.1016/S1574-0722(07)00029-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Bosch-Rosa et al. supplementary material

Appendix
Download Bosch-Rosa et al. supplementary material(File)
File 721 KB