Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-dlb68 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-17T06:58:31.903Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gender and generosity: does degree of anonymity or group gender composition matter?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

C. Bram Cadsby*
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON N1G 2W1, Canada
Maroš Servátka
Affiliation:
Department of Economics and Finance, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
Fei Song
Affiliation:
Ted Rogers School of Business Management, Ryerson University, Toronto, ON, Canada

Abstract

Employing a two-by-two factorial design that manipulates whether dictator groups are single or mixed-sex and whether procedures are single or double-blind, we examine gender effects in a standard dictator game. No gender effects were found in any of the experimental treatments for the mean or median levels of giving, or for the propensity to give nothing. However, females chose to give away half of their endowments with greater frequency than males in the pooled single-sex treatments.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Economic Science Association 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi: 10.1007/s10683-010-9242-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Andreoni, J., & Vesterlund, L. (2001). Which is the fair sex? Gender differences in altruism. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 293312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Ner, A., Kong, F., & Putterman, L. (2004). Share and share alike: gender-pairing, personality, and cognitive ability as determinants of giving. Journal of Economic Psychology, 25(5), 581589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolton, G., & Katok, E. (1995). An experimental test for gender differences in beneficent behavior. Economics Letters, 48(3-4), 287292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolton, G., & Ockenfels, A. (2000). ERC: a theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. American Economic Review, 90(1), 166193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boschini, A., Muren, A., & Persson, M. (2009). Constructing gender in the economics lab. www.ne.su.se/ paper/wp09_15.pdf. Accessed 19 April 2010.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: experiments on strategic interaction. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Charness, G. (2000). Responsibility and effort in an experimental labor market. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 42(3), 375384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G., & Rabin, M. (2002). Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(3), 817869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, D. J., & Kagel, J. H. (2009). Other regarding preferences: a selective survey of experimental results. http://www.econ.ohio-state.edu/kagel/Other_RegardingPreferences_survey.pdf. Accessed 19 April 2010. Forthcoming inGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
Cox, J. C. (2002). Trust, reciprocity, and other-regarding preferences: groups vs. individuals and males vs. females. In Zwick, R., & Rapoport, A. (Eds.), Experimental business research. Norwell: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Cox, J. C., & Deck, C. (2006). When are women more generous than men? Economic Inquiry, 44(4), 587598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufwenberg, M., & Muren, A. (2006). Generosity, anonymity, gender. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 61(1), 4249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (1998). Are women less selfish than men? Evidence from dictator experiments. Economic Journal, 108(448), 726735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (2008). Differences in the economic decisions of men and women: experimental evidence. In Plott, C., & Smith, V. (Eds.), Handbook of experimental economics results. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6(3), 347369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preference, property rights and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economics Behavior, 7(3), 346380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. (1996). Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games. American Economic Review, 86(3), 653660.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. Journal of Business, 59(4), 285300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kantowitz, B. H., Roediger, H. L., & Elmes, D. G. (2008). Experimental psychology (9th edn.). Belmont: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Rabin, M. (1993). Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics. American Economic Review, 83(5), 12811302.Google Scholar
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10(1), 8083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, F., Cadsby, C. B., & Morris, T. (2004). Other-regarding behavior and behavioral forecasts: females versus males as individuals and group representatives. International Journal of Conflict Management, 15(4), 340363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Cadsby et al. supplementary material

Experimental Materials Experiment Instructions - Double-Blind Procedure
Download Cadsby et al. supplementary material(File)
File 48.7 KB