Hostname: page-component-66644f4456-mv4w6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T13:40:48.848Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

Temperament as a moderator of the effects of parenting on children's behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2014

Elena Gallitto*
Affiliation:
Carleton University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Elena Gallitto, Department of Psychology, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON K1S 5B6, Canada; E-mail: e_gallitto@hotmail.com.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study examined the role of child temperament as moderator of the effect of parenting style on children's externalizing and internalizing behaviors. A series of structural equation models were fit to a representative sample of 2,631 Canadian children from the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth. In addition to testing for the presence of Temperament × Parenting interactions, these models also examined the direct and indirect effects of a number of additional contextual factors such as neighborhood problems, neighborhood cohesion, social support, and maternal depression. The results indicate that exposure to more positive parenting reduces behavior problems in children with difficult/unadaptable temperaments. No moderating effects of temperament on hostile parenting were found. Such results serve to highlight the pivotal role of positive features of the rearing environment as catalysts for the successful adaptation of children with difficult/unadaptable temperaments. The results of this modeling work also serve to emphasize the importance of considering the ways in which more distal factors can affect children's behavioral adaptation by contributing to changes in proximal family processes.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Early temperamental dispositions represent critically important antecedents of children's future psychosocial adjustment. Temperament is characterized by a combination of innate characteristics regarding specific emotional, motoric, attentional, and self-regulating processes that are first apparent in infancy and maintain a relative stability over time (Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Damon and Eisenberg1998). Such innate dispositions exert an impact on the infant's emotional reactivity to external stimuli and his/her capacity to adapt to the changing environment (Allport, Reference Allport1961; Buss & Plomin, Reference Buss and Plomin1984; Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Damon and Eisenberg1998). A number of studies have related children's temperamental disposition to the development of two distinct clusters of maladaptive behavioral symptoms, namely, externalizing behaviors that reflect the inability to promptly inhibit impulses and the tendency to manifest aggressive behavior, and internalizing behaviors such as excessive worrying, general low emotional tone, and social withdrawal. With respect to the development of externalizing behaviors, early negative emotionality, irritability, and frustration (i.e., difficult temperament; Bates, Freeland, & Lounsboury, Reference Bates, Freeland and Lounsboury1979) have consistently been associated with impulsive and aggressive conduct across childhood (Bates, Bayles, Bennet, Ridge, & Brown, Reference Bates, Bayles, Bennet, Ridge, Brown, Pepler and Rubin1991; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Murphy, Guthrie and Jones1997; Guerin, Gottfried, & Thomas, Reference Guerin, Gottfried and Thomas1997; Shaw, Owens, Giovannelli, & Winslow, Reference Shaw, Owens, Giovannelli and Winslow2001; Stringaris, Maughan, & Goodman, Reference Stringaris, Maughan and Goodman2010; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Wang, Deng, Eisenberg, Wolchik and Tein2008). Research has also identified early temperamental traits characterized by fearfulness and withdrawal from novelty (i.e., unadaptable temperament; Bates et al., Reference Bates, Freeland and Lounsboury1979) as important precursors of children's internalizing behaviors (Bates, Reference Bates, Wachs and Kohnstamm2000; Kagan, Reznick, Clarke, Snidman, & Garcia-Coll, Reference Kagan, Reznick, Clarke, Snidman and Garcia-Coll1984; Keiley, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, Reference Keiley, Bates, Dodge, Pettit and Columbus2002; Manassis & Bradley, Reference Manassis and Bradley1994; Rapee & Coplan, Reference Rapee and Coplan2010; Sanson, Hemphill, & Smart, Reference Sanson, Hemphill and Smart2004).

Notwithstanding the pivotal role that temperamental dispositions have in predicting problem behaviors in childhood, single risk factors are likely to account for only a small proportion of variance in developmental outcomes. For these reasons, current research has emphasized the need to incorporate other relevant ecological and contextual factors when studying temperament as a predictor of individual differences in behavior (Gallagher, Reference Gallager2002; Nigg, Reference Nigg2006). Among a multitude of contextual factors influencing children's behaviors, parenting practices appear to be an important aspect of children's psychological adjustment (Collins, Macobby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, Reference Collins, Macobby, Steinberg, Hetherington and Bornstein2000). Generally, positive parenting has been considered fundamental to all aspects of healthy child development. It is through sensitive and responsive parenting behavior that parents fulfill the child's basic needs for physical proximity, interpersonal relatedness, and intimacy, which are essential to the promotion of children's emotional, social, and intellectual growth (Ainsworth, Bell, & Stayton, Reference Ainsworth, Bell, Stayton and Richards1974; Kochanska, Reference Kochanska2002; Kochanska & Murray, Reference Kochanska and Murray2000; McCain & Mustard, Reference McCain and Mustard1999; Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, Reference Pettit, Bates and Dodge1997; Weatherston & Fitzgerald, Reference Weatherston, Fitzgerald, Tyano, Keren, Herrman and Cox2010). Conversely, children whose basic developmental needs for emotional closeness, intimacy, and security are not adequately met are at increased risk for compromised mental health. Children raised in hostile parenting environments have been shown to report an increased risk for the emergence of emotional and behavioral problems both in childhood and later in adolescence (Bayer et al., Reference Bayer, Hiscock, Obioha, Ukoumunne, Price and Wake2008; Rutter, Giller, & Hagell, Reference Rutter, Giller and Hagell1998; although note that an exception applies to studies conducted in high-risk areas where harsh/punitive parenting has been found to be related to better outcomes; Beyers, Bates, Pettit, & Dodge, Reference Beyers, Bates, Pettit and Dodge2003; Roche, Ghazarian, Little, & Leventhal, Reference Roche, Ghazarian, Little and Leventhal2011).

Despite evidence for the contributions of both parenting and temperament to children's behavior, predictive models based on the main effects of such variables serve to reduce complex interrelationships to relatively simple linear expressions, thereby failing to fully elucidate the mechanisms responsible for developmental outcomes. Conversely, research that emphasizes the interactive effects of biological and environmental factors might be better able to capture heterogeneity in developmental pathways. Such research has typically been conducted within the framework of either the diathesis stress or the differential susceptibility paradigms. The diathesis–stress/dual-risk model (Monroe & Simons, Reference Monroe and Simons1991; Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2000; Zuckerman, Reference Zuckerman1999) is based on the claim that early exposure to adversity contributes to the development of behavior and mental health problems in individuals manifesting early-life vulnerability factors, such as difficult temperament (Belsky, Hsieh, & Crnic, Reference Belsky, Hsieh and Crnic1998; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2012), physiological reactivity (Walker & Diforio, Reference Walker and Diforio1997), or genetic makeup (Gunthert et al., Reference Gunthert, Conner, Armeli, Tennen, Covault and Kranzler2007).

According to the differential susceptibility view (Belsky, Reference Belsky1997, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005; Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2007; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009, Reference Belsky and Pluess2010), children who manifest negative temperamental dispositions can exhibit a differential susceptibility to rearing influences, either for better or for worse. That is, children characterized as more temperamentally negative might not only be particularly susceptible to aversive parenting practices but also tend to display heightened behavioral malleability in face of favorable environmental influences (e.g., for fearful-inhibited infants see Gilissen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & van der Veer, Reference Gilissen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn and van der Veer2008; Gilissen, Koolstra, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van der Veer, Reference Gilissen, Koolstra, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van der Veer2007; for difficult temperament see Bradley & Corwyn, Reference Bradley and Corwyn2008; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Stoel, Kranenburg-Bakermans, van IJzendoorn, Juffer and Koot2009; van Zeijl et al., Reference van Zeijl, Mesman, Stolk, Alink, van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2007). A key premise of the differential susceptibility view is that such dispersion of vulnerability/plasticity genes is beneficial to future progeny in terms of both reproductive and inclusive fitness (Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2010). Similar to bet-hedging strategies, generating offsprings who differ in their responsivity to the environment would not only increase the chances of survival of the “physically fit” ones in the face of adversity but also prove to be advantageous for the vulnerable ones when exposed to favorable conditions (Belsky, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005). In such a way, the differential susceptibility framework has served to shed the light on the role that temperament has, not only as a source of risk, but also as a determinant of adaptive plasticity, thereby moving current thinking beyond the diathesis–stress-driven view.

In a more recent reformulation of their theory, Pluess and Belsky (Reference Pluess and Belsky2012) proposed the adoption of the term “vantage sensitivity” to indicate the propensity of an individual to prosper in a development-enhancing environment, although not necessarily be affected by adverse circumstances (as in the case of differential susceptibility). Correspondingly, vantage sensitivity represents the positive end, or the “bright” side of differential susceptibility, where increases in the level of functioning are manifested by individuals endowed with specific endogenous characteristics that render them more sensitive to exposure to high-quality environments. According to this latter view, temperamental traits are regarded as a marker of enhanced adaptability with respect to the promotion of both emotional and behavioral health. In sum, the research suggests that not all children are affected equally by their rearing experiences and that patterns of Parenting × Temperament interactions are important for understanding the complex relationships among proximal developmental processes, children's inherited dispositions, and behavioral outcomes.

Concurrent with recent advances made in the study of Parenting × Temperament interactions, an underpinning of research focused on the broader social–ecological framework has surged in the past few decades (for a review, see Kotchick & Forehand, Reference Kotchick and Forehand2002). In this vein, the bioecological model of Bronfenbrenner and Morris (Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner1998, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner2006) has provided a useful framework for research that aims to study the influence of multiple contextual factors on children's development. This model incorporates the dynamic interplay among innate biological processes (i.e., temperament), and the microlevel (i.e., family) and macrolevel (i.e., societal influences) factors that shape the individual's development over time (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner1998). In line with such a multidimensional/multifaceted approach, various contextual factors, such as maternal depression and neighborhood quality, are viewed as potential sources of influence on development that exert both direct effects on children's behavior and indirect ones through their impact on parental behavior. As suggested by a large body of studies, children exposed to maternal depression during their early years are at risk for the development of mental health and behavioral problems throughout the life course (Bureau, Easterbrooks, & Lyons-Ruth, Reference Bureau, Easterbrooks and Lyons-Ruth2009; Davis, Reference Davis2006; Lee & Gotlib, Reference Lee and Gotlib1991; Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, & Cibelli, Reference Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks and Cibelli1997; Weissman, Warner, Wickramaratne, Moreau, & Olfson, Reference Weissman, Warner, Wickramaratne, Moreau and Olfson1997; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Nomura, Warner, Pilowsky and Verdeli2006). The negative impact of parental depression extends to various aspects of children's mental health, including self-development, self-esteem, and affect regulation (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Toth1998; Cummings & Davies, Reference Cummings, Davies, Joiner and Coyne1999; Goodman & Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999). In addition, indirect effects of maternal depression on children's behavior are often manifested through parenting. Generally, symptoms of depression can interfere with mothers' ability to experience pleasure in their children's activities and engage in positive dyadic interactions with them (Leve, Kim, & Pears, Reference Leve, Kim and Pears2005; Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, & Repacholi, Reference Lyons-Ruth, Alpern and Repacholi1998; Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Baghurst, Sawyer, Prior, Clark and Arney2006; Rottenberg & Gotlib, Reference Rottenberg, Gotlib and Power2004). Similarly, depressed parents often display impaired attention, withdrawal, and self-absorption that reduce their ability to focus on inputs from their children and limit their effectiveness in directing behavior toward child-oriented goals (Dix & Meunier, Reference Dix and Meunier2009). As a consequence, depressed mothers are more likely to engage in insensitive and coercive parenting (Bugental, Reference Bugental, Sigel, McGillicuddy-DeLisi and Goodnow1992; Feng, Shaw, Skuban, & Lane, Reference Feng, Shaw, Skuban and Lane2007; Hasting & Grusec, Reference Hasting and Grusec1998; Patterson, Reference Patterson1982) and are generally emotionally unresponsive and less able to engage positively with their children (Jacob & Johnson, Reference Jacob and Johnson1997; Lovejoy, Graczyk, O'Hare, & Neuman, Reference Lovejoy, Graczyk, O'Hare and Neuman2000).

Distal features of the environment can also have an effect on children's developmental outcomes. With respect to such features, both neighborhood structural (i.e., lack of safety, problems, and social disorder) and functional (i.e., neighborhood collective efficacy) characteristics have been regarded as factors that can both directly and indirectly affect the adjustment of the developing child. The research on neighborhood structural characteristics shows that growing up in impoverished or violent communities puts children at increased risk for the development of problem behaviors (Attar, Guerra, & Tolan, Reference Attar, Guerra and Tolan1994; McLeod & Edwards, Reference McLeod and Edwards1995; Moren-Cross, Wright, LaGory, & Lanzi, Reference Moren-Cross, Wright, LaGory and Lanzi2006). Furthermore, children living in dangerous areas are more likely to be subjected to hostile parenting, given that caregivers tend to adopt such practices to a greater degree under more extreme circumstances (Earls, McGuire, & Shay, Reference Earls, McGuire and Shay1994; Pinderhughes, Nix, Foster, & Jones, Reference Pinderhughes, Nix, Foster and Jones2001). Moreover, neighborhood problems can also contribute to the risk of children's psychological disorders by increasing the prevalence of parental depression (Belle, Reference Belle1990; Brown, Bhrolchain, & Harris, Reference Brown, Bhrolchain and Harris1975; Kim, Reference Kim2010; Ross, Reference Ross2000; Ross & Mirowsky, Reference Ross and Mirowsky2001).

Conversely, the presence of social bonds, trust, and social supportive relationships among neighborhood residents plays a crucial role in sustaining well-being under condition of high stress (Cobb, Reference Cobb1976; Leavy, Reference Leavy1983). In support of this notion, findings based on a recent examination of Canadian 0- to 11-year-old children confirmed the role of neighborhood collective efficacy (i.e., cohesion, trust, and reciprocity) as one of the mechanisms by which distal features of the social environment might affect children's behavior (Kohen, Leventhal, Dahinten, & McIntosh, Reference Kohen, Leventhal, Dahinten and McIntosh2008). In this particular study, neighborhood collective efficacy was associated with lower ratings of maternal depression and higher family functioning scores that, in turn, led to more positive parenting, and ultimately to a decrease in children's behavioral problems. Similarly, it has been shown that support provided by both members of the family (i.e., close relative and partner) and other extrafamilial sources of assistance (i.e., friends and health professionals) are essential to the promotion of individual well-being and the improvement of the life of children (Belsky, Reference Belsky1994; Cutrona & Troutman, Reference Cutrona and Troutman1986; Power & Parke, Reference Power and Parke1984). For instance, mothers who have strong social support networks tend to be less punitive and more responsive with their children (Colletta, Reference Colletta1979; Powell, Reference Powell1980). Moreover, high levels of social support can help to buffer the effects of maternal depression on children's developmental outcomes through the enhancement of perceived self-efficacy in the parenting role (Cochran & Brassard, Reference Cochran and Brassard1979).

In general, distal features of the broader contextual living environment, such as neighborhood quality, in addition to more proximal intrafamilial factors, such as the presence of maternal depression, contribute to general well-being of both children and their family members. In this light, any attempt made to understand the manner in which children's behavioral outcomes are influenced by Parenting × Temperament interactions should also account for potential effects of the broader ecological context such as those associated with the immediate family environment as well as other community-level factors. Such a multilayered approach can provide the means through which to detect the multiple co-occurring facets of the social and physical environments that affect the development of children.

The Present Study

Previous investigations have provided useful insights into the moderating effects of temperament on the association between parenting and children's behavior. In the present study, an examination of such interaction effects is conducted while also estimating the simultaneous contribution of a number of other key intrafamilial processes and macrolevel factors associated with children's behavioral outcomes. These relationships are conceptualized within four structural equation models. Two are risk models, which, in addition to temperament, include neighborhood problems, maternal depression, and hostile parenting as potential contextual risk factors affecting children's externalizing and internalizing behaviors (see Figures 1 and 2). Within the first risk model, the direct effect that difficult temperament (i.e., early tendency to manifest heightened irritability and frustration in response to environmental demands) has on future externalizing behavior is examined, whereas within the second risk model, the direct effect that unadaptable temperament has on future internalizing behavior is examined. The term unadaptable temperament here is used to indicate the child's fearful reactions to novel aspects of the environment (e.g., new people, toys, or places), which has often been examined in relation to internalizing behaviors. Within each of these risk models, the direct effect of hostile parenting and both the direct and indirect effects of neighborhood problems and maternal depression on the respective behavioral outcomes are also examined. Within each risk model, Temperament × Parenting interactions are tested by taking into account the role of temperament as a moderator of the relationship between hostile parenting levels and behavioral outcomes. Specifically, it is hypothesized that the presence of externalizing behavior problems should be greatly enhanced for temperamentally difficult children exposed to more hostile parenting practices. Similarly, higher levels of hostile parenting should have a greater impact on unadaptable children, compared to their more adaptable counterparts, with respect to the manifestation of internalizing behavior problems. Note that, for both cases, the interactive effects of temperament and hostile parenting are expected to be significant and positive in magnitude (i.e., whereby the combination of higher scores on both the difficult/unadaptable temperament and hostile parenting measures serves to enhance the presence of externalizing/internalizing behavior problems in children).

Figure 1. Structural equation modeling results for the first risk model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Figure 2. Structural equation modeling results for the second risk model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. **p < .01, ***p < .001.

The other two models are promotive models, which, in addition to temperament and maternal depression, include neighborhood cohesion, social support, and positive parenting as potential contextual factors that could be assumed to promote better outcomes for children by reducing the chances of the future development of externalizing and internalizing behavior problems (see Figures 3 and 4). Within each of these promotive models, the direct effects of both neighborhood cohesion and positive parenting and the indirect effects of both neighborhood cohesion and social support on the respective behavioral outcomes are examined. Within each promotive model, Temperament × Parenting interactions are tested by taking into account the role of children's temperament as a moderator of the relationship between positive parenting levels and behavioral outcomes. Specifically, it is hypothesized that, in contrast to temperamentally easy children, difficult children should manifest fewer externalizing behavior problems when they are generally engaged in positive and supportive relationships with their mothers. Similarly, those children displaying unadaptable temperament should exhibit less internalizing behavior problems when their mothers tend to be positive and responsive caregivers. Here, for both cases, the interactive effects of temperament and positive parenting are expected to be significant and negative in magnitude (i.e., whereby the combination of higher scores on both difficult/unadaptable temperament and positive parenting measures serves to reduce the presence of externalizing/internalizing problems in children).

Figure 3. Structural equation modeling results for the first promotive model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. ***p < .001.

Figure 4. Structural equation modeling results for the second promotive model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. *p < .05. ***p < .001.

Method

Sample

The data for this research come from the Canadian National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (NLSCY). The NLSCY is a long-term study that includes repeated cross-sectional and longitudinal cohorts designed to give national estimates for the population of Canadian children. Statistics Canada has conducted the NLSCY over a 16-year period in biennial data-collection cycles. The main objectives of the NLSCY are to collect data on factors influencing child development, including social and environmental correlates of early childhood development, and to make that information available for people who are developing programs and policies focused on improving the life of children and youth (Statistics Canada, 1996). The original NLSCY sampling strategy relied on household selection through a multistage, stratified cluster, probability-sampling procedure based on basic demographic and other labor force information collected by Statistics Canada's Labour Force Survey. This technique involves the stratification and/or clustering of population units before sampling. City blocks are first selected based on specific characteristics (i.e., province and average income). Subsequently, households are selected from within those blocks, and children from each household are randomly selected (Michaud, Reference Michaud2001).

Data for the present study were obtained from two cycles of the survey, namely, Cycle 1 and Cycle 3. The first wave of data collection was conducted in 1994/95 at Cycle 1 on 13,439 residential households across Canada. It had an overall response rate of 86.3%, resulting in a sample of data for 22,831 newborn to 11-year-old children (Statistics Canada, 1996). The present study focuses on a subsample of children 2–3 years old (N = 3,909) for whom a series of potential predictors of behavioral problems were measured at Cycle 1. Subsequently, those children's behavioral outcomes were assessed at 6–7 years old using measures incorporated into Cycle 3 (1998–1999). Approximately 32% (N = 1,278) of the selected children were lost at Cycle 3 due to attrition. After deletion of the attrition sample, the matched longitudinal component across the two cycles contained N = 2,631 children. In the present study, the mean ages of the children were 2.57 years at Cycle 1 (SD = 0.49) and 6.50 at Cycle 3 (SD = 0.51). Among these children, 48.8% were males. The majority of them lived with both biological parents (81.4%). In 93% of the cases, the survey respondent was the mother of the child. Mother's average age at the time of the initial interview was 31.7 years (SD = 5.3). The majority of mothers had completed their high school studies (67.8%).

For the present research, an attrition analysis was performed to determine whether the loss of panel data from Cycle 1 to Cycle 3 might have had a significant impact on the final estimates and generalization of the study results. For this purpose, a series of cross-sectionally weighted independent t tests and contingency table analyses was conducted on the set of demographic and predictor variables measured at Cycle 1 to determine whether there were any significant differences between individuals used in the structural equation modeling (SEM) analyses and study dropouts (i.e., those with no follow-up data at Cycle 3). The attrition analysis revealed statistically significant differences only with respect to parental education, χ2 (3) = 8.65, p = .034, between the sample of children included in the study and those excluded due to attrition.

Table 1 presents a comparison between the original sample, the final attrition sample, and the Canadian population based upon available demographic information. As shown in Table 1, the similarities between the current subsample and national estimates (when available) provide some evidence for the generalizability of the results to the general Canadian population. It is also important to note that all of the following statistical analyses were conducted utilizing longitudinal survey weights (which were normalized to return the sample to its original size by dividing the weight variable by its average). The use of survey weights in a clustered stratified sample such as the NLSCY allows the results of statistical tests to be generalized to the population, thus increasing the likelihood that the sample remains representative of the original target population despite survey dropout.

Table 1. Demographic information

Subsequently, missing data on any of the study variables were imputed in LISREL 8.80 using multiple imputation (with 100 imputed data sets) based on the expectation-maximization algorithm (Schafer, Reference Schafer1997). Proportions of missing data for each of the study variables are listed in Table 2. In the expectation-maximization algorithm, the two expectation and maximization steps are iterated through multiple times using maximum likelihood (ML). In the expectation step, the distribution for the missing data is calculated based on the known values for the observed data, and estimates of the sums of squares and cross products are generated. In the maximization step, the unknown parameters are estimated using all of the other variables as predictors in a regression model, and the estimates of the sums of squares and cross products matrix obtained in the expectation step is then used to estimate a new covariance matrix along with the associated regression coefficients. This two-step procedure is repeated for several iterations until the difference between the estimated covariance matrices from step-to-step falls below some specified convergence criterion (Little & Rubin, Reference Little and Rubin1987; Rubin, Reference Rubin1987). In general, multiple imputation procedures have been shown to produce consistent and unbiased estimates when the missing data are either missing at random or missing completely at random (Kristman, Manno, & Cote, Reference Kristman, Manno and Cote2004). Moreover, the multiple imputation process attempts to reduce the impact of the loss of residual variability by introducing a random error to the imputation model through repeated random sampling with replacement (Enders, Reference Enders2010). Table 2 also presents the means and standard deviations for each of the study variables after imputation.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics (after imputation) and amount of missing data for all of the study variables in the final sample (N = 2,631)

Measures

Neighborhood characteristics (Cycle 1)

A revised version of the Simcha–Fagan Neighborhood Questionnaire was used in the NLSCY to assess characteristics of the neighborhood (McGuire, Reference McGuire1997). Five items were employed to measure the respondent's perception of neighborhood problems (i.e., litter, broken glass or garbage in the street or road, illicit drug trafficking, and vandalism). Mothers rated each item along a 3-point scale. All item scores were then reversed so that higher scores represented a greater prevalence of neighborhood problems. For this scale, the Cronbach α was 0.73 for the sample of respondents used in this study.

Five items were used to measure neighborhood collective efficacy, a construct that combines both social cohesion (i.e., mutual trust among neighbors and sharing common values) and informal social control (i.e., neighbors can rely on one another to monitor and supervise youth). Mothers rated each item along a 4-point scale. All item scores were then reversed so that higher scores represented a greater prevalence of neighborhood collective efficacy (although note that, for convenience, the term “neighborhood cohesion” is used to refer to this scale). For this scale, the Cronbach α was 0.87 for the sample of respondents used in this study.

Social support (Cycle 1)

The short version of the Social Provision Scale (Cutrona & Russell, Reference Cutrona, Russell, Jones and Perlman1987) was used to assess the degree of social support received by the respondents from family members and friends. The respondents were asked to report on a 4-point scale the extent to which each item describes their current social network. The scale is composed of six items in total. Examples of items are as follows: “I have family and friends who help me feel safe, secure and happy” and “There is someone I trust whom I would turn to for advice if I were having problems.” For this scale, the Cronbach α was 0.82 for the sample of respondents used in this study.

Maternal depression (Cycle 1)

Maternal depressive symptoms were assessed using the reduced version of the Center of Epidemiological Studies Depression Rating Scale (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). The scale is based on 12 items measured on a 4-point scale and is meant to assess the presence of depression in the general population. The frequency and severity of depressive symptoms in the preceding week were measured. Common symptoms include lack of interest in outdoor or fun activities, lack of appetite, apathy, hopelessness, loneliness, crying spells, and sleeping disturbances. For this scale, the Cronbach α was 0.81 for the sample of respondents used in this study.

Parenting quality (Cycle 1)

In the NLSCY, the quality of parenting was measured in terms of three dimensions of parental behaviors that assess positive, hostile–ineffective, and consistent parenting, respectively (where only the first two are of interest in the present study). Each of the items was scored on a 5-point scale indicating the frequency of the behavior in question. The positive interaction scale (consisting of five items in total) assesses parents' perspective on the positive interactions that they have with their offspring, including contingent praise, sensitivity, and enjoyment of activities with the children. For this scale, the Cronbach α was 0.73 for the sample of respondents used in this study. The hostile–ineffective parenting scale (consisting of seven items in total) is based on the parent-reported use of negative parenting techniques, including frequent punishment, harsh discipline, disapproval, and general management problems. For this scale, the Cronbach α was 0.71 for the sample of respondents used in this study.

Temperament (Cycle 1)

The temperament scales utilized in the NLSCY consist of adapted versions of the Infant Characteristics Questionnaire (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Freeland and Lounsboury1979), which was originally developed for use with 3- to 5-month-olds. The two versions used in the present study were adapted for use with both 24- and 35-month children (i.e., Lee & Bates, Reference Lee and Bates1985). The main intent of the scale is to obtain information from parents regarding the degree of difficulty of the child as well as the quality of their emotionality and reactivity. This scale is composed of 32 items divided into four main subscales reflecting four distinctive temperamental traits. Each of the items is ranked on a 7-point scale. For the purpose of the present study, two temperamental profiles are included in the subsequent analyses, namely, difficultness and unadaptability. Six items from the difficult temperament subscale were utilized to assesses the degree of irritability and moodiness of the child (Cronbach α = 0.75 for the sample of respondents used in this study). Five items from the unadaptability subscale were used to assess the degree of unadaptability to new situations (Cronbach α = 0.67 for the sample of respondents used in this study).

Behavioral problems (Cycle 3)

Caregiver's reports of children's competencies and behavioral problems were obtained using the Child Behavioral Checklist (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991). This measure allows for the examination of two broad groupings of syndromes: externalizing problems and internalizing problems. Items are scored on a 3-point scale for which a score of 0 indicates an absence of the behavior in question whereas scores of 1 and 2 represent ordered categories indicating increasing severity of the behavior. In the present study, physical aggression, indirect aggression, and hyperactivity items were used to provide a single measure of externalizing problems (Cronbach α = 0.87), whereas the emotional disorder subscale was used as an index of internalizing behaviors (Cronbach α = 0.80).

Statistical modeling

In the present study, SEM (LISREL 8.80; Jöreskog & Sörbom, Reference Jöreskog and Sörbom2001) was used to test whether the hypothesized models provided a good fit to the observed data. Given the ordinal nature of the data set, ML estimation procedures were used along with Satorra and Bentler's (Reference Satorra, Bentler, von Eye and Clogg1994) scaling corrections, allowing for the calculation of Satorra–Bentler scaled chi-square (SB χ2) values. This method of analysis, referred to as robust ML, has the advantage of correcting the chi-square and the standard errors (which are dubbed robust standard errors) for multivariate normality violations (Savalei, Reference Savalei2010).

Model fit indices

A commonly reported measure of model fit is the Pearson chi-square statistic χ2. Generally, significant χ2 values will be obtained if an SEM model is not supported by the sample data (i.e., there are discrepancies between observed and predicted covariance values). However, it is widely known that chi-square based statistics are extremely sensitive to sample sizes and model complexity (e.g., Kline, Reference Kline2005). This situation might then result in the rejection of reasonable models due to the presence of large samples and large numbers of latent factors. Hence, to help evaluate the present models, a series of additional comparative fit indices were also examined (Byrne, Reference Byrne1998), which included the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger & Lind, Reference Steiger and Lind1980), the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, Reference Bentler1990), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR; Jöreskog & Sörbom, Reference Jöreskog and Sörbom1986), and the expected cross validation index (ECVI; Browne & Cudeck, Reference Browne and Cudeck1989). Criterion values for these fit statistics are based on Hu and Bentler's (Reference Hu and Bentler1999) recommendations for determining a close fit of a structural model, namely, RMSEA < 0.06, CFI > 0.95, and SRMR < 0.08.

Latent interaction factors

Product-term interaction indicators can be obtained by cross-multiplying all of the indicator variables for each of the corresponding first-order factors. In the present study, in order to minimize correlations among the first-order and interaction factors (i.e., nonessential multicollinearity), each product indicator was then residual centered (Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Wen, Hau, Little, Bovaird and Widaman2007). That is, each product term was separately regressed against all first-order indicators simultaneously and the residuals from those regressions were used as indicators for the latent interaction factor. Compared to the mean-centering approach of Marsh, Wen, and Hau (Reference Marsh, Wen and Hau2004), residual centering has the advantage of being able to derive latent interaction factors without having to alter the means of the first-order latent factors (Jöreskog & Yang, Reference Jöreskog, Yang, Marcoulides and Schumacker1996).

Generally, though, using all possible product indicators (e.g., 6 × 7 = 42 for the difficult temperament and hostile parenting interaction) is not considered a parsimonious practice because it can result in nonconvergence problems for complex models with a large number of latent factors. However, selecting only one or two product indicators can result in a loss of information (Marsh, Wen, & Hau, Reference Marsh, Wen, Hau, Hancock and Mueller2006). In a recent simulation study, Saris, Batista-Foguet, and Coenders (Reference Saris, Batista-Foguet and Coenders2007) showed that the optimal way to estimate latent interaction factors is to select a set of the most reliable product indicators. For these reasons, in the present study confirmatory factor analyses were used to select 20 of the residual-centered product indicators with the highest factor loadings and the lowest measurement errors on the latent interaction factors as the indicators for each of those factors.

With respect to the imputation of any missing values for the product indicators, the “transform, then impute” method suggested by von Hippel (Reference von Hippel2009) was employed. This two-step procedure involves first computing the product term indicators from the incomplete data and then imputing the missing values on those indicators. In the present study, this second step was done after the imputation of the missing values for all of the noninteraction indicators had been completed (in order to prevent the possibility that the product terms would distort the imputation estimates for the nonproduct indicators if both were inserted in the imputation model in a single step). The use of the “transform and impute” method is justified by its effectiveness in producing more reliable estimates (Allison, Reference Allison2002) and because it helps to preserve the nature of the covariances among the raw and transformed variables (von Hippel, Reference von Hippel2009).

Covariates

To attenuate the effects of any potential confounding factors, covariate adjustment was implemented by regarding child's gender, maternal education, family income, and family structure as control variables. Dummy-coded variables were created for all control variables given their categorical nature (see Table 1). Subsequently, each of the study variable items was regressed onto the full set of covariates. The corresponding regression residuals were then saved and used as the indicators of the latent factors when estimating the structural models (Geldhof, Pornprasertmanit, Schoemann, & Little, Reference Geldhof, Pornprasertmanit, Schoemann and Little2013).

Results

Measurement models for the latent factors were assessed prior to testing the structural relationships among them. The fit of each of the four measurement models was deemed satisfactory (see Table 3). The estimated factor loadings were all significant at the .01 level and in the expected direction. Such results indicated that the relations among the observed indicator variables and their underlying latent factors were quite strong. The matrix of correlations between all of the latent study variables was also inspected for the presence of any large correlations between pairs of variables, and no multicollinearity problems were detected (see Table 4).

Table 3. Overall fit of the measurement models

Note: RMSEA, Root mean square error of approximation; 95% CI, confidence internal for RMSEA; CFI, comparative fit index; SRMR, standardized root mean square residual.

Table 4. Matrix of correlations between all of the latent factors

Note: NP, Neighborhood problems; NC, neighborhood cohesion; SS, social support; DP, parental depression; DF, difficult temperament; UN, unadaptable temperament; HP, hostile parenting; PP, positive parenting; INT, internalizing behavior; EXT, externalizing behavior.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Risk models

For both risk models, goodness of fit indices suggested the presence of a satisfactory fit for this sample, SB χ2 (2233) = 12888, p < .001; RMSEA = 0.043, 90% confidence interval (CI) = (0.042–0.043), CFI = 0.91, SRMR = 0.082, ECVI = 5.04 for externalizing behaviors; SB χ2 (1389) = 6339, p < .001; RMSEA = 0.037, 90% CI = (0.036–0.038), CFI = 0.91, SRMR = 0.070, and ECVI = 2.53 for internalizing behaviors. The full path diagrams for the risk models for externalizing and internalizing behaviors are shown in Figures 1 and 2, respectively. In these two figures, all path coefficients are positive and significant except for the path between neighborhood problems and hostile parenting, the paths connecting both neighborhood problems and the temperament by hostile parenting interaction to the externalizing outcome behaviors, and the path between the interaction and internalizing behaviors. As expected from the literature reviewed earlier, hostile parenting is directly related to the future manifestation of both externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Maternal depression is related to externalizing and internalizing behaviors both directly and indirectly through its relation to hostile parenting. Neighborhood problems is not directly related to externalizing behaviors but is related to it indirectly through its relation to maternal depression (but, surprisingly, not hostile parenting). Although difficult temperament is directly related to the future manifestation of externalizing behaviors, it does not moderate the relation between hostile parenting levels and such behaviors. Similarly, although unadaptable temperament is directly related to the future manifestation of internalizing behaviors, it does not moderate the relation between hostile parenting levels and such behaviors.

Promotive models

For both promotive models, the goodness-of-fit indices suggested the presence of a satisfactory fit for this sample, SB χ2 (2428) = 12816, p < .001; RMSEA = 0.040, 90% CI = (0.040–0.041), CFI = 0.92, SRMR = 0.050, ECVI = 5.03 for externalizing behaviors; SB χ2 (1721) = 8034, p < .001; RMSEA = 0.037, 90% CI = (0.037–0.038), CFI = 0.93, SRMR = 0.047, and ECVI = 3.18 for internalizing behaviors. Figure 3 shows the full diagram for the promotive model involving externalizing behaviors. In this figure, all path coefficients are significant except for the paths from neighborhood cohesion to both maternal depression and externalizing behaviors, and the path from maternal depression to positive parenting. As expected from the literature reviewed earlier, neighborhood cohesion is related to externalizing behaviors indirectly through its relation to positive parenting (but, surprisingly, not to maternal depression). Indirect effects of social support on externalizing behavior through both maternal depression and positive parenting are also present. Maternal depression has only a direct effect on externalizing behavior given that it is not related to positive parenting in this model. Furthermore, the direct effects of positive parenting and difficult temperament on externalizing behaviors are significant. However, the relationship between positive parenting and externalizing behaviors is now moderated by difficult temperament. The negative value of the path coefficient associated with this interaction indicates that that the size of the negative relation between positive parenting levels and externalizing behaviors is larger for higher levels of difficult temperament.

The full path diagram for the promotive model involving internalizing behaviors is shown in Figure 4. All path coefficients are significant except for the paths to internalizing behaviors leading from both neighborhood cohesion and positive parenting as well as the paths from neighborhood cohesion to maternal depression and from maternal depression to positive parenting. Hence, of the four nontemperament variables in this model, only maternal depression has a direct relation to the future manifestation of internalizing behaviors, with only social support being related to this outcome indirectly, through its relation to maternal depression. In contrast, unadaptable temperament is directly related to the future manifestation of internalizing behaviors, and although there is no direct main effect of positive parenting, the relationship between positive parenting and internalizing behaviors is moderated by unadaptable temperament. The negative value of the path coefficient associated with this interaction indicates that the size of the small negative relation between positive parenting levels and internalizing behaviors is larger for higher levels of unadaptable temperament.

Interaction analyses

To better characterize the nature of the significant interaction effects in the two promotive models, simple slope testing was conducted (Aiken & West, Reference Aiken and West1991; Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012). The average factor score for temperament, as well as scores one standard deviation below and above the average were used to derive simple regression lines for the effects of parenting. For these analyses, standardized latent factor scores for each of the temperament, parenting, interaction, and outcome factors were obtained for each of the two promotive models and used to estimate the required regression coefficients. Simple slope plots for the interactions are presented in Figure 5. The left plot in Figure 5 displays the simple slopes for the relation between externalizing behaviors and positive parenting for difficult (β = 0.208, p < .001), average (β = –0.088, p < .001), and easy (β = 0.032, p = .258) temperaments, respectively. Simple slopes for the relation between internalizing behavior and positive parenting are plotted on the right side of Figure 5 for unadaptable (β = –0.114, p < .001), average (β = –0.062, p = .002), and adaptable (β = –0.010, p = .721) temperaments, respectively. Consistent with the results for the interaction terms in each of the promotive SEM models, the simple slopes become substantially more negative as temperamental difficultness and unadaptability increases.

Figure 5. Simple slopes for the relationship between positive parenting and externalizing behavior for children with easy, average, and difficult temperament and between positive parenting and internalizing behavior for children with adaptable, average, and unadaptable temperament (dashed areas represent region of significance on X). Scores on positive parenting are standard deviations.

As part of these post hoc interaction analyses, regions of significance (RoS on X) were obtained for each of the promotive models to help determine whether the differential susceptibility or diathesis–stress models provides a better account for these data (Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012). The RoS on X test identifies the lower and upper bounds of values on positive parenting for which the regression of temperament and the outcome variable reaches statistical significance. For externalizing behaviors, these lower and upper bounds were 1.93 and 3.97, respectively, indicating that the heights of the regression lines were significantly different for all possible cases where the score of positive parenting was either lower than 1.93 or higher than 3.97 (where the actual range of the standardized latent positive parenting scores was –5.13 to 1.44). Hence, children with difficult temperament showed more externalizing problems when positive parenting scores were lower than 1.93, with the upper bound value 3.95 being way above the observed range of positive parenting, thereby providing substantial empirical evidence in support of the diathesis–stress model. For internalizing behaviors, the lower and upper bounds for the RoS on X were 0.32 and 4.89, respectively, indicating that the regression lines were significantly different for all possible cases where the score of positive parenting was either lower than 0.32 or higher than 4.89. Hence, children with unadaptable temperament showed more internalizing problems when positive parenting scores were lower than 0.32, with the upper bound value 4.89 being way above the observed range of positive parenting, thereby also providing substantial empirical evidence in support of the diathesis–stress model. The shaded areas in Figure 5 represents the region of significance within two standard deviations of the mean positive parenting scores for externalizing (left plot) and internalizing behaviors (right plot), respectively.

In addition to the RoS on X testing, the proportion of interaction (PoI) metric was used to quantify the proportion of the interaction uniquely attributable to differential susceptibility/diathesis stress, with values of 0.50 corresponding to differential susceptibility and values close to zero being reflective of diathesis stress (Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012). The PoI value was close to zero (PoI = 0.02) for externalizing behaviors, indicating that worse outcomes for children with difficult temperament far outweighed better outcomes, again providing evidence for diathesis stress. Similar results were obtained for internalizing behaviors, where the PoI value also approached zero (PoI = 0.07), indicating that, for children with unadaptable temperament, the negative effects on internalizing behaviors resulting from the lack of positive parenting prevailed over the positive impact on behavior due to the influence of positive parenting, once again providing a strong evidence for diathesis stress.

Discussion

Research on child development has increasingly focused on the ways in which individual attributes serve to moderate the effects of environmental exposure in shaping human development. Much of this work has been directed toward understanding the mechanisms through which temperament acts in combination with factors such as the quality of parenting to influence children's outcomes. A number of theoretical frameworks put forward to elucidate the dynamics underlying the complex interplay between nature and nurture have consequently been employed by developmental researchers as a means to classify such interactive effects as distinct categories of phenomena (Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2012). For instance, diathesis–stress/dual-risk views emphasize the synergistic effects that child-intrinsic vulnerability factors and adverse rearing experiences have with respect to increasing negative consequences for individuals who are most sensitive to the detrimental effects of low-quality environments. Nonetheless, such pathology-based accounts have recently been extended to incorporate a differential susceptibility view (Belsky, Reference Belsky1997; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009, Reference Belsky and Pluess2010), which proposes that vulnerability/plasticity factors not only predispose individuals to an increased risk of negative outcomes in the presence of adverse environments but also render them more susceptible to the beneficial effects of supportive experiences. Based on these premises, individuals are set to vary in their developmental plasticity due to innate temperamental dispositions that ultimately impact their levels of susceptibility to both positive and negative qualities of the environment. More recently, a reinterpretation of available research evidence has offered new insights based on the notion of vantage sensitivity, which represents the ability of individuals to prosper given the presence of specific endogenous characteristics if exposed to development-enhancing environmental conditions (Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2012). On this latter view, the presence of these specific sensitivity traits constitute an advantage with respect to the development (or production) of favorable outcomes in the presence of ecological contexts that provide adequate social resources and ambient support.

In the present study, Parenting × Temperament interactions were investigated within a number of SEMs containing various combinations of risk and promotive factors in order to clarify the distinct contribution such interactions make in determining children's behavior. With respect to the joint effects of temperament and parenting, the results of the two risk models examined here indicate that neither difficult nor unadaptable children exposed to hostile parenting in early childhood seem to be at a particular increased risk for the development of behavior problems at later age in comparison to easier or more adaptable children, respectively. That is, although direct influences of hostile parenting on both externalizing and internalizing behaviors were observed, the nature of the effects of hostile parenting on children's behavior problems were not conditional upon either of these two temperamental dimensions.

In contrast, the results of the two promotive models examined here indicate that temperamentally difficult/unadaptable children respond more negatively to the effects of unresponsive and uninvolved parenting (i.e., a lack of positive parenting) than do children without such temperamental characteristics. That is, in line with diathesis–stress models, the behavior of difficult/unadaptable children with respect to both externalizing and internalizing behavioral outcomes is worsened from having mothers who are less sensitive and responsive to their needs. However, it is also important to note that high levels of positive parenting resulted in all children achieving about the same levels of behavioral adjustment. Hence, although the pattern of interaction results are consistent with that expected according to diathesis stress, the fact that the presence of positive environmental influences serves to attenuate behavioral differences between temperamentally difficult/unadaptable children and those without such temperamental characteristics makes these latter results seem more in line with the notion that positive parenting represents an adjustment promoting factor rather than a risk factor. Moreover, note that because difficult and unadaptable children generally manifest either the same or worse behavioral problems rather than the same or better distinguishes this interaction effect from that expected according to the notion of vantage sensitivity.

In sum, the present work does provide evidence that the types of rearing strategies employed by parents of temperamentally difficult/unadaptable children when dealing with their children's demands can affect their future development. As shown here, a fundamental dimension for understanding the maladaptive response of such children is the extent to which their parents endorse rearing practices that are unresponsive to and disengaged from their offspring during the first years of life. Generally, parents have the power to nurture and instill positive qualities (i.e., self-esteem and self-efficacy) in the young child who strives to master ongoing challenges and integrate personal experiences into its own sense of “autonomous-relational self” (Kagitçibasi, Reference Kagitçibasi1996). Parental guidance is essential in order to enhance children's problem-solving skills and encourage the self-control that is necessary to promote normal development (Landry, Smith, & Swank, Reference Landry, Smith and Swank2006). On this view, it is within such appropriate rearing environments that temperamentally difficult/unadaptable children can develop adequate coping skills and manifest positive developmental outcomes that are on par with temperamentally easy/adaptable children.

An additional aspect of the models examined in this study is the fact that they included a number of other key contextually relevant risk and promotive factors. Accordingly, the bioecological system approach (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner1998, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner2006) was used as a framework to shed light on the multiple co-occurring facets of the social and physical environments that affect the development of children. With respect to the effects of those other factors, one interesting finding concerns the fact that a predominant mechanism through which poor neighborhood quality exerts its influence on children's future behavioral outcomes is indirectly by way of its impact on maternal depression. More specifically, consistent with previous research, neighborhood problems significantly contribute to higher levels of maternal depression, independently of the effects of the covariates controlled for in these analyses (Kim, Reference Kim2010; Ross, Reference Ross2000; Ross & Mirowsky, Reference Ross and Mirowsky2001). Higher levels of maternal depression, in turn, directly contributed to increases in both externalizing and internalizing behavior (Lyons-Ruth et al., Reference Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks and Cibelli1997). Furthermore, depressed mothers were significantly more likely to report higher levels of hostile parenting, corroborating prior research, which has demonstrated that depressed mothers tend to interact more negatively with their children (Bugental, Reference Bugental, Sigel, McGillicuddy-DeLisi and Goodnow1992; Feng et al., Reference Feng, Shaw, Skuban and Lane2007; Hasting & Grusec, Reference Hasting and Grusec1998).

In contrast with previous research, however, no direct effect of either neighborhood problems on hostile parenting practices (Earls et al., Reference Earls, McGuire and Shay1994; Pinderhughes et al., Reference Pinderhughes, Nix, Foster and Jones2001) or maternal depression on positive parenting practices (Jacob & Johnson, Reference Jacob and Johnson1997; Lovejoy et al., Reference Lovejoy, Graczyk, O'Hare and Neuman2000) was evident in this study. In contrast with previous research as well (e.g., Moren-Cross, 2006), neighborhood problems did not exert any direct effect on the future manifestation of externalizing behaviors. However, it was found that neighborhood problems directly contributed to the future development of internalizing behavior problems in this sample of children. Thus, these results provide some evidence for the unique predictive value of neighborhood problems with respect to the later emergence of internalizing behaviors. Such a result is consistent with the findings from recent research conducted by Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, and Earls (Reference Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn and Earls2005) on a community sample of 5-year-olds living in the Chicago area, which showed that living in disadvantaged areas might increase the likelihood of internalizing problems becoming manifested in children.

In addition to providing insight on the negative effects of poor neighborhood quality, the present results serve to highlight the role that both neighborhood cohesion and social support have to play with respect to contributing to enhancing positive parenting practices (and, hence, the lives of children; Cochran & Brassard, Reference Cochran and Brassard1979; Powell, Reference Powell1980). One explanation for such effects would likely be that greater social support and higher levels of cohesion in the neighborhood can help to foster self-confidence and reinforce positive attributions and behavior (Perkins, Hughey, & Speer, Reference Perkins, Hughey and Speer2002; Thompson, Flood, & Goodwin, Reference Thompson, Flood, Goodwin, Cicchetti and Cohen2006). However, in contrast with prior findings, higher levels of neighborhood cohesion were not associated with significant decreases in maternal depression (i.e., after accounting for the effects of the other covariates). Conversely, social support seemed to be a particularly relevant and contributing factor to decreasing maternal depression. It could be argued that self-perceived social support should serve to alleviate symptoms of depression by reducing stress and isolation in individuals. Moreover, the presence of a strong social support network would also provide parents with opportunities for discussions regarding how to handle children that misbehave, thus helping them in their efforts to engage in positive interactions with their children (Furstenberg, Reference Furstenberg and Wilson1993). Such findings are consistent with prior studies that have indicated that characteristics of the social environment (i.e., social capital) are likely to exert an impact upon children's behavior through their influence on proximal processes at the family level (Dorsey & Forehand, Reference Dorsey and Forehand2003).

Several limitations of this study should be noted because they provide directions for future research. For example, the data employed relied almost exclusively on maternal reports, which then increased the likelihood of shared method invariance bias. Such bias can lead to erroneous conclusions by overestimating the correlation between true scores due to correlated errors attributable to participants' response sets (Campbell & Fiske, Reference Campbell and Fiske1959; Fiske, Reference Fiske, Brinberg and Kidder1982). However, the use of SEM does serve to minimize this phenomenon by specifically allowing for correlations among the error terms (Cole, Reference Cole2006). Furthermore, any sharing of variance due to such bias is much more likely to affect the validity of main effects than it would interaction effects (especially after such effects have explicitly been residualized on the main effects). However, future researchers may want to replicate this study with different measurement methods and multiple informants for all of the constructs.

Another inevitable issue with longitudinal survey data is attrition. Although the sample examined here was relatively large, it may be somewhat unrepresentative due to attrition. Although the use of normalized longitudinal weights serves to attenuate the effects of attrition, it is important to acknowledge that the manner in which the loss of participants due to attrition may have affected the results. In order to be inclusive, researchers should strive to preserve data from more disadvantaged families by either increasing response rates for such individuals or using high-risk population surveys.

Moreover, the dynamics underlying the effects of various proximal and distal factors on children's behavior might also depend on other characteristics such as culture and ethnicity. For instance, the mechanisms through which neighborhood disorder, maternal depression, and parenting influence the behavior of children can vary across different ethnic–cultural groups (Ho, Bluestein, & Jenkins, Reference Ho, Bluestein and Jenkins2008; Pachter, Auinger, Palmer, & Weitzman, Reference Pachter, Auinger, Palmer and Weitzman2006). Given that Canada represents a multicultural society, culturally relevant characteristics related to minority populations require special consideration. The NLSCY sample was selected based on demographic information provided by the Labor Force Survey, which targets 98% of the Canadian population, not including inmates of institutions, full-time members of the Canadian armed forces, children who lived in institutions for more than 6 months, and aboriginal children living on reserve. Although convenient and economic, such sampling procedures could lead to selection bias, thus contributing to underrepresentation of minority groups. Future research should therefore attempt to address these issues and employ surveys that include minority children as target populations.

Moreover, other important components of the child's ecological system, such as the school and peer group, should be given some consideration. Nowadays, young children are spending more time in childcare centers, increasing their exposure to the influence of other socializing forces outside the family. For instance, positive interactions with teachers and other caretakers can assume a positive role in promoting children's feelings of self-worth and belonging that are essential to their healthy development (Birch & Ladd, Reference Birch and Ladd1998; Gillian, Reference Gillian1998; Pianta, Reference Pianta1999). This situation invites consideration of how childhood professionals might either support or interfere with children's socialization processes and socioemotional development. For these reasons, future researchers might want to include such educational settings within the conceptual and operational framework proposed here (for some recent work in this regard, see Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2010).

Finally, it is important to consider the possibility of a conceptual overlap between the temperament and problem behavior variables. Although these two constructs might have been operationalized in a slightly different fashion (i.e., in terms of normative behavior in the case of temperament and maladaptive responding in the case of problem behavior), the extent to which they might represent measures of the same trait is an open issue. Future research might attempt to derive refined measures of such traits by identifying confounded items and using factor analysis to determine the unique variance associated with each factor (Lemery, Essex, & Smider, Reference Lemery, Essex and Smider2002).

Nonetheless, one of the major strengths of the current research is that it is one of the few large-scale longitudinal studies to examine the simultaneous relationships of both proximal and distal factors with the purpose of elucidating the dynamics underlying the successful adaptation of children with difficult/unadaptable temperament. Analysis of such data sets with the statistical techniques used here has the potential to improve the breadth and depth of the knowledge about children's behavioral problems and its precursors. Such findings should be useful to researchers and mental health professionals engaged in the assessment of both the risk and promotive factors associated with the emergence of mental problems in vulnerable populations and, hence, aid them in the rebalancing of a child's environment.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Revised Child Behavior Checklist and the Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant–mother attachment and social development: Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In Richards, M. P. M. (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 99135). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Allison, P. D. (2002). Missing data series: Quantitative applications in the social sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. (1961). Patterns and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Attar, B. K., Guerra, N. G., & Tolan, P. H. (1994). Neighborhood disadvantage, stressful life events, and adjustment in urban elementary-school children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 391400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, J. E. (2000). Adjustment style in childhood as a product of parenting and temperament. In Wachs, T. D. & Kohnstamm, G. A. (Eds.), Temperament in context (pp. 173200). Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Bates, J. E., Bayles, K., Bennet, D. V., Ridge, B., & Brown, M. M. (1991). Origins of externalizing behavior problems at eight years of age. In Pepler, D. J. & Rubin, K. H. (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 93122). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. B., & Lounsboury, M. L. (1979). Measures of infant difficultness. Child Development, 50, 794803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayer, J. K., Hiscock, H., Obioha, C., Ukoumunne, O. C., Price, A., & Wake, M. (2008). Early childhood aetiology of mental health problems: A longitudinal population-based study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 11661174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belle, D. (1990). Poverty and women's mental health. American Psychologist, 45, 385389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (1994). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 8396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to environmental influence: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F. (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 139159). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Hsieh, J. K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys' externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2010). Review article: Children's differential susceptibility to effects of parenting. Family Science, 1, 1425.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2012). Differential susceptibility to long-term effects of quality of child care on externalizing behavior in adolescence? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36, 210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fix index in structural equation models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beyers, J. M., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2003). Neighborhood structure, parenting processes, and the development of youths' externalizing behaviors: A multilevel analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 3553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Children's interpersonal behaviors and the teacher–child relationship. Developmental Psychology, 34, 934946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2008). Infant temperament, parenting, and externalizing behavior in first grade: A test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 124131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Lerner, R. M. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 9931028). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 793828). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, G., Bhrolchain, M., & Harris, T. (1975). Social-class and psychiatric disturbance among women in an urban population. Sociology, 9, 225254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1989). Single sample cross-validation indices for covariance structures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 24, 445455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bugental, D. B. (1992). Affective and cognitive processes within threat-oriented family systems. In Sigel, I. E., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V., & Goodnow, J. J. (Eds.), Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children (2nd ed., pp. 219248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bureau, J., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2009). Maternal depressive symptoms in infancy: Unique contribution to children's depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence? Development and Psychopathology, 21, 519537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Byrne, B. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (1998). Maternal depressive disorder and contextual risk: Contribution to the development of attachment insecurity and behavior problems in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 283300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 300314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cochran, M., & Brassard, J. (1979). Child development and personal social networks. Child Development, 50, 601616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, D. A. (2006). Coping with longitudinal data in research on developmental psychopathology. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 2025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colletta, N. (1979). Support systems after divorce: Incidence and impact. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 837846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, W. A., Macobby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case of nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (1999). Depressed parents and family functioning: Interpersonal effects and children's functioning and development. In Joiner, T. E. & Coyne, J. C. (Eds.), The interactional nature of depression: Advances in interpersonal approaches (pp. 299327). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1987). The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. In Jones, W. H. & Perlman, D. (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 1, pp. 3767). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Cutrona, C. E., & Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social support, infant temperament, and parenting self-efficacy: A mediational model to postpartum depression. Child Development, 57, 15071518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, N. M. (2006). Depression in children and adolescents. Journal of School Nursing, 21, 311317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dix, T., & Meunier, L. N. (2009). Depressive symptoms and parenting competence: An analysis of 13 regulatory processes. Developmental Review, 29, 4568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorsey, S., & Forehand, R. (2003). The relation of social capital to child psychosocial adjustment difficulties: The role of positive parenting and neighborhood dangerousness. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 25, 1123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Earls, F., McGuire, J., & Shay, S. (1994). Evaluating community intervention to reduce risk of child abuse: Methodological strategies in conducting neighborhood surveys. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 473485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Murphy, B. C., Guthrie, I. K., Jones, S., et al. (1997). Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of children's social functioning from regulation and emotionality. Child Development, 68, 642664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Feng, X., Shaw, D. S., Skuban, E. M., & Lane, T. (2007). Emotional exchange in mother–child dyads: Stability, mutual influence, and associations with maternal depression and child problem behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 714725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, D. W. (1982). Convergent and discriminant validation in measurements and research strategies. In Brinberg, D. & Kidder, L. (Eds.), New directions for methodology of social and behavioral science: Forms of validity in research (No. 12, pp. 7792). San Francisco, CA: Jossey–Bass.Google Scholar
Furstenberg, F. F. Jr. (1993). How families manage risk and opportunity in dangerous neighborhoods. In Wilson, W. J. (Ed.), Sociology and the public agenda (pp. 231258). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallager, K.C. (2002). Does child temperament moderate the influence of parenting on adjustment? Developmental Review, 22, 623643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geldhof, G. J., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., & Little, T. D. (2013). Orthogonalizing through residual centering: Extended applications and caveats. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 73, 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillian, R. (1998). The importance of schools and teachers in child welfare. Child and Family Social Work, 3, 1325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilissen, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & van der Veer, R. (2008). Parent–child relationship, temperament, and physiological reactions to fear-inducing film clips: Further evidence for differential susceptibility. Journal of Child Experimental Psychology, 99, 182195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilissen, R., Koolstra, C. M., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van der Veer, R. (2007). Physiological reactions of preschoolers to fear-inducing film clips: Effects of temperamental fearfulness and quality of the parent–child relationship. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 187195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review, 106, 458490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A.W., & Thomas, C.W. (1997). Difficult temperament and behavior problems: A longitudinal study from 1.5 to 12 years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 7190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunthert, K. C., Conner, T. S., Armeli, S., Tennen, H., Covault, J., & Kranzler, H. R. (2007). Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety reactivity in daily life: A daily process approach to gene–environment interaction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 762768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasting, P. D., & Grusec, J. E. (1998). Parenting goals as organizers of responses to parent–child disengagement. Developmental Psychology, 34, 465479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, C., Bluestein, D. N., & Jenkins, J. M. (2008). Cultural differences in the relationship between parenting and children's behavior. Developmental Psychology, 44, 507522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, T., & Johnson, S. L. (1997). Parent–child interaction among depressed fathers and mothers: Impact on child functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 391409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jöreskog, K., & Yang, F. (1996). Nonlinear structural equation models: The Kenny–Judd model with interaction effects. In Marcoulides, G. A. & Schumacker, R. E. (Eds.), Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 5788). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1986). LISREL VI: Analysis of linear structural relationships by maximum likelihood, instrumental variables, and least squares methods. Uppsala: University of Uppsala, Department of Statistics.Google Scholar
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2001). LISREL 8: User's reference guide (2nd ed.). Lincolnwood, IL: SSI Scientific Software International.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. J., Reznick, S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 22122225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagitçibasi, C. (1996). The autonomous-relational self. European Psychologist, 1, 180186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keiley, M. K., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Effects of temperament on the development of externalizing and internalizing behaviors over 9 years. In Columbus, F. (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 6, pp. 256288). Huntington, NY: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Kim, J. (2010). Neighborhood disadvantage and mental health: The role of neighborhood disorder and social relationships. Social Science and Research, 39, 260271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (2002). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 191195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. T. (2000). Mother–child mutually responsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 71, 417431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohen, D. E., Leventhal, T., Dahinten, V. S., & McIntosh, C. N. (2008). Neighborhood disadvantage: Pathways of effects for young children. Child Development, 79, 156169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotchick, B. A., & Forehand, R. (2002). Putting parenting in perspective: A discussion of the contextual factors that shape parenting practices. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 11, 255269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kristman, V., Manno, M., & Cote, P. (2004) Loss to follow-up in cohort studies: How much is too much? European Journal of Epidemiology, 19, 751760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: Establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 42, 627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leavy, R. C. (1983). Social support and psychological disorder: A review. Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 321.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C. L., & Bates, J. E. (1985). Mother–child interaction at age two years and perceived difficult temperament. Child Development, 56, 13141325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (1991). Adjustment of children of depressed mothers: A 10 month follow-up. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 473477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Revealing the relation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding: Expert ratings and factor analyses. Child Development, 73, 867882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leve, L. D., Kim, H. K., & Pears, K. C. (2005). Childhood temperament and family environment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing trajectories from age 5 to 17. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 505520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O'Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 561592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1998). Disorganized infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile–aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom. Child Development, 64, 572585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Cibelli, C. D. (1997). Infant attachments strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems at age 7. Developmental Psychology, 33, 681692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manassis, K., & Bradley, S. J. (1994). The development of childhood anxiety disorders: Toward an integrated model. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 345366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Wen, Z., & Hau, K.-T. (2004). Structural equation models of latent interactions: Evaluation of alternative estimation strategies and indicator construction. Psychological Methods, 9, 275300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., Wen, Z., & Hau, K.-T. (2006). Structural equation models of latent interactions and quadratic effects. In Hancock, G. H. & Mueller, R. O. (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 225265). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Wen, Z., Hau, K.-T., Little, T. D., Bovaird, J. A., & Widaman, K. F. (2007). Unconstrained structural equation models of latent interactions: Contrasting residual- and mean-centered approaches. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 570580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCain, M., & Mustard, F. (1999). Reversing the real brain drain: Early years study, final report. Toronto: Canadian Institute for Advanced Research.Google Scholar
McGuire, J. B. (1997). The reliability and validity of a questionnaire describing neighborhood characteristics relevant to families and young children living in urban areas. Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 551566.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, J. D., & Edwards, K. (1995). Contextual determinants of children's responses to poverty. Social Forces, 73, 14871516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesman, J., Stoel, R., Kranenburg-Bakermans, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., Koot, M. H., et al. (2009). Predicting growth curves of early childhood externalizing problems: Differential susceptibility of children with difficult temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 625636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michaud, S. (2001). The National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth—Overview and changes after three cycles. Canadian Studies in Population, 28, 391405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller-Lewis, L. R., Baghurst, P. A., Sawyer, M. G., Prior, M. R., Clark, J. J., Arney, F. M., et al. (2006). Early childhood externalizing behavior problems: Child, parenting, and family-related predictors over time. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 34, 891906.Google Scholar
Monroe, S. M., & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis–stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 406425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moren-Cross, J. L., Wright, D. R., LaGory, M., & Lanzi, R. G. (2006). Perceived neighborhood characteristics and problem behavior among disadvantaged children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 36, 273294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pachter, L. M., Auinger, P., Palmer, R., & Weitzman, M. (2006). Do parenting and the home environment, maternal depression, neighborhood, and chronic poverty affect child behavioral problems differently in different racial-ethnic groups? Pediatrics, 117, 13291338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.Google Scholar
Perkins, D. D., Hughey, J., & Speer, P. W. (2002). Community psychology perspectives on social capital theory and community development practice. Journal of the Community Development Society, 33, 3352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. E. (1997). Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children's adjustment: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 68, 908923.Google Scholar
Pianta, R. (1999). Enhancing relationships between children and teachers. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinderhughes, E. E., Nix, R., Foster, E. M., & Jones, D. (2001). Parenting in context: Impact of neighborhood poverty, residential stability, public services, social networks and danger on parental behaviors. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 941953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care. Developmental Psychology, 46, 379390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2012). Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 901916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, D. R. (1980). Personal social networks as a focus for primary prevention of child maltreatment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 1, 232239.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, T. G., & Parke, R. D. (1984). Social network factors and transition to parenthood. Sex Roles, 10, 949972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapee, R. M., & Coplan, R. J. (2010). Conceptual relations between anxiety disorder and fearful temperament. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 127, 1731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roche, K. M., Ghazarian, S. R., Little, T. D., & Leventhal, T. (2011). Understanding links between punitive parenting and adolescent adjustment: The relevance of context and reciprocal associations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roisman, G. I., Newman, D. A., Fraley, R. C., Haltigan, J. D., Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2012). Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: Recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 389409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, C. E. (2000). Neighborhood disadvantage and adult depression. Journal of Mental Health and Social Behavior, 41, 177187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2001). Neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 258276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rottenberg, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2004). Socioemotional functioning in depression. In Power, M. (Ed.), Mood disorders: A handbook of science and practice (pp. 6177). Hoboken, NJ; Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial behavior by young people. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Developmental system and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 297312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections between temperament and social development: A review. Social Development, 13, 142170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saris, W. E., Batista-Foguet, J. M., & Coenders, G. (2007). Selection of indicators for the interaction term in structural equation models with interaction. Quality & Quantity, 41, 5572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In von Eye, A. & Clogg, C. C. (Eds.), Latent variable analysis: Applications to developmental research (pp. 399419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Savalei, V. (2010). Small sample statistics for incomplete nonnormal data: Extensions of complete data formulae and a Monte Carlo comparison. Structural Equation Modeling, 17, 241264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. Monographs on statistics and applied probability. London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Owens, E. B., Giovannelli, J., & Winslow, E. B. (2001). Infant and toddler pathways leading to early externalizing disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 3643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (1996). National longitudinal survey of children and youth: User's handbook and micro data guide. Ottawa, ON: Author.Google Scholar
Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980). Statistically based tests for numbers of common factors. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
Stringaris, A., Maughan, B., & Goodman, R. (2010). What's in a disruptive disorder? Temperamental antecedents of oppositional defiant disorder: Findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 474483.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A., Flood, M. F., & Goodwin, R. (2006). Social support and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 137). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Stolk, M. N., Alink, L. R. A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., et al. (2007). Differential susceptibility to discipline: The moderating effect of child temperament on the association between maternal discipline and early childhood externalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 626636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Hippel, P. T. (2009). How to impute interactions and squares and other transformed variables. Sociological Methodology, 39, 265291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, E. F., & Diforio, D. (1997). Schizophrenia: A neural diathesis–stress model. Psychological Review, 104, 667685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weatherston, D., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2010). Role of parenting in the development of the infant's interpersonal abilities. In Tyano, S., Keren, M., Herrman, H., & Cox, J. (Eds.), Parenthood and mental health: A bridge between infant and adult psychiatry (pp. 181194). Chichester: Wiley–Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissman, M. M., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., Moreau, D., & Olfson, M. (1997). Offspring of depressed parents: 10 years later. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 932940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Nomura, Y., Warner, V., Pilowsky, D., & Verdeli, H. (2006). Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 10011008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xue, Y., Leventhal, T., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Earls, J. F. (2005). Neighborhood residence and mental health problems of 5- to 11-year-olds. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 554563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, Q., Wang, Y., Deng, X., Eisenberg, N., Wolchik, S. A., & Tein, J.-T. (2008). Relations of parenting and temperament to Chinese children's experience of negative life events, coping efficacy, and externalizing problems. Child Development, 79, 493513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (1999). Vulnerability to psychopathology: A biosocial model. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Structural equation modeling results for the first risk model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Structural equation modeling results for the second risk model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Structural equation modeling results for the first promotive model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. ***p < .001.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Structural equation modeling results for the second promotive model. All path coefficients have been standardized and dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. *p < .05. ***p < .001.

Figure 4

Table 1. Demographic information

Figure 5

Table 2. Descriptive statistics (after imputation) and amount of missing data for all of the study variables in the final sample (N = 2,631)

Figure 6

Table 3. Overall fit of the measurement models

Figure 7

Table 4. Matrix of correlations between all of the latent factors

Figure 8

Figure 5. Simple slopes for the relationship between positive parenting and externalizing behavior for children with easy, average, and difficult temperament and between positive parenting and internalizing behavior for children with adaptable, average, and unadaptable temperament (dashed areas represent region of significance on X). Scores on positive parenting are standard deviations.