Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T14:59:42.431Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using a developmental perspective to examine the moderating effects of marriage on heavy episodic drinking in a young adult sample enriched for risk

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2020

Seung Bin Cho
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Pusan National University, Busan, South Korea Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Rebecca L. Smith*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Kathleen Bucholz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Grace Chan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, CT, USA
Howard J. Edenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Victor Hesselbrock
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, CT, USA
John Kramer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Vivia V. McCutcheon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
John Nurnberger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Marc Schuckit
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California-San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Yong Zang
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Danielle M. Dick
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Jessica E. Salvatore
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Richmond, VA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Jessica E. Salvatore, Department of Psychology, Box 842018, Richmond, VA 23284-2018, USA; E-mail: jesalvatore@vcu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Many studies demonstrate that marriage protects against risky alcohol use and moderates genetic influences on alcohol outcomes; however, previous work has not considered these effects from a developmental perspective or in high-risk individuals. These represent important gaps, as it cannot be assumed that marriage has uniform effects across development or in high-risk samples. We took a longitudinal developmental approach to examine whether marital status was associated with heavy episodic drinking (HED), and whether marital status moderated polygenic influences on HED. Our sample included 937 individuals (53.25% female) from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism who reported their HED and marital status biennially between the ages of 21 and 25. Polygenic risk scores (PRS) were derived from a genome-wide association study of alcohol consumption. Marital status was not associated with HED; however, we observed pathogenic gene-by-environment effects that changed across young adulthood. Among those who married young (age 21), individuals with higher PRS reported more HED; however, these effects decayed over time. The same pattern was found in supplementary analyses using parental history of alcohol use disorder as the index of genetic liability. Our findings indicate that early marriage may exacerbate risk for those with higher polygenic load.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Epidemiological data consistently demonstrate that being married (relative to being single or separated/divorced/widowed) is associated with lower alcohol use and lower odds of alcohol use disorder (Bachman, O'Malley, & Johnston, Reference Bachman, O'Malley and Johnston1984; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang and Hasin2015; Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999). The protective effects of marriage are often explained in terms of role incompatibility (Yamaguchi & Kandel, Reference Yamaguchi and Kandel1985) and social control processes (Craddock, vanDellen, Novak, & Ranby, Reference Craddock, vanDellen, Novak and Ranby2015), whereby individuals match their behaviors with the socially normative expectations of the spousal role (Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer, & Emery, Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016), and spouses monitor and control one another's health behaviors, such as drinking (Craddock et al., Reference Craddock, vanDellen, Novak and Ranby2015; Umberson, Reference Umberson1992). Above and beyond these main effects, marital status moderates genetic influences on alcohol use behaviors such that genetic risk is typically attenuated among married compared with unmarried individuals (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Salvatore, Maes, Korhonen, Latvala, Aliev and Dick2017; Heath, Jardine, & Martin, Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Prescott & Kendler, Reference Prescott and Kendler2001). Such findings are consistent with the idea that genetic influences are attenuated in environments in which there is a high degree of social control (Shanahan & Hofer, Reference Shanahan and Hofer2005).

Although the protective and moderating effects of marriage on alcohol use are among the most consistent findings in the epidemiological (Leonard & Eiden, Reference Leonard and Eiden2007) and genetic epidemiological literatures (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Salvatore, Maes, Korhonen, Latvala, Aliev and Dick2017; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Prescott & Kendler, Reference Prescott and Kendler2001), the majority of these studies have examined these effects at a single point in time, typically in midlife adults or in age-mixed samples (e.g., Dinescu et al., Reference Dinescu, Turkheimer, Beam, Horn, Duncan and Emery2016; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989; Horn et al., Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016). This has left a gap in our understanding of these associations and the gene–environment interaction processes from a developmental perspective. In particular, marriage may not have a protective effect when it occurs developmentally off-time, such as among those who marry relatively early. Although there is no standard definition of what constitutes early marriage, researchers often use a cut-point below the national median, with some using 23 (Uecker & Stokes, Reference Uecker and Stokes2008) and others using 25 years of age (Eickmeyer & Hemez, Reference Eickmeyer and Hemez2017). Early marriage is associated with negative consequences in the areas of educational attainment, finances, and health (Dupre & Meadows, Reference Dupre and Meadows2007; Elder, Reference Elder1994; Lehrer, Reference Lehrer2004; Uecker, Reference Uecker2014). Many of these consequences are long-lasting and continue to negatively impact individuals, specifically women, throughout adulthood (Loughran & Zissimopoulous, Reference Loughran and Zissimopoulos2004; Uecker, Reference Uecker2012). Early marriage is also associated with more role confusion and conflict (Elder, Reference Elder1994), lower relationship stability, and poorer relationship quality (Lehrer, 2004). In short, the negative outcomes associated with early marriage contrast with the protective effect typically associated with marriage observed in older samples.

It is important to recognize that some of the poor outcomes associated with early marriage may also reflect selection effects. Individuals who marry young also tend to be less educated, have fewer aspirations to obtain more education (Eickmeyer & Hemez, Reference Eickmeyer and Hemez2017; Uecker & Stokes, Reference Uecker and Stokes2008), and have less-educated parents who also married young (Uecker & Stokes, Reference Uecker and Stokes2008). There is also some evidence that individuals who marry at younger ages engage in more substance use as adolescents (Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999) and are more likely to have a history of psychiatric illness (Forthofer, Kessler, Story, & Gotlib, Reference Forthofer, Kessler, Story and Gotlib1996). Thus, individuals who marry relatively early (and their partners) may have a number of personal liabilities that limit the protective effects of marriage (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Heath, Bucholz, Madden, Agrawal, Statham and Martin2007).

A second limitation of prior studies on marriage and alcohol outcomes is that they have typically focused on population- or community-based samples (e.g., Bachman et al., Reference Bachman, O'Malley and Johnston1984; Horn et al., Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999). Risk and protective factors for alcohol outcomes in population-based samples may differ from those in high-risk individuals, such as among individuals with family histories of alcohol use disorder (Hill, Shen, Lowers, & Locke, Reference Hill, Shen, Lowers and Locke2000). A family history of alcohol use disorder is associated with a greater risk for problematic alcohol use and the development of alcohol problems (Cotton, Reference Cotton1979; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015). Of particular relevance for this study is that a parental history of alcohol problems is associated with a greater likelihood of having a spouse with an alcohol use disorder (Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Lönn, Long, Sundquist, Kendler, Sundquist and Edwards2018). Considering that marriage to a spouse with an alcohol problem is a risk factor for the onset of alcohol problems (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Leonard & Eiden, Reference Leonard and Eiden2007), high-risk individuals may be placed at even greater risk given their decreased likelihood of entering into the types of marital relationships with prosocial partners that are likely to have a protective effect on alcohol misuse.

Current study

To address these gaps in the literature, we report findings from a longitudinal study of a sample of young adults enriched for risk who were followed between the ages of 21 and 25. We examined (a) whether marriage was associated with frequency of heavy episodic drinking (HED) and (b) whether marital status moderated measured genetic influences (as measured with a genome-wide polygenic risk score (PRS)) to predict HED frequency over time. Genome-wide PRS reflect a state-of-the-science approach to index one's level of genetic predisposition for a given trait or behavior (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Lee, Mehta, Vinkhuyzen, Dudbridge and Middeldorp2014). This approach uses the results from a genome-wide association study (GWAS) in a large-scale discovery sample to calculate personalized indices of genetic risk in a target sample. Common genetic variants, or single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), are tested for their association with a given trait/behavior in the discovery sample (Bogdan, Baranger, & Agrawal, Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018; Maier, Visscher, Robinson, & Wray, Reference Maier, Visscher, Robinson and Wray2018; Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Aliev, Edwards, Evans, Macleod, Hickman and Dick2014; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Lee, Mehta, Vinkhuyzen, Dudbridge and Middeldorp2014). Then, the effect sizes from the discovery GWAS are used to calculate the weighted linear composite corresponding to the number of risk-increasing alleles carried by each individual in the target sample.

If marriage has a consistent protective effect across development, we would expect that marriage would be associated with reductions in HED (Bachman et al., Reference Bachman, O'Malley and Johnston1984; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang and Hasin2015; Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999). Similarly, if marriage has a uniformly moderating protective effect on genetic risk across development, we would expect that genetic risk would be attenuated for those who are married compared with those who are unmarried (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Salvatore, Maes, Korhonen, Latvala, Aliev and Dick2017; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Prescott & Kendler, Reference Prescott and Kendler2001). However, given that early marriage is associated with poorer outcomes across a range of domains (e.g., educational attainment, finances, and health (Dupre & Meadows, Reference Dupre and Meadows2007; Elder, Reference Elder1994; Lehrer, Reference Lehrer2004; Uecker, Reference Uecker2014)), we did not advance directional hypotheses for either research question.

Method

Participants

We used data from the Prospective Study sample of the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA). COGA is a collaborative research project between multiple sites in the USA, with the goal of identifying genetic influences on alcohol use disorders and related psychiatric outcomes (Begleiter et al., Reference Begleiter, Reich, Hesslbrock, Porjesz, Li, Schuckit and Rice1995). Families with alcohol-dependent probands, based on both the third, revised edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987)) and the Feighner criteria (Feighner et al., Reference Feighner, Robins, Guze, Woodruff, Winokur and Munoz1972), were recruited from inpatient and outpatient alcohol clinics across six study sites in the USA and followed longitudinally. Unascertained comparison families from the population were also recruited from various sources (e.g., driver's license records and dental clinic records). The Institutional Review Boards of all participating institutions approved the study and written consents were obtained from all participants. Detailed description about the design of COGA can be found in previously published papers (Begleiter et al., Reference Begleiter, Reich, Hesslbrock, Porjesz, Li, Schuckit and Rice1995; Foroud et al., Reference Foroud, Edenberg, Goate, Rice, Flury, Koller and Reich2000; Reich et al., Reference Reich, Edenberg, Goate, Williams, Rice, Eerdewegh and Begleiter1998).

The Prospective Study was launched in 2004 as a part of COGA, with the goal of examining how genetic risk unfolds across development and in conjunction with the environment. Offspring between ages 12 and 22 with at least one parent in either the clinically ascertained group or the unascertained comparison group who had completed an interview in the original COGA study were recruited to participate and followed longitudinally (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017). In this study, we included 937 (776 from the clinically ascertained group) European ancestry participants (based on ancestral principal components derived from genetic data) between the ages of 21 and 25 with available genetic data. We limited our sample to only participants of European ancestry so that our analytic sample was ancestrally matched to the discovery sample used to calculate genome-wide PRS . Participants were followed up biennially, with each participant assessed up to three times between ages 21 and 25, resulting in a total of 1,691 assessments. In total, 34.8%, 50.0%, and 15.3% of participants completed one, two, and three assessments, respectively.

Measures

HED frequency

Frequency of HED was measured at each assessment by asking, “How often did you have five or more drinks in 24 hr during the last 12 months?” Responses included 13 options, ranging from “never” to “every day,” which were converted into frequencies by taking the midpoint of each response option. For example, the responses “every day” and “two days per week (100–149 days)” corresponded to 365 and 124.5 days per year, respectively. Those who reported they did not drink during the past year were coded as zero.

Marital status

Marital status was measured at each assessment. Six response options were combined to create a binary variable for each time point. “Married” and “living as married” were combined and coded as married (1). All other response options (i.e., “widowed,” “separated,” “divorced,” and “never married”) were combined and coded as unmarried (0). (We note that there were no widowed participants in the sample.) This approach is consistent with previous studies of the moderating effect of relationship status on genetic risk for alcohol phenotypes (e.g., Heath et al., Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989). Individuals whose marital status changed (e.g., from married to divorced) were coded differently across time.

Genotyping

Genotyping was performed using the Illumina 1 M and Illumina OmniExpress (Illumina, San Diego, CA), and Smokescreen (BioRelm, Walnut, CA) arrays. The reported pedigree structure was assessed using a pruned set of 1,519,440 SNPs. Family structures were altered, as needed, and SNP genotypes were tested for Mendelian inconsistencies (PedCheck (O'Connell & Weeks, Reference O'Connell and Weeks1998)) with the revised family structure. Genotype inconsistencies were set to missing. Genotypes were imputed to 1000 Genomes (EUR and AFR, Phase 3, b37, October 2014; build hg19) using SHAPEIT (Delaneau, Zagury, & Marchini, Reference Delaneau, Zagury and Marchini2013) and then IMPUTE2 (Howie, Fuchsberger, Stephens, Marchini, & Abecasis, Reference Howie, Fuchsberger, Stephens, Marchini and Abecasis2012). Imputed SNPs with information (INFO) scores <0.30 or individual genotype probability scores <0.90 were excluded, as were palindromic SNPs (A/T or C/G), monomorphic SNPs, SNPs with a genotyping rate of <95%, SNPs that did not pass the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) (p < 1 × 10−6), and SNPs with a minor allele frequency (MAF) <0.05%. In total, 6,881,872 SNPs were available for analysis after passing quality control and data cleaning thresholds.

Genome-wide PRS of alcohol use in the COGA sample were constructed based on GWAS summary statistics of alcohol consumption, measured in grams of alcohol per day, from the Alcohol Genome-Wide Association (AlcGen) and Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Plus (CHARGE+) consortia (Schumann et al., Reference Schumann, Liu, O'Reilly, Gao, Song, Xu and Elliott2016). After removing palindromic SNPs (which can be ambiguous with respect to the reference allele when going across samples), we used the clump and score procedures in PLINK (Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira, Bender and Sham2007) to sum each individual's total number of minor alleles from the score SNPs, with each SNP weighted by the negative log of the GWAS association p-value and sign of the association coefficient (beta). Clumping was done with respect to the linkage disequilibrium (LD) pattern in the 1000 Genome Phase 3 sample using a 500 kb physical distance and an LD threshold of r 2 ≥ .25. Thus, PRS were constructed of SNPs that captured independent genetic association signals from the AlcGen and CHARGE+ GWAS. Following conventions for polygenic scoring using the pruning and thresholding approach (Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018), we calculated a series of scores in COGA that included SNPs meeting increasingly stringent p-value thresholds in the AlcGen and CHARGE+ discovery GWAS (Schumann et al., Reference Schumann, Liu, O'Reilly, Gao, Song, Xu and Elliott2016; p < .50, p < .40, p < .30, p < .20, p < .10, p < .05, p < .01, p < .001, and p < .0001).

Data analysis

To examine whether marital status moderated the association between the PRS and HED among young adults and whether the association among marital status (treated as a time-varying variable), PRS, and HED changed across time, we used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with log link by assuming HED follows a Poisson distribution. In this model, HED frequency was predicted by age, marital status, PRS, two-way interactions, and the three-way interaction among age, PRS, and marital status. Age was centered at 21 years. Sex was coded as female (0) or male (1) and included as a covariate. The first three ancestral principal components were also included as covariates to control for potential population stratification. Random effects of intercept and age were included to incorporate repeated assessments.

Following standard practice (Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018; Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009), we first conducted a series of preliminary analyses to select the PRS p-value threshold that provided the best fit and maximized effect sizes. In these models, the log of HED frequency was regressed onto age, PRS, marital status, the two-way interactions, and the three-way interaction among age, PRS, and marital status. The selected PRS was standardized and used in all subsequent analyses. We then fit GLMMs with marital status and the selected PRS separately to examine the main effects of the predictors on HED. In each model, marital status or PRS and its interaction with age were included as predictors for HED. We then fit and interpreted the full model with marital status, the selected PRS, the two-way interactions, and the three-way interaction among age, PRS, and marital status. We used the GLIMMIX procedure in SAS and maximum likelihood estimator based on Laplace approximation for parameter estimation.

To check the robustness of the results, we ran two series of sensitivity analyses. First, we fit the same model with parental history of alcohol dependence (PHAD), in place of the PRS, to represent latent genetic risk for alcohol use problems (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015). A PHAD variable was created from parents’ alcohol dependence diagnosis based on the fourth edition of the DSM (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria. PHAD was coded dichotomously as no PHAD (0) or either mother or father was diagnosed with alcohol dependence (1). Participants were coded as missing if both parents’ information was missing or if one parent did not have an alcohol disorder diagnosis and the other parent's information was missing. This resulted in 743 participants included in the sensitivity analyses. Parameters of PHAD and its interaction with marital status and age were compared with those from the original model.

Next, we conducted a secondary series of analyses to examine whether our pattern of results was robust when controlling for divorce/separation, college attendance, gene-by-covariate and covariate-by-environment interactions, and family ascertainment status. First, the original model was fit after removing participants who reported divorce or separation (n = 36) from the sample. This allowed us to rule out the possibility that our observed effects were due to the inclusion of divorced/separated individuals in the unmarried group in view of evidence that divorce and separation are associated with greater alcohol problems (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang and Hasin2015; Kessler, Walters, & Forthofer, Reference Kessler, Walters and Forthofer1998). Second, we fit a variation of the original model including college attendance as a time-varying covariate. College students engage in more HED compared with their non-student age-matched peers (Johnston, O'Malley, Miech, Bachman, & Schulenberg, Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Miech, Bachman and Schulenberg2015; O'Malley & Johnston, Reference O'Malley and Johnston2002; Slutske, Reference Slutske2005), so controlling for college attendance allowed us to examine whether our observed effects were driven by college attendance. Third, to address concerns that gene-by-environment effects may be confounded by the effects of gene-by-covariate and covariate-by-environment interactions (Keller, Reference Keller2014), we fit a model including these interaction terms. Finally, to control for potential differences between the clinically ascertained and unascertained comparison families, we fit a variation of the original model including family ascertainment status as a time-invariant covariate.

Results

Descriptive statistics of marital status and HED frequencies by age are summarized in Table 1. More participants were married or living as married at older ages than at younger ages. Among unmarried participants of all ages, the majority were never married rather than separated or divorced. On a descriptive level, engagement in HED was generally lower among married (or living as married) participants compared with those who were unmarried.

Table 1. Marital status and HED frequency by age

Note: Married includes those living as married. HED frequency was measured in days over the last 12 months.

Table 2 summarizes the fit statistics (AIC (Akaike information criterion) and BIC (Bayesian information criterion)) and effect sizes for the preliminary analyses to inform selection of the PRS p-value threshold. The lowest AIC and BIC values were observed for the PRS based on a p-value threshold of <.0001, indicating that the best model fit was obtained with PRS based on that threshold. Similarly, the absolute values of t for parameter estimates related to PRS were the largest with PRS based on p < .0001, indicating that the predictive power of PRS with that threshold was highest. Thus, a PRS based on a p < .0001 threshold was carried forward into subsequent analyses.

Table 2. Parameter estimates and fit statistics for Alc-PRS (genome-wide PRS for alcohol) thresholds

Note: t-values shown are the absolute values. Analogous to the full model, age was centered at 21 years and HED frequency was log-transformed. The first three ancestral principal components and sex were included as covariates.

Before fitting a full model with all interactions between PRS and marital status, we fit separate models with either PRS or marital status to examine the main effects of each variable (see Table 3). The results from both models indicated that neither PRS nor marital status was predictive of HED frequency, and none of the associations changed as a function of age. The only significant association was between HED and age, indicating that HED frequency decreased between ages 21 and 25.

Table 3. HED as a function of marital status and Alc-PRS (genome-wide PRS for alcohol) and their interaction with time

Note: Bold type indicates p < .05. Bold italic type indicates p < .001. b indicates unstandardized regression coefficient.

Abbreviations. PC, Principal component for genetic ancestry; SE, standard error.

Parameter estimates from the full model that included PRS, marital status, the two-way interactions, and the three-way interaction among PRS, marital status, and age are summarized in Table 4. We found a significant negative three-way interaction among PRS, marital status, and age, and we focus on the interpretation of this three-way interaction effect in view of the fact that this higher-order interaction modifies the main effects and lower-order interactions. Figure 1 depicts the pattern of the three-way interaction among PRS, marital status, and age, plotted at three illustrative ages (21, 23, and 25 years). Each plot shows the expected frequencies of HED by combinations of marital status and PRS, which was split at the median.

Figure 1. HED as a function of the three-way interaction among Alc-PRS (genome-wide PRS for alcohol), marital status, and time. Notes. Confidence intervals (CIs) are not symmetric because they were converted from expected log-transformed counts. The first three ancestral principal components and sex were included as covariates. HED frequency was measured in days over the last 12 months.

Table 4. HED as a function of Alc-PRS(genome-wide PRS for alcohol), marital status, time, and their interaction

Note: Bold type indicates p < .05. Bold italic type indicates p < .001.

Abbreviations. PC, Principal component for genetic ancestry.

With age centered at 21 years, HED was similar among unmarried participants with high and low PRS. As illustrated in Figure 1, unmarried individuals with a higher PRS were predicted to engage in HED 8.97 days per year compared with 11.59 days per year for those with a lower PRS. However, among married participants, HED was higher among those with a higher PRS compared with those with a lower PRS. Married individuals with a higher PRS were predicted to engage in HED 34.85 days per year compared with 4.72 days per year for those with a lower PRS.

This pattern of effects changed over time, indicated by the negative three-way interaction. Centered at the illustrative age of 23 years, HED was similar across all conditions. Unmarried individuals with a lower PRS were predicted to engage in HED 8.06 days per year compared with 6.96 days per year for those with a higher PRS. Married individuals with a lower PRS were predicted to engage in HED 5.99 days per year compared with 7.05 days per year for those with a higher PRS. With age centered at 25 years, HED was similar among unmarried participants with high and low PRS. Unmarried individuals with a higher PRS were predicted to engage in HED 5.72 days per year compared with 5.02 days per year for those with a lower PRS. Among married participants, however, a higher PRS was associated with lower HED. Individuals with a higher PRS were predicted to engage in HED 1.86 days per year compared with 11.14 days per year for those with a lower PRS. Thus, the nature of marriage's moderating effect on the association between PRS and HED changed between the ages of 21 and 25.

Sensitivity analyses with parameters estimated from the same GLMM model fit with PHAD in place of PRS are summarized in Table 5 and depicted in Figure 2. At age 21, consistent with the results from the model with the PRS, we found a significant positive interaction between PHAD and marital status, indicating that PHAD was associated with higher HED among married participants. Although the three-way interaction among age, PHAD, and marital status was not statistically significant, the direction of the negative three-way interaction was consistent with the result from the model using the PRS, indicating that the initial difference of the association between PHAD and HED by marital status was attenuated with age.

Figure 2. HED as a function of the three-way interaction among PHAD, marital status, and time. Notes. CIs are not symmetric because they were converted from expected log-transformed counts. The first three ancestral principal components and sex were included as covariates. HED frequency was measured in days over the last 12 months.

Table 5. Sensitivity analyses of HED as a function of PHAD, marital status, time, and their interaction

Note: Bold type indicates p < .05. Bold italic type indicates p < .001.

Abbreviations. PC, Principal component for genetic ancestry.

In a secondary set of sensitivity analyses, we examined whether our pattern of results was robust to the effects of divorce and separation, college attendance, gene-by-covariate and covariate-by-environment interactions, and family ascertainment status. We observed the same pattern of results after removing individuals who reported divorce or separation from the sample and when controlling for college attendance. Likewise, we observed the same pattern of results after controlling for all gene-by-covariate and covariate-by-environment interactions. Lastly, we observed the same pattern of results when controlling for family ascertainment status. None of the effects significantly changed with the inclusion of these covariates or interaction terms, which guided our decision to report the results with the fewest parameters in this paper (full results are available upon request from the corresponding author).

Discussion

The primary goals of the present study were to examine, in a sample of young adults enriched for risk, (a) whether marriage was associated with HED and (b) whether marital status moderated measured genetic influences to predict HED over time.

We first examined the relationship between marital status and HED, and we found no association. This null effect is surprising, as previous research demonstrated a protective effect of marriage on alcohol use (Bachman et al., Reference Bachman, O'Malley and Johnston1984; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang and Hasin2015; Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999). These contradictory results could be due to discrepancies in sample characteristics, as previous research typically focused on midlife or age-mixed (e.g., Dinescu et al., Reference Dinescu, Turkheimer, Beam, Horn, Duncan and Emery2016; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989; Horn et al., Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016), population- or community-based samples (e.g., Bachman et al., Reference Bachman, O'Malley and Johnston1984; Horn et al., Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999). In contrast, the present study included a sample of young adults enriched for risk.

A sample of young adults enriched for risk, such as the sample in this study, may differ in important ways from previously studied samples for two reasons. First, the protective effect of marriage is typically explained by role incompatibility (Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985; Horn et al., Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016), which may not be relevant for young adults. Young adults may not perceive a conflict between high levels of alcohol use and the socially normative expectations of the spousal role; therefore, there may be less impetus to reduce alcohol use upon the transition to marriage. Second, the null effect of marriage may be attributable to selection effects of a sample enriched for risk. Previous research demonstrates that individuals with a predisposition for alcohol problems are more likely to have a spouse with an alcohol use disorder (Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Lönn, Long, Sundquist, Kendler, Sundquist and Edwards2018), which may actually put individuals at greater risk for problematic alcohol use (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Leonard & Eiden, Reference Leonard and Eiden2007). This suggests that individuals who are at higher risk for problematic alcohol use, like many of those in the present sample, are at increased risk of choosing partners with higher levels of alcohol use. Thus, the absence of a protective marriage effect in our sample may reflect that marriage to a partner with problematic alcohol use undermines the protective effect of marriage (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016).

Next, to test whether marital status was a relevant moderator of genetic influences on alcohol use over time, we examined the interactions among marital status, PRS, and age to predict HED. Among married individuals at age 21, we found that HED was higher among those with higher PRS compared with those with lower PRS. This finding is indicative of a pathogenic gene-by-environment interaction effect among those who marry early (i.e., by age 21), suggesting that early marriage does not have the same protective benefit in terms of attenuating genetic predispositions as seen in older samples (Dinescu et al., Reference Dinescu, Turkheimer, Beam, Horn, Duncan and Emery2016; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Jardine and Martin1989; Horn et al., Reference Horn, Xu, Beam, Turkheimer and Emery2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Lönn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016).

Interestingly, we found that the pathogenic gene-by-environment interaction effect decayed over time. HED was similar across all conditions by age 23 and, by age 25, the effect was reversed. At age 25, we found that married individuals with higher PRS had lower HED compared to those with lower PRS. In contrast, at age 25, HED was similar among all unmarried participants, regardless of genetic risk. Our finding adds developmental nuance to the extant literature on gene-by-environment interaction effects for alcohol outcomes in that it further suggests that marriage is not a uniformly protective environment. Moreover, it underscores the importance of examining intersecting risk and protective factors, with particular consideration of the potential ramifications of developmentally off-time events (e.g., being at greater genetic risk and marrying young).

Although PRS reflect the state-of-the-science when considering measured genetic risk for complex behavioral outcomes such as alcohol use (Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018; Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Aliev, Edwards, Evans, Macleod, Hickman and Dick2014), we also recognize that, at present, they account for just a fraction of the variation. In order to examine whether our observed polygenic gene-by-environment effects were spurious, we ran two sets of sensitivity analyses.

First, we ran a parallel set of analyses using PHAD as an index of one's genetic predisposition. Consistent with our polygenic analyses, we found that, among married individuals at age 21, a PHAD was associated with higher HED compared with those without this parental history. However, the pattern of effects at age 25 differed across the PRS and parental history analyses. Specifically, there were no differences in HED among married individuals as a function of PHAD, but there were differences as a function of PRS. Thus, although the polygenic risk and parental history models are consistent in demonstrating that the pathogenic gene-by-environment effect decays over time, the inconsistent results across the polygenic risk and parental history models at age 25 caution against any strong conclusions about the exact nature of this decay (and whether the effect observed at age 21 fully reverses).

Second, we ran a set of set of analyses to examine whether our pattern of results was robust when controlling for divorce and separation status, college attendance, gene-by-covariate and covariate-by-environment interactions, and family ascertainment status. Prior research suggests that divorce and separation are associated with greater alcohol problems (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang and Hasin2015; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Walters and Forthofer1998) and that college students are more likely to engage in HED than their non-college peers (Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Miech, Bachman and Schulenberg2015; O'Malley & Johnston, Reference O'Malley and Johnston2002; Slutske, Reference Slutske2005). We observed the same pattern of effects after removing divorced and separated individuals from the unmarried group as was observed in the primary PRS analyses, allowing us to rule out the possibility that our observed effects were due to the inclusion of this subgroup. Likewise, we observed the same pattern of effects when controlling for college attendance, suggesting that our findings were not driven by individuals’ college student status. Next, we reran our analyses controlling for gene-by-covariate and covariate-by-environment interactions to address any concerns that our gene-by-environment effects were confounded (Keller, Reference Keller2014). When controlling for these interaction terms, we found a pattern of results that was consistent with those observed in the primary PRS analyses. Finally, after controlling for family ascertainment status, we observed the same pattern of effects as in our primary PRS analyses. This suggests that group-level differences between the clinically ascertained and unascertained community comparison families did not influence our results.

It is worth noting the differences in the association between PRS and HED across the main effects and interactive effects models. In the main effects model, we found no association between PRS and HED. However, in the interaction model where we examined marriage as a moderator of the association between PRS and HED, a statically significant main effect of PRS emerged. Importantly, main effects cannot be directly interpreted in the presence of an interaction effect (Aiken & West, Reference Aiken and West1991). Moreover, the null effect of the PRS in the main effects model, and its moderation by marital status in the interaction model, underscores that genetic influences on complex traits such as HED can have a differential impact depending on the environment. Consistent with this possibility, in this study we observed a crossover effect for the PRS as a function of the participants’ marital status and age. This suggests that the association between PRS and HED is dependent on marital status, such that genetic liability had a stronger impact on HED among individuals who married relatively young.

Implications

The findings of this study showed that marriage was not uniformly protective among a sample of young adults enriched for risk. Our findings suggest that, although early marriage itself was not a risky environment, this environment seemed to exacerbate risk for those with higher polygenic load. These results add an interesting developmental perspective on some of the earliest gene-by-environment effects in the field and highlight the importance of utilizing a gene-by-environment-by-development approach (Vrieze, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Vrieze, Iacono and McGue2012). Moreover, it emphasizes the need to examine intersecting risk and protective factors within this framework. These findings can aid future clinical work aimed at reducing HED by informing risk profiling. As our findings suggest that early marriage exacerbates the effects of genetic risk for alcohol misuse, individuals who marry young and who are genetically predisposed to alcohol problems may be an especially important group to target to reduce heavy drinking.

Limitations

The findings of this study should be considered in the context of several limitations. First, the study included offspring of either clinically ascertained families or unascertained comparison families. Thus, the offspring of the unascertained comparison families may not necessarily have the same risk factors as the offspring from the clinically ascertained families (although we note that comparison families were not excluded on the basis of a history of substance use disorders). Second, because the COGA Prospective Study employs a rolling enrollment strategy and we limited our sample to participants between 21 and 25 years of age, some participants were only eligible for one assessment while still in our specified age range. However, it is of note that the model we employed can incorporate different numbers of assessments and periods between assessments across participants so that we were able to maximize the sample size from the available data. Third, we did not have any data on the characteristics of our participants’ partners, which would likely influence the drinking frequency of the participants, nor did we have information regarding the relationship length (particularly for those who were living as married), to consider the potential moderating effects of these factors.

Lastly, there was an imperfect correspondence between our sample and the discovery sample used to create polygenic scores. The discovery sample consisted of older individuals from a population-based sample (Schumann et al., Reference Schumann, Liu, O'Reilly, Gao, Song, Xu and Elliott2016), while the target sample in the present study consisted of a group of young adults enriched for risk. Moreover, our study only included participants of European ancestry; therefore, it is possible that our findings may not extend to individuals of other ancestral backgrounds. Future research should address these limitations by utilizing a better matched and more ethnically diverse discovery sample, although we note that most large-scale GWAS efforts for complex traits and behaviors such as alcohol are limited in this respect at this time given the massive sample sizes required to detect small effects. Additionally, our study incorporated individuals from a sample enriched for risk, so it is unclear if our findings would generalize to other populations.

Conclusions and future directions

Previous studies of marital status as a moderator of genetic risk for alcohol outcomes have typically utilized cross-sectional studies, without considering the role of development. We examined whether marriage was a relevant moderator of genetic risk on alcohol use using a developmental framework in a sample of young adults enriched for risk. We observed pathogenic gene-by-environment effects among married individuals at age 21, but these effects decayed over time. Additionally, this work could be expanded to examine more critically the pathogenic effect of early marriage on genetic risk for alcohol use. Finally, future research should examine whether these findings extend to developmentally off-time marriages in the opposite direction (i.e., individuals whose first marriage occurs much later in life), particularly in view of findings that individuals who are at risk for alcohol problems (by virtue of family history) or have alcohol problems themselves tend to marry later (Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Lönn, Long, Sundquist, Kendler, Sundquist and Edwards2018; Waldron et al., Reference Waldron, Heath, Lynskey, Bucholz, Madden and Martin2011). Overall, our findings highlight the importance of utilizing a gene-by-environment-by-development approach and considering the consequences of developmentally off-time events.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) (Principal Investigators B. Porjesz, V. Hesselbrock, H. Edenberg, and L. Bierut), which includes 11 different centers: University of Connecticut (V. Hesselbrock); Indiana University (H. J. Edenberg, J. Nurnberger Jr., and T. Foroud); University of Iowa (S. Kuperman and J. Kramer); SUNY Downstate (B. Porjesz); Washington University in St. Louis (L. Bierut, J. Rice, K. Bucholz, and A. Agrawal); University of California at San Diego (M. Schuckit); Rutgers University (J. Tischfield and A. Brooks); Department of Biomedical and Health Informatics, The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia; Department of Genetics, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania (L. Almasy), Virginia Commonwealth University (D. Dick), Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (A. Goate); Howard University (R. Taylor).

Other COGA collaborators include: L. Bauer (University of Connecticut); J. McClintick, L. Wetherill, X. Xuei, Y. Liu, D. Lai, S. O'Connor, M. Plawecki, and S. Lourens (Indiana University); G. Chan (University of Iowa; University of Connecticut); J. Meyers, D. Chorlian, C. Kamarajan, A. Pandey, and J. Zhang (SUNY Downstate); J.-C. Wang, M. Kapoor, and S. Bertelsen (Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai); A. Anokhin, V. McCutcheon, and S. Saccone (Washington University); J. Salvatore, F. Aliev, J. Su, S. I-Chun Kuo, and B. Cho (Virginia Commonwealth University); Mark Kos (University of Texas Rio Grande Valley). A. Parsian and H. Chin are the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) Staff Collaborators.

We continue to be inspired by our memories of Henri Begleiter and Theodore Reich, founding Principal Investigator and Co-Principal Investigator of COGA, and also owe a debt of gratitude to other past organizers of COGA, including Ting-Kai Li, P. Michael Conneally, Raymond Crowe, and Wendy Reich, for their critical contributions. This national collaborative study is supported by NIH Grant U10AA008401 from the NIAAA and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA). Funding support for GWAS genotyping performed at the Johns Hopkins University Center for Inherited Disease Research was provided by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, the NIH GEI (U01HG004438), and the NIH contract "High throughput genotyping for studying the genetic contributions to human disease" (HHSN268200782096C). GWAS genotyping was also performed at the Genome Technology Access Center in the Department of Genetics at Washington University School of Medicine which is partially supported by NCI Cancer Center Support Grant #P30 CA91842 to the Siteman Cancer Center and by ICTS/CTSA Grant# UL1RR024992 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the NIH, and NIH Roadmap for Medical Research. Additional support for this project comes from K01AA024152 (JES) and K02AA018755 (DMD).

Footnotes

Denotes shared first authorship.

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised (3rd, text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th, text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Bachman, J. G., O'Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (1984). Drug use among young adults: The impacts of role status and social environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 629645. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.3.629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, P. B., Salvatore, J. E., Maes, H. H., Korhonen, T., Latvala, A., Aliev, F., … Dick, D. M. (2017). Social relationships moderate genetic influences on heavy drinking in young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78, 817826. doi:10.15288/jsad.2017.78.817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begleiter, Henri, Reich, Theodore, Hesslbrock, Victor, Porjesz, Bernice, Li, Ting-Kai, Schuckit, Marc A., … Rice, John P. (1995). The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism. Alcohol Health & Research World, 19(3), 228236. Retrieved from https://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/ahrw19-3/228%E2%80%93236.pdfGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R., Baranger, D. A. A., & Agrawal, A. (2018). Polygenic risk scores in clinical psychology: Bridging genomic risk to individual differences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 14, 119157. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084847CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., McCutcheon, V. V., Agrawal, A., Dick, D. M., Hesselbrock, V. M., Kramer, J. R., … Porjesz, B. (2017). Comparison of parent, peer, psychiatric, and cannabis use influences across stages of offspring alcohol involvement: Evidence from the COGA Prospective Study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 41, 359368. doi:10.1111/acer.13293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cotton, N. S. (1979). The familial incidence of alcoholism: A review. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 40, 89116. doi:10.15288/jsa.1979.40.89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craddock, E., vanDellen, M. R., Novak, S. A., & Ranby, K. W. (2015). Influence in relationships: A meta-analysis on health-related social control. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 37, 118130. doi:10.1080/01973533.2015.1011271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delaneau, O., Zagury, J.-F., & Marchini, J. (2013). Improved whole-chromosome phasing for disease and population genetic studies. Nature Methods, 10, 56. doi:10.1038/nmeth.2307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinescu, D., Turkheimer, E., Beam, C. R., Horn, E. E., Duncan, G., & Emery, R. E. (2016). Is marriage a buzzkill? A twin study of marital status and alcohol consumption. Journal of Family Psychology, 30, 698707. doi:10.1037/fam0000221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupre, M. E., & Meadows, S. O. (2007). Disaggregating the effects of marital trajectories on health. Journal of Family Issues, 28(5), 623652. doi: 10.1177/0192513X06296296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eickmeyer, Kasey J., & Hemez, Paul. (2017). Family Profiles (Vol. FP-17-23). Bowling Green, OH: National Center for Family & Marriage Research. doi: 10.25035/ncfmr/fp-17-23Google Scholar
Elder, G. H. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change: Perspectives on the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 415. doi:10.2307/2786971Google Scholar
Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G., & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26, 57. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1972.01750190059011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foroud, T., Edenberg, H. J., Goate, A., Rice, J., Flury, L., Koller, D. L., … Reich, T. (2000). Alcoholism susceptibility loci: Confirmation studies in a replicate sample and further mapping. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 933945. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb04634.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forthofer, Melinda S., Kessler, Ronald C., Story, Amber L., & Gotlib, Ian H. (1996). The effects of psychiatric disorders on the probability and timing of first marriage. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37(2), 121. doi: 10.2307/2137268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, B. F., Goldstein, R. B., Saha, T. D., Chou, S. P., Jung, J., Zhang, H., … Hasin, D. S. (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 757766. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, J. D., Heath, A. C., Bucholz, K. K., Madden, P. A. F., Agrawal, A., Statham, D. J., & Martin, N. G. (2007). Spousal concordance for alcohol dependence: Evidence for assortative mating or spousal interaction effects? Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 31, 717728. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00356.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., & Martin, N. G. (1989). Interactive effects of genotype and social environment on alcohol consumption in female twins. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50, 3848. doi:10.15288/jsa.1989.50.38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, S. Y., Shen, S., Lowers, L., & Locke, J. (2000). Factors predicting the onset of adolescent drinking in families at high risk for developing alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 265275. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00841-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, E. E., Xu, Y., Beam, C. R., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2013). Accounting for the physical and mental health benefits of entry into marriage: A genetically informed study of selection and causation. Journal of Family Psychology, 27, 3041. doi:10.1037/a0029803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howie, B., Fuchsberger, C., Stephens, M., Marchini, J., & Abecasis, G. R. (2012). Fast and accurate genotype imputation in genome-wide association studies through pre-phasing. Nature Genetics, 44, 955959. doi:10.1038/ng.2354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Miech, R. A., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2015). Monitoring the future. National survey results on drug use: 1975–2014: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Retrieved from http://www.monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/mtf-overview2014.pdfGoogle Scholar
Keller, M. C. (2014). Gene × environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: The problem and the (simple) solution. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 1824. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ji, J., Edwards, A. C., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2015). An extended Swedish national adoption study of alcohol use disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 211218. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Lönn, S. L., Salvatore, J., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2016). Effect of marriage on risk for onset of alcohol use disorder: A longitudinal and co-relative analysis in a Swedish national sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 911918. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., & Forthofer, M. S. (1998). The social consequences of psychiatric disorders, III: Probability of marital stability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 10921096. doi:10.1176/ajp.155.8.1092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrer, Evelyn L. (2004). Religion as a determinant of economic and demographic behavior in the United States. Population and Development Review, 30(4), 707726. doi: 10.1111/padr.2004.30.issue-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, K. E., & Eiden, R. D. (2007). Marital and family processes in the context of alcohol use and alcohol disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 285310. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091424CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, K. E., & Rothbard, J. C. (1999). Alcohol and the marriage effect. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement(s13), 139146. doi:10.15288/jsas.1999.s13.139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughran, David, & Zissimopoulos, Julie. (2004). Are there gains to delaying marriage? The effect of age at first marriage on career development and wages (Vol. IDEAS Working Paper Series, pp. 138). Santa Monica, CA: RAND. Retrieved from https://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/working_papers/2004/RAND_WR207.pdfGoogle Scholar
Maier, R. M., Visscher, P. M., Robinson, M. R., & Wray, N. R. (2018). Embracing polygenicity: A review of methods and tools for psychiatric genetics research. Psychological Medicine, 48, 10551067. doi:10.1017/S0033291717002318CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connell, J. R., & Weeks, D. E. (1998). PedCheck: A program for identification of genotype incompatibilities in linkage analysis. American Journal of Human Genetics, 63, 259266. doi:10.1086/301904CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2002). Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among American college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement(s14), 2339. doi:10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2001). Associations between marital status and alcohol consumption in a longitudinal study of female twins. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62, 589604. doi:10.15288/jsa.2001.62.589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, S., Neale, B., Todd-Brown, K., Thomas, L., Ferreira, M. A. R., Bender, D., … Sham, P. C. (2007). PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, 559575. doi:10.1086/519795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S. M., Wray, N. R., Stone, J. L., Visscher, P. M., O'Donovan, M. C., Sullivan, P. F., … Sklar, P. (2009). Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature, 460, 748752. doi:10.1038/nature08185.Google ScholarPubMed
Reich, T., Edenberg, H. J., Goate, A., Williams, J. T., Rice, J. P., Eerdewegh, P. V., … Begleiter, H. (1998). Genome-wide search for genes affecting the risk for alcohol dependence. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81, 207215. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19980508)81:3<207::AID-AJMG1>3.0.CO;2-T3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Edwards, A. C., Evans, D. M., Macleod, J., Hickman, M., … Dick, D. M. (2014). Polygenic scores predict alcohol problems in an independent sample and show moderation by the environment. Genes, 5, 330346. doi:10.3390/genes5020330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvatore, J. E., Lönn, S. L., Long, E. C., Sundquist, J., Kendler, K. S., Sundquist, K., & Edwards, A. C. (2018). Parental alcohol use disorder and offspring marital outcomes. Addiction, 114, 8191. doi:10.1111/add.14405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schumann, G., Liu, C., O'Reilly, P., Gao, H., Song, P., Xu, B., … Elliott, P. (2016). KLB is associated with alcohol drinking, and its gene product β-Klotho is necessary for FGF21 regulation of alcohol preference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 1437214377. doi:10.1073/pnas.1611243113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 60, 6576. doi:10.1093/geronb/60.Special_Issue_1.65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S. (2005). Alcohol use disorders among US college students and their non-college-attending peers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 321327. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.321CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uecker, Jeremy E. (2012). Marriage and mental health among young adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 53(1), 6783. doi: 10.1177/0022146511419206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uecker, Jeremy E. (2014). Religion and early marriage in the United States: Evidence from the Add Health study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 53(2), 392415. doi: 10.1111/jssr.12114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uecker, Jeremy E., & Stokes, Charles E. (2008). Early marriage in the United States. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70(4), 835846. doi: 10.1111/jomf.2008.70.issue-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umberson, D. (1992). Gender, marital status and the social control of health behavior. Social Science & Medicine, 34, 907917. doi:10.1016/0277-9536(92)90259-SCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrieze, S. I., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2012). Confluence of genes, environment, development, and behavior in a post Genome-Wide Association Study world. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 11951214. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000648CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldron, M., Heath, A. C., Lynskey, M. T., Bucholz, K. K., Madden, P. A. F., & Martin, N. G. (2011). Alcoholic marriage: Later start, sooner end. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 632642. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01381.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Lee, S. H., Mehta, D., Vinkhuyzen, A. A. E., Dudbridge, F., & Middeldorp, C. M. (2014). Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 55, 10681087. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12295CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi, K., & Kandel, D. B. (1985). On the resolution of role incompatibility: A life event history analysis of family roles and marijuana use. American Journal of Sociology, 90(6), 12841325. doi: 10.1086/228211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Marital status and HED frequency by age

Figure 1

Table 2. Parameter estimates and fit statistics for Alc-PRS (genome-wide PRS for alcohol) thresholds

Figure 2

Table 3. HED as a function of marital status and Alc-PRS (genome-wide PRS for alcohol) and their interaction with time

Figure 3

Figure 1. HED as a function of the three-way interaction among Alc-PRS (genome-wide PRS for alcohol), marital status, and time. Notes. Confidence intervals (CIs) are not symmetric because they were converted from expected log-transformed counts. The first three ancestral principal components and sex were included as covariates. HED frequency was measured in days over the last 12 months.

Figure 4

Table 4. HED as a function of Alc-PRS(genome-wide PRS for alcohol), marital status, time, and their interaction

Figure 5

Figure 2. HED as a function of the three-way interaction among PHAD, marital status, and time. Notes. CIs are not symmetric because they were converted from expected log-transformed counts. The first three ancestral principal components and sex were included as covariates. HED frequency was measured in days over the last 12 months.

Figure 6

Table 5. Sensitivity analyses of HED as a function of PHAD, marital status, time, and their interaction