Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T16:13:48.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Three forms of trust and their association

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2011

Ken Newton*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Southampton and Wissenschaftszentrum, Berlin, Germany
Sonja Zmerli*
Affiliation:
Institute of Political Science, Darmstadt University of Technology and Institute of Social and Political Research, Goethe-University Frankfurt/Main, Germany
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article investigates the relationships between particular social trust, general social trust, and political trust and tests a variety of political, social-psychological, and social capital theories of them. This sort of research has not been carried out before because until the World Values Survey of 2005–07 there has been, to our knowledge, no comparative survey that includes measures of particular and other forms of trust. The new data challenge a common assumption that particular social trust is either harmful or of little importance in modern democracies and shows that it has strong, positive associations with other forms of trust. However, the relationships are not symmetrical and particular social trust seems to be a necessary but not sufficient cause of general social trust, and both forms of social trust appear to be necessary, but not sufficient conditions for political trust. Strong evidence of mutual associations between different forms of trust at both the individual micro level and the contextual macro level supports theories of rainmaker effects, the importance of political institutions, and the significance of social trust for political trust. In more ways than one, social trust, not least of a particular type, seems to have an important bearing on social and political stability.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © European Consortium for Political Research 2011

Introduction: on social and political trust

In a recent and comprehensive overview of the voluminous and rapidly expanding literature, Peter Nannestad (Reference Nannestad2008: 432) concludes that ‘The question of trust is a huge puzzle that is not even near solution’. This article tries to solve a part of the puzzle exploring the three-cornered set of associations between particular social trust, general social trust, and political trust. This work focuses on only a part of the puzzle, but at the same time it is at the heart of the much larger project of political sociology conceived as a study of the relationship between politics and the wider society. It also goes to the heart of social capital theory and its claim that contextual effects (the rainmaker effect) have a strong impact on individual trust, as well as considering the impact of institutions on different forms of trust. In other words, it considers both micro theories of a bottom-up, social-psychological, and individual nature, and top–down theories that focus on government, social institutions, and aggregate levels of trust.

Much of the current interest in trust derives from social capital theory, which argues that social trust is intimately related to political trust and hence to the health and stability of democracy. It is said that the slow erosion of social trust, and the voluntary associations and social networks that help to produce it, explain the declining levels of support for political leaders and the institutions of government across much of the western world (Putnam, Reference Putnam2000, Reference Putnam2002; Dalton, Reference Dalton2004). In this regard, many writers find it useful to distinguish between particular and general social trust, reasoning that the latter is most important for social capital in a large-scale, mobile, and socially mixed society – a society of strangers (Sahlins, Reference Sahlins1972; Newton, Reference Newton1999: 20–21; Warren, Reference Warren1999: 8–12). However, there is considerable disagreement in the contemporary literature about (1) the nature and the origins of particular and general social trust, (2) the relationship of particular trust to general trust, and (3) the relationship of different forms of social trust to political trust. Hence, there is also disagreement about what combinations of different forms of trust contribute to social and political stability.

This paper examines the nature, associations, and origins of three kinds of trust. The first part discusses the nature of different kinds of trust, and the second focuses on three models of them to be found in the voluminous and growing social science literature. The third part describes the data used in the study, together with some preliminary empirical results on which the methods and approaches are based. The fourth part proceeds by way of correlation and multi-level analysis to examine the associations between the three types of trust and the independent variables linked with them. The final section returns to the three models of trust, drawing some conclusions about them and more general issues.

The nature of particular, general, and political trust

Particular and general social trust

For some, particular trust is based on knowledge of and close contact with others gained from close and constant contact with others. Hence, Hardin states, ‘for me to trust you, I have to know a fair amount about you’ (Hardin, Reference Hardin2000: 34). Similarly, Rose (Reference Rose1994: 29) finds that east Europeans ‘know who they can trust and trust who they know’, which is generally a fairly small circle of family, friends, and colleagues (see also Luhmann, Reference Luhmann1979: 43; Yamagishi and Yamagishi, Reference Yamagishi and Yamagishi1994; Offe, Reference Offe1999: 56). In contrast, Uslaner (2000–Reference Uslaner2001: 573, Reference Uslaner2008a: 102) refers to knowledge-based trust as strategic trust, and distinguishes it from particular trust, which is ‘faith only in your own kind’. Strangers, he says, are presumed to be untrustworthy, and he gives the example of religious fundamentalists who see non-believers as heathens.

Empirically, there is probably a good deal of overlap between ‘knowledge based’ particular trust and ‘own-kind’ particular trust, but theoretically they are distinct. In daily life, most people probably come into closest contact with people like themselves, and they probably develop their most trusting relations with people they come into close and constant contact with, but we cannot assume either of these claims. Do people usually, invariably, or inevitably trust others because they are of the same family, church, ethnic group, neighbourhood, profession, class, status, gender, or generation? Do we trust the person in the next office because he or she is of the same class, status, education, age, ethnic group, and nationality as us? These are subjects for empirical research rather than matters of definition.

In this article, we try to avoid empirical speculation by defining particular trust in a neutral way. Following the standard Oxford English Dictionary definition, the word ‘particular’ is used here as an adjective describing the noun ‘trust’ where social trust is associated with specific people or groups of people, whether known or in-group others. General trust is not limited in this way. It extends in a more abstract manner to people as a whole in an unselective and unspecific manner. General trust is inclusive. General trust is the belief that most people can be trusted, even if you do not know them personally, and even if they are not like you socially (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2001Reference Uslaner2002: 573). Accordingly, Misztal (Reference Misztal1996: 72) suggests that social trust ranges along a continuum from the personal (particular) to the abstract (general), while others see this as a continuum of ‘thick’ and ‘thin’ trust, the latter being a basis of the weak, low-density ties of large-scale society (Granovetter, Reference Granovetter1973, Reference Granovetter1983).

Since it is often assumed that general trust is most important for modern western society, most comparative research ignores particular trust and what little research is published on the subject is impressionistic or else based on plausible assumptions or limited case studies. To our knowledge, there was no systematic comparative study of particular social trust until the World Values survey of 2005–07, the basis of this work, which breaks new research ground and produces new and novel conclusions. However, it seems reasonable to hypothesize with Uslaner (Reference Uslaner1999: 123) that everyone must trust someone, and consequently that particular trust is fairly widespread in all societies. Much more is known about general social trust, which is a relatively rare commodity in some countries because it involves risks, unknowns, and short-cuts (Luhmann, Reference Luhmann1988; Kollock, Reference Kollock1994: 319; Misztal, Reference Misztal1996: 18). In only eight of the 93 nations covered in the World Values study of 2000 does more than half the population say that ‘most people can be trusted’, and five of these are in the small group of affluent nations in northwestern Europe. In another 19 nations, mostly Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development countries, between a third and a half express attitudes of general trust, but in much of central and east Europe, Africa, Latin America, and Asia, a minority of 25% is willing to express general trust (Newton, Reference Newton2007: 346). Nevertheless, general trust is vital to modern, large-scale, mobile, and heterogeneous society in which the weak ties of daily life require risk-taking with strangers and casual acquaintances.

The relationship between particular and general social trust

Recent studies have begun to uncover the relationship between particular and general social trust, but with very different results. Some have found that they are distinct and sometimes incompatible sets of attitudes (Yamagishi and Yamagishi, Reference Yamagishi and Yamagishi1994: 144; Yamagishi et al., Reference Yamagishi, Jin and Miller1998; Newton, Reference Newton1999: 180; Stolle, Reference Stolle2001, Reference Stolle2002; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002: 54), while others find that the two can coexist, or that specific trust can promote general trust (Whiteley, Reference Whiteley1999; Yosano and Hayashi, Reference Yosano and Hayashi2005; Glanville and Paxton, Reference Glanville and Paxton2007; Freitag and Traunmüller, Reference Freitag and Traunmüller2009). A third possibility is suggested in an experimental study finding cultural differences between Americans, who tend to trust strangers if they are of the same in-group as themselves, and Japanese, who place greater trust in strangers if they are members of the same personal social network, irrespective of in-group/out-group differences (Yuki et al., Reference Yuki, Maddux, Brewer and Takemura2005). The inconsistent results of research add to the puzzle of trust; are particular and general social trust mutually exclusive or mutually supportive, or does this depend on culture and context?

Social and political trust

Another major part of the trust puzzle is the association between different forms of social and political trust. For some years, empirical research failed to deliver clear support for the social capital theory that social and political trust are linked at the individual level. At best, the evidence was weak and patchy, and at worst it showed no clear associations between social and political trust (Wright, Reference Wright1976: 104–110; Craig, Reference Craig1993: 27; Orren, Reference Orren1997; Hall, Reference Hall1999; Kaase, Reference Kaase1999; Newton, Reference Newton1999: 180–185, Reference Newton2001, Reference Newton2006a: 84–85; Newton and Norris, Reference Newton and Norris2000: 62–66; Uslaner, 2000–Reference Uslaner2001: 586, Reference Uslaner2002, Reference Uslaner2008a: 111; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005). At the same time, there is better evidence that cross-national levels of aggregate social and political trust are positively associated (Inglehart, Reference Inglehart1997, Reference Inglehart1999; Rothstein, Reference Rothstein1998; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Pharr and Dalton2000: 26; Newton and Norris, Reference Newton and Norris2000: 52–73; Newton, Reference Newton2001; Paxton, Reference Paxton2002; Torpe, Reference Torpe2003; Freitag, Reference Freitag2003a; Van der Meer, Reference Van der Meer2003; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Kumlin and Rothstein, Reference Kumlin and Rothstein2005; Kumlin, Reference Kumlin2007; Oskarson, Reference Oskarson2007; Svallfors, Reference Svallfors2007; Gabriel and Walter-Rogg, Reference Gabriel and Walter-Rogg2008; Listhaug and Ringdal, Reference Listhaug and Ringdal2008; Rothstein and Stolle, Reference Rothstein and Stolle2008a, Reference Rothstein and Stolleb; Freitag and Bühlmann, Reference Freitag and Bühlmann2009).

More recent studies, however, have found stronger associations between social and political trust at the individual level. Country studies in the United States, Switzerland, Germany, Sweden and Japan find a close tie between general social and political trust (Freitag Reference Freitag2003a, Reference Freitagb; Glanville and Paxton, Reference Glanville and Paxton2007; Bäck and Kestilä, Reference Bäck and Kestilä2008) and cross-national studies at the individual levels have produced similar evidence (Jagodzinski and Manabe, Reference Jagodzinski and Manabe2004: 85–97; Zmerli and Newton, Reference Zmerli and Newton2008; Freitag and Bühlmann, Reference Freitag and Bühlmann2009). The conclusion that social and political trust are, indeed, associated at both individual and cross-national aggregate levels revives a range of research questions concerning the political importance of social capital.

Although bits of the picture have been filled in, the puzzle of trust remains, not least the triangular set of relations between particular and general social trust and political trust. A reading of the large and growing social science literature suggests that there are three main models of trust. One claims that different types of social and political trust tend to fit together in a mutually reinforcing pattern, another argues the opposite, and the third suggests a more complicated pattern in which some but not all forms of trust fit together. We will refer to these as the compatible model, the incompatible model and the conditional model.

Three models of social and political trust

Model 1: the compatible model

The simplest model presents all three types of trust as a single syndrome; those who are trusting in one realm of life are usually trusting in the others. Two main schools of thought contribute to this view. The first is the macro approach to social capital arguing that social and political structures and institutions are major influences on levels of trust. Societies with dense networks of social relations and voluntary associations, and with embedded institutions that enforce or encourage trustworthy behaviour (police, courts, civil service and welfare institutions), will develop high levels of social and political trust. This, in turn, will reinforce the institutions and norms of civil society and so create a virtuous spiral (Putnam, Reference Putnam1993, Reference Putnam2000; Rothstein, Reference Rothstein1998; Tyler, Reference Tyler1998; Weingast, Reference Weingast1998; Rahn et al., Reference Rahn, Brehm and Carlson1999; Knack, Reference Knack2000; Levi and Stoker, Reference Levi and Stoker2000; Newton and Norris, Reference Newton and Norris2000; Paxton, Reference Paxton2002; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Inglehart and Welzel, Reference Inglehart and Welzel2005; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2008b).

Social psychology offers a second theory for expecting that most forms of trust are closely associated in the same individual. Glanville and Paxton (Reference Glanville and Paxton2007) label it ‘the psychological propensity model’. It argues that trust is a core personality characteristic, learned mainly in early life and intimately linked with other personality characteristics, especially a sense of control over life, a belief in interpersonal cooperation and a sunny and optimistic disposition. In contrast, the distrusting are a misanthropic breed with a dismal view of fate, human nature, and the possibility of cooperation (Erikson, Reference Erikson1950; Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1956, Reference Rosenberg1957; Allport, Reference Allport1961; Cattell, Reference Cattell1965; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner1999: 138, Reference Uslaner2002: 79–86; 2000–Reference Uslaner2001: 571). If the social psychology school is right, trust and distrust of different kinds are unlikely to mix in the same individual.

The two theories are supported by some empirical evidence. Uslaner (Reference Uslaner2002: 32–33), for example, finds a small and positive correlation between particular and general social trust, and Herring et al. (Reference Herring, Jankowski and Brown1999) find little evidence in their study of African–Americans to suggest that strong in-group identity is associated with strong out-group dislike. Whiteley (Reference Whiteley1999) finds that measures of particular and general trust form a single cumulative scale. Bahry et al. (Reference Bahry2005: 525, 529) find positive correlations between in-group and out-group trust and conclude that ‘Faith in one’s own and faith in major out-groups are not mutually exclusive, but complementary’. Glanville and Paxton (Reference Glanville and Paxton2007: 240) find no evidence that trust in any one domain (particular trust) hinders the development of general trust.

Other research finds a mutually reinforcing relationship between democracy, social trust, and political trust (Putnam, Reference Putnam1993, Reference Putnam2000: 136–137; Knack and Keefer, Reference Knack and Keefer1997; Inglehart, Reference Inglehart1999; Booth and Richard, Reference Booth and Richard2001; Newton, Reference Newton2001; Paxton, Reference Paxton2002; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005). Zmerli and Newton (Reference Zmerli and Newton2008) and Zmerli et al. (Reference Zmerli, Newton and Montero2007) find a strong and significant positive correlation between individual general social trust and political trust in two separate studies covering democracies in Europe and the United States. Conversely, the political systems of the communist bloc generated both low political and general social trust (Sztompka, Reference Sztompka2000; Mishler, and Rose Reference Mishler and Rose2001).

The empirical implications of the compatibility model are that (hypothesis 1) the three types of trust are positively associated. However, (hypothesis 2) while social psychologists argue that this is a feature of individual personality characteristics, (hypothesis 3) social capital theorists argue that the association may also be influenced by top–down social climates of trust and by social and political institutions that reinforce trustworthiness.

Model 2: the incompatible model

The second model is almost, but not quite, the reverse of the compatible model. It is based on reasons for believing that particular social trust may be incompatible with both general social trust and political trust. In his influential study of ‘Montegrano’ in southern Italy, Banfield (Reference Banfield1958) argues that the local culture of amoral familism entailed trust in the family and automatic distrust of all others, including politicians who are presumed to be only self-interested. More recent work also argues that interpersonal trust (particular trust) is not an instance of a more general impersonal phenomenon, and nor can it simply be transferred to others or to other contexts (Cohen, Reference Cohen1999: 221). Hooghe (Reference Hooghe2008: 578) and Hardin (Reference Hardin2002) say it should not be assumed that we will trust strangers simply because we trust the people we know. There is little theory or evidence to tell us how particular trust can be extended to a general form, which leaves a hole in the theory claiming that the particular can lead to the general.

Some research goes an important step further by claiming that particular and general trust are inimical. A recent study of Russia points out that particular trust is generally assumed to be a zero-sum entity (Bahry et al., Reference Bahry2005: 525). Uslaner (Reference Uslaner1999: 124–125) writes, ‘the more dependent we are on our close associates and kin, the more we think of the world in terms of “we” and “they”. We won’t trust “most people” ’ (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner1999: 124). Warren (Reference Warren1999: 318) remarks that particular trust in members of the same family, clan, or group is often combined with general distrust of strangers. There is evidence that particular and general trust load on discrete factors (Yamagishi and Yamagishi, Reference Yamagishi and Yamagishi1994; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002: 54), and that general trust is weak among members of associations with strong in-group trust (Stolle, Reference Stolle1998: 503–504).

Other empirical work finds little evidence of associations between social and political trust at either individual or aggregate levels (Kaase, Reference Kaase1999: Newton, Reference Newton2006a: 84–86, Reference Newton2006b). This may be because the two are different things, social trust being influenced largely by social variables, including class, education, and membership of voluntary associations, and political trust being associated mainly with political variables such as political interest, party identification, and use of the political media (Abramson, Reference Abramson1983; Lawrence, Reference Lawrence1997; Mishler and Rose, Reference Mishler and Rose2001; Anderson and LoTempio, Reference Anderson and LoTempio2002).

The empirical implications of the incompatibility model are that there will be (hypothesis 4) a negative correlation between particular and general social trust, and (hypothesis 5) a negative or random association between social trust and political trust.

Model 3: the conditional model

The third model of trust suggests that there is no necessary incompatibility between particular and general social trust, and that the two may be positively associated in some cases, but not necessarily in most or all cases. There are three reasons for advancing this possibility, one logical and two empirical.

The logical argument is simple: it is difficult to conceive of an individual who is high on general trust but low on particular trust, since someone who trusts in general must logically trust in particular. The reverse is not true, however: to trust particular and selected others does not necessarily entail trusting people in general. At the same time, since everyone must trust someone, total distrust is probably as rare as total trust. Even the inhabitants of Montegrano had, perforce, to trust their own family, while distrusting all others.

The second argument for the conditional model is drawn from social psychology research. Much of it assumes, at least implicitly, that in-group identity is necessarily associated with out-group hostility (cf. Brewer, Reference Brewer1999: 430), but recent work shows that in-group attachment is independent of attitudes towards out-groups. Distrust of out-groups depends partly on competition for resources, how much the in-group feels threatened, and on the trade-off between the benefits of closure of in-group boundaries versus the opportunities of opening up to outside groups (Brewer, Reference Brewer1979, Reference Brewer1999, Reference Brewer2007; Yamagishi et al., Reference Yamagishi, Jin and Miller1998; Hewstone et al., Reference Hewstone, Rubin and Willis2002: 575–604; Voci, Reference Voci2006).

The third argument supporting the conditional model of trust concerns the wider cultural and institutional context in which in-groups and out-groups exist. Lines of demarcation can change turning out-groups into in-groups, or vice versa. Bates et al. (Reference Bates, Figueiredo and Weingast1998) gave an example of how political change caused Serbs and Croats to turn from being peaceful social groups with a high rate of inter-marriage into hostile in- and out-groups. In times of war and crisis, national populations tend to forget their internal differences and concentrate on common and external problems. Liberal and egalitarian cultures may make it easier in some countries to combine particular and general social trust compared with other countries, and countries with established, corruption-free, and power-sharing democratic governments, low levels of inequality, Protestant traditions, a strong rule of law, and universal social services seem to make it easier for individuals to combine all three forms of trust (Freitag, Reference Freitag2003a; Rothstein and Stolle, Reference Rothstein and Stolle2003; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Rothstein and Uslaner, Reference Rothstein and Uslaner2005; Kumlin and Rothstein, Reference Kumlin and Rothstein2005; Neller, Reference Neller2008; Freitag and Bühlmann, Reference Freitag and Bühlmann2009), compared with non-democratic countries where particular social trust seems to accompany a lack of political and general social trust.

Evidence of conditionality can be found in the very different social contexts of Russia and Germany. Along with other studies of Russia, Bahry et al. (Reference Bahry2005: 530) find that some individuals express high particular with low general social trust, but they also find others who combine high particular and general social trust. Freitag and Traunmüller (Reference Freitag and Traunmüller2009) also find evidence of an overlap between general and particular trust in Germany. Trusting one’s own group, or particular other ethnic groups, does not necessarily preclude a high level of general social trust, but is not necessarily associated with it either. It may depend upon circumstances.

The conditional model suggests (hypothesis 6) that while all those expressing general trust must necessarily express specific trust, the reverse is not necessarily true and that (hypothesis 7) not all of those who trust in particular will necessarily trust in general, although (hypothesis 8) some will combine both forms of social trust. Similarly, (hypothesis 9) the more open, democratic, and egalitarian a society the more its citizens will combine all three forms of trust.

A note of caution

Often assumed to be much less important than general social trust for modern society, particular trust has slipped into the background of research. The recent Handbook of Social Capital (Castiglione et al., Reference Castiglione, van Deth and Wolleb2008) mentions it once, in passing, in its 720 pages. There are a few single-country surveys that include questions about both general and particular social trust, alongside questions about political trust, but until the most recent World Values survey we know of no cross-national study that includes batteries of questions about all three kinds of trust. Therefore, the three models outlined above are built on weak foundations of formal logic, brave assumptions, and some contradictory evidence, some of it circumstantial or based on single case studies. All three models have a prima facie plausibility, but Models 1 and 2 are contradictory and both cannot be right.

Data and methods

Since this study focuses on the relationship between social and political trust, it selects from the World Values survey of 2005–07 a set of countries with the highest democratic scores in the Polity IV project. There is little sense in analysing survey responses to questions about political trust in non-democratic countries, where, apart from anything else, the absence of freedom of speech makes it difficult to give honest answers. In 78 countries covered in the 1999–2000 World Values study, confidence in parliament was highest in Vietnam and China, with scores of 98% and 95%, respectively (Newton, Reference Newton2007: 347). One of the longest and most advanced democracies in the world, Norway, had the same political trust score as Iran (70%), and Sweden (51%) was barely higher than Zimbabwe (50%). Combining Polity IV democracy scores of 9 and 10 and the World Values data on trust produces a list of 22 democratic countries distributed across Europe, Asia, Africa, Oceania, and North and South America.Footnote 1 The World Values survey was carried out between 2005 and 2007. Sampling and fieldwork methods varied but included face-to-face interviews and mailed questionnaires.Footnote 2 In the 22 selected democracies, 29,163 respondents participated in the survey. In order to test for macro- and micro-level effects on our three individual types of trust, we run multi-level analyses that control for varying intercepts.Footnote 3 The 2005–07 World Values survey includes a battery of questions about particular and general social trust as well as confidence in political institutions.

Social trust

The World Values survey asks a set of six questions about social trust.

I’d like to ask you how much you trust people from various groups. Could you tell me for each whether you trust people from this group completely, somewhat, not very much or not at all?

Your family

Your neighbourhood

People you know personally

People you meet for the first time

People of another religion

People of another nationality.

The first three deal with forms of particular trust involving known others with whom respondents have close ties (family and those they know personally) and people in their neighbourhood. These three measures cover particular social trust in the sense that they ask about particular, specific, and selected others. The last three questions cover general trust in unspecified and possibly different others who are not known personally. In addition, World Values asks: ‘Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you can’t be too careful in dealing with people?’. This is the ‘standard’ general social trust question used in many surveys. Respondents are offered a ‘Yes/No’ option for this question, as against the four rating scale of the other six questions, which makes comparison difficult, but it provides a useful benchmark against which to judge the general nature of responses to the last three questions, and vice versa.

Principal component analysis of the pooled social trust data for 22 countries yields a two component solution, one weighted primarily on the measures of particular trust in known others and the other on the four measures of general trust (Table 1). This confirms the validity of the three general trust questions, and vice versa (see also Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002: 54). Although the six or seven trust measures cluster in two groups, these are not completely distinct, because trust in the neighbourhood and in people known personally show positive loadings on both components, though not the heaviest ones. This suggests that there are two types of social trust corresponding to the particular–general distinction, but that they overlap to some degree.Footnote 4 Similar results have appeared in national studies of the United States, Germany, and Japan (Yosano and Hayashi, Reference Yosano and Hayashi2005; Glanville and Paxton, Reference Glanville and Paxton2007; Freitag and Traunmüller, Reference Freitag and Traunmüller2009), but Table 1 provides cross-national confirmation of these studies. It is consistent with empirical propositions 6 and 7 of the conditional model.

Table 1 Principal component analyses of measures of social trust, promax rotation, structure matrix, with and without the standard general trust item

The newly released World Values survey integrated data set presents equilibrated weights for the 2005 survey in which N = 1000 or 1500. All tables in this paper are based on equilibrated data in which N = 1000.

Political trust

The World Values survey asks the same questions about a set of six political and governmental institutions as follows:

I am going to name a number of organisations. For each one, could you tell me how much confidence you have in them: is it a great deal of confidence, quite a lot of confidence, not very much confidence or none at all?

The organisations named are parliament, the government, political parties, the justice system, the civil service, and the police. The World Values survey also asks about a further set of private institutions, but since this study focuses on interpersonal social trust and political trust, they are not included in this study.

In common with previous research (Listhaug and Wiberg, Reference Listhaug and Wiberg1995; Zmerli and Newton, Reference Zmerli and Newton2008), Table 2 shows that principal component analysis of the six World Values questions tapping political confidence reveals a single component. As with social trust, the principal component analysis shows a common cross-national pattern of responses to political confidence questions.

Table 2 Principal component analysis of six measures of confidence in political institutions

Countries weighted by equilibrated weight (N = 1000).

At this stage, we could conduct two kinds of analysis, one focusing on country variations and explanations of them, the other trying to build high-level cross-national generalizations about trust, irrespective of national differences. Notwithstanding the existence of national variations that are both interesting and important, this article chooses the second approach. The fact that different types of trust cluster in a clearly patterned way when individual data for 22 nations are pooled shows that it is also meaningful to take individuals as the unit of analysis, leaving national comparisons for later work. Correlation and multi-level analysis (see below) support this approach. In addition, analysing pooled individual data across 22 nations helps to set a broadly based framework of research within which particular country variations may be more meaningfully examined at a later stage.

The second choice involves using trust measures based either on factor loadings or indices. We choose to use indices here.Footnote 5 The first index measures particular trust in family, neighbours, and people known personally, and the second measures general trust in people met for the first time and those of other nationalities and religions. Both indices are based on the 4-point rating scale of the trust questions, ranging from 1 (low trust) to 4 (high trust) and have 13 parameter values. The standard general social trust question is dropped from the analysis for three reasons. First, as the authors have shown elsewhere (Zmerli and Newton, Reference Zmerli and Newton2008), interviewees are offered a ‘Yes/No’ response option that does not discriminate as effectively as a 4-item rating scale, and second, a single-item measure of social trust is not as sensitive as a 3-item scale. Third, as the figures in Table 1 show, the 3-item scale in the World Values survey is a better measure of general social trust than the standard question. The political trust index is built in the same way using the six political confidence questionnaire items. It ranges from 1 (low trust) to 4 (high trust) and has 37 parameter values.

The study proceeds by way of correlation analysis to show the strength, direction, and consistency of the associations between the three types of trust in the 22 countries, before moving to multi-level analysis to test the strength of their associations with each other and with other individual and contextual variables.

Analysis and results

Cross-national patterns

Table 3 shows the distribution of national trust scores ranked in descending order of general trust. As hypothesized, particular trust is higher than general trust in every country and usually by a substantial margin. The average general trust score for all countries weighted equally is 2.3 on a 4-point scale. For particular trust it is 3.3, which is 43% higher. However, the hypothesis that general social trust is more common than political trust is not supported by the figures. Across the 22 countries, political and general social trust have the same mean of 2.3 and in 13 countries political trust is equal to, or higher than, general social trust. High levels of political compared with general trust are found in a mixture of countries, including South Africa, Switzerland, Finland, India, Cyprus, and Germany, which adds to the puzzle.

Table 3 Means of social and political trust by country, ranked by general social trust

aEntries are based on the weighted means of the three trust indices. Average scores and Ns are based on World values equilibrated weight (N = 1000). Countries in order of descending mean of general social trust.

Correlations between the three types of trust show that they are positively and significantly associated (Table 4). All correlations except two are statistically significant at 0.01, and those between particular and general trust are most generally the highest, as one might expect.Footnote 6 The correlations are generally within the same, rather small range in each column showing, once again, strong cross-national consistency. One surprise, however, is that in 10 of the 22 cases, the association between political and particular social trust is stronger than that between political and general social trust. Therefore, as social capital theory would have it, there is a close and positive correspondence between social and political trust at the individual level in all countries, but in some countries particular trust is a more important correlate than general trust. These results are not consistent with the incompatible model of trust, and seem to fit the conditional model better than does the compatible model. Nevertheless, the strong association between particular social and political trust is unexpected.Footnote 7 It also seems from Table 3 that all three forms of trust are generally higher in wealthier and established democracies, a finding that is consistent with previous cross-national studies (Inglehart, Reference Inglehart1997, Reference Inglehart1999; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Newton, Reference Newton2007: 347).

Table 4 Correlations between indices of three types of trust. Pearson’s r, by country

aResults are based on equilibrated weights (N = 1000); ***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05.

The claim that everyone must trust someone is confirmed by the fact that of the 28,031 individuals giving valid answers, only 31 (0.1%) claimed to have no social trust of any of the six kinds. Similarly, no more than 0.6% professed the strange combination of general but not particular trust, a finding that is consistent with hypothesis 6. It may be that 0.1% and 0.6% figures are noise created by interviewer error, miscoding, or failure on the part of those interviewed to understand or hear the questions.Footnote 8 Particular trust is the normal position for nine out of ten people in democratic countries, where 92% express high particular trust (score 2.6 or more on the particular trust index). It is virtually impossible to develop a sense of general trust in the absence of particular trust, but particular trust is not automatically associated with general trust. No less than 99% of those with high general trust have high particular trust, whereas only 45% of those with high particular trust also have high general trust. This combination of figures is consistent with the conditional model, but not with the compatible model or the incompatible model. Nor are the figures consistent with the social psychology school claiming that there is a personality type for whom trust is a core characteristic. It seems that people choose where to place their trust, distinguishing between those close to them (family, neighbours, and those they know well) and those who are not (members of other nations, religions, and those they have just met). Trust is not a general personality characteristic, but something involving choices and distinctions, a suggestion that fits the conditional model.

There is a similar asymmetry between social and political trust, in which the great majority of those with political trust are socially trusting, but only one third to 40% of those who are socially trusting are also politically trusting. This also suggests that social trust, either particular or general, is a necessary but not a sufficient cause of political trust. This also suggests that social trust conditions political trust, although it also indicates an unexpectedly important role for particular trust.

In the next stage of the analysis, we use multi-level modelling in order to put the associations between different forms of trust to a more rigorous test and to estimate the effects of individual and contextual variables on them.

Multi-level analysis

A reading of the already voluminous literature on trust suggests a fairly short and consistent list of individual variables associated with it, namely membership of voluntary associations, education, church attendance, age, gender. For political trust, we can add political identity and political interest.Footnote 9 The list of contextual variables covers corruption, ethnic heterogeneity, social equality, and various measures of democratic development, durability, and performance.

Individual variables

Associational membership

The enormous importance attached by writers from de Tocqueville to John Stuart Mill to modern studies of social capital, civil society, and voluntary activity should make this a particularly strong determinant of both social and political trust. However, recent summaries of the very large and growing literature on voluntary associations and trust reveal very different, inconclusive, and sometimes contrary findings (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002; Stolle, Reference Stolle2007: 667–669; Hooghe, Reference Hooghe2008: 568–593; Rossteutscher, Reference Rossteutscher2008: 216–224; Rothstein and Stolle, Reference Rothstein and Stolle2008a, Reference Rothstein and Stolleb: 277). The relationship between trust and voluntary activity remains an important part of the trust puzzle.

Education

As Uslaner (Reference Uslaner2008a: 108) observes, ‘virtually every study of generalized trust, in every setting, has found that education is a powerful predictor of trust’.

Gender and age

Gender and age have been included as standard control variables in most trust studies, although their effects are usually small and variable. Women are sometimes less trusting than men, and the old sometimes more trusting than the young, but this pattern varies over time and from one country to another (Newton, Reference Newton1999: 182–183; Whiteley, Reference Whiteley1999: 34–35).

Church attendance

As a proxy for conservative value orientations, church attendance has proven to be positively linked with political trust (see, e.g. Zmerli, Reference Zmerli2004).

Political interest and identity

Social trust may be more strongly associated with social variables compared with political trust, which is more closely related to political variables, especially political interest, partisanship, and support for the ruling party or parties, and the left–right scale (Newton, Reference Newton1999; Newton and Norris, Reference Newton and Norris2000; Rothstein and Stolle, Reference Rothstein and Stolle2008b: 282–283). Unfortunately, left–right, partisanship, and government support are either not available in the World values data or else produce a large number of missing cases, and therefore only political interest can be included as an independent variable.

Contextual variables

Homogeneity and ethnic fractionalization

Social heterogeneity is often argued to be a cause of low trust (Helliwell, Reference Helliwell1996; Knack and Keefer, Reference Knack and Keefer1997; Hero, Reference Hero1998, Reference Hero2003; Alesina and La Ferrara, Reference Alesina and La Ferrara2000; Glaeser et al., Reference Glaeser, Laibson, Scheinkman and Soutter2000; Zak and Knack, Reference Zak and Knack2001; Costa and Kahn, Reference Costa and Kahn2003; Eisenberg, Reference Eisenberg2006). The indicator used here is the Alesina and La Ferrara index of ethnic fractionalization (Reference Alesina and La Ferrara2002).

Corruption

Corruption in public life is also argued to be a cause of low trust (Svensson, Reference Svensson1998; Alesina et al., Reference Alesina, Baqir and Easterly1999; Annett, Reference Annett1999; Easterly, Reference Easterly2000; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2005).

Democratic quality and durability

The better established and developed a democratic system, the more likely it is to sustain comparatively high levels of social and political trust (Dunn, Reference Dunn1993; Putnam, Reference Putnam1993: 111–115; Anderson and Guillory, Reference Anderson and Guillory1997; Brehm and Rahn, Reference Brehm and Rahn1997; Inglehart, Reference Inglehart1999; Newton and Norris, Reference Newton and Norris2000: 70; Booth and Richard, Reference Booth and Richard2001: 55; Paxton, Reference Paxton2002). This study uses the duration of democracy and the Economist quality of democracy index as its measures.

Government effectiveness and performance

The more effectively and impartially a political system performs for its citizens, the more likely it is to generate high levels of political trust and to sustain high levels of social trust (Knack and Keefer, Reference Knack and Keefer1997: 1275–1276; Tyler, Reference Tyler1998; Levi and Stoker, Reference Levi and Stoker2000; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002: 223–229; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Rothstein and Stolle, Reference Rothstein and Stolle2008a, Reference Rothstein and Stolleb). Three World Bank indicators are used, measuring government effectiveness, rule of law, and regulatory quality.

Equality and inequality

Previous research shows social and economic equality to have a positive association with social and political trust (Putnam, Reference Putnam2000: 354–361; Warren, Reference Warren2004: 143; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2008a: 113). The indicator used here is the Gini index.Footnote 10

In addition, we intend to scrutinize whether and to what extent the interrelationships between our three types of trust are affected by country-specific variations of the contextual variables. For this purpose, we create cross-level interactions and include them in the multi-level models.

Tables 5, 6 and 7 present multi-level analyses of each type of trust at the individual level. Since the macro variables create problems of colinearity, each table presents them separately. Each set of tables includes both macro and micro measures of the other two types of trust as independent variables in order to test for their aggregate (‘rainmaker’ effects) and individual level effects on other types of trust.Footnote 11 Each set of tables includes both fixed effects based on the cross-national consistencies in the pooled individual-level data for 22 countries and cross-level interactions.

Table 5 Macro- and micro-level determinants of particular social trust, multi-level analysis, and random intercept models

Table 5 (Continued)

***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05; for the coding of the variables see Appendix. Predictors are centred on their grand means. Countries weighted by equilibrated weight (N = 1000); explained variances calculated according to Bryk and Raudenbush (Reference Bryk and Raudenbush1992). The intra-class correlation coefficient based on the empty model indicates that 83.1% of the variance are explained by the first level and 16.9% by the second level.

Table 6 Macro- and micro-level determinants of general social trust, multi-level analysis, and random intercept models

Table 6 (Continued)

***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05; for the coding of the variables see Appendix. Predictors are centred on their grand means. Countries weighted by equilibrated weight (N = 1000); explained variances calculated according to Bryk and Raudenbush (Reference Bryk and Raudenbush1992). The intra-class correlation coefficient based on the empty model indicates that 74.7% of the variance are explained by the first level and 25.3% by the second level.

Table 7 Macro- and micro-level determinants of political trust, multi-level analysis, random intercept models

Table 7 (Continued)

***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05; for the coding of the variables see Appendix. Predictors are centred on their grand means. Countries weighted by equilibrated weight (N = 1000); explained variances calculated according to Bryk and Raudenbush (Reference Bryk and Raudenbush1992). The intra-class correlation coefficient based on the empty model indicates that 81.2% of the variance are explained by the first level and 18.8% by the second level.

Together, the multi-level tables present a rich collection of statistics, from which we may draw six general conclusions. First, the numerous significant cross-level interaction effects show significant country variations, but the fixed effects also show strong patterns in the pooled individual-level data for the 22 countries. There are, in other words, generally strong consistencies across countries as well as variations between them. Second, the trust levels of a country as a whole have a significant impact on individual levels. Consistent with the rainmaker hypothesis (hypothesis 3), aggregate social trust of the particular and general variants have large and significant impacts on individual levels of social and political trust. This evidence is also supported by the significant cross-level interactions, which reveal that aggregate trust levels impact on the strength and direction of micro level determinants of trust. Third, and following from this point, all three forms of trust form a fairly tight triangle of interdependent relationships, although particular and general social trust are more closely associated with each other than with political trust, again consistent with the conditional model. Fourth, particular trust is not incompatible with either general social trust or political trust, being positively and significantly associated with them at both the aggregate and individual levels, at least in the democratic nations covered in this study.Footnote 12 Particular social trust seems to play an important part in creating and sustaining high levels of general and political trust. Fifth, contextual political variables do not make much of a contribution to particular social trust, and they are more important for general and (not surprisingly) for political trust. The overarching, society-wide impact of democratic government, rule of law, government effectiveness, and regulatory quality would seem to have an impact on general trust in unknown others, but less so on particular trust in close and known others. This suggests a slight modification of hypothesis 3 to read that aggregate and institutional macro variables have a stronger impact on general social and political trust than on particular forms of trust.

Conclusion

This study investigates the relationships between particular social trust, general social trust, and political trust. Contemporary research has largely overlooked particular social trust, because it is regarded as being of little importance for modern society or else a damaging influence on it. Interest in the association between general social trust and political trust at the individual level has also been undermined by a series of earlier studies, suggesting that they are weakly and patchily correlated, although more recent work has challenged this conclusion by finding much closer associations. The latest wave of the World Values survey includes a set of questions on all three kinds of trust that makes it possible for the first time to conduct a cross-national study of the associations between particular and general social trust and the associations between political trust and the two types of social trust. The 4-point rating scales of the trust questions also make them more sensitive measures than the ‘Yes/No’ option attached to the standard social trust question.

An overview of the literature suggests three different models of trust: the first argues that different kinds of trust are mutually reinforcing and compatible; the second argues, on the contrary, that particular social trust drives out general social trust, and that particular social trust is unlikely to be accompanied by political trust; the third, a modification of the two others, suggests that there is no necessary compatibility or incompatibility of the three types of trust, but that particular social trust should condition the development of general and political trust.

The evidence of the 2005–07 World Values survey is most consistent with the conditional model. Cross-tabulations, correlations, and multi-level analysis of the three types of trust show that they are positively and significantly associated, but in different ways and to varying extents. An overwhelming majority of the population of democratic societies (more than 90%) professes particular trust of one form or another, but less than half are high on general trust, and slightly more than the third are high on political trust. While (of logical necessity) all but a tiny minority (0.6%) of those with a high general trust score are also high on particular trust, fewer than half (45%) of those claiming particular trust also claim high general trust. There is a similar conditionality between social and political trust; almost all of those who are politically trusting have a high score on the measure of particular social trust, but the reverse is not true. It would seem from these results that particular trust is the foundation on which general social trust and political trust are based, but building these forms of trust on the foundations does not inevitably or even generally occur.

The evidence refutes the idea that particular social trust drives out or undermines general social trust, a finding that supports some social capital and social-psychological writing. More than that, it points to the crucial importance of particular social trust as a platform on which general and political trust may be, but is not necessarily, built. This raises the question of who extends one form of trust to another and under what sorts of circumstances.

Conversely, the results are not consistent with the social psychology theory that social trust is a pervasive core personality characteristic – what Glanville and Paxton (Reference Glanville and Paxton2007) call ‘the psychological propensity model’. The evidence shows that individuals do not have a general propensity to trust or not to trust, but that they choose whom and what to trust and combine varying degrees of trust or distrust in different objects.

The evidence is consistent with social capital theory in two significant ways. First, it suggests that there is indeed a significant and positive relationship between general social trust and political trust, and it goes a step further in finding that particular social trust also has an impact on political trust, as it does on general social trust. In sum, there is a strong set of triangular relations between the three types of trust, as some social capital theory predicts. Second, the evidence is consistent with the rainmaker effect, whereby aggregate levels of trust in society have an influence on individual levels – a top–down view of trust and trustworthiness. Similarly, the evidence suggests strong institutional and macro influences on individual trust levels, including the quality of democracy, government effectiveness, and the rule of law and regulatory quality in society. Here again, it goes a step further in finding that aggregate political trust and the institutions of government have little effect on individual particular trust compared with general social and political trust. This is probably because particular trust, so far as it is based on personal knowledge and close social contact, is less likely to be affected by wider contextual influences that have an impact on society as a whole.

Trust variables apart, other individual-level variables have little impact on the three types of trust. The effects of age, gender, and church attendance are small even when they are significant, while those of education and active membership are barely larger. It is not surprising that education has no association with the measures of particular trust based on personal knowledge and contacts, but most studies have found a strong and enduring connection between education and general social trust, which is not replicated here. And in spite of all the literature on the importance of voluntary associations for social and political trust, the evidence here, as in some other studies, is not particularly convincing. Nor do the aggregate measures of equality or ethnic fractionalization have an impact, although some previous studies have found these to be good predictors of either political or social trust, or both. However, our small national n of 22 Polity IV democracies may explain the ‘under performance’ figures for ethnic fractionalization and the Gini index.

Last, the study suggests that particular social trust is a key part of the trust puzzle discussed in the opening section of this paper: it seems to make things possible but not inevitable; it seems to be a necessary but not a sufficient cause of more general forms of social and political trust. At any rate, there seem to be good reasons for devoting more attention to particular social trust in order to understand how and why it may develop into general social and political trust. To this extent, particular social trust may play a significant role in strengthening social integration and democratic stability.

Acknowledgements

We are particularly grateful to Daniel Fuß, Daniel Stegmüller, Tom van der Meer, and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful advice and comments.

Appendix 1: Micro and macro variables and their coding

Footnotes

1 See Table 3.

2 See World Values survey 2005–07 Codebook for further details (http://www.wvsevsdb.com/wvs/WVSDocs.jsp?Idioma=I).

3 In accordance with Hox (Reference Hox2002), we tested in a stepwise manner the presence of random slope effects. The corresponding variance components were weak and often insignificant. For reasons of simplicity and parsimony, we decided to restrict the multi-level analyses to random intercept models.

4 This overlap is also confirmed by Mokken’s scale analysis. Owing to lack of space, the results are not presented here, but they show significant and similar hierarchical patterns for the pooled and cross-national data. The scale analysis suggests that particular social trust lays the foundation for the development of general social trust.

5 Indices are preferred over factor scores because they can be interpreted more intuitively and involve fewer missing values (see also Zmerli and Newton, Reference Zmerli and Newton2008 for arguments and evidence justifying the use of indices rather than factor loadings). The two indices of social trust are calculated by summing each individual's answers to the three trust questions and dividing by the number of valid responses. In the case of political confidence, six responses are summed and divided by the number of valid responses. The WV trust scores have been recoded so that 1 is low and 4 is high. A recoding also applies to the standard general trust question (see Appendix 1).

6 The figures in the last two columns for Spain are anomalous and may be erroneous. It does not fit with previous work of this kind on Spain (see Zmerli et al., Reference Zmerli, Newton and Montero2007; Zmerli and Newton, Reference Zmerli and Newton2008), which shows the country conforming to the general pattern in Europe. We also note in passing that the country figures presented in tables 3 and 4 are not intended to highlight the national differences that undoubtedly exist, but to show that it is possible to reach high-level generalizations about individual trust patterns in 22 democracies, notwithstanding country variations.

7 At the aggregate level, there are also strong positive correlations between all three types of trust, with Pearson’s r’s of 0.88, 0.71, and 0.58 between particular and general, particular and political, and general and political trust, respectively. Each of them is significant at the 0.000 level.

8 In some ways, cross-tabulations tell us more about the associations between different kinds of trust than correlations, but they consume far more space. For the cross-tabulations, see the authors’ paper presented to the ECPR General Conference, Potsdam, September 2009 (http://www.ecprnet.eu/default.asp).

9 The ‘mean world’ effect of the mass media should also be included, but it is difficult to test World Values using the World Values file and there is a large amount of missing data for our 22 countries.

10 The details and sources of macro and micro variables are provided in Appendix 1.

11 Macro variables are not included in the first two columns where only the effects of cross-level interactions and micro measures are analysed.

12 Note that a study covering democratic and non-democratic countries would be expected to show high levels of general trust in democracies and high levels of particular trust in non-democracies.

References

Abramson, P. (1983), Political Attitudes in America: Formation and Change, San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Alesina, A., Baqir, R. Easterly, W. (1999), ‘Public goods and ethnic divisions’, Quarterly Journal of Economics November: 12431284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alesina, A. La Ferrara, E. (2000), ‘Participation in heterogeneous communities’, The Quarterly Journal of Economics 115(3): 847904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alesina, A. La Ferrara, E. (2002), ‘Who trusts others?’, Journal of Public Economics 85: 207234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G.W. (1961), Pattern and Growth in Personality, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Anderson, C.J. Guillory, C.A. (1997), ‘Political institutions and satisfaction with democracy: a cross-national analysis of consensus and majoritarian systems’, American Political Science Review 91: 6681.Google Scholar
Anderson, C.J. LoTempio, A.J. (2002), ‘Winning, losing, and political trust in America’, British Journal of Political Science 32: 335351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Annett, A. (1999), ‘Ethnic and religious division, political instability, and government consumption’. International Monetary Fund, mimeo, March.Google Scholar
Bäck, M. Kestilä, E. (2008), ‘Social capital and political trust in Finland: an individual-level assessment’, Scandinavian Political Studies 32: 171194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahry, D., et al. (2005), ‘Ethnicity and trust: evidence from Russia’, American Political Science Review 99: 521532.Google Scholar
Banfield, E. (1958), The Moral Basis of a Backward Society, New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Bates, R.H., Figueiredo, R.J.P. Jr. Weingast, B.R. (1998), ‘The politics of interpretation: rationality, culture and transition’, Politics and Society 26: 603642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, J.A. Richard, P.B. (2001), ‘Civil society and political context in central America’, in B. Edwards, M. Foley and M. Diani (eds), Beyond Tocqueville: Civil Society and the Social Capital Debate in Comparative Perspective, Hanover, NH: Tufts University/University Press of New England, pp. 4355.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. Rahn, W. (1997), ‘Individual-level evidence for the causes and consequences of social capital’, American Journal of Political Science 41: 9991023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M.B. (1979), ‘In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: a cognitive-motivational analysis’, Psychological Bulletin 86: 307324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M.B. (1999), ‘The psychology of prejudice: ingroup love or outgroup hate?’, Journal of Social Issues 55: 429444.Google Scholar
Brewer, M.B. (2007), ‘The importance of being we: human nature and intergroup relations’, American Journal of Psychology 62: 726738.Google ScholarPubMed
Bryk, A.B. Raudenbush, S.W. (1992), Hierarchical Linear Models. Applications and Data Analysis Methods, London: Sage.Google Scholar
Castiglione, D., van Deth, J.W. Wolleb, G. (eds) (2008), The Handbook of Social Capital, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cattell, R.B. (1965), The Scientific Analysis of Personality, Baltimore: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1999), Democracy and Trust, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Costa, D.L. Kahn, M.E. (2003), ‘Civic engagement and community heterogeneity: an economist’s perspective’, Perspectives on Politics 1: 103111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, S. (1993), The Malevolent Leaders: Popular Discontent in America, Boulder, Col.: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Dalton, R.J. (2004), Democratic Challenges: Democratic Choice: The Erosion of Political Support in Advanced Industrial Democracies, Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delhey, J. Newton, K. (2005), ‘Predicting cross-national levels of social trust: global pattern or Nordic exceptionalism?’, European Sociological Review 21: 311327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J. (1993), ‘Trust’, in R.E. Goodin and P. Pettit (eds), A Companion to Contemporary Political Philosophy, Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 638644.Google Scholar
Easterly, W. (2000), ‘Can institutions resolve ethnic conflict’. The World Bank, Development Research Group, Policy Research Working Paper 2482.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, A. (2006), ‘Equality, trust and multiculturalism’, in F. Kay and R. Johnson (eds), Social Capital, Diversity, and the Welfare State, Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, pp. 6794.Google Scholar
Erikson, E.H. (1950), Childhood and Society, New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Freitag, M. (2003a), ‘Social capital in (dis)similar democracies: the development of generalized trust in Japan and Switzerland’, Comparative Political Studies 36: 936966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freitag, M. (2003b), ‘Beyond Tocqueville: the origins of social capital in Switzerland’, European Sociological Review 19: 217232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freitag, M. Bühlmann, M. (2009), ‘Crafting trust: the role of political institutions in a comparative perspective’, Comparative Political Studies 42: 15371566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freitag, M. Traunmüller, R. (2009), ‘Spheres of trust: an empirical analysis of the foundations of particularized and generalized trust’, European Journal of Political Research 48: 782803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, O.W. Walter-Rogg, M. (2008), ‘Social capital and political trust’, in H. Meulemann (ed.), Social Capital in Europe: Similarity of Countries and Diversity of People? Multi-level Analyses of the European Social Survey 2002, Leiden: Brill Academic Publishing, pp. 219250.Google Scholar
Glaeser, E.L., Laibson, D.I., Scheinkman, J.A. Soutter, C.L. (2000), ‘Measuring trust’, The Quarterly Journal of Economics 115: 811846.Google Scholar
Glanville, J.L. Paxton, P. (2007), ‘How do we learn trust? A confirmatory tetrad analysis of the sources of generalized trust’, Social Psychology Quarterly 70: 230242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granovetter, M.S. (1973), ‘The strength of weak ties’, American Journal of Sociology 78: 13601380.Google Scholar
Granovetter, M.S. (1983), ‘The strength of weak ties: a network theory revisited’, Sociological Theory 1: 201233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, P. (1999), ‘Social capital in Britain’, British Journal of Political Science 29: 417459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardin, R. (2000), ‘The public trust’, in S. Pharr and R.D. Putnam (eds), Disaffected Democracies: What’s Troubling the Trilateral Countries?, Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 3151.Google Scholar
Hardin, R. (2002), Trust and Trustworthiness, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Hero, R.E. (1998), Faces of Inequality: Social Diversity in American Politics, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hero, R.E. (2003), ‘Social capital and racial inequality in America’, Perspectives on Politics 1(March): 113122.Google Scholar
Helliwell, J.F. (1996), ‘Do borders matter for social capital? Economic growth and civil culture in US states and Canadian provinces’. NBER Working paper no. Q5863.Google Scholar
Herring, M., Jankowski, T.B. Brown, R.E. (1999), ‘Pro-black doesn’t mean anti-white: the structure of African-American group identity’, The Journal of Politics 61: 363386.Google Scholar
Hewstone, M., Rubin, M. Willis, H. (2002), ‘Intergroup bias’, Annual review of Psychology 53: 575604.Google Scholar
Hooghe, M. (2008), ‘Associations and socialization’, in D. Castiglione, J. van Deth and G. Wolleb (eds), Handbook of Social Capital, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 568593.Google Scholar
Hox, J. (2002), Multilevel Analysis: Techniques and Applications, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R. (1997), Modernization and Postmodernization, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Inglehart, R. (1999), ‘Trust, well-being and democracy’, in M.E. Warren (ed.), Democracy and Trust, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 88120.Google Scholar
Inglehart, R. Welzel, C. (2005), Modernization, Cultural Change and Democracy: The Human Development Sequence, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jagodzinski, W. Manabe, K. (2004), ‘How to measure interpersonal trust? A comparison of two different measures’, ZA-Information 55: 8597.Google Scholar
Kaase, M. (1999), ‘Interpersonal trust, political trust and non-institutionalised political participation in Western Europe’, West European Politics 22: 123.Google Scholar
Knack, S. (2000), ‘Social capital and the quality of government: evidence from the states’, American Journal of Political Science 46: 772785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knack, S. Keefer, P. (1997), ‘Does social capital have an economic payoff? A cross-country investigation’, Quarterly Journal of Economics 112: 12511288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollock, P. (1994), ‘The emergence of exchange structure: an experimental study of uncertainty, commitment and trust’, American Journal of Sociology 100: 313345.Google Scholar
Kumlin, S. (2007), ‘Overloaded or undermined? European welfare states in the face of performance dissatisfaction’, in S. Svallfors (ed.), The Political Sociology of the Welfare State. Institutions, Social Cleavages, and Orientations, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, pp. 80116.Google Scholar
Kumlin, S. Rothstein, B. (2005), ‘Making and breaking social capital: the impact of welfare state institutions’, Comparative Political Studies 38: 339365.Google Scholar
Lawrence, R.Z. (1997), ‘Is it really the economy, stupid?’, in J.S. Nye, P.D. Zelikow and D.C. King (eds), Why Americans Distrust Government, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, pp. 111132.Google Scholar
Levi, M. Stoker, L. (2000), ‘Political trust and trustworthiness’, in N.W. Polsby (ed.), Annual Review of Political Science, Vol. 30, Palo Alto, Ca: Annual Reviews, pp. 475508.Google Scholar
Listhaug, O. Ringdal, K. (2008), ‘Trust in political institutions’, in H. Ervasti, T. Fridberg, M. Hjerm and K. Ringdal (eds), Nordic Social Attitudes in a European Perspective, Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Listhaug, O. Wiberg, M. (1995), ‘Confidence in political and private institutions’, in H.-D. Klingemann and D. Fuchs (eds), Citizens and the State, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 298322.Google Scholar
Luhmann, N. (1979), Trust and Power, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Luhmann, N. (1988), ‘Familiarity, confidence, trust: problems and alternatives’, in D. Gambetta (ed.), Trust: Making and Breaking Cooperative Relations, Oxford: Blackwells, pp. 94107.Google Scholar
Mishler, W. Rose, R. (2001), ‘What are the origins of political trust? Testing institutional and cultural theories in post-communist societies’, Comparative Political Studies 34: 3062.Google Scholar
Misztal, B. (1996), Trust in Modern Societies, Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Nannestad, P. (2008), ‘What have we learned about generalized trust if anything?’, Annual Review of Political Science 11: 413436.Google Scholar
Neller, K. (2008), ‘What makes people trust in their fellow citizens’, in H. Meulemann (ed.), Social Capital in Europe, Boston: Brill Academic, pp. 103133.Google Scholar
Newton, K. (1999), ‘Social and political trust in established democracies’, in P. Norris (ed.), Critical Citizens: Global Support for Democratic Governance, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 169187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newton, K. (2001), ‘Trust, social capital, civil society, and democracy’, International Political Science Review 22: 201214.Google Scholar
Newton, K. (2006a), ‘Institutional confidence and social trust: aggregate and individual relations’, in M. Torcal and J.R. Montero (eds), Political Disaffection in Contemporary Democracies: Social Capital, Institutions and Politics, London: Routledge, pp. 81100.Google Scholar
Newton, K. (2006b), ‘Political support: social capital, civil society and political and economic performance’, Political Studies 54: 846864.Google Scholar
Newton, K. (2007), ‘Social and political trust’, in R.J. Dalton, and H.-D. Klingemann (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Behaviour, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 342361.Google Scholar
Newton, K. Norris, P. (2000), ‘Confidence in public institutions: faith, culture, or performance?’, in S. Pharr and R.D. Putnam (eds), Disaffected Democracies: What’s Troubling the Trilateral Countries?, Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 5273.Google Scholar
Offe, C. (1999), ‘How can we trust our fellow citizens’, in M.E. Warren (ed.), Democracy and Trust, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 4287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orren, G. (1997), ‘Fall from grace: the public’s loss of faith in government’, in J.S. Nye, P.D. Zelikow and D.C. King (eds), Why People Don’t Trust Government, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, pp. 77107.Google Scholar
Oskarson, M. (2007), ‘Social risk, policy dissatisfaction, and political alienation: a comparison of six European countries’, in S. Svallfors (ed.), The Political Sociology of the Welfare State. Institutions, Social Cleavages, and Orientations, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, pp. 117148.Google Scholar
Paxton, P. (2002), ‘Social capital and democracy: an inter-dependent relationship’, American Sociological Review 67: 254277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, R.D. (1993), Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in Modern Italy, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, R.D. (2000), Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community, New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Putnam, R.D. (ed.) (2002), Democracies in Flux, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, R., Pharr, S.J. Dalton, R.J. (2000), ‘Introduction: what’s troubling the trilateral democracies?’, in S.J. Pharr and R.D. Putnam (eds), Disaffected Democracies: What’s Troubling the Trilateral Countries?, Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 327.Google Scholar
Rahn, W.M., Brehm, J. Carlson, N. (1999), ‘National elections as institutions for generating social capital’, in T. Skocpol and M. Fiorina (eds), Civil Engagement in American Democracy, Washington, DC: Brookings Institute, pp. 110160.Google Scholar
Rose, R. (1994), ‘Postcommunism and the problem of trust’, Journal of Democracy 5: 1830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, M. (1956), ‘Misanthropy and political ideology’, American Sociological Review 21: 690695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, M. (1957), ‘Misanthropy and attitudes towards international affairs’, The Journal of Conflict Resolution 1: 340345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossteutscher, S. (2008), ‘Social capital and civic engagement: a comparative perspective’, in D. Castiglione, J.W. van Deth and G. Wolleb (eds), Handbook of Social Capital, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 208240.Google Scholar
Rothstein, B. (1998), Just Institutions Matter: The Moral and Political Logic of the Universal Welfare State, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothstein, B. Stolle, D. (2003), ‘Social capital, impartiality, and the welfare state: an institutional approach’, in M. Hooghe and D. Stolle (eds), Generating Social Capital: Civil Society and Institutions in Comparative Perspective, Basingstoke: Palgrave, pp. 191210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothstein, B. Stolle, D. (2008a), ‘The state and social capital: an institutional theory of generalized trust’, Comparative Politics 40: 441467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothstein, B. Stolle, D. (2008b), ‘Political institutions and generalized trust’, in D. Castiglione, J.W. van Deth and G. Wolleb (eds), Handbook of Social Capital, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 273302.Google Scholar
Rothstein, B. Uslaner, R. (2005), ‘All for all: equality corruption and social trust’, World Politics 58: 4172.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (1972), Stone Age Economics, Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Stolle, D. (1998), ‘Bowling together, bowling alone: the development of generalized trust in voluntary associations’, Political Psychology 19: 497526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolle, D. (2001), ‘Getting to trust: an analysis of the importance of institutions, families, personal experiences, and group membership’, in P. Dekker and E.M. Uslaner (eds), Social Capital and Participation in Everyday Life, London: Routledge, pp. 118132.Google Scholar
Stolle, D. (2002), ‘Trusting strangers: the concept of generalized trust in perspective’, in G.S. Schaal (ed.), Österreichische Zeitschrift für Politikwissenschaft, Vol. 4, Schwerpunktheft, pp. 397412.Google Scholar
Stolle, D. (2007), ‘Social capital’, in R. Dalton and H.-D. Klingemann (eds), Oxford Handbook of Political Behaviour, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 655674.Google Scholar
Svallfors, S. (ed.) (2007), The Political Sociology of the Welfare State. Institutions, Social Cleavages, and Orientations, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Svensson, J. (1998), ‘Foreign aid and rent-seeking’. World Bank, Development Economics, Research Group, Policy Research Working Paper 1880.Google Scholar
Sztompka, P. (2000), Trust. A Sociological Theory, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Torpe, L. (2003), ‘Social capital in Denmark: a deviant case?’, Scandinavian Political Studies 26: 2748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T. (1998), ‘Trust and democratic governance’, in V. Braithwaite and M. Levi (eds), Trust and Governance, New York: Russell Sage Foundation, pp. 269294.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (1999), ‘Democracy and social capital’, in M.E. Warren (ed.), Democracy and Trust, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 121150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2000–2001), ‘Producing and consuming trust’, Political Science Quarterly 115: 569590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2002), The Moral Foundations of Trust, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2005), ‘Trust and corruption’, in J.G. Lambsdorff, M. Taube and M. Schramm (eds), The New Institutional Economics of Corruption, London: Routledge, pp. 7692.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2008a), ‘Trust as a moral value’, in D. Castiglione, J.W. van Deth and G. Wolleb (eds), Handbook of Social Capital, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 101121.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2008b), ‘The foundations of trust: macro and micro’, Cambridge Journal of Economics 32: 289294.Google Scholar
Van der Meer, J. (2003), ‘Rain or fog? An empirical examination of social capital’s rainmaker effects’, in M. Hooghe and D. Stolle (eds), Generating Social Capital, Basingstoke: Palgrave, pp. 133152.Google Scholar
Voci, A. (2006), ‘The link between identification and in-group favouritism: effects of threat to social identity and trust-related emotions’, British Journal of Social Psychology 45: 265284.Google Scholar
Warren, M.E. (1999), ‘Democratic theory and trust’, in M.E. Warren (ed.), Democracy and Trust, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 310345.Google Scholar
Warren, M.E. (2004), ‘What does corruption mean in a democracy’, American Journal of Political Science 48: 327342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weingast, B. (1998), ‘Constructing trust: the political and economic roots of ethnic and regional violence’, in V. Haufler, K. Soltan and E.M. Uslaner (eds), Where is the New Institutionalism Now?, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, pp. 163200.Google Scholar
Whiteley, P.F. (1999), ‘The origins of social capital’, in J.W. van Deth, M. Maraffi, K. Newton and P.F. Whiteley (eds), Social Capital and European Democracy, London: Routledge, pp. 2544.Google Scholar
Wright, J.D. (1976), The Dissent of the Governed, New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Jin, N. Miller, A.S. (1998), ‘In-group bias and culture of collectivism’, Asian Journal of Social Psychology 1: 315328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T. Yamagishi, M. (1994), ‘Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan’, Motivation and Emotion 18: 129166.Google Scholar
Yosano, A. Hayashi, N. (2005), ‘Social stratification, intermediary groups and the creation of trustfulness’, Sociological Theory and Methods 20: 2744.Google Scholar
Yuki, M., Maddux, W.W., Brewer, M.B. Takemura, K. (2005), ‘Cross-cultural differences in relationship- and group-based trust’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31: 4862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zak, P.J. Knack, S. (2001), ‘Trust and growth’, The Economics Journal 111: 295321.Google Scholar
Zmerli, S. (2004), ‘Politisches Vertrauen und Unterstützung’, in J.W. van Deth (ed.), Deutschland in Europa, Wiesbaden: VS Verlag Sozialwissenschaften, pp. 229256.Google Scholar
Zmerli, S. Newton, K. (2008), ‘Social trust and attitudes towards democracy’, Public Opinion Quarterly 72: 706724.Google Scholar
Zmerli, S., Newton, K. Montero, J.R. (2007), ‘Trust in people, confidence in political institutions, and satisfaction with democracy’, in J.W. van Deth, J.R. Montero and A. Westholm (eds), Citizenship and Involvement in European Democracies, London: Routledge, pp. 3565.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Principal component analyses of measures of social trust, promax rotation, structure matrix, with and without the standard general trust item

Figure 1

Table 2 Principal component analysis of six measures of confidence in political institutions

Figure 2

Table 3 Means of social and political trust by country, ranked by general social trust

Figure 3

Table 4 Correlations between indices of three types of trust. Pearson’s r, by country

Figure 4

Table 5 Macro- and micro-level determinants of particular social trust, multi-level analysis, and random intercept models

Figure 5

Table 5 (Continued)

Figure 6

Table 6 Macro- and micro-level determinants of general social trust, multi-level analysis, and random intercept models

Figure 7

Table 6 (Continued)

Figure 8

Table 7 Macro- and micro-level determinants of political trust, multi-level analysis, random intercept models

Figure 9

Table 7 (Continued)