Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-07T06:34:18.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The chicken and egg of anxiety and depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2015

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentaries to Special Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

The paper of Kessler and co-workers on epidemiologic relationships between anxiety and depression is a major contribution, in that it presents data on a very large number of subjects studied across multiple countries and ethnicities. The findings confirm the earlier, pioneering work of Kessler's group (Magee et al. Reference Magee, Eaton, Wittchen, McGonagle and Kessler1996) as well as others including our own (Regier et al. Reference Regier, Rae, Narrow, Kaelber and Schatzberg1998; Ohayon & Schatzberg, Reference Ohayon and Schatzberg2010) on two major points. Major depression is characterised by high rates of co-occurring anxiety disorders and anxiety disorders more commonly presage the onset of the depression than vice versa. Both our and Kessler's group reported a number of years ago that social anxiety or phobia in adolescence occurred some 10 years before the onset of the depression in adulthood (Magee et al. Reference Magee, Eaton, Wittchen, McGonagle and Kessler1996; Regier et al. Reference Regier, Rae, Narrow, Kaelber and Schatzberg1998). The relationship between these disorders has been the subject of some study and there are considerable speculations but the definitive answers have remained somewhat elusive. Recent work using functional imaging may provide us some clues to shared characteristics as well as potential differences.

In the paper, Kessler and co-workers noted that both disorders may reflect variants of an internalising disorder but this still does not address the sequence of the onsets. Why would anxiety presage depression? One possibility is that some anxiety disorders such as social phobia impair the subject's ability to succeed at school or work and that failure results ion demoralisation and ultimately depression. That would fit a model that depression is essentially learned similar to the helplessness that results from exposure of lower animals to specific stressors. The model would also suggest that anxiety is in some ways primary or virtually instinctual – an observation consistent with the primate work of Kalin's group looking at fears of snakes and stranger intrusion (Oler et al. Reference Oler, Fox, Shelton, Rogers, Dyer, Davidson, Shelledy, Oakes, Blangero and Kalin2010). As indicated in the Kessler's paper, a number of years ago our group reported that cognitive behaviour therapy administered to socially phobic adolescents reduced the risk of recurrence of depression during the following 1 year (Hayward et al. Reference Hayward, Varady, Albano, Thienemann, Henderson and Schatzberg2000) in keeping with the depression being learned and preventable.

Another model might be that depression reflects biological processes involving depletion of monoamines over time. Here, one could posit that catecholamines released during highly stressful periods eventually deplete stores and result in a low catecholamine state and depression. There is little to support uniquely low catecholamine levels in unipolar depression. However, there do appear to be unipolar depressives with very high catecholamine metabolite levels (Schatzberg et al. Reference Schatzberg, Orsulak, Rosenbaum, Maruta, Kruger, Cole and Schildkraut1982) whose norepinephrine system might be viewed as functionally inefficient requiring greater turnover to evoke transmission. In depression, high catecholamine and metabolite levels appear to be positively correlated with cortisol (Rosenbaum et al. Reference Rosenbaum, Maruta, Schatzberg, Orsulak, Jiang, Cole and Schildkraut1983), whereas norepinephrine and cortisol levels are negatively correlated in healthy states (i.e., higher norepinephrine or metabolite levels are associated with lower cortisol levels). Whether this difference has to do with comorbid anxiety or the stages of the disorders is unclear, but it could explain a progression from anxiety to major depression where specific abnormalities in these two systems may not be consistently concordant between the disorders. This area would require further study.

Another approach has been to explore possible differences in functional imaging in depression and anxiety. Etkin's group has done elegant work here in exploring subjects’ ability to implicitly regulate emotional conflict using functional magnetic resonance imaging (f-MRI). In many ways, implicit emotion regulation more closely approaches the innate ways we handle emotions because the challenge is free from explicit or conscious responses. Generalised anxiety disorder patients (with or without major depression) and major depressives all demonstrated deficits involving activation and connectivity of the amygdala and anterior cingulate (Etkin & Schatzberg, Reference Etkin and Schatzberg2011). These data would fit a common ‘internalising’ biology as hypothesised by Kessler. However, only the generalised anxiety patients (comorbid with depression or not) failed to implicitly regulate emotional conflict. The major depression-only patients were able to overcome the deficit by also activating anterior lateral prefrontal cortical regions bilaterally. This activation correlated with successful implicit regulation. These data point to key compensatory abilities in depression that may explain why not all subjects with major depression experience comorbid anxiety. Thus, the relative ability to deal with negative emotions may underlie differences among the disorders.

An important issue in the relationship of anxiety and depression has to do with whether anxiety is occurring as a full anxiety disorder (i.e., categorical) or rather as a set of symptoms that fall short of a specific anxiety disorder diagnosis (dimensional model). Using f-MRI connectivity data, Oathes et al. (Reference Oathes, Patenaude, Schatzberg and Etkin2015) from Etkin's group recently reported that neither a dimensional nor categorical model explained the findings on f-MRI. Rather, the relationships appear to involve both categorical and dimensional relationships. This state of affairs is in some ways indicated in the recently promulgated Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Editon (DSM5) where both categorical and dimensional approaches are recommended.

Much of the recent interest in the relationship of these disorders stems from large-scale clinical trials in major depression. In the Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression study (STAR*D), anxiety as a dimension largely predicted poorer responses to monotherapy with the antidepressant citalopram in Phase I as well as various combinations and switches in later phases (Fava et al. Reference Fava, Rush, Alpert, Balasubramani, Wisniewski, Carmin, Biggs, Zisook, Leuchter, Howland, Warden and Trivedi2008). In the more recent International Study to Predict Optimised Treatment in Depression (i-SPOT-D), anxiety as a dimension predicted poorer responses to escitalopram, sertraline or venlafaxine (Saveanu et al. Reference Saveanu, Etkin, Duchemin, Goldstein-Piekarski, Gyurak, Debattista, Schatzberg, Sood, Day, Palmer, Rekshan, Gordon, Rush and Williams2015). Fully comorbid anxiety disorder/major depression was not a predictor of poorer rate of response than that seen in non-comorbid major depression (Arnow et al. in press). Thus, anxiety symptoms per se are the key predictors of poorer response to monotherapy in major depression and require alternative strategies.

A number of strategies have been reported and these involve using agents with 5HT-2 antagonist properties or sedating benzodiazepines. For example, clonazepam added to the Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor (SSRI) fluoxetine appears to produce greater response than the SSRI alone (Papakostas et al. Reference Papakostas, Clain, Ameral, Baer, Brintz, Smith, Londborg, Glaudin, Painter and Fava2010). Benefit has also been reported with the atypical antipsychotic quetiapine (Montgomery et al. Reference Montgomery, Locklear, Svedsäter and Eriksson2014) and the antidepressant mirtazapine (Schatzberg et al. Reference Schatzberg, Kremer, Rodrigues and Murphy2002) both of which have potent 5-HT2 antagonist effects and thus are calming and potentially sedating. These studies indicate the anxiolytic effects of the commonly used SSRI or serotonin noreprinephrine reuptake inhibitor (SNRI) antidepressants fail to provide sufficient relief of the anxiety symptoms to provide overall relief from the depression. Working backwards, these results would suggest that anxiety as a dimension in the context of depression needs to be addressed specifically but that the presence of anxiety may represent a different biological process than fully comorbid anxiety and depression.

Experience with treating anxious depressives may tell us something about the staging from anxiety to depression. Clinically, psychopharmacologists all the time treat and write about refractory depression – i.e., patients who fail to respond to one or more adequate trials of antidepressants. Less commonly do we write about refractory anxiety. One explanation is that refractory anxiety develops into major depression as we discussed above. Thus, we commonly do not see anxious patients who become refractory. They tend to respond when treated adequately and if not, they may develop major depression. This situation has been less well studied than the converse but could provide clues regarding the nature of the relationships.

The area of anxiety and depression has been largely enhanced by the work of Kessler and co-workers over the past two decades and the paper in this issue is entirely in keeping with their important contributions to the field.

References

Arnow, BA, Blasey, C, Williams, LM, Palmer, D, Rekshan, W, Schatzberg, AF, Etkin, A, Kulkarni, J, Luther, JF, Rush, AJ (2015, in press). Are depression subtypes relevant to predicting antidepressant response?: a report from the iSPOT-D trial. American Journal of Psychiatry.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etkin, A, Schatzberg, AF (2011). Common abnormalities and disorder-specific compensation during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety and major depressive disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 968978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fava, M, Rush, AJ, Alpert, JE, Balasubramani, GK, Wisniewski, SR, Carmin, CN, Biggs, MM, Zisook, S, Leuchter, A, Howland, R, Warden, D, Trivedi, MH (2008). Difference in treatment outcome in outpatients with anxious versus nonanxious depression: a STAR*D report. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 342351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayward, C, Varady, S, Albano, AM, Thienemann, M, Henderson, L, Schatzberg, AF (2000). Cognitive-behavioral group therapy for social phobia in female adolescents: results of a pilot study. Journal of the American Academy Child of Adolescent Psychiatry 39, 721726.Google Scholar
Magee, WJ, Eaton, WW, Wittchen, HU, McGonagle, KA, Kessler, RC (1996). Agoraphobia, simple phobia, and social phobia in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry 53, 159168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, SA, Locklear, JC, Svedsäter, H, Eriksson, H (2014). Efficacy of once-daily extended release quetiapine fumarate in patients with different levels of severity of generalized anxiety disorder. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 29, 252262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oathes, DJ, Patenaude, B, Schatzberg, AF, Etkin, A (2015). Neurobiological signatures of anxiety and depression in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry 77, 385393.Google Scholar
Ohayon, MM, Schatzberg, AF (2010). Social phobia and depression: prevalence and comorbidity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68, 235243.Google Scholar
Oler, JA, Fox, AS, Shelton, SE, Rogers, J, Dyer, TD, Davidson, RJ, Shelledy, W, Oakes, TR, Blangero, J, Kalin, NH (2010). Amygdalar and hippocampal substrates of anxious temperament differ in their heritability. Nature 466, 864868.Google Scholar
Papakostas, GI, Clain, A, Ameral, VE, Baer, L, Brintz, C, Smith, WT, Londborg, PD, Glaudin, V, Painter, JR, Fava, M (2010). Fluoxetine-clonazepam cotherapy for anxious depression: an exploratory, post-hoc analysis of a randomized, double blind study. International Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 25, 1721.Google Scholar
Regier, DA, Rae, DS, Narrow, WE, Kaelber, CT, Schatzberg, AF (1998). Prevalence of anxiety disorders and their comorbidity with mood and addictive disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement 34, 2428.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, AH, Maruta, T, Schatzberg, AF, Orsulak, PJ, Jiang, NS, Cole, JO, Schildkraut, JJ (1983). Toward a biochemical classification of depressive disorders, VII: urinary free cortisol and urinary MHPG in depressions. American Journal of Psychiatry 140, 314318.Google Scholar
Saveanu, R, Etkin, A, Duchemin, AM, Goldstein-Piekarski, A, Gyurak, A, Debattista, C, Schatzberg, AF, Sood, S, Day, CV, Palmer, DM, Rekshan, WR, Gordon, E, Rush, AJ, Williams, LM (2015). The International Study to Predict Optimized Treatment in Depression (iSPOT-D): outcomes from the acute phase of antidepressant treatment. Journal of Psychiatric Research 61, 112.Google Scholar
Schatzberg, AF, Orsulak, PJ, Rosenbaum, AH, Maruta, T, Kruger, ER, Cole, JO, Schildkraut, JJ (1982). Toward a biochemical classification of depressive disorders, V: heterogeneity of unipolar depressions. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 471475.Google Scholar
Schatzberg, AF, Kremer, C, Rodrigues, HE, Murphy, GM Jr(2002). Mirtazapine vs. Paroxetine Study Group. Double-blind, randomized comparison of mirtazapine and paroxetine in elderly depressed patients. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10, 541550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar