Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:25:47.139Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural substrates of trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability: Mechanisms of heterotypic comorbidity between externalizing disorders and unipolar depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2016

Aimee Zisner
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
Theodore P. Beauchaine*
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Theodore P. Beauchaine, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 225 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210; E-mail: beauchaine.1@osu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Trait impulsivity, which is often defined as a strong preference for immediate over delayed rewards and results in behaviors that are socially inappropriate, maladaptive, and short-sighted, is a predisposing vulnerability to all externalizing spectrum disorders. In contrast, anhedonia is characterized by chronically low motivation and reduced capacity to experience pleasure, and is common to depressive disorders. Although externalizing and depressive disorders have virtually nonoverlapping diagnostic criteria in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, heterotypic comorbidity between them is common. Here, we review common neural substrates of trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability, which include both low tonic mesolimbic dopamine activity and low phasic mesolimbic dopamine responding to incentives during reward anticipation and associative learning. We also consider how other neural networks, including bottom-up emotion generation systems and top-down emotion regulation systems, interact with mesolimbic dysfunction to result in alternative manifestations of psychiatric illness. Finally, we present a model that emphasizes a translational, transdiagnostic approach to understanding externalizing/depression comorbidity. This model should refine ways in which internalizing and externalizing disorders are studied, classified, and treated.

Type
Special Issue Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

In child and adolescent psychopathology research, most psychiatric disorders are classified as either internalizing syndromes, including depressive and anxiety disorders, or externalizing syndromes, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), disruptive behavior disorders, and substance use disorders (SUDs). The internalizing/externalizing distinction follows from a long tradition of empirically based taxonomy in developmental psychopathology (see Beauchaine & Klein, Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press). In such research, factor analysis consistently parses behavioral symptoms of psychopathology into broadband internalizing and externalizing dimensions (e.g., Achenbach, Reference Achenbach, Kreutzer, DeLuca and Caplan2011; Achenbach, & Edelbrock, Reference Achenbach and Edelbrock1983).

More recent structural analyses of symptoms expressed among both twin samples and population-based samples of adults confirm the internalizing/externalizing distinction. These analyses invariably yield a hierarchical latent structure of psychopathology in which (a) higher order internalizing and externalizing factors account for much of the covariation among specific lower order syndromes (i.e., disorders), and (b) higher order internalizing and externalizing factors are themselves correlated (e.g., Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Aggen, Knudsen, Røysamb, Neale and Reichborn-Kjeennerud2011; Krueger, Reference Krueger1999; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Hicks, Patrick, Carlson, Iacono and McGue2002; Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman, & Rahouz, Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Tuvblad, Zheng, Raine, & Baker, Reference Tuvblad, Zheng, Raine and Baker2009; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Krueger, Hobbs, Markon, Eaton and Slade2013). This pattern emerges regardless of the age of participants.

Population-level covariation among syndromes within the internalizing and externalizing spectra suggests that comorbidity of disorders at the individual level should be common (see, e.g., Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, Reference Angold, Costello and Erkanli1999; Beauchaine, Hinshaw & Pang, Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Klein & Riso, Reference Klein, Riso and Costello1993). Homotypic comorbidity refers to cases in which an individual experiences two or more internalizing disorders, or two or more externalizing disorders. Given results from factor analyses, and given substantial overlap in criterion sets, homotypic comorbidity has proved to be the rule rather than the exception in child, adolescent, and adult psychopathology research, both in the United States and abroad (see, e.g., Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello and Erkanli1999; Ferdinand, Dieleman, Ormel, & Verhulst, Reference Ferdinand, Dieleman, Ormel and Verhulst2007; Gau et al., Reference Gau, Ni, Shang, Soong, Wu and Lin2010; Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McGonagle, Zhao, Nelson, Hughes and Eshleman1994; Maughan, Rowe, Messer, Goodman, & Meltzer, Reference Maughan, Rowe, Messer, Goodman and Meltzer2004).

In contrast to homotypic comorbidity, heterotypic comorbidity refers to cases in which an individual experiences at least one internalizing disorder and at least one externalizing disorder (Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello and Erkanli1999). Even though internalizing and externalizing disorders comprise largely nonoverlapping criterion sets, heterotypic comorbidity is exceedingly common (e.g., Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, Reference Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler and Angold2003; Gilliom & Shaw, Reference Gilliom and Shaw2004; Keiley, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, Reference Keiley, Bates, Dodge and Pettit2000; Marmorstein & Iacono, Reference Marmorstein and Iacono2003). For example, up to half of preschool and school-age children with ADHD experience a comorbid mood disorder (Wilens et al., Reference Wilens, Biederman, Brown, Tanguay, Monuteaux and Blake2002), and externalizing disorders in childhood predict depression in adulthood (Loth, Drabick, Leibenluft, & Hulvershorn, Reference Loth, Drabick, Leibenluft and Hulvershorn2014). Furthermore, worsening externalizing problems across time predict worsening internalizing problems, particularly among children with high baseline levels of psychopathology (Gilliom & Shaw, Reference Gilliom and Shaw2004; Keiley et al., Reference Keiley, Bates, Dodge and Pettit2000). However, heterotypic comorbidity is not a transient phenomenon of childhood, and is also observed in adolescent, young adult, and adult samples (see Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Ball, Monuteaux, Mick, Spencer and McCreary2008; Chan, Dennis, & Funk, Reference Chan, Dennis and Funk2008; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005; Krueger, McGue, & Iacono, Reference Krueger, McGue and Iacono2001).

Artifactual, Spurious, and True Comorbidity

From a validity standpoint, sources of comorbidity can be divided into three overarching categories (see, e.g., Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello and Erkanli1999; First, Reference First2005; Klein & Riso, Reference Klein, Riso and Costello1993; Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld2003). These include artifactual comorbidity, which occurs when one disease entity is mistakenly split into multiple diagnoses; spurious comorbidity, which occurs when distinct disease entities are assigned shared diagnostic criteria; and true comorbidity, which occurs when an individual suffers from separate disease entities. To date, most research on comorbidity has focused either on tabulating symptoms to determine whether rates of diagnostic co-occurrence exceed chance levels (see Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello and Erkanli1999; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McGonagle, Zhao, Nelson, Hughes and Eshleman1994, Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005), or on evaluating patterns of symptoms among large population-based and twin samples using advanced statistical modeling (e.g., Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006; Tackett, Waldman, Van Hulle, & Lahey, Reference Tackett, Waldman, Van Hulle and Lahey2011).

Although these approaches yield important descriptive information about diagnostic co-occurrence and about heritabilities of comorbidity, they provide no information about neurobiological mechanisms. This is a significant limitation because etiology-based diagnosis is often a precondition for determining whether apparent comorbidity is artifactual, spurious, or true (see Beauchaine & Marsh, Reference Beauchaine, Marsh, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; First, Reference First2005; Jensen, Reference Jensen2003; Preskorn & Baker, Reference Preskorn and Baker2002). Lacking specification of etiology, we must infer psychopathology solely from symptoms, which are often nonspecific, insensitive markers of disease state (see Beauchaine, Lenzenweger, & Waller, Reference Beauchaine, Lenzenweger and Waller2008; Meehl, Reference Meehl1995).

Advances in behavioral genetics, molecular genetics, and neuroimaging have led to significant progress in identifying etiological underpinnings of many mental disorders, and have produced key insights into possible mechanisms of both homotypic and heterotypic comorbidity (see, e.g., Baskin-Sommers & Foti, Reference Baskin-Sommers and Foti2015; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Rhee, Lahey, & Waldman, Reference Rhee, Lahey and Waldman2015). Such efforts lend increasing support to a neuroscience-informed, trait-based approach to characterizing psychopathology in which patterns of functional interaction among a limited number of brain systems give rise to internalizing and externalizing vulnerability (see, e.g., Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). For example, high trait impulsivity, which has well characterized neural substrates, confers vulnerability to all externalizing syndromes, including the hyperactive/impulsive and combined presentations of ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder (CD), SUDs, and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD; see Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Neuhaus, & Beauchaine, Reference Neuhaus, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press). Trait impulsivity is therefore a primary vulnerability to homotypic comorbidity among externalizing disorders (Beauchaine, Zisner, & Sauder, in press).

Objectives

Other behavioral traits are transdiagnostic vulnerabilities to both internalizing and externalizing syndromes (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015b; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). For example, emotion dysregulation, often defined as the inability to modulate affective states in the service of adaptive behavior, is observed across a wide range of psychiatric disorders, including major depressive disorder (MDD; e.g., Ehring, Tuschen-Caffier, Schnülle, Fischer, & Gross, Reference Ehring, Tuschen-Caffier, Schnülle, Fischer and Gross2010), externalizing spectrum disorders (e.g., Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007), and anxiety disorders (e.g., Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, & Fresco, Reference Mennin, Heimberg, Turk and Fresco2005), to name but a few. We and others have summarized the role of emotion dysregulation in conferring transdiagnostic vulnerability to psychopathology in recent reviews (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). The purpose of this paper is to describe how mesolimbic dopamine (DA) dysfunction confers transdiagnostic vulnerability to both externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression.Footnote 1 We synthesize accumulating research on common neural substrates of trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability, and present several interpretations of relations among these tendencies. As we demonstrate in sections to follow, considerable evidence now supports the following set of conjectures:

  1. 1. Trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability (hallmarks of externalizing spectrum disorders and depression) derive from chronically low tonic mesolimbic DA activity and blunted phasic DA responding during anticipation of incentives.

  2. 2. A primary route to heterotypic comorbidity is through this transdiagnostic neural vulnerability in incentive processing.

  3. 3. Hypofunctioning of the mesolimbic DA system affects approach motivation, producing a chronically aversive mood state characterized by low positive affectivity and irritability, which are common to both externalizing disorders and unipolar depression.

  4. 4. At the neural level of analysis, externalizing disorders and unipolar depression are distinguished from one another by individual differences in activity and reactivity of other neural systems.

In sections to follow, we contend that the neural basis of heterotypic comorbidity between externalizing spectrum disorders,Footnote 2 including the hyperactive/impulsive and combined presentations of ADHDFootnote 3 and unipolar depression, is shared neural vulnerability in the mesolimbic DA system. We content further that behavioral differences between these disorders arise from impairments in other neural circuits that interact with the mesolimbic DA system to affect behavior. One such circuit is the septohippocampal system, including the amygdala, a region that is hyperresponsive to negatively valenced stimuli among those with depression. We argue that deficient lateral prefrontal regulation of the amygdala portends risk for depression, whereas deficient orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal regulation of mesolimbic reward circuitryFootnote 4 portends risk for externalizing spectrum disorders. Both of these regulatory mechanisms may be disrupted within an individual, giving rise to heterotypic comorbidity. We recognize that other central DA systems and other neurotransmitter systems affect mood state, and interact functionally with the mesolimbic DA system to confer vulnerability or resilience to psychopathology, including both homotypic and heterotypic comorbidity (e.g., Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016). We and others have considered such relations elsewhere (e.g., Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Zalewski, Crowell, & Potapova, Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Zalewski, Crowell and Potapova2011; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015; Sauder, Beauchaine, Gatze-Kapp, Shanon, & Aylward, Reference Sauder, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Shannon and Aylward2012). Our primary objectives in writing this paper are to describe the role of mesolimbic DA circuitry in conferring vulnerability to trait impulsivity and anhedonia, and to a chronically aversive mood state characterized irritability, which is common to both externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression.

Key Terms and Concepts

The term temperament refers to emotional and behavioral predispositions that are expressed very early in life and arise from heritable individual differences in brain structure and function (see, e.g., Kagan, Reference Kagan, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press; Rothbart, & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Damon and Eisenberg1998). According to prominent developmental models, temperamental predispositions interact with environmental influences throughout childhood and adolescence to eventuate in adult personality (e.g., De Pauw & Mervielde, Reference De Pauw and Mervielde2010; Goldsmith, Lemery, Aksan, & Buss, Reference Goldsmith, Lemery, Aksan, Buss, Molfese and Molfese2000; Rothbart, Reference Rothbart2007). Adult personality traits are “relatively enduring patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that distinguish individuals from one another” (Roberts & Mroczek, Reference Roberts and Mroczek2008, p. 31; see McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa2003). Temperament and personality both reflect quantifiable, continuously distributed individual differences in the population (see Haslam, Holland, & Kuppens, Reference Haslam, Holland and Kuppens2012; Nettle, Reference Nettle2006). Extreme levels of certain temperament and personality traits (e.g., impulsivity, inhibition, and neuroticism) can confer vulnerability to psychopathology (see Beauchaine & Neuhaus, Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press; Kagan, Reference Kagan, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press), especially when coupled with deficiencies in emotion regulation (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). Extreme variation in temperament and personality usually does not reflect qualitative differences between affected individuals and others in the general population (e.g., Clark, Reference Clark2005; Livesley & Jang, Reference Livesley and Jang2005; Miller, Lyman, Widiger, & Leukefeld, Reference Miller, Lyman, Widiger and Leukefeld2001). Because temperament and personality traits are distributed along continua, setting boundaries for what is considered to be clinically elevated symptom expression can be challenging and is somewhat arbitrary.

Trait impulsivity

Conceptualizations of the neurobiological basis of impulsivity have evolved over time (see Neuhaus & Beauchaine, Reference Neuhaus, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press). Hippocrates viewed impulsive tendencies as the product of excessive yellow bile (Titchener, Reference Titchener1921). In the 18th century, Franz Joseph Gall argued based on phrenology that the absence of the mental faculty of “cautiousness” resulted in impulsivity (Spurzheim, Reference Spurzheim1834). In the 19th century, John Harlow provided a detailed account of perhaps the most famous head injury of all time: that suffered by Phineas P. Gage (Harlow, Reference Harlow1848). His observations of Gage were consistent with assumptions of brain–behavior relationships that were popular at the time. In particular, specific regions of the frontal cortex were believed to underlie precautious motivations, with damage to these regions resulting in disinhibition. Many also assumed that behavior was the outcome of competing mental forces. Among healthy individuals, more primitive urges were effectively constrained by inhibitory control mechanisms. Thus, damage to brain regions responsible for inhibiting impulses created disequilibrium, so “animal propensities” prevailed (Harlow, 1948).

The notion that adaptive behavior derives from a relative equilibrium between self-gratifying and restrained motivations pervades major theories of impulsivity. For example, Eppinger and Hess (1910/1915) asserted that vagotonia, a term used to describe an imbalance in the autonomic nervous system favoring the parasympathetic over the sympathetic division, was used to explain a range psychological and medical phenomena, including neurasthenia, hysteria, nervousness, and impulsive tendencies.

Several more recent theories of impulsivity posit that individual differences in approach behavior derive from activity and reactivity of mesolimbic structures. For example, in his prominent neurobiological theory of personality, Gray (Reference Gray1987a, Reference Gray1987b) proposed a mesolimbic DA behavioral approach system as the neural substrate of appetitive motivation. Subsequently, DA theories of approach motivation were used to explain impulsive, reward-seeking behaviors often observed in ADHD, CD, and other externalizing disorders (e.g., Fowles, Reference Fowles1988; Quay, Reference Quay1993). Although these early theories correctly attributed impulsivity to mesolimbic neural dysfunction, they incorrectly assumed that excessive DA activity predisposed affected individuals to impulsive behavior. As described in detail here and elsewhere, subsequent research reveals that externalizing spectrum disorders are characterized by low rather than high mesolimbic DA activity and reactivity (see Beauchaine & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine and Gatzke-Kopp2012; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015).

Contemporary accounts of impulsivity operationalize the construct in a number of ways. In its most general sense, impulsivity refers to deficiencies in self-control over behavior (see, e.g., Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). The extent to which one exhibits impulsive tendencies varies continuously across the population, and normal variation in this trait is not maladaptive. Rather, normal variation in trait impulsivity is reflected in core aspects of personality, including novelty seeking, sensation seeking, extraversion, and exuberance, all of which may foster adaptive functioning, including creative and flexible thinking (see, e.g., Degnan et al., Reference Degnan, Hane, Henderson, Moas, Reeb-Sutherland and Fox2011; Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase, & Russell, Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005).

In contrast, excessive trait impulsivity, the focus of this paper, confers vulnerability to clinically significant psychopathology. As noted above, high trait impulsivity is expressed as a preference for immediate, smaller rewards over larger, delayed rewards, and often results in behaviors that are socially inappropriate, maladaptive, and short-sighted (see Neuhaus & Beauchaine, Reference Neuhaus, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). As we discuss in detail below, such behaviors reflect an overarching problem with effectively anticipating rewards and optimizing goal-directed behavior (Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005).

In discussing trait impulsivity, it is important to note that others conceptualize the construct without a clinical aspect in mind. For example, Whiteside and Lynam (Reference Whiteside and Lynam2001) attribute four etiologically distinct personality facets, including urgency, lack of planning, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking to impulsive behavior, and Patton, Stanford, and Barratt (Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995) distinguish between motor impulsiveness (actions without forethought), nonplanning impulsiveness (overemphasis on the present), and attentional impulsiveness (difficulty maintaining focus on stimuli). In addition, impulsivity is sometimes operationalized as behavioral performance on circumscribed tasks, such as go/no-go (continuous performance), stop-signal, choice serial reaction time, delay-discounting, and cost/benefit decision-making tasks (e.g., Bezdjian, Baker, Lozano, & Raine, Reference Bezdjian, Baker, Lozano and Raine2009; Bickel, Odum, & Madden, Reference Bickel, Odum and Madden1999; Dimoska & Johnstone, Reference Dimoska and Johnstone2007). Go/no-go, stop-signal, and choice serial reaction time tasks may best capture inattentiveness and impulsive action (i.e., premature responding and/or failure to inhibit responses), whereas other behavioral tasks, such as delay-discounting and cost/benefit decision-making tasks, may best capture impulsive choice. However, common self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity capture only partially overlapping, and in some cases more distinct, aspects of impulsivity (Reynolds, Ortengren, Richards, & de Wit, Reference Reynolds, Ortengren, Richards and de Wit2006; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Eagle, Economidou, Theobald, Mar and Murphy2009).

Given heterogeneity in definitions of and approaches to measuring impulsivity, a well-supported case for a particular operational definition is crucial. We prefer to operationalize impulsivity for human participants using ADHD hyperactive–impulsive scale scores over other measures for three primary reasons: (a) hyperactive–impulsive symptoms of ADHD are highly heritable (e.g., Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Perlis, Doyle, Smoller, Goralnick and Holmgren2005), far more so than most alternative measures of impulsivity (73%–85% of the variance in ADHD symptoms is explained by heritability; Nikolas & Burt, Reference Nikolas and Burt2010; Willcutt, Reference Willcutt and Barchin press); (b) trait impulsivity among those with ADHD is associated with well-replicated physiological and neural substrates (e.g., Beauchaine, Katkin, Strassberg, & Snarr, Reference Beauchaine, Katkin, Strassberg and Snarr2001; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Sylvers, Mead and Chipman-Chacon2006; Hart, Radua, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, Reference Hart, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2012; Plichta & Scheres, Reference Plichta and Scheres2014); and (c) ADHD predicts functional outcomes including academic and occupational underachievement, and confers vulnerability to other externalizing spectrum disorders across development (see Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010; Neuhaus & Beauchaine, 2013; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015).

Anhedonia

Anhedonia, a term that derives from ancient Greek (ἀν- an-, “without” + ἡδονή hēdonē, “pleasure”), was formally coined by Théodule-Armand Ribot to describe an inability to experience pleasure (Ribot, Reference Ribot1896). Even before the term emerged, diminished response to pleasure was considered to be a significant if not essential presenting feature of depression (Bevan, Reference Bevan1899; Bucknill & Tuke, Reference Bucknill and Tuke1874; Clouston, Reference Clouston1896), and it was incorporated into early influential theories of schizophrenia (Bleuler, Reference Bleuler1911; Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1919). Interest in the role of anhedonia in psychopathology dwindled in the first half of the 20th century; but it was revitalized in the 1960s by the psychoanalyst Sandor Rado, who posited that anhedonia was one of two key symptoms that marked genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. Rado (Reference Rado1956, Reference Rado1962) suggested that anhedonia could be transmitted genetically, and that it interfered with normal living among individuals with schizophrenia, and among their family members who did not experience psychotic features. Paul Meehl (Reference Meehl1962, Reference Meehl1973) expanded on this work by suggesting that low hedonic capacity is a characteristic of and heritable trait vulnerability to both depression and schizophrenia.

Anhedonia became a defining feature of mood disorders following Donald Klein's conceptualization of endogenous depression. He asserted that for some, depression is a biologically derived pathology, with onset of anhedonia being the first observable sign of a depressive episode (Klein, Reference Klein1974). Klein's work contributed to inclusion of anhedonia as a criterion for major depressive disorder, beginning with DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980).

Klein (Reference Klein1974), DSM-III (APA, 1980), and many others describe anhedonia as diminished interest or capacity to experience pleasure in response to stimuli that are ordinarily rewarding. However, “diminished interest” implies that hedonic capacity must change from high to low over some interval of time (e.g., during a major depressive episode). In contrast, we and others assert that for some, anhedonia is both traitlike and state dependent. We therefore define trait anhedonia as a relatively stable personality attribute, characterized by chronically low motivation, and inability to experience pleasure (e.g., Loas, Reference Loas1996; McCabe, Reference McCabe and Ritsner2014; Shankman, Nelson, Harrow, & Faull, Reference Shankman, Nelson, Harrow and Faull2010). However, we recognize that anhedonia captures partly dissociable processes that may be compromised differentially. These include anticipatory versus consummatory responses to reward, social versus physical (sensory) anhedonia, differences between interest/motivation to seek rewards versus pleasure derived from rewards, and differential responding to distal versus proximal reinforcers (see Shankman et al., Reference Shankman, Katz, DeLizza, Sarapas, Gorka, Campbell and Ritsner2014). As elaborated in later sections, the aspect of trait anhedonia that is of greatest interest here is diminished positive affect in anticipation of rewards, a deficit related directly to reduced interest in and motivation to seek pleasurable stimuli.

Like trait impulsivity, anhedonia is operationalized in a number of ways. Common measures include the original and revised Chapman Physical and Social Anhedonia Scales (Chapman, Chapman, & Raulin, Reference Chapman, Chapman and Raulin1976), the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (Snaith et al., Reference Snaith, Hamilton, Morley, Humayan, Hargreaves and Trigwell1995), and the Fawcett–Clark Pleasure Scale (Fawcett, Clark, Scheftner, & Gibbons, Reference Fawcett, Clark, Scheftner and Gibbons1983), which provide some convergent psychometric properties (Leventhal, Chasson, Tapia, Miller, & Pettit, Reference Leventhal, Chasson, Tapia, Miller and Pettit2006). Anticipatory and consummatory aspects of anhedonia are distinguished from one another using the Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (Gard, Gard, Kring, & John, Reference Gard, Gard, Kring and John2006). Examples of common laboratory-based indices of anhedonia include self-report, psychophysiological, and hemodynamic responses to tasting sucrose and chocolate (e.g., Berlin, Givry-Steiner, Lecrubier, & Puech, Reference Berlin, Givry-Steiner, Lecrubier and Puech1998; Chentsova-Dutton & Hanley, Reference Chentsova-Dutton and Hanley2010), and to hearing pleasant musical stimuli (e.g., Keller et al., Reference Keller, Young, Kelley, Prater, Levitin and Menon2013), as well as behavioral responses, such as developing a response bias toward more frequently rewarding stimuli (Pizzagalli, Jahn, & O'Shea, Reference Pizzagalli, Jahn and O'Shea2005).

As a general rule across self-report and laboratory-based measures, high anhedonia scores capture low positive affectivity, a collection of emotions related to low levels of joy, energy, excitement, and confidence (Watson, Reference Watson, Snyder and Lopez2002). This contrasts with the personality characteristic of negativity affectivity, which manifests as a general tendency to experience distress, nervousness, and hostility, which may or may not co-occur with low positive affectivity (Watson & Tellegen, Reference Watson and Tellegen1985).Footnote 5 Here, we operationalize trait anhedonia as deficits in incentive processing prior to the experience of reward (i.e., low interest and motivation to expend energy to receive rewards; diminished anticipatory responses to rewards). However, we recognize that the term anhedonia conventionally encapsulates deficiencies relating to both obtaining and directly experiencing rewarding stimuli, and that one or both of these functions can be affected within individuals.

DA neural circuitry

DA system organization

DA is a monoamine neurotransmitter that is implicated in neural processing of motivation, incentive salience, learning, and movement, among other functions. DA projections are traditionally divided into four primary neural systems, including the mesolimbic, mesocortical, nigrostriatal, and tuberoinfundibular pathways, which ascend from the midbrain or hypothalamus (Beaulieu & Gainetdinov, Reference Beaulieu and Gainetdinov2011; Björklund & Dunnett, Reference Björklund and Dunnett2007; Taber, Black, Porrino, & Hurley, Reference Taber, Black, Porrino and Hurley2012). Although these pathways are far more interconnected, both structurally and functionally, than originally thought, it is still useful to subdivide DA neural circuits for heuristic purposes (Björklund & Dunnett, Reference Björklund and Dunnett2007; Tisch, Silberstein, Limousin-Dowsey, & Jahanshahi, Reference Tisch, Silberstein, Limousin-Dowsey and Jahanshahi2004; Wise, Reference Wise2009).

The two DA projections that are of greatest relevance to trait impulsivity and anhedonia are the mesolimbic and mesocortical pathways, both of which contain neurons that ascend from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the midbrain. In the mesolimbic pathway, the medial forebrain bundle connects the VTA to the ventral striatum (VS), including the nucleus accumbens (NAcc; both the core and shell) and ventral regions of the caudate nucleus and putamen (Voorn, Vanderschuren, Groenewegen, Robbins, & Pennartz, Reference Voorn, Vanderschuren, Groenewegen, Robbins and Pennartz2004). This pathway is associated most strongly with motivation, incentive salience, and impulsivity, and is implicated in the pathogenesis of all externalizing spectrum disorders, including ADHD, ODD, CD, substance abuse disorders, and ASPD (see Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010, in press; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Beauchaine, Neuhaus, et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Gatzke-Kopp, Reid, Brekke and Olliges2016; Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007; Shannon, Sauder, Beauchaine, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Shannon, Sauder, Beauchaine and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). The mesocortical pathway includes DA projections from the VTA to cortical regions, including the prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and cingulate cortices. This pathway, which is associated with error monitoring, executive functions, evaluating incentive magnitude, and maintenance of goal-directed behaviors, exerts increasing top-down control over the mesolimbic DA system as the frontal cortices mature across development (see, e.g., Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., in press; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Casey, Reference Casey2015; Macdonald, Goines, Novacek, & Walker, Reference Macdonald, Goines, Novacek and Walker2016 [this issue]). Due to extensive feedforward and feedback connections between the mesolimbic and mesocortical pathways, they are sometimes referenced jointly as the mesocorticolimbic pathway. However, we prefer to retain a conceptual distinction between the two pathways. This is because top-down inhibitory control of mesolimbic circuitry by cortical regions differs depending on the region and strength of functional connectivity between cortical and subcortical sites (Lodge, Reference Lodge2011). The complex relationship between the mesolimbic and mesocortical pathways suggests that it may be best to regard the pathways as influenced by one another instead of as a single dopaminergic pathway. Pharmacologically induced elevations in prefrontal DA levels decrease DA activity in mesolimbic structures, whereas decreases in prefrontal DA increase DA activity in mesolimbic structures (Louilot, LeMoal, & Simon, Reference Louilot, LeMoal and Simon1989). Disruption in this feedback/feedforward system, as evidenced by altered functional connectivity, may be one neural substrate of impulsivity (e.g., Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Sauder, Beauchaine and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Tisch et al., Reference Tisch, Silberstein, Limousin-Dowsey and Jahanshahi2004).

The third DA pathway, the nigrostriatal pathway, projects from the substantia nigra pars compacta to the dorsal striatum, including dorsal portions of the putamen and caudate. This pathway is most commonly associated with movement, including motoric dysfunction (e.g., Parkinson disease). However, it also plays a role in habit formation and addiction (Everitt & Robbins, Reference Everitt and Robbins2013). Finally, the tuberoinfundibular pathway contains DA projections from the hypothalamus to the pituitary gland.

DA receptors

DA binds to G-coupled protein receptors that are present in the central nervous system and other locations throughout the body. DA receptor subtypes are grouped as either D1-like (comprising D1 and D5 receptors) or D2-like (comprising D2, D3, and D4 receptors) and are differentiated on the basis of structural, biochemical, and pharmacological properties (Beaulieu & Gainetdinov, Reference Beaulieu and Gainetdinov2011). Receptor activation and subsequently elicited responses are functions of a number of factors, including variation in receptor genes, differences in receptor densities between different brain regions and across individuals, and whether receptors are located on presynaptic or postsynaptic neurons (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Rubinstein, Phillips, Lessov, Burkhart-Kasch and Zhang1998; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Fowler, Logan, Gatley and Gifford1999; see Beaulieu & Gainetdinov, Reference Beaulieu and Gainetdinov2011). Depending on DA levels that derive from opposing functions and differing ligand affinities of different receptors, DA and its agonists can elicit opposite behavioral effects (e.g., stimulating vs. inhibiting locomotor activity, heart rate, and pain sensitivity; Calabrese, Reference Calabrese2001). Responses to DA agonism can be biphasic, such that low doses induce different or opposing effects compared to high doses (e.g., Strömbom, Reference Strömbom1976).

DA and reward processing

Reward processing encompasses both endogenous and exogenous responses to reinforcers, including neural responding, and how reward contingencies are used to predict, assess, and guide goal-directed behavior. The role of the mesolimbic DA system in reward processing was first studied in the mid-1950s by Olds and Milner, who demonstrated that rats implanted with stimulating electrodes in regions of this system, including the VTA and striatum, habitually self-stimulate, sometimes to the point of death (Olds & Milner, Reference Olds and Milner1954; see Milner, Reference Milner1991). Later animal studies, some of which we describe below, expanded this work, and identified midbrain DA function as fundamental in processing incentives, and in guiding future, goal-directed behaviors. These findings and others that followed provide the basis for conceptualizing the mesolimbic DA system as the brain's primary reward circuit and provide a compelling neurobiological explanation for the relationship between reward and motivation.

Incentive processing includes both anticipatory and consummatory phases of reward responding. During reward anticipation, an organism is motivated to approach potential reinforcers. During reward consummation, the organism experiences some degree of pleasure/satisfaction from receiving reinforcement.Footnote 6 To maximize access to reinforcers, organisms must learn to anticipate them accurately based on potentially dynamic and ambiguous reward contingencies, a process known as reward learning. The distinction between reward anticipation and receipt has received considerable support from animal and neuroimaging work (discussed below; see Berridge & Kringelbach, Reference Berridge and Kringelbach2015; Berridge & Robinson, Reference Berridge and Robinson1998; O'Doherty, Reference O'Doherty2004), and emerges in multiples lines of research. For example, separating reward anticipation from receipt parallels the concepts of “wanting” (motivation to pursue rewards) versus “liking” (satisfaction from receiving rewards) described by Robinson and Berridge in their highly influential incentive salience theory of addiction (Reference Robinson and Berridge1993, Reference Robinson and Berridge2008; Berridge & Kringelbach, Reference Berridge and Kringelbach2015). According to incentive salience theory, wanting (anticipating) is mediated by the NAcc core among other regions, whereas liking is mediated by neural processes in the NAcc shell (e.g., Berridge & Kringelbach, Reference Berridge and Kringelbach2015; Berridge & Robinson, Reference Berridge and Robinson2003; Saddoris, Cacciapaglia, Wightman, & Carelli, Reference Saddoris, Cacciapaglia, Wightman and Carelli2015).

The computational task of accurately anticipating reinforcers is accomplished in part through appropriate tonic (low-amplitude, baseline) and phasic (high-amplitude, event-related) activity of midbrain DA neurons (Schultz, Reference Schultz1998). For example, unexpected rewards elicit burst firing of DA neurons that project to the NAcc in the VS (Tremblay, Hollerman, & Schultz, Reference Tremblay, Hollerman and Schultz1998). The magnitude and time course of this spike in DA provides a temporal window for an organism to learn which behaviors increase the likelihood of receiving rewards, resulting in reinforcement of these behaviors (i.e., motivated responses; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Once the organism can predict, based on cues, the probability and magnitude of an impending reward, phasic DA reactivity shifts forward from occurring during consummation of the reinforcer to the temporal interval between cue and reward (Bressan & Crippa, Reference Bressan and Crippa2005). However, phasic DA responding continues to occur during receipt of rewards that are unexpected, or are larger in magnitude than expected. In contrast, phasic DA responding dampens following receipt of rewards that are worse than expected, or expected but absent (Bressan & Crippa, Reference Bressan and Crippa2005; Schultz, Reference Schultz1998). During reward anticipation, midbrain DA neurons encode relative value of expected rewards (magnitude of the reward weighted by probability and delay), such that large, immediate, and highly probable gains elicit the most robust neural firing (Kobayashi & Schultz, Reference Kobayashi and Schultz2008; Tobler, Fiorillo, & Schultz, Reference Tobler, Fiorillo and Schultz2005). The VS, which receives direct input from midbrain DA neurons, and from other structures implicated in reward processing, including the amygdala, insula, and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), also indexes the relative value of predicted rewards (Knutson, Adams, Fong, & Hommer, Reference Knutson, Adams, Fong and Hommer2001), whereas reduced activation signals reward devaluation (Gottfried, O'Doherty, & Dolan, Reference Gottfried, O'Doherty and Dolan2003). Thus, DA-mediated activity of the VS and other structures is integral to associative learning, and associative learning is reflected in the ability to anticipate future rewards.

Phasic changes in mesolimbic neural responding have been assessed directly in animals (e.g., Tremblay et al., Reference Tremblay, Hollerman and Schultz1998) and indirectly through functional neuroimaging with humans. One type of functional magnetic resonance imaging task commonly used with humans is the monetary incentive delay task, adapted from tasks used in primate research to study reward processing (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Adams, Fong and Hommer2001; Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997). Parameters of the monetary incentive delay task vary across studies, but the basic framework is as follows: for each trial, participants are presented with an incentive cue that signals the possibility of winning money, avoiding losing money, or no change during that trial. Participants are then challenged to respond to a target quickly, and their performance determines the monetary outcome for that trial. This paradigm is advantageous because it separates reward anticipation from reward receipt, and has been used widely to investigate neural correlates of reward processing in clinical and nonclinical samples (e.g., Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, May, Siegle, Ladouceur, Ryan and Carter2006; Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas, & Gotlib, Reference Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas and Gotlib2008; Lutz & Widmer, Reference Lutz and Widmer2014; Sauder, Derbidge, & Beauchaine, Reference Sauder, Derbidge and Beauchaine2016).

Although phasic DA responding has received considerable attention in research on reward processing, tonic levels of DA in the mesolimbic system also play a role in evaluating and responding to incentives (Goto, Otani, & Grace, Reference Goto, Otani and Grace2007; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Tonic levels of DA, as assessed through DA concentrations in extrasynaptic space, are normally insufficient to stimulate postsynaptic DA receptors (Grace, Reference Grace, Solanto, Arnsten and Castellanos2001). However, tonic DA modulates phasic DA activity (Bressan & Crippa, Reference Bressan and Crippa2005). For example, tonic DA can bind to DA terminal autoreceptors and other receptor sites in the extrasynaptic space, inhibiting phasic release of DA (Grace & Bunney, Reference Grace, Bunney, Borroni and Kupfer1995). Through such processes, excessively high or low tonic DA can compromise reward learning by altering the relative strength of phasic signals to reinforcers (see Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005; Schultz, Reference Schultz1998).

As alluded to above, incentive processing is related closely to motivation, defined as the degree to which an organism will exert energy in goal-directed behaviors. Like reward processing, motivation derives in part from tonic and phasic DA in the VS, and communication among structures in the mesolimbic DA circuit, including the VS (see Niv, Reference Niv2007). For example, chronically elevated extracellular DA is associated with enhanced motivation (Cagniard, Balsam, Brunner, & Zhuang, Reference Cagniard, Balsam, Brunner and Zhuang2006), whereas DA depletion and DA receptor blockage in the VS are associated with reduced motivation (Salamone, Cousins, & Bucher, Reference Salamone, Cousins and Bucher1994). Thus, intact ability to neurally encode the relative value of rewards and anticipate future rewards is important for stimulating motivated behaviors in order to obtain rewards that are “worth” the effort.

Several key takeaways from research on incentive processing are worth noting before we move forward in our discussion: (a) effective reward processing is contingent on tonic levels of DA, and on phasic modulation of DA in mesolimbic structures; (b) reward anticipation and reward consummation are dissociable, and reward anticipation may be compromised in organisms that possess intact consummatory responses; and (c) the ability to appraise reward value and anticipate future rewards influences motivation to engage in goal-directed behaviors.

Neural Responding to Incentives and Trait Impulsivity

Contrary to some early accounts, which erroneously attributed pathologically impulsive behavior to excessive mesolimbic DA activity and reactivity (for a review, see Brenner, Beauchaine, & Sylvers, Reference Brenner, Beauchaine and Sylvers2005), research conducted in the past 10–15 years identifies chronically low tonic DA and diminished DA reactivity to incentives in the VS as important neural substrates of clinically expressed impulsivity (e.g., Scheres, Milham, Knutson, & Castellanos, Reference Scheres, Milham, Knutson and Castellanos2007; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Newcorn, Fowler, Telang and Solanto2007; see also Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011; Gatzke-Kopp, & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007; Neuhaus & Beauchaine, 2013; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). Low phasic DA responding is associated with impaired reward processing, which manifests in a number of ways, including compromised ability to differentially respond to rewards based on their frequency and magnitude (Luman, Van Meel, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, & Geurts, Reference Luman, Van Meel, Oosterlaan, Sergeant and Geurts2009), failure to establish associative learning contingencies (Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005), and prolonged extinguishing of previously rewarded behaviors (Gatzke-Kopp et al., Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Shannon, Chipman-Chacon, Fleming and Crowell2009). As described below, impaired associative learning caused by a hypofunctional mesolimbic DA system is a critical factor underlying poor impulse control.

Animal studies

Converging evidence from comparative studies suggests that impulsive behavior is associated with deficiencies in mesolimbic DA function (see Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Selective lesioning of the NAcc core, a DA-rich area of the VS, increases impulsive choice in rats (Cardinal, Pennicott, Lakmali, Robbins, & Everitt, Reference Cardinal, Pennicott, Lakmali, Robbins and Everitt2001). Similarly, overexpression of the DA transporter in the NAcc, which results in reduced levels of extracellular DA in this region, is associated with both increased impulsive choice (greater preference for immediate, smaller rewards over larger, delayed rewards), and risk proneness (greater preference for large, uncertain rewards over small but likely rewards; Adriani et al., Reference Adriani, Boyer, Gioiosa, Macri, Dreyer and Laviola2009).

The role of DA neurocircuitry in impulsivity is especially evident in animal studies that select and compare groups based on (a) differences in inherent VS responding and (b) baseline differences in impulsive behaviors. Both impulsive choice and impulsive action (failure to inhibit behavior when it is advantageous to do so) are associated with abnormally low DA release in the VS in response to electrical stimulation (Diergaarde et al., Reference Diergaarde, Pattij, Poortvliet, Hogenboom, de Vries and Schoffelmeer2008). Among laboratory animals that are highly impulsive, as assessed by poor behavioral inhibition on the stop-signal task, DA agonism shortens reaction time to inhibit action and thereby improves impulse control (Eagle, Tufft, Goodchild, & Robbins, Reference Eagle, Tufft, Goodchild and Robbins2007; Feola, de Wit, & Richards, Reference Feola, de Wit and Richards2000). In contrast, among animals with fast reaction times at baseline (high impulse control), studies by Eagle et al. and Feola et al. demonstrate that DA agonism either exhibits no effect on impulse control or lengthens reaction time.

Impulsive animals also exhibit an increased proclivity to engage in drug use. For example, inherently low D2/3 receptor availability in the NAcc is associated prospectively with higher rates of cocaine self-administration (Dalley et al., Reference Dalley, Fryer, Brichard, Robinson, Theobald and Lääne2007). Combined, such findings suggest low tonic and phasic DA release are associated with impulse control problems.

It is important to note that relations between DA and impulsivity may be obscured entirely when inherent individual differences in mesolimbic function are not considered, because these individual differences modulate effects of DA-acting drugs. Studies that do not consider individual differences in baseline impulsivity overwhelmingly associate DA agonism with increases in impulsive action, which is in direct contrast with well-replicated findings linking low DA to impulsivity among highly impulsive animals (see D'Amour-Horvat & Leyron, Reference D'Amour-Horvat and Leyton2014). In addition to individual differences in DA neurocircuitry function, other factors relating to dosing and drug administration schedules should be considered when conducting and interpreting studies concerning pharmacological manipulation of DA in animals.Footnote 7

It is important to note that there is still uncertainty regarding the precise nature and causes of DA deficiency associated with trait impulsivity. A number of possible mechanisms exist, including abnormalities attributed to DA release, density, and function of the DA transporter and/or DA receptors, and functional connectivity between regions among and outside central DA systems, as well as effects of particular stimulus conditions. In all likelihood, more than one such factor drives behavioral effects, and factors underlying impulsive tendencies may differ among individuals (see Beauchaine et al., in press; Dalley, Mar, Economidou, & Robbins, Reference Dalley, Mar, Economidou and Robbins2008). Molecular genetic accounts suggest that trait impulsivity and its various behavioral manifestations across development are inherited multifactorially. Because many genes are involved, which interact with one another and with environment to affect behavior, no single genetic locus is necessary or sufficient to confer clinically expressed impulsivity (e.g., Gizer, Otto, & Ellingson, Reference Gizer, Otto, Ellingson, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016).

Neuroimaging research with humans

Findings from neuroimaging studies among humans with ADHD and other externalizing spectrum disorders who exhibit clinically significant impulsivity parallel and in some cases clarify findings from animal studies. Single photon emission computed tomography, positron emission tomography (PET), and magnetic resonanc imaging studies implicate compromised mesolimbic and/or mesocortical DA function in ADHD (e.g., Ludolph et al., Reference Ludolph, Kassubek, Schmeck, Glaser, Wunderlich and Buck2008; Vles, Feron, & Hendrikson, Reference Vles, Feron and Hendriksen2003; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Newcorn, Fowler, Telang and Solanto2007, Reference Volkow, Wang, Kollins, Wigal, Newcorn and Telang2009), CD (e.g., Rubia, Smith, et al., Reference Rubia, Smith, Halari, Matukura, Mohammad and Taylor2009), SUDs (e.g., Martin-Soelch et al., Reference Martin-Soelch, Leenders, Chevalley, Missimer, Kunig and Magyar2001; Volkow, Fowler, & Wang, Reference Volkow, Fowler and Wang2004), and antisocial traits (e.g., Oberlin et al., Reference Oberlin, Dzemidzic, Bragulat, Lehigh, Talavage and O'Connor2012). Children with ADHD exhibit smaller VS volumes, which correspond with severity of hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (Carmona et al., Reference Carmona, Proal, Hoekzema, Gispert, Picado and Moreno2009; Sauder et al., Reference Sauder, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Shannon and Aylward2012). Individuals with ADHD also demonstrate reduced striatal responding to smaller, immediate rewards and larger, delayed rewards in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (e.g., Plichta et al., Reference Plichta, Vasic, Wolf, Lesch, Brummer and Jacob2009).Footnote 8 Furthermore, those with ADHD (e.g., Carmona et al., Reference Carmona, Hoekzema, Ramos-Quiroga, Richarte, Canals and Bosch2012; Furukawa et al., Reference Furukawa, Bado, Tripp, Mattos, Wickens and Bramati2014; Hoogman et al., Reference Hoogman, Aarts, Zwiers, Slaats-Willemse, Naber and Onnink2011; Scheres et al., Reference Scheres, Milham, Knutson and Castellanos2007; Ströhle et al., Reference Ströhle, Stoy, Wrase, Schwarzer, Schlagenhauf and Huss2008), early-onset criminal offending (Cohn et al., Reference Cohn, Veltman, Pape, van Lith, Vermeiren and van den Brink2015), and SUDs (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Schlagenhauf, Wustenberg, Hein, Kienast and Kahnt2009; Wrase et al., Reference Wrase, Schlagenhauf, Kienast, Wüstenberg, Bermpohl and Kahnt2007) all exhibit VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation and/or reward outcome compared with controls. These findings are consistent with hypofunctionality of DA circuitry observed in impulsive animals.

No clear evidence has emerged from animal research to describe the relationship between phasic DA responses to immediate rewards and impulsivity (Luman, Tripp, & Scheres, Reference Luman, Tripp and Scheres2010), and competing theories suggest that such responses are suppressed (e.g., Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005) or unaffected (e.g., Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008). In some neuroimaging research, ADHD is associated with similar or perhaps even greater VS responses to reward outcomes (as opposed to reward anticipation), compared with controls (Furukawa et al., Reference Furukawa, Bado, Tripp, Mattos, Wickens and Bramati2014; Scheres et al., Reference Scheres, Milham, Knutson and Castellanos2007; Wilbertz et al., Reference Wilbertz, van Elst, Delgado, Maier, Feige and Philipsen2012). This is consistent with theories that link timing and duration over magnitude of DA responding to ADHD (see also Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005).

Some of the strongest neuroimaging evidence for deficient mesolimbic responding as a neural substrate of impulsivity derives from studies that assess neurobiological consequences of DA agonists among those with ADHD. DA agonists, including methylphenidate, increase extracellular DA levels in the striatal pathway (e.g., Volkow, Fowler, Wang, Ding, & Gatley, Reference Volkow, Fowler, Wang, Ding and Gatley2002). Among both children and adults with ADHD, methylphenidate normalizes (a) attenuated striatal responses to reward (Vles et al., Reference Vles, Feron and Hendriksen2003; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Fowler, Wang, Ding and Gatley2002), (b) frontocingulate underreactivity during error processing (Rubia, Halari, Mohammad, Taylor, & Brammer, Reference Rubia, Halari, Mohammad, Taylor and Brammer2011), and (c) functional connectivity deficits between mesolimbic and mesocortical brain regions (Rubia, Halari, Cubillo, Mohammad, & Taylor, Reference Rubia, Halari, Cubillo, Mohammad and Taylor2009). Thus, DA agonists, which increase availability of DA in the striatum, decrease impulsive behaviors by increasing DA availability to levels experienced by individuals without these symptoms (e.g., Hinshaw, Henker, Whalen, Erhardt, & Dunnington, Reference Hinshaw, Henker, Whalen, Erhardt and Dunnington1989; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999; see Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013).

Translational models of externalizing behavior

As described above, the strength of a given reinforcer to motivate future behaviors is dependent on several factors, including the magnitude and predictability of the reinforcer and the interval between rewarded behavior and receipt of the reinforcer (see Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). These factors are coded by phasic neural firing in the VS and interconnected brain regions, which signal relative values of expected reinforcers. Associative learning is affected adversely when tonic DA firing is suppressed (Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008). Under such conditions, an organism is less able to predict and assign value to reinforcers, because the temporal window within which reward learning can occur is narrowed. As a result, larger and more frequent incentives are required to form behavior–reward associations, and to predict future reward outcomes (Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). It should be noted that more frequent trials of nonreward may also be required for extinction of learned behavior–reward associations.

It follows from patterns of neural activity and reactivity described above that impulsivity emerges as a preference for immediate over delayed incentives given a general failure to associate behaviors with rewards that are temporally distant from one another (see Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Thus, immediate but smaller rewards are appealing compared to larger delayed rewards, because immediate choices are more easily associated with reward and therefore reinforced. Impulsive action may reflect slower learning of which behaviors to select in order to progress toward one's goals (i.e., maximize reward yields). For example, the go/no-go task requires the organism to respond to presentation of “go” cues and withhold responses following “no-go” cues. Failure to inhibit “no-go” responses may occur when responses for “go” cues are sufficiently reinforced, but extinction processes for erroneous, ineffective, or insufficient behavior are impaired. This leads to less differential responding between cue types. Consequently, an organism may exhibit generalized responding to cues regardless of type, and fail to withhold responses appropriately (see Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). This is consistent with neuroimaging research demonstrating a failure to migrate neural activity from mesolimbic structures to the anterior cingulate when previously rewarded behaviors are extinguished among boys with ADHD and/or CD (Gatzke-Kopp et al., Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Shannon, Chipman-Chacon, Fleming and Crowell2009).

Contemporary ontogenic process models assert that heritable trait impulsivity is a transdiagnostic vulnerability to all externalizing spectrum disorders (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010, in press; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Beauchaine, Shader, & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine, Shader, Hinshaw, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016). As noted above, children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD, which often begins a developmental trajectory to more severe externalizing conduct, exhibit diminished DA reactivity to incentives (see Plichta & Scheres, Reference Plichta and Scheres2014; Rubia, Reference Rubia2011). This hypofunctionality may emerge at least in part through low tonic DA (e.g., Vles et al., Reference Vles, Feron and Hendriksen2003; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Newcorn, Fowler, Telang and Solanto2007; see Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Although impulsive, reward-seeking behaviors upregulate DA levels temporarily, with associated increases in positive affect, they do so only transiently, and do not address underlying DA deficiency (Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Thus, low tonic DA, combined with low phasic responding to reward cues, may lead to a disproportionately high preference for immediate over delayed rewards and diminished ability to act with foresight.

In addition to providing a neurobiological explanation for impulse control problems, the DA deficiency model of impulsivity may also account for mechanisms that underlie responses to certain interventions. For example, raising intrinsically low levels of tonic DA via administration of DA agonists improves associative learning among those with ADHD by extending the interval during which behavior–reward contingencies are encoded (see Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007). DA agonists may also reduce novelty- and sensation-seeking behaviors by diminishing an organism's dependence on immediately available rewarding stimuli to increase striatal activation (Arnsten, Reference Arnsten2006). Other neural processes, such as error detection, may also improve when reinforcement contingencies are presented sufficiently close together to accommodate a shorter window for associative learning (see Luman, Oosterlan, & Sergeant, Reference Luman, Oosterlaan and Sergeant2005). For example, providing immediate, performance-based rewards is associated with improved task accuracy and increased neural responding to error and task-relevant stimuli among children with ADHD (Rosch & Hawk, Reference Rosch and Hawk2013). It is also a core component of effective treatments for more severe conduct problems (e.g., Ialongo, Poduska, Werthamer, & Kellam, Reference Ialongo, Poduska, Werthamer and Kellam2001).

Interim conclusions

Considerable evidence from animal research and neuroimaging studies with humans suggests that for many individuals, trait impulsivity derives from chronically low tonic DA and diminished DA reactivity to incentive cues in the VS and other mesolimbic structures (see Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011). As a result, trait impulsive individuals exhibit deficits in associative learning and a preference for immediate but small rewards over larger delayed rewards. This preference for immediacy also manifests in reduced motivation to engage in tasks that require sustained effort, and tasks that are not intrinsically motivating (see Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Raising DA to normal levels improves impulse control by facilitating associative reward learning, increasing positive affect (see Ashby, Isen, & Turken, Reference Ashby, Isen and Turken1999), and reducing reliance on external reinforcers that are immediately available.

Neural Circuitry of Anhedonia

Support for midbrain DA deficiency as a neural basis of anhedonia is also extensive, and derives from animal studies, human neuroimaging experiments, and behavioral studies conducted with both subclinical and clinically depressed samples (Gorwood, Reference Gorwood2008). Findings from these studies indicate that anhedonia is characterized by DA deficiencies in associative reward learning and diminished motivation to exert effort to obtain incentives. Some, including Eisenberg et al. (Reference Eisenberger, Berkman, Inagaki, Rameson, Mashal and Irwin2010), even operationalize anhedonia as diminished VS activation during reward anticipation, highlighting the importance of DA neural circuitry in expression of this trait.

Animal studies

Because animals cannot self-report state or trait anhedonia, it must be inferred from behavior under carefully controlled experimental conditions. For example, anhedonia is sometimes operationalized as aberrantly low likelihood of seeking pleasurable stimuli despite intact motor function. Animal models suggest that DA is neither necessary nor sufficient for experiencing pleasure from consuming appetitive stimuli (i.e., liking), but may be involved critically in motivational aspects of reward processing (i.e., wanting). Manipulating mesolimbic DA through pharmacological agonism/antagonism, electrical stimulation, and lesions induced by neurotoxins, such as 6-hydroxydopamine, alters an animal's propensity to expend energy to gain rewards, but does not influence hedonic effects of rewards, such as food palatability (see Berridge & Robinson, Reference Berridge and Robinson1998). For example, mice that are engineered genetically to have synaptic DA levels that are 70% higher than normal expend more energy to obtain sweet rewards, but are no more likely to derive pleasure from sweets themselves, as assessed through orofacial reactions (Peciña, Cagniard, Berridge, Aldridge, & Zhuang, Reference Peciña, Cagniard, Berridge, Aldridge and Zhuang2003). Similarly, mice that are engineered genetically to be incapable of producing DA exhibit a preference for sucrose and saccharin over water, just as control mice, but initiate licking behavior for the sweet-tasting substances less frequently (Cannon & Palmiter, Reference Cannon and Palmiter2003). The authors suggest that DA is not required to develop a taste preference for sweets, but may be involved in goal-directed behavior to obtain sweets. This parallels findings among depressed humans, who experience hedonic effects of sweet tastes similarly to healthy controls (Dichter, Smoski, Kampov-Polevoy, Gallop, & Garbutt, Reference Dichter, Smoski, Kampov-Polevoy, Gallop and Garbutt2010).

Animals with deficient mesolimbic DA levels also exhibit a preference for low-cost/low rewards when high-cost/high rewards are available, which is sometimes considered to be a behavioral indicator of anhedonia (see Salamone, Correa, Farrar, & Mingote, Reference Salamone, Correa, Farrar and Mingote2007). This preference is maintained when reward delays are made equivalent across high- and low-cost reward trials (Floresco, Maric, & Ghods-Sharif, Reference Floresco, Maric and Ghods-Sharifi2008), and disappears when low and high rewards require equal energy expenditure (Denk et al., Reference Denk, Walton, Jennings, Sharp, Rushworth and Bannerman2005). Thus, animals understand reward contingencies yet choose options that require less effort. Correa, Carlson, Wisniecki, and Salamone (Reference Correa, Carlson, Wisniecki and Salamone2002) also reported that animals with selective DA lesions in the NAcc are just as likely as controls to exert effort for rewards that require one lever press, but respond less frequently when rewards require five lever presses. Such findings implicate low DA in diminished motivation, or reduced goal-directed behaviors to receive reward.

Neuroimaging research with humans

Anhedonia is often viewed as a core component of depression and schizophrenia, and like impulsivity, is expressed along a continuum in the population (Keller et al., Reference Keller, Young, Kelley, Prater, Levitin and Menon2013). Nonpathological anhedonia involves both reduced ability to anticipate and adapt one's behavior to obtain incentives and reduced motivation to work for incentives. For example, Pizzagalli et al. (Reference Pizzagalli, Jahn and O'Shea2005) reported that undergraduates with high self-reported anhedonia failed to show increases in response bias for rewards, whereas participants with low self-reported anhedonia exhibited this response bias. Thus, the high anhedonia group failed to adjust their behavior based on prior rewarded experiences, suggesting deficient ability to anticipate rewards to maximize gains. Pizzagalli et al. propose that this behavioral pattern may reflect the anhedonic phenotype of depression. Undergraduate students with higher self-reported anhedonia are also less willing to expend effort to receive rewards in laboratory-based tasks that are designed to evaluate reward motivation (Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert, & Zald, Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009).

To our knowledge, only two studies have assessed the relationship between anhedonia and neural responding to incentives in nonclinical, non–high-risk samples. Both studies used versions of the monetary incentive delay task. The first study, conducted by Wacker, Dillon, and Pizzagalli (Reference Wacker, Dillon and Pizzagalli2009), included a predominately nonclinical sample (N = 28). Reduced NAcc volumes and diminished activation in the NAcc to monetary incentives were both associated with anhedonia (as assessed by the anhedonic depression subscale of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Weber, Assenheimer, Clark, Strauss and McCormick1995), but not with other symptoms of depression and anxiety. The authors failed to find compromised activation to reward cues, which is more consistent with the anhedonia literature. However, their paradigm allowed for monetary gains on 50% of reward trials. This reward schedule is lower than that used in similar tasks from other studies (e.g., ~66% of trials were rewarded in Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Adams, Fong and Hommer2001). Thus, participants were less able to predict reward outcomes, which, as described above, results in elicitation of greater phasic NAcc firing during reward receipt, but not during presentation of reward cues. Wacker et al. (Reference Wacker, Dillon and Pizzagalli2009) acknowledge that this difference in methods complicates interpretation of their findings. It is therefore unclear whether anhedonia involves dysfunctional neural responding to anticipatory, consummatory, or both phases of reward processing.

The second study was performed by Eisenberger et al. (Reference Eisenberger, Berkman, Inagaki, Rameson, Mashal and Irwin2010), who, unlike Wacker et al. (Reference Wacker, Dillon and Pizzagalli2009), excluded participants with a current DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis. Participants (N = 39) were randomly assigned to placebo or a low-dose injection of the endotoxin Escherichia coli. Endotoxin was administered to induce an inflammatory response, because inflammation is implicated in onset and maintenance of depression (Miller, Maletic, & Raison, Reference Miller, Maletic and Raison2009). Compared with placebo, endotoxin exposure was associated with both blunted VS activation to reward cues (but not reward receipt) and increased self-reported and observer-assessed depressed mood.Footnote 9 Group differences in VS activation mediated the relationship between endotoxin exposure and increases in observer-rated depressed mood. Although the purpose of this study was to investigate the role of inflammation in depression, findings support a functional relationship between blunted VS responses to reward cues and depressive symptoms.

Two other neuroimaging studies have investigated anhedonia among healthy adults without past or current mental illness, but neither included functional tasks that assessed reward responding to incentives. The first study, conducted by Harvey, Pruessner, Czechowska, and Lepage (Reference Harvey, Pruessner, Czechowska and Lepage2007), required participants to view images of positive, negative, and neutral valences. Among other findings, higher anhedonia scores, as measured by the Chapman Revised Physical Anhedonia Scale, were associated positively with activation in the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and negatively with anterior caudate volumes, which extended to the VS in a less stringent follow-up analysis. Although the researchers' hypotheses of enhanced vmPFC activation and reduced striatal volumes were supported by their findings, they did not identify reduced VS activation to pleasant stimuli as predicted.

The second study, conducted by Keller et al. (Reference Keller, Young, Kelley, Prater, Levitin and Menon2013), investigated the relationship between trait anhedonia (measured using the positive affect factor of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire—Short Form) and neural responding to musical stimuli. Trait anhedonia was associated with reduced pleasantness ratings of stimuli, and with hypoactivation of the right NAcc, basal forebrain, bilateral hypothalamus, and cortical regions, including orbitofrontal, anterior, and posterior cingulate, and ventromedial prefrontal cortices. Trait anhedonia correlated negatively with effective connectivity between mesolimbic and paralimbic regions, including the bilateral insula and the OFC.

Anhedonia in MDD

As noted above, anhedonia characterizes several psychiatric disorders, including major depression, schizophrenia, and Parkinson disease (Winograd-Gurvich, Fitzgerald, Georgiou-Karistianis, Bradshaw, & White, Reference Winograd-Gurvich, Fitzgerald, Georgiou-Karistianis, Bradshaw and White2006). Although anhedonia is not required for a DSM-5 diagnosis of major depression, anhedonia and depressed mood are the two core diagnostic features of the disorder (APA, 2013). Reported estimates of clinically significant anhedonia among adults with major depression vary widely from 37% to 92% (Buckner, Joiner, Pettit, Lewinsohn, & Schmidt, Reference Buckner, Joiner, Pettit, Lewinsohn and Schmidt2008; Lemke, Puhl, Koethe, & Winkler, Reference Lemke, Puhl, Koethe and Winkler1999; Pelizza & Ferrari, Reference Pelizza and Ferrari2009). Similarly, estimates of anhedonia among depressed children and adolescents range from 10% to 57% (Luby, Mrakotsky, Heffelfinger, Brown, & Spitznagel, Reference Luby, Mrakotsky, Heffelfinger, Brown and Spitznagel2004; Yorbik, Birmaher, Axelson, Williamson, & Ryan, Reference Yorbik, Birmaher, Axelson, Williamson and Ryan2004). Such large discrepancies may be attributable to use of different measures of anhedonia, different thresholds for determining clinical significance, and differences in symptom presentation and severity among study samples. Regardless, these findings indicate that anhedonia is a common but not ubiquitous feature of depression.

Because anhedonia is so common to major depression, disentangling state-dependent effects of mental illness from trait anhedonia in psychiatric populations remains a challenge. Nevertheless, mesolimbic DA dysfunction in depression is reported widely (e.g., Lambert, Johansson, Ågren, & Friberg, Reference Lambert, Johansson, Ågren and Friberg2000; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Krüger, Wilson, Christensen, Goulding and Schaffer2001). Striatal hypoactivation to positively valenced stimuli is a well-replicated finding among depressed adolescent and adult samples (e.g., see Forbes & Dahl, Reference Forbes and Dahl2011; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, May, Siegle, Ladouceur, Ryan and Carter2006, Reference Forbes, Hariri, Martin, Silk, Moyles and Fisher2009; Groenewold, Opmeer, de Jonge, Aleman, & Costafreda, Reference Groenewold, Opmeer, de Jonge, Aleman and Costafreda2013; Morgan, Olino, McMakin, Ryan, & Forbes, Reference Morgan, Olino, McMakin, Ryan and Forbes2013). For responses to incentives more specifically, current depression is associated with failure to develop a response bias toward rewarding stimuli (e.g., Henriques, Glowacki, & Davidson, Reference Henriques, Glowacki and Davidson1994; Pizzagalli, Iosifescu, Hallett, Ratner, & Fava, Reference Pizzagalli, Iosifescu, Hallett, Ratner and Fava2008), impaired reward-based reversal learning (Robinson, Cools, Carlisi, Sahakian, & Drevets, Reference Robinson, Cools, Carlisi, Sahakian and Drevets2012), and blunted VS responses to anticipation and receipt of monetary incentives (e.g., Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Hariri, Martin, Silk, Moyles and Fisher2009; Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk and Bogdan2009; Smoski et al., Reference Smoski, Felder, Bizzell, Green, Ernst and Lynch2009). Pizzagalli et al. (Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk and Bogdan2009) also demonstrated that anhedonic symptoms and depression severity correlate negatively with smaller caudate volumes. The one exception to this body of literature is a study by Knutson et al. (Reference Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas and Gotlib2008), who found that NAcc activation did not differ between depressed participants and controls during reward anticipation or receipt. The authors suggest that their sample of depressed adults may not have exhibited VS hyporeactivity during reward anticipation as they predicted specifically because anhedonic features were not sufficiently prominent.

The null finding by Knutson et al. (Reference Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas and Gotlib2008) should not be considered anomalous. As presented above, anhedonia in depression is common, but not universal. A large body of research indicates that depression emerges from a multitude of equifinal pathways involving genetic-, neural-, neuroendocrine-, and trait-level vulnerabilities that interact with environmental risk factors, including adversity in childhood, ongoing stressors, and social instability (see Charney & Manji, Reference Charney and Manji2004; Kendler, Gardner, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Gardner and Prescott2002; Kim, Reference Kim2008; Klein, Kotov, & Bufferd, Reference Klein, Kotov and Bufferd2011). Trait anhedonia may be a characteristic feature of a subpopulation of individuals with major depression, which may account for why only a subset of depressed individuals exhibit mesolimbic DA dysfunction when interindividual variability in reward responding is considered (Foti, Carlson, Sauder, & Proudfit, Reference Foti, Carlson, Sauder and Proudfit2014). Accordingly, greater attention to the extent to which individual differences in reward processing predict alternative phenotypic presentations of illness and treatment response may be especially valuable (see Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Hariri, Martin, Silk, Moyles and Fisher2009; e.g., Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Olino, McMakin, Ryan and Forbes2013). The importance of identifying clinical subgroups on the basis of etiological vulnerabilities and not collections of behavioral symptoms is revisited multiple times in this article.

Anhedonia in populations at high risk for major depression

Anhedonia may be present prior to and/or after major depressive episodes, and thus may be best characterized as a traitlike vulnerability (McCabe, Reference McCabe and Ritsner2014). Self-reports of anhedonia predict the onset of major depression 1 year later (Dryman & Eaton, Reference Dryman and Eaton1991), and undergraduate students and individuals with histories of depression report individual differences in hedonic capacity that are stable across long periods of time (Clark, Fawcett, Salazar-Grueso, & Fawcett, Reference Clark, Fawcett, Salazar-Grueso and Fawcett1984; Shankman et al., Reference Shankman, Nelson, Harrow and Faull2010; Watson & Walker, Reference Watson and Walker1996). Anhedonia is also experienced both during and between major depressive episodes (Schrader, Reference Schrader1997). Despite such findings, and a long-established theoretical argument for anhedonia as a trait vulnerability to depression, very few laboratory-based studies have investigated prospective links between anhedonia and depression, and studies of anhedonia in cases of remitted depression cannot rule out scarring effects.

Remitted depression

Select studies have included samples of individuals with remitted depression to investigate possible traitlike features associated with susceptibility. Compared to healthy controls, individuals with remitted depression may be less able to modulate their behavior based on reinforcement history (i.e., develop a response bias to rewarding outcomes), reflecting deficient reward learning (Pechtel, Dutra, Goetz, & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel, Dutra, Goetz and Pizzagalli2013). Furthermore, using fMRI, McCabe, Cowen, and Harmer (Reference McCabe, Cowen and Harmer2009) reported reduced activation in the VS and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to a pleasant stimulus (chocolate taste) among individuals with remitted depression compared to healthy controls, suggesting mesolimbic dysfunction may persist following depressive episodes. These results are compatible with those obtained by Hasler et al. (Reference Hasler, Fromm, Carlson, Luckenbaugh, Waldeck and Geraci2008), who induced acute catecholamine depletion, including DA, via oral administration of α-methylparatyrosine among adults with a history of major depression and healthy controls. Using PET, Hasler et al. compared neural glucose activity before and after depletion, and anticipated increased glucose metabolism in regions implicated in depression due to the inhibitory role of striatal DA projections on glutamate release. Consistent with their hypotheses, depressive and anhedonic symptoms (assessed by the Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale and the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale, respectively) increased to a greater extent in the remitted depressed group than in the control group. Furthermore, PET revealed an association between catecholamine depletion and increased metabolic activity of glucose in the VS across groups. Anhedonic symptoms also correlated with heightened metabolic activity in the VS following catecholamine depletion. Hasler et al. suggest that individuals with a history of major depression may be more vulnerable to experiencing depressive and anhedonic symptoms due to greater sensitivity of reward circuitry to catecholamine reduction.

The aforementioned studies by McCabe et al. (Reference McCabe, Cowen and Harmer2009) and Hasler et al. (Reference Hasler, Fromm, Carlson, Luckenbaugh, Waldeck and Geraci2008) both implicate VS function in remitted depression, but neither investigated reward anticipation specifically. Dichter, Kozink, McClernon, and Smoski (Reference Dichter, Kozink, McClernon and Smoski2012) investigated neural responding to both reward anticipation and outcomes among individuals with remitted major depression using a monetary incentive delay task and failed to find VS hypofunctionality. Instead, they reported hyperactivation in the pregenual ACC, right midfrontal gyrus, and right cerebellum to reward anticipation, and hypoactivation in the OFC, right frontal pole, left insular cortex, and left thalamus. The authors interpret these findings as evidence of aberrant frontostriatal responding due to connectivity among these regions, which are directly or indirectly implicated in reward processing.

Individuals with familial risk for depression

Another approach to investigating anhedonia as a vulnerability to major depression is to study individuals with a family history of the disorder (Rice, Harold, & Thapar, Reference Rice, Harold and Thapar2002; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Nomura, Warner, Pilowsky and Verdeli2006). High internalizing symptoms predict reduced neural responding to reward among siblings, over and above ratings of positive and negative affect (Weinberg, Liu, Hajcak, & Shankman, Reference Weinberg, Liu, Hajcak and Shankman2015). In addition, across studies, children of depressed parents exhibit hypofunctionality in neural reward circuitry (McCabe, Woffindale, Harmer, & Cowen, Reference McCabe, Woffindale, Harmer and Cowen2012; Olino et al., Reference Olino, McMakin, Morgan, Silk, Birmaher and Axelson2014; see Proudfit, Reference Proudfit2014). For example, youth with strong familial vulnerability to depression (but not a personal history of depression) exhibit diminished activation in the VS specifically to anticipation of incentives (Olino et al., Reference Olino, McMakin, Morgan, Silk, Birmaher and Axelson2014). In addition, the few longitudinal studies that have investigated depression among vulnerable youth samples report that diminished reward-seeking behavior and aberrant neural reward processing are associated prospectively with depressive symptoms, and may predict later emergence of depression (e.g., Bress, Meyer, & Proudfit, Reference Bress, Meyer and Proudfit2015; Bress, Smith, Foti, Klein, & Hajcak, Reference Bress, Smith, Foti, Klein and Hajcak2012; Rawal, Collishaw, Thapar, & Rice, Reference Rawal, Collishaw, Thapar and Rice2013). These results suggest that youth with family histories of depression exhibit similar neural reward circuitry deficits seen in depression, and that these deficits may reflect an underlying vulnerability for developing the disorder.

Anhedonia, self-inflicted injury (SII), and borderline personality disorder

As summarized above, both anhedonia, assessed via self-report, and blunted VS responding to incentives, assessed through neuroimaging, are exhibited by vulnerable and currently depressed samples. Another subpopulation of interest for the present discussion is individuals with a history of SII, including those who have attempted suicide and/or engage in nonsuicidal self-harm (see Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, McCauley, Smith, Vasilev and Stevens2008; Kaufman, Crowell, & Stepp, Reference Kaufman, Crowell, Stepp, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016; Nock, Reference Nock2010). Individuals who engage in these behaviors are both trait impulsive and anhedonic, and lie at the nexus of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology (see Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbridge, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Crowell, Derbidge, & Beauchaine, Reference Crowell, Derbidge, Beauchaine and Nock2014; Crowell, Kaufman, & Beauchaine, Reference Crowell, Kaufman, Beauchaine, Tackett and Sharp2014). Anhedonia predicts suicidal ideation (Winer et al., Reference Winer, Nadorff, Ellis, Allen, Herrera and Salem2014) and violence toward self and others (Fawcett et al., Reference Fawcett, Scheftner, Fogg, Clark, Young and Hedeker1990; Nordström, Schalling, & Asberg, Reference Nordström, Schalling and Asberg1995; Sadeh, Javdani, Finy, & Verona, Reference Sadeh, Javdani, Finy and Verona2011). Furthermore, as with other groups characterized by anhedonia, those who have attempted suicide exhibit deficits in reward processing, including impaired reward/punishment-based reversal learning (Dombrovski et al., Reference Dombrovski, Clark, Siegle, Butters, Ichikawa and Sahakian2010), a high preference for immediate over delayed rewards, and difficulties anticipating positive future events (see van Heeringen, Bijttebier, & Godfrin, Reference van Heeringen, Bijttebier and Godfrin2011).

Research on the neural substrates of reward responding in SII is still in its infancy, but structural brain alterations in mesolimbic circuitry are implicated in major depression among individuals with a history of suicide attempts (Jia et al., Reference Jia, Huang, Wu, Zhang, Lui and Zhang2010), and those at high risk for suicide based on family history (Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Koch, Schachtzabel, Schultz, Sauer and Schlösser2011). A history of self-harm is also associated with reduced functional connectivity in striatal circuitry, both during depressive episodes and in remitted depression (Marchand, Lee, Johnson, Thatcher, & Gale, Reference Marchand, Lee, Johnson, Thatcher and Gale2013; Marchand et al., Reference Marchand, Lee, Johnson, Thatcher, Gale and Wood2012). In the only study to date that has specifically evaluated neural correlates of reward responding among self-injurers, adolescents who engaged in SII exhibited less striatal activation in anticipation of incentives compared with controls (Sauder et al., Reference Sauder, Derbidge and Beauchaine2016).

Although depressed adolescents who engage in SII share many similarities with other depressed groups (e.g., high negative affectivity; low positive affectivity), they are more likely to follow a developmental course toward borderline personality disorder (BPD; see Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Crowell, Beauchaine, & Linehan, Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Crowell, Kaufman, & Lenzenweger, Reference Crowell, Kaufman, Lenzenweger, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2013), and to engage in suicidal behaviors, gestures, and threats (diagnostic criteria of the disorder; APA, 2013). Although emotion dysregulation is regarded as a core feature of BPD (see Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Linehan, Reference Linehan1993), trait impulsivity and anhedonia are also common, and correlate with severity of symptoms (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Marissen, Arnold, & Franken, Reference Marissen, Arnold and Franken2012). In the only study to date to investigate reward responding in BPD, participants with BPD or ASPD exhibited blunted prefrontal and striatal responding to monetary incentives (Völlm et al., Reference Völlm, Richardson, McKie, Elliott, Dolan and Deakin2007), suggesting deficient neural reward processing.Footnote 10

Interim conclusions

The construct of anhedonia typically encompasses reward processing related to both anticipatory/motivational and consummatory aspects of incentives. It is likely that some anhedonic individuals have deficiencies in both domains, whereas others have deficiencies restricted to only one or the other. However, findings from behavioral neuroscience and human neuroimaging studies suggest that distinguishing between motivation and the experience of pleasure is warranted, and that DA neurotransmission may be more critically involved in the former component. From this perspective, growing evidence suggests that high trait anhedonia may reflect poor associative reward learning and diminished motivation to exert effort to obtain rewards, both of which are mediated by a hypofunctioning mesolimbic DA system. These findings parallel research on the neural bases of trait impulsivity, described above. Major depression is a heterogeneous construct, and anhedonia is not equivalent to, or a necessary component of, depression. Additional research on anhedonia in the context of clinical, high-risk, and nonclinical populations, as well as disentangling the neural substrates of different aspects of depression, including anhedonia, may help to further clarify relationships between DA neural circuitry responding and trait anhedonia.

Trait Irritability as an Aversive Mood State

Emotions are often described as valenced responses to stimuli, whereas mood is described as a slower moving, longer lasting pattern of affective responding that is less tied to discrete events (Rottenberg & Gross, Reference Rottenberg and Gross2003; Watson, Reference Watson2000). Multiple neurotransmitter systems, including serotonin, norepinephrine, and DA, are associated with mood modulation (see, e.g., Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Zalewski, Crowell and Potapova2011; Ruhé, Mason, & Schene, Reference Ruhé, Mason and Schene2007). Here, we emphasize tonic mesolimbic DA dysfunction as an important neural substrate of trait irritability, a tendency to be easily and excessively annoyed, angered, inpatient, and aggressive (see Bettencourt, Talley, Benjamin, & Valentine, Reference Bettencourt, Talley, Benjamin and Valentine2006; Leibenluft & Stoddard, Reference Leibenluft and Stoddard2013). Although trait irritability refers to a negative mood state, it is exhibited through emotional outbursts and disproportionately intense aggressive acts to provocations. Thus, trait irritability is closely related to the concepts of emotion/mood dysregulation and emotional lability (Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg, & Leibenluft, Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014; Snaith & Taylor, Reference Snaith and Taylor1985). However, it is worth noting that trait irritability is a mood state that is largely heritable, whereas emotion (dys)regulation is affected much more strongly by socialization mechanisms (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., in press; Goldsmith, Pollak, & Davidson, Reference Goldsmith, Pollak and Davidson2008).

With the exception of ODD and ASPD, irritability is not a diagnostic feature of externalizing disorders or adult major depression (APA, 2013). However, irritability often co-occurs with trait impulsivity and anhedonia and is frequently experienced by those with externalizing and depressive disorders, as discussed below. We argue that low tonic and low phasic mesolimbic DA is a neural substrate of trait irritability, which is experienced as a persistent aversive mood state. Furthermore, we assert that trait irritability has transdiagnostic neural substrates that link it to both trait impulsivity and anhedonia based on a common etiology (i.e., mesolimbic DA dysfunction), which helps to account for higher than expected rates of comorbidity between depression and externalizing disorders.

Neural bases of trait irritability

Compromised mesolimbic DA function, including low tonic and diminished anticipatory responses to incentives, is a core neural substrate of anhedonia and chronic irritability among both clinical and nonclinical populations (e.g., Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014; Yamawaki, Okada, Okamoto, & Liberzon, Reference Yamawaki, Okada, Okamoto and Liberzon2012). PET studies demonstrate that low tonic midbrain DA activity is associated with trait irritability among healthy individuals (Laakso et al., Reference Laakso, Wallius, Kajander, Bergman, Eskola and Solin2003), whereas high DA activity is associated with a pleasant mood state and high hedonic capacity (see, e.g., Ashby et al., Reference Ashby, Isen and Turken1999; Depue, Luciana, Arbisi, Collins, & Leon, Reference Depue, Luciana, Arbisi, Collins and Leon1994). Thus, tonic DA hypofunctioning affects how incentives are experienced subjectively (i.e., liking), which in turn affects behavior (Berridge & Kringelbach, Reference Berridge and Kringelbach2015; Blum et al. Reference Blum, Braverman, Holder, Lubar, Monastra and Miller2000, Reference Blum, Chen, Braverman, Comings, Chen and Arcuri2008; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Among healthy individuals, the magnitude of DA release in the VS in response to DA agonism correlates with hedonic responding (Drevets et al., Reference Drevets, Gautier, Price, Kupfer, Kinahan and Grace2001), and upregulating DA availability increases hedonic expectations for positive future life events (Sharot, Shiner, Brown, Fan, & Dolan, Reference Sharot, Shiner, Brown, Fan and Dolan2009). Such findings suggest that raising mesolimbic DA levels is associated with improved mood and hedonic capacity.

It is interesting to note that some individuals report an unpleasant subjective reaction to DA agonism, which may reflect individual differences in tonic mesolimbic DA function (Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Fowler, Logan, Gatley and Gifford1999). Healthy individuals who report liking the effects of infusions of methylphenidate, a DA reuptake inhibitor, have fewer D2 receptors in the striatum compared to those who report disliking the experience, and more D2 receptors correlate with greater intensity of unpleasantness (Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Fowler, Logan, Gatley and Gifford1999). Because D2 receptors are autoreceptors, a lack of this receptor type may reflect compensation for diminished DA activity in reward circuitry (Blum et al., Reference Blum, Braverman, Holder, Lubar, Monastra and Miller2000). Thus, Volkow et al. (Reference Volkow, Wang, Fowler, Logan, Gatley and Gifford1999) conclude that DA agonism among individuals with intrinsically low mesolimbic DA activity is more likely to be experienced as pleasant because it raises DA levels to within an optimal range for enjoying the experience. In contrast, those with intrinsically higher mesolimbic DA activity are more likely to find the experience aversive because DA agonism pushes activity outside an optimal range of function. In addition, depressed individuals also rate subjective effects of DA agonism as more pleasant than healthy controls, and exhibit comparatively low mesolimbic functioning following dextroamphetamine administration (Tremblay et al., Reference Tremblay, Naranjo, Graham, Herrmann, Mayberg and Hevenor2005). Low DA synthesis capacity in the striatum and midbrain is also associated with aggressiveness (Schlüter et al., Reference Schlüter, Winz, Henkel, Prinz, Rademacher and Schmaljohann2013), which, as noted above, is often observed with irritability. Collectively, these findings lend further support to the supposition that low tonic and low phasic DA contribute to trait irritability and altered responses to reinforcers.

Contribution to externalizing disorders

Several contemporary models of ADHD assert that chronic irritability motivates hyperactivity and excessive reinforcement-seeking behavior, hallmarks of the combined and hyperactive–impulsive presentations (Blum et al. Reference Blum, Braverman, Holder, Lubar, Monastra and Miller2000, Reference Blum, Chen, Braverman, Comings, Chen and Arcuri2008; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005). Sagvolden et al. (Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005) assert that reinforcement-seeking behaviors provoke phasic DA release, which serves to temporarily reduce feelings of irritability, anhedonia, and boredom caused by low tonic DA activity. As noted above, however, any hedonic value gained from these behaviors is transient, and does not address the underlying DA deficiency. Thus, searches for more frequent and larger incentives are undertaken to alleviate the aversive mood state (see Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011; Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Coch, Dawson and Fischer2007; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005).

Contribution to depression and SII

Although depression can result from interactions among a multitude of biological vulnerabilities and environmental risk factors, a core etiological factor for many with the disorder is hypofunctioning DA-mediated reward processing. Deficient neural responding to incentives is a neural substrate of low positive affectivity, anhedonia, and irritability (see Forbes & Dahl, Reference Forbes and Dahl2005, Reference Forbes and Dahl2011; Nestler & Carlezon, Reference Nestler and Carlezon2006). Lower` striatal responding in anticipation of monetary incentives is associated with lower positive affect in real-world settings (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Hariri, Martin, Silk, Moyles and Fisher2009), and predicts increases in depressive symptoms over time (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Olino, McMakin, Ryan and Forbes2013). Dampened VS activation to reward is also associated with impaired mood reactivity to positive events (Foti et al., Reference Foti, Carlson, Sauder and Proudfit2014). Like anhedonia, irritability is particularly relevant to certain subpopulations with depression, including those with notable mood/emotion dysregulation and emotional lability. Irritability is associated with histories of suicidal ideation, suicide attempts, and nonsuicidal self-injury (e.g., Conner, Meldrum, Wieczorek, Duberstein, & Welte, Reference Conner, Meldrum, Wieczorek, Duberstein and Welte2004; Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Lalovic, Lesage, Seguin, Tousignant and Turecki2004; Herpertz, Reference Herpertz1995; Pendse, Westrin, & Engström, Reference Pendse, Westrin and Engström1999; Stålenheim, Reference Stålenheim2001). Individuals who repeatedly engage in SII are likely to experience negative/irritable mood states and use self-harm as a coping strategy to mitigate emotional distress (Brown, Comtois, & Linehan, Reference Brown, Comtois and Linehan2002; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Nixon, Cloutier, & Aggarwal, Reference Nixon, Cloutier and Aggarwal2002; Nock, Prinstein, & Sterba, Reference Nock, Prinstein and Sterba2009; Taylor, Peterson & Fischer, Reference Taylor, Peterson and Fischer2012). Trait irritability may also help to explain the high co-occurrence of self-harm and aggression directed toward others (Leibenluft & Stoddard, Reference Leibenluft and Stoddard2013; O'Donnell, House, & Waterman, Reference O'Donnell, House and Waterman2015).

Vulnerability to externalizing disorders, depression, and heterotypic comorbidity

Trait irritability is common among individuals who are impulsive and anhedonic, and confers vulnerability to externalizing spectrum disorders and depression. Seroczynski, Bergeman, and Coccaro (Reference Seroczynski, Bergeman and Coccaro1999) reported that impulsivity and irritability share overlapping genetic influences, a relationship that is stronger than that linking impulsivity and physical aggression. Greater irritability correlates with greater severity of hyperactive–impulsive ADHD symptoms (Sobanski et al., Reference Sobanski, Banaschewski, Asherson, Buitelaar, Chen and Franke2010), and raising mesolimbic DA levels via psychostimulant administration improves irritability among children with ADHD (de la Cruz et al., Reference de la Cruz, Simonoff, McGough, Halperin, Arnold and Stringaris2015). Such findings have led some to suggest that chronic irritability is a core feature of ADHD (e.g., Downson & Blackwell, Reference Downson and Blackwell2010; Skirrow, McLoughlin, Kuntsi, & Asherson, Reference Skirrow, McLoughlin, Kuntsi and Asherson2009).

Irritability is also associated with more severe anhedonia among depressed adolescents (Gabbay et al., Reference Gabbay, Johnson, Alonso, Evans, Babb and Klein2015), and is a common experience during major depressive episodes among adults, even when excluding for bipolar spectrum features (Fava et al., Reference Fava, Hwang, Rush, Sampson, Walters and Kessler2010; Judd, Schettler, Coryell, Akiskal, & Fiedorowicz, Reference Judd, Schettler, Coryell, Akiskal and Fiedorowicz2013; Perlis et al., Reference Perlis, Fava, Trivedi, Alpert, Luther and Wisniewski2009). Trait irritability and depression share significant heritability, and irritability in childhood predicts depression in early adulthood (Savage et al., Reference Savage, Verhulst, Copeland, Althoff, Lichtenstein and Roberson-Nay2015). Of note, irritability concurrent with all major depressive episode criteria except sad mood or anhedonia is exceptionally rare, which suggests it is linked closely to core features of major depression (Fava et al., Reference Fava, Hwang, Rush, Sampson, Walters and Kessler2010; Kovess-Masfety et al., Reference Kovess-Masfety, Alonso, Angermeyer, Bromet, de Girolamo and de Jonge2013).

We discuss irritability here because it frequently co-occurs with both externalizing spectrum disorders and depression, and because it is likely an important vulnerability to heterotypic comorbidity. Irritable children and adolescents with ADHD have significantly higher rates of depressive disorders compared to those who are not irritable (Ambrosini, Bennett, & Elia, Reference Ambrosini, Bennett and Elia2013; Sobanski et al., Reference Sobanski, Banaschewski, Asherson, Buitelaar, Chen and Franke2010). Similarly, chronic but not episodic irritability in early adolescence predicts ADHD and ODD during late adolescence, as well as MDD in early adulthood (Leibenluft, Cohen, Gorrindo, Brook, & Pine, Reference Leibenluft, Cohen, Gorrindo, Brook and Pine2006). Moreover, irritability in adolescence is associated with concurrent ADHD, mood disorders, ODD, and CD, and predicts MDD and dysthymia 20 years later (Stringaris, Psych, Cohen, Pine, & Leibenluft, Reference Stringaris, Psych, Cohen, Pine and Leibenluft2009). Finally, irritability is associated with high rates of comorbidity of impulse-control disorders and depression among adults (Fava et al., Reference Fava, Hwang, Rush, Sampson, Walters and Kessler2010), and may mark more severe psychopathology (Judd et al. Reference Judd, Schettler, Coryell, Akiskal and Fiedorowicz2013; Perlis et al., Reference Perlis, Fava, Trivedi, Alpert, Luther and Wisniewski2009; Sobanski et al., Reference Sobanski, Banaschewski, Asherson, Buitelaar, Chen and Franke2010).

Interim conclusions

Trait irritability, which is the tendency to be annoyed, angered, and impatient, is an aversive mood state that is characterized by low tonic mesolimbic DA activity and low phasic mesolimbic DA responding to incentives. Therefore, it shares neural bases with both trait impulsivity and anhedonia. As a transdiagnostic, trait-level vulnerability to psychopathology, irritability is often experienced by individuals with ADHD and individuals with major depression and appears to be especially likely among individuals comorbid for both disorders. Among those with ADHD, irritability may serve as a motivator to engage in hyperactive and reward-seeking behaviors, which elicit mesolimbic DA release, temporarily improving a chronically aversive mood state.

Neural Accounts of Heterotypic Comorbidity

Higher than expected rates of comorbidity between externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression are difficult to explain when considering that the two classes of psychopathology have almost no overlapping diagnostic criteria, and are often ascribed different etiological bases, with dopaminergic theories predominating for impulse-control disorders and serotonergic and noradrenergic theories predominating for depression. Here, we offer a framework to help explain, at least in part, high rates of heterotypic comorbidity between externalizing disorders and unipolar depression by suggesting that low tonic mesolimbic DA, and low phasic responding during reward anticipation, comprise core neural substrates of trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability, all of which are vulnerabilities to both externalizing disorders and unipolar depression, as described above.

It is important to note that midbrain DA dysfunction does not invariably result in psychopathology, but rather interacts dynamically with other subcortical neural systems, with cortical neural systems, and with environmental risk factors to shape and maintain patterns of behavior (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., in press; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). According to this view, externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression share mesolimbic DA dysfunction, yet have other, nonoverlapping neural substrates (e.g., serotonergic and noradrenergic; discussed below). Moreover, midbrain DA dysfunction is especially likely to eventuate in psychopathology when coupled with compromised prefrontally mediated emotion regulation capabilities (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007).

Subcortical moderators of midbrain DA activity and heterotypic comorbidity

Although externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression are both characterized by anhedonia, avolition, and irritability, they differ in at least one important way: those with externalizing disorders exhibit excessive approach behavior (e.g., novelty, sensation, and reward seeking), whereas those with depression and oft co-occurring anxiety exhibit passive avoidance behavior (i.e., withdrawal; see, e.g., Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016). At the symptom level, this difference is difficult to reconcile with the high rates of heterotypic comorbidity described above. However, the apparent discrepancy can be resolved by differentiating between neural substrates of approach and passive avoidance tendencies. Toward this end, it is important to consider that complex human behaviors are subserved by multiple neurobiological systems that interact functionally with one another (see, e.g., Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Zalewski, Crowell and Potapova2011; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine and Cicchetti2016).

Here we consider how one such network, the septohippocampal system, interacts with midbrain DA function to affect behavior. The septohippocampal system includes extensive serotonergic (5-HT) projections from the raphe nuclei, and noradrenergic projections from the locus ceruleus, to a number of brain regions, including limbic structures (e.g., amygdala and hippocampus), the anterior cingulate cortex, and more frontal regions (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McNaughton & Corr, Reference McNaughton and Corr2004). The septohippocampal system and the amygdalaFootnote 11 (Gray's behavioral inhibition system) is a phylogenetically old neural system that responds to competing motivational objectives (i.e., approach-approach, approach-avoidance, and avoidance-avoidance). When such situations are encountered, septohippocampal activation promotes passive avoidance until the organism decides what action to take. Individual differences in activity and reactivity of this system give rise to corresponding individual differences in trait anxiety (Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McNaughton & Corr, Reference McNaughton and Corr2004). More broadly, the septohippocampal system helps us narrow attention, assign emotional significance to events, and encode them into memory (see Phan, Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, Reference Phan, Wager, Taylor and Liberzon2002; Phelps, Reference Phelps2004). Excessive septohippocampal activity confers vulnerability to anxiety disorders and depression (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000), whereas deficient septohippocampal activity potentiates vulnerability to externalizing behavior (see Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016). McNaughton and Corr (Reference McNaughton and Corr2004) conceptualize depression in particular as a behavioral manifestation of excessive septohippocampal system activity, which results in maladaptive responses to threat under circumstances in which the most appropriate response is to approach threat in order to obtain incentives. Instead, those who are depressed fail to approach because aversive outcomes of approach behavior are perceived as unavoidable, and outweigh any possible gains that could be obtained through action.

Single disorders

Low midbrain DA responding to reward, coupled with low septohippocampal system activity/reactivity to negatively valenced stimuli, confers vulnerability to externalizing disorders, normal midbrain DA responding to reward, coupled with excessive septohippocampal system activity/reactivity to negatively valenced stimuli, confers vulnerability to internalizing disorders (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015; Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McNaughton & Corr, Reference McNaughton and Corr2004). This assertion is supported by findings from both peripheral psychophysiological and neuroimaging studies. For example, externalizing symptoms correspond with diminished septohippocampal function, as evidenced by (a) morphological abnormalities in limbic regions, including the hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, and amygdala (e.g., Benegal, Antony, Venkatasubramanian, & Jakakumar, Reference Benegal, Antony, Venkatasubramanian and Jayakumar2007; Plessen et al., Reference Plessen, Bansal, Zhu, Whiteman, Amat and Quackenbush2006); (b) blunted amygdalar reactivity to fearful expressions (e.g., Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Mitchell, Reid, Sims and Kosson2008; see Marsh & Blair, Reference Marsh and Blair2008); and (c) low electrodermal responding (e.g., Gao, Raine, Venables, Dawson, & Mednick, Reference Gao, Raine, Venables, Dawson and Mednick2010; Isen, Iacono, Malone, & McGue, Reference Isen, Iacono, Malone and McGue2012), which serves as an index of septohippocampal function in appropriately designed experiments (Fowles Reference Fowles1980, Reference Fowles1988).

In the absence of externalizing symptoms, internalizing psychopathology, including anxiety and unipolar depression, is characterized by excessive septohippocampal system activity/reactivity to negatively valenced stimuli (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McNaughton & Corr, Reference McNaughton and Corr2004). For example, depression is associated with morphological abnormalities in the amygdala (Hamilton, Seimer, & Gotlib, Reference Hamilton, Siemer and Gotlib2008) and smaller hippocampal volumes (Campbell, Marriott, Nahmias, & MacQueen, Reference Campbell, Marriott, Nahmias and MacQueen2004). A number of studies indicate that depression is characterized by heightened activation to negative stimuli in the amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, and prefrontal regions (see Hamilton et al., Reference Hamilton, Etkin, Furman, Lemus, Johnson and Gotlib2012; Kerestes, Davey, Stephanou, Whittle, & Harrison, Reference Kerestes, Davey, Stephanou, Whittle and Harrison2014). Furthermore, nonsuicidal depression, depressive symptoms, and trait neuroticism are often associated with normal or heightened electrodermal responding (e.g., Norris, Larsen, & Cacioppo, Reference Norris, Larsen and Cacioppo2007; Papousek & Schulter, Reference Papousek and Schulter2001; Thorell et al., Reference Thorell, Wolfersdorf, Straub, Steyer, Hodgkinson and Kaschka2013), suggesting high septohippocampal system reactivity in this subpopulation (Fowles, Reference Fowles1980, Reference Fowles1988). Such findings support excessive septohippocampal system activity/reactivity as a partial neural substrate of unipolar depression.

Heterotypic comorbidity

Given opposing roles of the septohippocampal system in internalizing versus externalizing disorders, a logical next question is, “How do neural correlates of heterotypic comorbidity differ from those for single disorders along the internalizing and externalizing spectra?” This question is especially pertinent given high prevalence rates of heterotypic comorbidity and evidence from nonhuman research suggesting that these systems interact functionally (e.g., the basolateral amygdala has direct projections to the NAcc, which can influence impulse control and reward-seeking behaviors; Stuber et al., Reference Stuber, Sparta, Stamatakis, van Leeuwen, Hardjoprajitno and Cho2011; Winstanley, Theobald, Cardinal, & Robbins, Reference Winstanley, Theobald, Cardinal and Robbins2004). However, few neuroimaging studies have evaluated associations between Internalizing × Externalizing symptoms and neural function. One notable exception is a neuroanatomical analysis by Sauder, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Shannon, and Aylward (Reference Sauder, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Shannon and Aylward2012), who determined that, among adolescent males with ADHD and/or CD, comorbid internalizing symptoms correlated negatively with gray matter volumes in anterior cingulate, mesolimbic, and septohippocampal regions compared to adolescent males without internalizing symptoms. Similarly, reduced hippocampal volumes are associated with depressive symptoms, but not ADHD symptoms, among children with ADHD (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Siciliano, Wang, Liu, Sonuga-Barke and Greenhill2014). Adolescents with ADHD and CD also display increased amygdalar activation to negative stimuli relative to controls (Herpertz et al., Reference Herpertz, Huebner, Marx, Vloet, Fink and Stoecker2008), suggesting aberrant emotional responding among comorbid externalizers. Among adults with ADHD, depressive symptoms correlate with larger amygdala volumes (Frodl et al., Reference Frodl, Stauber, Schaaff, Koutsouleris, Scheuerecker and Ewers2010), and a history of major depression is associated with smaller hippocampal volumes (Onnink et al., Reference Onnink, Zwiers, Hoogman, Mostert, Kan and Buitelaar2014). Such findings provide preliminary support for a pattern of septohippocampal functioning more typical of internalizers among externalizers with internalizing symptoms compared to individuals with externalizing psychopathology alone.

Similarly, compared to individuals with internalizing symptoms alone, individuals with BPD and/or those who engage in self-injury score high on both internalizing and externalizing symptoms (Allely, Reference Allely2014; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, McCauley, Smith, Stevens and Sylvers2005, Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009, Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Hsiao, Vasilev, Yaptangco and Linehan2012; Derbidge & Beauchaine, Reference Derbidge, Beauchaine, Lewis and Rudolph2014; Eaton et al., Reference Eaton, Krueger, Keyes, Skodol, Markon and Grant2011), and exhibit patterns of neural and psychophysiological responding that are similar to those of externalizers (e.g., Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Hsiao, Vasilev, Yaptangco and Linehan2012; Vasilev, Crowell, Beuchaine, Mead, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Vasilev, Crowell, Beauchaine, Mead and Gatzke-Kopp2009; see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2012; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). As described above, these subgroups also exhibit aberrant mesolimbic DA functioning to incentives similar to those with externalizing psychopathology (Sauder et al., Reference Sauder, Derbidge and Beauchaine2016).

Functional outcomes

The research presented above suggests that heterotypically comorbid internalizing and externalizing symptoms are associated with co-occurring functional impairment in neural systems that differentiate between pure disorders. At the behavioral level of analysis, anxiety confers some protective effects among those with externalizing psychopathology. For example, those with ADHD and comorbid anxiety are less impulsive and exhibit a less severe response inhibition deficit than those with ADHD alone (see Shatz & Rostain, 2006). Furthermore, children and adolescents with CD and comorbid anxiety are less physically aggressive, regarded less negatively by peers, and experience fewer police contacts than youth with CD alone (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Lahey, Russo, Frick, Christ and McBurnett1991). Externalizing youth with comorbid anxiety are also more sensitive to punishment, and therefore more concerned with negative consequences of their behavior compared to nonanxious externalizing youth, which likely accounts for why this externalizing subgroup exhibits fewer behavior problems and better responses to behavioral treatment (e.g., Beauchaine, Neuhaus, et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Gatzke-Kopp, Reid, Brekke and Olliges2016; Jensen et al., Reference Jensen, Hinshaw, Kraemer, Lenora, Newcorn and Abikoff2001). Other research, however, suggests that externalizers with internalizing symptoms exhibit more severe conduct problems and greater risk of long-term social and academic impairment than noncomorbid individuals (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Ball, Monuteaux, Mick, Spencer and McCreary2008; Blackman, Ostrander, & Herman, Reference Blackman, Ostrander and Herman2005; Daviss, Reference Daviss2008; Ezpeleta, Domènech, & Angold, Reference Ezpeleta, Domènech and Angold2006).

Comorbid internalizing and externalizing psychopathology shape the ways in which disorders present in other ways as well. Anhedonia in BPD is related to poor impulse control, and excessive approach behaviors, but anhedonia among healthy controls is associated with the opposite pattern: withdrawal behaviors (Marissen et al., Reference Marissen, Arnold and Franken2012). Compared to depression, BPD is associated with elevated risk for suicide attempts, but comorbidity between BPD and depression is associated with more serious and more frequent suicide attempts than depression or BPD alone (Soloff, Lynch, Kelly, Malone, & Mann, Reference Soloff, Lynch, Kelly, Malone and Mann2000). High trait impulsivity is a known vulnerability to self-harm (Allely, Reference Allely2014; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009), and some have suggested that ASPD and BPD may reflect sex-moderated manifestations of a single core vulnerability (see Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Paris, Reference Paris1997; Phillipsen, Reference Philipsen2006).

Interim conclusions

Thus far, we have demonstrated that high trait impulsivity, conferred by low tonic and low phasic mesolimbic DA function, is an important vulnerability to externalizing spectrum disorders, whereas high trait impulsivity combined with high trait anxiety (subserved by excessive septohippocampal and amygdalar activity/reactivity) is implicated in comorbid externalizing and internalizing conditions. Trait anhedonia and irritability, which emerge from the same subcortical, mesolimbic substrates as trait impulsivity, are transdiagnostic vulnerabilities to both externalizing and internalizing spectrum disorders, and thus, heterotypic comorbidity.

Although chronic dysfunction in mesolimbic circuitry versus septohippocampal circuitry confer vulnerability to distinct trait-level predispositions (trait impulsivity and trait anxiety, respectively), functional interactions between neural systems affect how these vulnerabilities present (e.g., anhedonia in externalizing disorders contributes to excessive approach, whereas anhedonia in internalizing disorders contributes to passive avoidance). Investigations into neural correlates of Internalizing × Externalizing interactions are only beginning to emerge, and should shed light on how interdependent neural circuits influence one another in both clinical and healthy populations.

Cortical Moderators of Midbrain DA Activity

Until now, we have emphasized the roles of subcortical mesolimbic and (to a lesser extent) septohippocampal systems in conferring vulnerability to psychopathology. It is important to note, however, that many individuals who are high on trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and/or trait anxiety do not exhibit functional impairment (e.g., Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015; Dimoska & Johnstone, Reference Dimoska and Johnstone2007; Karcher, Martin, & Kerns, in press), and therefore do not meet diagnostic criteria for any psychiatric disorder (e.g., Bar-Haim et al., Reference Bar-Haim, Fox, Benson, Guyer, Williams and Nelson2009; Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Pruessner, Czechowska and Lepage2007).

These subcortical circuits have been conceptualized as bottom-up, emotion generation systems, which mediate approach- and avoidance-related affect. In contrast, inhibitory control over subcortical activity and reactivity is effected through prefrontal, top-down, emotion regulation systems. In order for approach and passive avoidance to become clinically problematic, they must occur in conjunction with poor cortically mediated emotion regulation (see, e.g., Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007, in press; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). Cortical regions of particular interest include (a) the ACC, which subserves conflict and performance-monitoring functions, including error detection and generation of regulating emotional responses (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, Reference Botvinick, Cohen and Carter2004; Etkin, Egner, & Kalisch Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011; Hajcak, McDonald, & Simons, Reference Hajcak, McDonald and Simons2004); (b) the OFC, which monitors affective value of incentives, subserves reward- and emotion-related decision-making functions, and overlaps structurally with portions of the ventromedial PFC (described below; Kringelbach, Reference Kringelbach2005; Koenigs & Grafman, Reference Koenigs and Grafman2009); (c) lateral regions of the PFC, including the ventrolateral PFC, which is involved in selecting and retrieving information, as well as volitional response inhibition, and the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), which is involved in higher order executive functions, such as working memory, integrating multiple sources of information, and organizing goal-directed behavior (Leung & Cai, Reference Leung and Cai2007; Tanji & Hoshi, Reference Tanji and Hoshi2008); and (d) the ventromedial PFC, which is associated with top-down emotion processing, including emotional reappraisal, expression, and regulation (Etkin et al., Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011). These regions either directly or indirectly modulate mesolimbic and septohippocampal function, and are therefore implicated in both externalizing and internalizing psychopathology.

Externalizing psychopathology

Volitional regulation of impulsivity requires modulation of the striatum by interconnected neural systems including the anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Heatherton, 2011; Heatherton & Wagner, 2011). When mesolimbic DA dysfunction is combined with deficient functioning of these cortical regions, externalizing psychopathology, beginning with ADHD and progressing to more severe externalizing disorders in high-risk environments, is especially likely (Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010, in press; Beauchaine, Neuhaus, et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Gatzke-Kopp, Reid, Brekke and Olliges2016; Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). This pattern of neural dysfunction is frequently exhibited among externalizers. For example, in addition to striatal abnormalities, which are widely reported in ADHD (see above), the disorder is also associated with ACC abnormalities, including hypofunctionality across a range of tasks (see Bush, Valera, & Seidman, Reference Bush, Valera and Seidman2005), reduced volumes (Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012; Makris et al., Reference Makris, Biederman, Valera, Bush, Kaiser and Kennedy2007; Seidman et al., Reference Seidman, Valera, Makris, Monuteaux, Boriel and Kelkar2006), and reduced connectivity between the ACC and striatum (e.g., Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Sauder, Beauchaine and Gatzke-Kopp2009). In addition, individuals with ADHD often display diminished error-related negativity, an electrophysiological signal generated by the ACC that reflects error monitoring (Shiels & Hawk, Reference Shiels and Hawk2010). Given the crucial role of this region in performance- and goal-related processing, disruptions in the ACC may contribute to altered ability to adjust one's behavior based on feedback, an ability that is compromised in ADHD and other externalizing disorders (e.g., Bush, Reference Bush2011; Gatzke-Kopp et al., Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Beauchaine, Shannon, Chipman-Chacon, Fleming and Crowell2009).

Higher order processing of incentives is mediated in large part by the OFC, a region that is also compromised in externalizing disorders. For example, individuals with ADHD have smaller OFC volumes (Hesslinger et al., Reference Hesslinger, Van Elst, Thiel, Haegele, Hennig and Ebert2002), but exhibit stronger functional connectivity between OFC, striatum, and ACC (Tomasi & Volkow, Reference Tomasi and Volkow2012). This is compatible with functional evidence from reward-based tasks suggesting that individuals with ADHD may also have OFCs that fail to differentiate between rewards of differing value (Wilbertz et al., Reference Wilbertz, van Elst, Delgado, Maier, Feige and Philipsen2012), and are hyperresponsive to certain types of gains (Ströhle et al., Reference Ströhle, Stoy, Wrase, Schwarzer, Schlagenhauf and Huss2008). Orbitofrontal dysfunction may contribute to increased saliency of rewards once those rewards are of sufficient magnitude to initiate mesolimbic reactivity.

Internalizing psychopathology

As discussed above, the vmPFC, OFC, and ACC are implicated in both reward processing and top-down emotion regulation. Unipolar depression is associated with dysfunctional reactions to emotion-evoking stimuli in regions related to affective processing, including the vmPFC, OFC, and ACC (e.g., see Kerestes et al., Reference Kerestes, Davey, Stephanou, Whittle and Harrison2014; Koenigs & Grafman, Reference Koenigs and Grafman2009; Murray, Wise, & Drevets, Reference Murray, Wise and Drevets2011; Stuhrmann, Suslow, & Dannlowski, Reference Stuhrmann, Suslow and Dannlowski2011). However, it is important to note that motivated/volitional regulatory control of emotions occurs through inhibition of the amygdala by lateral regions of the PFC (see Buhle et al., Reference Buhle, Silvers, Wager, Lopez, Onyemekwu and Kober2014; Clauss, Avery, & Blackford, Reference Clauss, Avery and Blackford2015; Heatherton, 2011; Heatherton & Wagner, 2011). The lateral PFC, primarily implicated in cognitive control, does not project directly to the amygdala, but rather exerts inhibitory effects though its connections with medial regions of the PFC, which have dense projections to and from the amygdala (Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin, & Davidson, Reference Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007; Öngür & Price, Reference Öngür and Price2000; Urry et al., Reference Urry, Van Reekum, Johnstone, Kalin, Thurow and Schaefer2006). For example, cognitive reappraisal of affective stimuli is associated with activation of the lateral PFC and deactivation of the amygdala (see Buhle et al., Reference Buhle, Silvers, Wager, Lopez, Onyemekwu and Kober2014), whereas unsuccessful cognitive reappraisal (increased negative emotion) may reflect failure of the lateral PFC to suppress amygdala responding (Wagner, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist, & Ochsner, Reference Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist and Ochsner2008).

Depression may involve failure of the lateral PFC to suppress the amygdala via the medial PFC (Johnstone et al., Reference Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007), an assertion supported by hypofunctionality of the lateral PFC and reduced functional connectivity between the dlPFC and the amygdala among depressed individuals (see Koenigs & Grafman, Reference Koenigs and Grafman2009; e.g., Dannlowski et al., Reference Dannlowski, Ohrmann, Konrad, Domschke, Bauer and Kugel2009; Johnstone et al., Reference Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007; Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer, & Thase, Reference Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer and Thase2007). Given the role of these lateral regions in regulating emotions, appraisals and attributions initiated by the dlPFC to dampen effects of negative, emotionally evocative stimuli among healthy individuals may not be applied effectively among those who are depressed, contributing to rumination and heightened negative affect (see Gotlib & Hamilton, Reference Gotlib and Hamilton2008; Hamilton et al., Reference Hamilton, Etkin, Furman, Lemus, Johnson and Gotlib2012).

Emotion regulation

Emotion regulation refers to processes that guide the experience and expression of emotions to facilitate adaptive behavior (Thompson, Reference Thompson and Thompson1990). Emotion regulation skills, subserved by effective cortical regulation of subcortical emotion generation circuits, protect against development of psychopathology (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). As noted above, subcortical vulnerabilities to trait impulsivity, anxiety, and anhedonia are necessary but insufficient to produce functional impairment. Rather, the combination of subcortical dysfunction and deficient top-down modulation of subcortical systems makes psychopathology most likely (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, 2015; Beauchaine & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine and Gatzke-Kopp2012; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Fineberg et al., Reference Fineberg, Potenza, Chamberlain, Berlin, Menzies and Bechara2010). This reflects a primary role of the prefrontal cortex: to appropriately modulate/inhibit responses in the service of effective behavior and emotion regulation (see, e.g., Dillon & Pizzagalli, Reference Dillon and Pizzagalli2007; Etkin et al., Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011; Schore, Reference Schore1996).

Developmental and environmental considerations

The brain undergoes profound structural changes during development, with cortical regions reaching maturity comparatively later than phylogenetically older, subcortical structures (Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein and Vaituzis2004; Toga, Thompson, & Sowell, Reference Toga, Thompson and Sowell2006). Among typically developing individuals, peak volume of the PFC occurs around ages 10–11, and neural maturation into adulthood includes cortical thinning in the parietal and right dorsal frontal areas, and cortical thickening in language-processing areas in the frontal and temporal lobes (see Lenroot & Giedd, Reference Lenroot and Giedd2006; Toga et al., Reference Toga, Thompson and Sowell2006). Adolescence in particular is a developmental period marked by a number of neural changes, including enhanced DA innervation of the frontal cortex and increased DA concentrations in cortical and subcortical regions (Wahlstrom, Collins, White, & Luciana, Reference Wahlstrom, Collins, White and Luciana2010).

Underdevelopment of prefrontal regions that subserve self-regulatory functions, combined with low mesolimbic DA activity/reactivity, is a shared vulnerability to impulsivity and anhedonia in internalizing and externalizing disorders across the life span (see Beauchaine et al., in press). For example, in adolescence, the absence of fully intact self-regulatory regions, which continue to develop into adulthood (Lenroot & Giedd, Reference Lenroot and Giedd2006), combined with normal upregulation of DA functioning, lead to increased saliency of and motivation to seek rewards that then manifest as increased risk-taking and novelty-seeking behaviors (see Chambers, Taylor, & Potenza, Reference Chambers, Taylor and Potenza2003; Wahlstrom et al., Reference Wahlstrom, Collins, White and Luciana2010). Thus, among typically developing adolescents, impulsive tendencies are subserved by DA hyperresponsivity to reward unchecked by inhibitory mechanisms. Similarly, Forbes and Dahl (2012) suggest that adolescents with low reward responding are vulnerable to depression because of imbalances in frontostriatal reward circuitry, which are especially discrepant compared with those of typically developing individuals during adolescence. In other words, a trajectory toward depression may be more likely among individuals with inherent mesolimbic DA vulnerability, coupled with underdevelopment of higher order cortical regions involved in self-regulation and emotion regulation.

Like depression, externalizing vulnerability results at least in part from underdevelopment of frontostriatal circuitry (see, e.g., Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor, & Rubia, Reference Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor and Rubia2012). Although ~65% of adults who experienced childhood ADHD maintain at least some symptoms into adulthood (Faraone, Biederman, & Mick, Reference Faraone, Biederman and Mick2006), individuals with ADHD whose symptoms remit with age may gain self-regulatory skills that match their unaffected peers through eventual maturation of frontostriatal circuitry and/or recruitment of alternative circuitry to compensate for enduring frontostriatal deficits (Vaidya, Reference Vaidya, Stanford and Tannock2012). For example, the medial PFC, which has direct projections to the striatum, develops more slowly among children with ADHD, and children with more impairing and persistent ADHD-related impairment have medial PFCs that are and remain thinner into adolescence compared to those with better outcomes and controls (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lerch, Greenstein, Sharp, Clasen and Evans2006, Reference Shaw, Malek, Watson, Sharp, Evans and Greenstein2012). Similarly, fronto-striato-parietal dysfunction among adults with persistent ADHD correlates negatively with the severity of ADHD symptoms (Cubillo et al., Reference Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor and Rubia2012; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Krick, Retz, Hengesch, Retz-Junginger and Reith2010). Finally, among boys with CD, most of whom experience homotypic comorbidity with other externalizing disorders, normal maturation (gray matter pruning) of the PFC is not observed in late childhood/early adolescence (De Brito et al., Reference De Brito, Mechelli, Wilke, Laurens, Jones and Barker2009). Thus, failures of prefrontal maturation influence the presence, persistence, and severity of symptoms of both internalizing and externalizing disorders.

Given the significance of bottom-up and top-down neural function in the expression of psychopathology, it is important to address environmental influences on prefrontal and subcortical neurodevelopment. Early-life adversity, including child abuse and neglect, malnutrition, and prenatal exposure to neurotoxins, substances of abuse, and high maternal stress, are associated with epigenetic changes that can have profound effects on neural development and function (see Archer, Oscar-Berman, Blum, & Gold, 2012; Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011). For instance, the structure and function of DA pathways are altered by both chronic and acute stress, possibly through physiological responses enacted by the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Pani, Porcella, & Gessa, Reference Pani, Porcella and Gessa2000). Environmental insults can result in hyper- and hypofunctioning of DA neural circuitry, depending on the severity and chronicity of insult, and are associated with increased risk for psychopathologies characterized by aberrant motivation/reward-processing, including ADHD, substance abuse, and depression (see Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Zalewski, Crowell and Potapova2011; Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011).

Physical abuse in childhood predicts smaller OFC volumes, which in turn are associated with social difficulties with peers and executive functioning deficits (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Chung, Avants, Shirtcliff, Gee and Davidson2010). In addition, adversity accrued across childhood is associated with smaller PFC volumes (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Chung, Avants, Rudolph, Shirtcliff and Gee2012), as is being raised in poor neighborhoods (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Hair, Shen, Shi, Gilmore and Wolfe2013). Moreover, neglect in childhood is associated with more diffuse frontal white matter organization (Hanson, Adluru, et al., 2013), and chronic exposure to stress results in less dendritic branching and lower neural spine densities in the PFC among rodents (e.g., Holmes & Wellman, Reference Holmes and Wellman2009).

Neural vulnerabilities to psychopathology that emerge early in development may be exacerbated by environmental stress to make psychopathology more likely. For example, among rodents, a history of chronic stress predicts functional alterations in neural, behavioral, and pharmacological responses to acute stress (e.g., Finlay, Zigmond, & Abercrombie, Reference Finlay, Zigmond and Abercrombie1995; Isgor, Kabbaj, Akil, & Watson, Reference Isgor, Kabbaj, Akil and Watson2004; Katz, Roth, & Carroll, Reference Katz, Roth and Carroll1981). Similarly, among humans, exposure to childhood trauma predicts aberrant tonic HPA axis functioning and physiological sensitization to stress, which in turn predict symptoms of depression (see Heim & Binder, Reference Heim and Binder2012; Heim, Newport, Mletzko, Miller, & Nemeroff, Reference Heim, Newport, Mletzko, Miller and Nemeroff2008). Such findings suggest that early-life stressors can elicit neural and physiological changes that affect responses to acute stressors, which may then increase risk for the emergence of psychiatric symptoms (Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Zalewski, Crowell, & Potapova, 2011).

Neural vulnerabilities for psychopathology that emerge early in development can also be exacerbated by the psychopathology that they portend once psychiatric illness develops. As reviewed above, for example, low mesolimbic function, an important neural substrate of trait impulsivity, confers vulnerability to all externalizing disorders, including SUDs (Zisner & Beauchaine, Reference Zisner, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2015). Strong and recurrent exposure to stimulants provokes downregulation of tonic DA activity in the NAcc, but sensitized phasic DA release specifically to these stimulants (see Thomas, Beurrier, Bonci, & Malenka Reference Thomas, Beurrier, Bonci and Malenka2001; Vezina, Reference Vezina2004). These changes, combined with the diminished PFC regulatory functioning that occurs in addiction, lead to greater impulsive decision making and higher risk of relapse (see Goldstein & Volkow, Reference Goldstein and Volkow2011; Kalivas, Reference Kalivas2008; Schoenbaum & Shaham, Reference Schoenbaum and Shaham2008). Among individuals with tonic DA levels that are inherently low, neural dysfunction elicited by substance dependence may be particularly devastating.

Interim conclusions

Heterotypic comorbidity emerges at least in part from a common neural vulnerability (low tonic DA and blunted DA-mediated responses to anticipation of incentives) with concurrent impairments in other neural circuits (e.g., hyperresponsitivity of the septohippocampal system to negative stimuli), which manifest as both overlapping and independent psychopathological symptoms. However, subcortically mediated deficits in approach and avoidance behavior are not necessarily impairing in the absence of co-occurring PFC dysfunction given critical roles of specific PFC regions in self-regulation and emotion regulation. Without PFC dysfunction, individual differences in trait impulsivity produce ordinary variation in personality, not psychopathology (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine and Gatzke-Kopp2012; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007). Similarly, trait anxiety and negative affectivity, which are vulnerabilities to unipolar depression, are less likely to result in functional impairment in the presence of strong emotion regulation capabilities. Furthermore, cumulative effects of neurobiological vulnerabilities, such as the degree to which an individual produces blunted mesolimbic responses or excessive septohippocampal responses, within the context of environmental risk factors, such as childhood trauma and substance abuse, influence the ways in which such neural deficits develop, manifest, and are maintained (see Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015a; Beauchaine et al., Reference Neuhaus, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Sauder et al., Reference Sauder, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Shannon and Aylward2012; Shannon, Beauchaine, Brenner, Neuhaus, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Shannon, Beauchaine, Brenner, Neuhaus and Gatzke-Kopp2007).

Implications

Advances in neuroscience provide increasing support for transdiagnostic approaches to psychopathology (see, e.g., Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine and Quinn2010). The framework presented here implies that diagnosing psychiatric illness purely on the basis of behavioral symptoms (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, anhedonia, or depressed mood) obscures common etiological mechanisms among what have traditionally been classified as distinct diagnostic entities. For example, low tonic mesolimbic DA and diminished phasic responding to incentives are neural substrates of both externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression, and contribute to heterotypic comorbidity. This may help to explain why externalizing disorders confer vulnerability to later depression (e.g., Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Molina, Pelham, Applegate, Dahlke and Overmyer2010; Meinzer et al., Reference Meinzer, Lewinsohn, Pettit, Seeley, Gau and Chronis-Tuscano2013; Seymour, Chronis-Tuscano, Iwamoto, Kurdziel, & MacPherson, Reference Seymour, Chronis-Tuscano, Iwamoto, Kurdziel and MacPherson2014).

Failure to distinguish between heterogeneous subgroups, such as depressed adolescents who engage in SII versus those who do not, may also obscure important etiological substrates of psychopathology (see, e.g., Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Hsiao, Vasilev, Yaptangco and Linehan2012). Heterogeneity within current diagnostic classes is an ongoing challenge in molecular genetics research on all psychiatric disorders (see, e.g., Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Gizer, Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Gizer, Beauchaine and Hinshawin press). Behavioral genetics studies yield strong evidence for heritable influences on psychopathology, including externalizing disorders, unipolar depression, and their comorbidity, yet molecular genetics research explains very little of this heritability by specific alleles (Faraone & Mick, Reference Faraone and Mick2010; Sullivan, Neale, & Kendler, Reference Sullivan, Neale and Kendler2000; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Pergadia, Blackwood, Penninx, Gordon and Nyholt2012). In the spirit of the Research Domain Criteria initiative (e.g., Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine and Quinn2010), greater attention to common genetic, biological/physiological, and trait-level vulnerabilities that span across diagnoses, instead of searching within single, behaviorally heterogeneous diagnoses, is expected to provide greater insight into etiological underpinnings of mental illness.

Issues of group heterogeneity also emerge when we fail to consider that multiple combinations of vulnerabilities and risk factors can produce equifinal outcomes. With regard to the framework described herein, we emphasize the importance of distinguishing between bottom-up, emotion-generation processes versus top-down emotion-regulation processes. Subcortical mesolimbic DA function confers individual differences in trait impulsivity, trait anhedonia, and trait irritability, which may be expressed as psychopathology when coupled with anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex functional deficiencies.

Greater appreciation for etiological similarities and differences between complex psychological and behavioral phenomena has meaningful implications for research and clinical practice (for a detailed discussion, see Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Brenner, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Brenner and Gatzke-Kopp2008). For instance, when individuals are studied on the basis of their etiological similarities instead of behavioral/diagnostic similarities, more homogeneous research samples may be obtained. This reduces interindividual variability and increases statistical power, allowing for easier detection of interactions among variables. Furthermore, when relevant biological vulnerabilities are similar across diagnostically distinct groups, other variables, such as environmental risk factors, can be studied more closely to identify how specific factors transact with biological vulnerabilities to protect against or potentiate psychopathology. For example, environmental risk factors, including coercive/ineffective parenting, deviant peer group affiliations, and neighborhood criminality, all mediate the relationship between ADHD and more severe externalizing comportment across development (Beauchaine et al., 2009, in press; Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013). In the absence of these risk factors, ADHD may not progress to more serious externalizing disorders.

Finally, individuals with incipient or existing psychopathology will be more easily identified when reliable, predictive biomarkers and risk factors are found. Such advances can also guide prevention and treatment programs that target specific deficits and needs of individuals with or at elevated risk for psychiatric illness, and restrict these programs to only individuals who are likely to benefit from them.

Artifactual, Spurious, and True Comorbidity Revisited

Despite largely nonoverlapping diagnostic criteria, externalizing spectrum disorders and unipolar depression frequently co-occur. Historically, this has been perplexing, especially considering that these disorders are commonly attributed to different neural substrates. Here, we assert that high rates of heterotypic comorbidity between externalizing disorders and unipolar depression can be accounted for in part by a common neural substrate: low tonic mesolimbic DA and attenuated DA responses to anticipation of reward, which are associated with trait-level vulnerabilities (high impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability) common to a number of psychiatric syndromes spanning diagnostic boundaries.

At the beginning of this article, we outlined three subtypes of comorbidity, including (a) artifactual (mistakenly splitting one disease entity into multiple diagnoses), (b) spurious (assigning shared diagnostic criteria to distinct disease entities), and (c) true (when an individual suffers from separate disease entities). When we consider psychopathology from a transdiagnostic perspective in which neural vulnerabilities cut across traditional psychiatric boundaries, heterotypic comorbidity between externalizing disorders and unipolar depression, at least for some individuals, is likely more artifactual than real. Behavioral genetics studies indicate shared heritability, and mesolimbic neural response patterns that give rise to trait impulsivity, anhedonia, and irritability, are shared, not distinct. The convergence of findings from the externalizing and depression literatures, across multiple levels of analysis, supports common trait-level vulnerabilities as substrates for heterotypic comorbidity, and contributes to growing support for translational and transdiagnostic perspectives in psychopathology research.

Footnotes

1. Although we restrict our discussion of internalizing disorders primarily to unipolar depression given space constraints, we acknowledge high rates of comorbidity between unipolar depression and anxiety (see, e.g., Hankin et al., 2016). We do not consider bipolar depression, which is distinct etiologically from unipolar depression, and is characterized by different patterns of heritability and central nervous system function (e.g., Cuellar, Johnson, & Winters, Reference Cuellar, Johnson and Winters2005).

2. A core tenet of developmental psychopathology is that multiple etiological pathways can lead to phenotypically similar, equifinal outcomes (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). The model elaborated herein is not intended to explain all cases of heterotypic comorbidity; rather, it describes one common pathway (see Beauchaine et al., in press).

3. We exclude the inattentive presentation of ADHD given consistent evidence that it is distinct etiologically from the hyperactive/impulsive and combined presentations (see, e.g., Diamond, Reference Diamond2005; Lee, Burns, Beauchaine, & Becker, Reference Lee, Burns, Beauchaine and Becker2015; Milich, Balentine, & Lynam, Reference Milich, Balentine and Lynam2001). Thus, throughout this article ADHD refers to the hyperactive/impulsive and combined presentations.

4. Herein, reward circuitry is restricted to discussion of networks that respond to incentives. Although brain regions implicated in punishment overlap with those for reward, punishment is not an emphasis of this work.

5. Although older conceptions of positive and negative affectivity implied/proposed that the constructs represent poles of a single dimension, neuroscientific evidence suggests that the brain comprises broad systems that engage flexibly to process valence-non-specific content, and that no single affect-processing region responds exclusively to positive or negative stimuli (Lindquist, Satpute, Wager, Weber, & Barrett, Reference Lindquist, Satpute, Wager, Weber and Barrett2015). Instead, certain brain regions/subregions, and individual neurons, may exhibit some degree of relative preference for valence-specific stimuli (Bartra, McGuire, & Kable, Reference Bartra, McGuire and Kable2013; Chikazoe, Lee, Kriegeskorte, & Anderson, Reference Chikazoe, Lee, Kriegeskorte and Anderson2014; Morrison & Salzman, Reference Morrison and Salzman2009). Inclusion in the positive and negative valence systems of the Research Domain Criteria highlights the importance of fine-tuning models of emotion across multiple levels of analysis to determine relationships between positive and negative affect (National Institute of Mental Health, 2014).

6. Incentives can be primary reinforcers, such as food and sex, which relate directly to survival and procreation, or secondary reinforcers, such as money, beauty, and social approval, which do not relate directly to survival (Krach, Paulus, Bodden, & Kircher, Reference Krach, Paulus, Bodden and Kircher2010; Kühn & Gallinat, Reference Kühn and Gallinat2012; Liu, Hairston, Schrier, & Fan, Reference Liu, Hairston, Schrier and Fan2011). Like primary reinforcers, secondary reinforcers elicit activation in the VS and other brain regions implicated in reward processing, including the OFC and cingulate cortex. Neural processing of social reward is more complex (see Krach et al., Reference Krach, Paulus, Bodden and Kircher2010) and is not considered in this article.

7. Pharmacologic agonism and antagonism of DA has produced equivocal behavioral and neurobiological results, often complicating interpretations of the relation between midbrain DA function and impulsive behavior. One way to reconcile these findings is to consider differences between acute and chronic administration of pharmacologic agents. For example, a single, large dose of amphetamine increases D1 receptor binding in the rat striatum, whereas chronic amphetamine administration decreases D1 receptor binding in the same region (Howlett & Nahorski, Reference Howlett and Nahorski1979). Another consideration concerns dose dependency. For example, low versus high doses of the DA agonists, including d-amphetamine and methylphenidate, elicit opposing effects on neural firing rates in the caudate nucleus/putamen of the striatum (Rebec & Segal, Reference Rebec and Segal1978). Furthermore, factors related to administration scheduling and dose can interact to produce additional complexities. Following administration of chronic low-dose amphetamine, acute increases in amphetamine elicit either depressed or enhanced neural firing in the caudate nucleus/putamen, whereas administration of chronic high-dose amphetamine followed by acute increases in amphetamine elicit neural excitation in this region (Kamata & Rebec, Reference Kamata and Rebec1983). Such factors, in addition to individual differences among animals (see text), and inherent difficulties measuring impulsivity in animal models, must be considered.

8. It is important to note that fMRI does not assess neurotransmitter function, whereas PET and single photon emission computed tomography do.

9. Eisenberger et al. (Reference Eisenberger, Berkman, Inagaki, Rameson, Mashal and Irwin2010) operationalized anhedonia as reduced VS activation during reward anticipation, and did not quantify anhedonia through self-report and/or observer ratings. Instead, participants reported the extent to which they felt unhappy, blue, lonely, gloomy, and worthless, and observers rated the extent to which participants appeared unhappy and gloomy.

10. Although a sample consisting of BPD-only participants is preferable for the present discussion, BPD and ASPD share core etiological substrates, and may reflect sex-specific manifestations of a common pathophysiology (see Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009). Thus, collapsing across these groups may still yield interpretable findings for BPD.

11. The amygdala, also called the amygdaloid complex, comprises distinct groupings of cells, including the basolateral and central nuclei, which serve specific, but related functions related to emotion processing (Sah, Faber, Lopez de Armentia, & Power, Reference Sah, Faber, De Armentia and Power2003). The basolateral amygdala, which has received the most attention in the depression literature thus far, may best be conceptualized as the amygdala itself, with projections to target regions that enable more specialized functions (Davis & Whalen, Reference Davis and Whalen2001). For example, the basolateral amygdala projects to the striatum, leading to instrumental approach behaviors, whereas its projections to the central amygdala are involved in attending to salient information and generating fear responses (Davis & Whalen, Reference Davis and Whalen2001). Although these functional differences are relevant, the amygdala is not typically subdivided into specific nuclei in human neuroimaging, and thus is not specified in the present work unless relevant for discussion.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (2011). Child Behavior Checklist. In Kreutzer, J. S., DeLuca, J., & Caplan, B. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 546552). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlingron, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Adriani, W., Boyer, F., Gioiosa, L., Macri, S., Dreyer, J. L., & Laviola, G. (2009). Increased impulsive behavior and risk proneness following lentivirus-mediated dopamine transporter over-expression in rats’ nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience, 159, 4758.Google Scholar
Allely, C. S. (2014). The association of ADHD symptoms to self-harm behaviours: A systematic PRISMA review. BMC Psychiatry, 14, 133.Google Scholar
Ambrosini, P. J., Bennett, D. S., & Elia, J. (2013). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder characteristics: II. Clinical correlates of irritable mood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 145, 7076.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 5787.Google Scholar
Archer, T., Oscar-Berman, M., Blum, K., & Gold, M. (2012). Neurogenics and epigenetics of impulsive behaviour: Impact on reward circuitry. Journal of Genetic Syndromes and Gene Therapy, 3, 1000115.Google Scholar
Arnone, D., McKie, S., Elliott, R., Thomas, E. J., Downey, D., Juhasz, G., et al. (2012). Increased amygdala responses to sad but not fearful faces in major depression: Relation to mood state and pharmacological treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 841850.Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. (2006). Stimulants: Therapeutic actions in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 23762383.Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529550.Google Scholar
Bar-Haim, Y., Fox, N. A., Benson, B., Guyer, A. E., Williams, A., Nelson, E. E., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of reward processing in adolescents with a history of inhibited temperament. Psychological Science, 20, 10091018.Google Scholar
Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2013). The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage, 76, 412427.Google Scholar
Baskin-Sommers, A., & Foti, D. (2015). Abnormal reward functioning across substance use and major depression: Considering reward as a transdiagnostic mechanism. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 227239.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray's motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183214.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2012). Physiological markers of emotion and behavior dysregulation in externalizing psychopathology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 7986.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015a). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, 875896.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015b). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: A transdiagnostic biomarker of emotion dysregulation and psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 4347.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2012). Instantiating the multiple levels of analysis perspective in a program of study on externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 10031018.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., & Gizer, I. (in press). Genetic and environmental influences on behavior. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007). Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74, 174184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Hinshaw, S. P., & Pang, K. L. (2010). Comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and early-onset conduct disorder: Biological, environmental, and developmental mechanisms. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 17, 327336.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Katkin, E. S., Strassberg, Z., & Snarr, J. (2001). Disinhibitory psychopathology in male adolescents: Discriminating conduct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder through concurrent assessment of multiple autonomic states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 610624.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., & Klein, D. N. (in press). Classifying psychopathology: The DSM, empirically-based taxonomies, and the Research Domain Criteria. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Klein, D. N., Crowell, S. E., Derbidge, C., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2009). Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A Biology × Sex × Environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 735770.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Schizotypy, taxometrics, and disconfirming theories in soft science: Comment on Rawlings, Williams, Haslam, and Claridge. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 16521662.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., & Marsh, P. (2006). Taxometric methods: Enhancing early detection and prevention of psychopathology by identifying latent vulnerability traits. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 931967). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., & McNulty, T. (2013). Comorbidities and continuities as ontogenic processes: Toward a developmental spectrum model of externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 15051528.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., & Neuhaus, E. (in press). Impulsivity and vulnerability to psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Neuhaus, E., Brenner, S. L., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2008). Ten good reasons to consider biological processes in prevention and intervention research. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 745774.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Neuhaus, E., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Reid, M. J., Brekke, A., Olliges, A., et al. (2016). Electrodermal responding predicts responses to, and may be altered by, preschool intervention for ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 293303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Neuhaus, E., Zalewski, M., Crowell, S. E., & Potapova, N. (2011). The effects of allostatic load on neural systems subserving motivation, mood regulation, and social affiliation. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 975999.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Shader, T. M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2016). An ontogenic process model of externalizing psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 485501). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic biomarker of psychopathology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 338350.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Zisner, A., & Sauder, C. L. (in press). Trait impulsivity and the externalizing spectrum. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology.Google Scholar
Beaulieu, J. M., & Gainetdinov, R. R. (2011). The physiology, signaling, and pharmacology of dopamine receptors. Pharmacological Reviews, 63, 182217.Google Scholar
Beck, A., Schlagenhauf, F., Wustenberg, T., Hein, J., Kienast, T., Kahnt, T., et al. (2009). Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 734742.Google Scholar
Benegal, V., Antony, G., Venkatasubramanian, G., & Jayakumar, P. N. (2007). Imaging study: Gray matter volume abnormalities and externalizing symptoms in subjects at high risk for alcohol dependence. Addiction Biology, 12, 122132.Google Scholar
Berlin, I., Givry-Steiner, L., Lecrubier, Y., & Puech, A. J. (1998). Measures of anhedonia and hedonic responses to sucrose in depressive and schizophrenic patients in comparison with healthy subjects. European Psychiatry, 13, 303309.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron, 86, 646664.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28, 309369.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 507513.Google Scholar
Bettencourt, B., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 751777.Google Scholar
Bevan, L. W. (1899). A textbook of mental diseases. London: Charles Griffin.Google Scholar
Bezdjian, S., Baker, L. A., Lozano, D. I., & Raine, A. (2009). Assessing inattention and impulsivity in children during the Go/NoGo task. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 365383.Google Scholar
Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 447454.Google Scholar
Biederman, J., Ball, S. W., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T. J., McCreary, M., et al. (2008). New insights into the comorbidity between ADHD and major depression in adolescent and young adult females. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 426434.Google Scholar
Björklund, A., & Dunnett, S. B. (2007). Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: An update. Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 194202.Google Scholar
Blackman, G. L., Ostrander, R., & Herman, K. C. (2005). Children with ADHD and depression: A multisource, multimethod assessment of clinical, social, and academic functioning. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8, 195207.Google Scholar
Bleuler, E. (1911). Dementia praecox oder gruppe der schizophrenien/dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Blum, K., Braverman, E. R., Holder, J. M., Lubar, J. F., Monastra, V. J., Miller, D., et al. (2000). The reward deficiency syndrome: A biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive and compulsive behaviors. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 32(S1), 1112.Google Scholar
Blum, K., Chen, A. L. C., Braverman, E. R., Comings, D. E., Chen, T. J., Arcuri, V., et al. (2008). Attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder and reward deficiency syndrome. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 4, 893918.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539546.Google Scholar
Brenner, S. L., Beauchaine, T. P., & Sylvers, P. D. (2005). A comparison of psychophysiological and self-report measures of BAS and BIS activation. Psychophysiology, 42, 108115.Google Scholar
Bress, J. N., Meyer, A., & Proudfit, G. H. (2015). The stability of the feedback negativity and its relationship with depression during childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Bress, J. N., Smith, E., Foti, D., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Neural response to reward and depressive symptoms in late childhood to early adolescence. Biological Psychology, 89, 156162.Google Scholar
Bressan, R. A., & Crippa, J. A. (2005). The role of dopamine in reward and pleasure behaviour—Review of data from preclinical research. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 111(S427), 1421.Google Scholar
Brown, M. Z., Comtois, K. A., & Linehan, M. M. (2002). Reasons for suicide attempts and nonsuicidal self-injury in women with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 198202.Google Scholar
Buckner, J. D., Joiner, T. E., Pettit, J. W., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Schmidt, N. B. (2008). Implications of the DSM's emphasis on sadness and anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 159, 2530.Google Scholar
Bucknill, J. C., & Tuke, D. H. (1874). A manual of psychological medicine. London: Churchill Google Scholar
Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H., et al. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 29812990.Google Scholar
Bush, G. (2011). Cingulate, frontal, and parietal cortical dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 11601167.Google Scholar
Bush, G., Valera, E. M., & Seidman, L. J. (2005). Functional neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and suggested future directions. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 12731284.Google Scholar
Cagniard, B., Balsam, P. D., Brunner, D., & Zhuang, X. (2006). Mice with chronically elevated dopamine exhibit enhanced motivation, but not learning, for a food reward. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 13621370.Google Scholar
Calabrese, E. J. (2001). Dopamine: Biphasic dose responses. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 31, 563583.Google Scholar
Campbell, S., Marriott, M., Nahmias, C., & MacQueen, G. M. (2004). Lower hippocampal volume in patients suffering from depression: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 598607.Google Scholar
Cannon, C. M., & Palmiter, R. D. (2003). Reward without dopamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1082710831.Google Scholar
Cardinal, R. N., Pennicott, D. R., Lakmali, C., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (2001). Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science, 292, 24992501.Google Scholar
Carmona, S., Hoekzema, E., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Richarte, V., Canals, C., Bosch, R., et al. (2012). Response inhibition and reward anticipation in medication-naïve adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A within-subject case-control neuroimaging study. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 23502361.Google Scholar
Carmona, S., Proal, E., Hoekzema, E. A., Gispert, J. D., Picado, M., Moreno, I., et al. (2009). Ventro-striatal reductions underpin symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 972977.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J. (2015). Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 295319.Google Scholar
Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 10411052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, Y.-F., Dennis, M. L., & Funk, R. R. (2008). Prevalence and comorbidity of major internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and adults presenting to substance abuse treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 34, 1424.Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Raulin, M. L. (1976). Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 374382.Google Scholar
Charney, D. S., & Manji, H. K. (2004). Life stress, genes, and depression: Multiple pathways lead to increased risk and new opportunities for intervention. Science Signaling, 225, re5.Google Scholar
Chentsova-Dutton, Y., & Hanley, K. (2010). The effects of anhedonia and depression on hedonic responses. Psychiatry Research, 179, 176180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chikazoe, J., Lee, D. H., Kriegeskorte, N., & Anderson, A. K. (2014). Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 11141122.Google Scholar
Chronis-Tuscano, A., Molina, B. S., Pelham, W. E., Applegate, B., Dahlke, A., Overmyer, M., et al. (2010). Very early predictors of adolescent depression and suicide attempts in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 10441051.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597600.Google Scholar
Clark, D. C., Fawcett, J., Salazar-Grueso, E., & Fawcett, E. (1984). Seven-month clinical outcome of anhedonic and normally hedonic depressed inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 12161220.Google Scholar
Clark, L. A. (2005). Temperament as a unifying basis for personality and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 505521.Google Scholar
Clauss, J. A., Avery, S. N., & Blackford, J. U. (2015). The nature of individual differences in inhibited temperament and risk for psychiatric disease: A review and meta-analysis. Progress in Neurobiology, 127, 2345.Google Scholar
Clouston, T. S. (1896). Clinical lectures on mental diseases. London: Churchill.Google Scholar
Cohn, M. D., Veltman, D. J., Pape, L. E., van Lith, K., Vermeiren, R. R. J. M., van den Brink, W., et al. (2015). Incentive processing in persistent disruptive behavior and psychopathic traits: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in adolescents. Biological Psychiatry, 78, 615624.Google Scholar
Conner, K. R., Meldrum, S., Wieczorek, W. F., Duberstein, P. R., & Welte, J. W. (2004). The association of irritability and impulsivity with suicidal ideation among 15- to 20-year-old males. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 34, 363373.Google Scholar
Corr, P. J., & McNaughton, N. (2016). Neural mechanisms of low trait anxiety and risk for externalizing behavior. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 220238). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Correa, M., Carlson, B. B., Wisniecki, A., & Salamone, J. D. (2002). Nucleus accumbens dopamine and work requirements on interval schedules. Behavioural Brain Research, 137, 179187.Google Scholar
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837844.Google Scholar
Crowell, S., Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Sylvers, P., Mead, H., & Chipman-Chacon, J. (2006). Autonomic correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 174178.Google Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., Hsiao, R. C. J., Vasilev, C. A., Yaptangco, M., Linehan, M. M., et al. (2012). Differentiating adolescent self-injury from adolescent depression: Possible implications for borderline personality development. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 4557.Google Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., & Linehan, M. (2009). A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending Linehan's theory. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 495510.Google Scholar
Crowell, S., Beauchaine, T. P., McCauley, E., Smith, C., Stevens, A. L., & Sylvers, P. (2005). Psychological, autonomic, and serotonergic correlates of parasuicidal behavior in adolescent girls. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 11051127.Google Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., McCauley, E., Smith, C. J., Vasilev, C. A., & Stevens, A. L. (2008). Parent–child interactions, peripheral serotonin, and self-inflicted injury in adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 1521.Google Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Derbidge, C., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2014). Developmental approaches to understanding self-injury and suicidal behaviors. In Nock, M. K. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of suicide and self-injury (pp. 183205). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Kaufman, E., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2014). A biopsychosocial model of borderline personality development: Theory and empirical evidence. In Tackett, J. & Sharp, C. (Eds.), Handbook of borderline personality disorder in children and adolescents (pp. 143158). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Kaufman, E. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2013). The development of borderline personality and self-inflicted injury. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 577609). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Smith, A., Taylor, E., & Rubia, K. (2012). A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. Cortex, 48, 194215.Google Scholar
Cuellar, A. K., Johnson, S. L., & Winters, R. (2005). Distinctions between bipolar and unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 307339.Google Scholar
Dalley, J. W., Fryer, T. D., Brichard, L., Robinson, E. S., Theobald, D. E., Lääne, K., et al. (2007). Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science, 315, 12671270.Google Scholar
Dalley, J. W., Mar, A. C., Economidou, D., & Robbins, T. W. (2008). Neurobehavioral mechanisms of impulsivity: Fronto-striatal systems and functional neurochemistry. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 90, 250260.Google Scholar
D'Amour-Horvat, V., & Leyton, M. (2014). Impulsive actions and choices in laboratory animals and humans: Effects of high vs. low dopamine states produced by systemic treatments given to neurologically intact subjects. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 432.Google Scholar
Dannlowski, U., Ohrmann, P., Konrad, C., Domschke, K., Bauer, J., Kugel, H., et al. (2009). Reduced amygdala-prefrontal coupling in major depression: Association with MAOA genotype and illness severity. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 12, 1122.Google Scholar
Davis, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 1334.Google Scholar
Daviss, W. B. (2008). A review of co-morbid depression in pediatric ADHD: Etiologies, phenomenology, and treatment. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 18, 565571.Google Scholar
De Brito, S. A., Mechelli, A., Wilke, M., Laurens, K. R., Jones, A. P., Barker, G. J., et al. (2009). Size matters: Increased grey matter in boys with conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits. Brain, 132, 843852.Google Scholar
Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 47, 765780.Google Scholar
Denk, F., Walton, M. E., Jennings, K. A., Sharp, T., Rushworth, M. F. S., & Bannerman, D. M. (2005). Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort. Psychopharmacology, 179, 587596.Google Scholar
de la Cruz, L. F., Simonoff, E., McGough, J. J., Halperin, J. M., Arnold, L. E., & Stringaris, A. (2015). Treatment of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and irritability: Results from the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 6270.Google Scholar
De Pauw, S. S., & Mervielde, I. (2010). Temperament, personality and developmental psychopathology: A review based on the conceptual dimensions underlying childhood traits. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 41, 313329.Google Scholar
Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 485498.Google Scholar
Derbidge, C. M., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2014). A developmental model of self-inflicted injury, borderline personality, and suicide risk. In Lewis, M. & Rudolph, K. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 521542). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2005). Attention-deficit disorder (ADHD without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from ADHD (with hyperactivity). Development and Psychopathology, 17, 807825.Google Scholar
Dichter, G. S., Kozink, R. V., McClernon, F. J., & Smoski, M. J. (2012). Remitted major depression is characterized by reward network hyperactivation during reward anticipation and hypoactivation during reward outcomes. Journal of Affective Disorders, 136, 11261134.Google Scholar
Dichter, G. S., Smoski, M. J., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Gallop, R., & Garbutt, J. C. (2010). Unipolar depression does not moderate responses to the Sweet Taste Test. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 859863.Google Scholar
Diergaarde, L., Pattij, T., Poortvliet, I., Hogenboom, F., de Vries, W., Schoffelmeer, A. N., et al. (2008). Impulsive choice and impulsive action predict vulnerability to distinct stages of nicotine seeking in rats. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 301308.Google Scholar
Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2007). Inhibition of action, thought, and emotion: A selective neurobiological review. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12, 99114.Google Scholar
Dimoska, A., & Johnstone, S. J. (2007). Neural mechanisms underlying trait impulsivity in non-clinical adults: Stop-signal performance and event-related potentials. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 31, 443454.Google Scholar
Dombrovski, A. Y., Clark, L., Siegle, G. J., Butters, M. A., Ichikawa, N., Sahakian, B. J., et al. (2010). Reward/punishment reversal learning in older suicide attempters. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 699707.Google Scholar
Downson, J. H., & Blackwell, A. D. (2010). Impulsive aggression in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121, 103110.Google Scholar
Drevets, W. C., Gautier, C., Price, J. C., Kupfer, D. J., Kinahan, P. E., Grace, A. A., et al. (2001). Amphetamine-induced dopamine release in human ventral striatum correlates with euphoria. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 8196.Google Scholar
Dryman, A., & Eaton, W. W. (1991). Affective symptoms associated with the onset of major depression in the community: Findings from the US National Institute of Mental Health Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 84, 15.Google Scholar
Eagle, D. M., Tufft, M. R., Goodchild, H. L., & Robbins, T. W. (2007). Differential effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on stop-signal reaction time task performance in the rat, and interactions with the dopamine receptor antagonist cis-flupenthixol. Psychopharmacology, 192, 193206.Google Scholar
Eaton, N. R., Krueger, R. F., Keyes, K. M., Skodol, A. E., Markon, K. E., Grant, B. F., et al. (2011). Borderline personality disorder co-morbidity: Relationship to the internalizing–externalizing structure of common mental disorders. Psychological Medicine, 41, 10411050.Google Scholar
Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: Spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion, 10, 563572.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Berkman, E. T., Inagaki, T. K., Rameson, L. T., Mashal, N. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2010). Inflammation-induced anhedonia: Endotoxin reduces ventral striatum responses to reward. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 748754.Google Scholar
Eppinger, H., & Hess, L. (1915). Vagotonia: A clinical study in negative neurology. New York: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Co. (Original work published 1910)Google Scholar
Ernst, C., Lalovic, A., Lesage, A., Seguin, M., Tousignant, M., & Turecki, G. (2004). Suicide and no Axis I psychopathology. BMC Psychiatry, 4, 7.Google Scholar
Etkin, A., Egner, T., & Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 8593.Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: Devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 19461954.Google Scholar
Ezpeleta, L., Domènech, J. M., & Angold, A. (2006). A comparison of pure and comorbid CD/ODD and depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 704712.Google Scholar
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychological Medicine, 36, 159166.Google Scholar
Faraone, S. V., & Mick, E. (2010). Molecular genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33, 159180.Google Scholar
Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., et al. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 13131323.Google Scholar
Fava, M., Hwang, I., Rush, A. J., Sampson, N., Walters, E. E., & Kessler, R. C. (2010). The importance of irritability as a symptom of major depressive disorder: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 856867.Google Scholar
Fawcett, J., Clark, D. C., Scheftner, W. A., & Gibbons, R. D. (1983). Assessing anhedonia in psychiatric patients: The Pleasure Scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 7984.Google Scholar
Fawcett, J., Scheftner, W. A., Fogg, L., Clark, D. C., Young, M. A., Hedeker, D., et al. (1990). Time-related predictors of suicide in major affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 11891194.Google Scholar
Feola, T. W., de Wit, H., & Richards, J. B. (2000). Effects of d-amphetamine and ethanol on a measure of behavioral inhibition in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 838848.Google Scholar
Ferdinand, R. F., Dieleman, G., Ormel, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2007). Homotypic versus heterotypic continuity of anxiety symptoms in young adolescents: Evidence for distinctions between DSM-IV subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 325333.Google Scholar
Fineberg, N. A., Potenza, M. N., Chamberlain, S. R., Berlin, H. A., Menzies, L., Bechara, A., et al. (2010). Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes: A narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 591604.Google Scholar
Finlay, J. M., Zigmond, M. J., & Abercrombie, E. D. (1995). Increased dopamine and norepinephrine release in medial prefrontal cortex induced by acute and chronic stress: Effects of diazepam. Neuroscience, 64, 619628.Google Scholar
First, M. B. (2005). Mutually exclusive versus co-occurring diagnostic categories: The challenge of diagnostic comorbidity. Psychopathology, 38, 206210.Google Scholar
Floresco, S. B., Maric, T. L., & Ghods-Sharifi, S. (2008). Dopaminergic and glutamatergic regulation of effort- and delay-based decision making. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 19661979.Google Scholar
Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2005). Neural systems of positive affect: Relevance to understanding child and adolescent depression? Development and Psychopathology, 17, 827850.Google Scholar
Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2011). Research review: Altered reward function in adolescent depression: What, when and how? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 315.Google Scholar
Forbes, E., Hariri, A., Martin, S., Silk, J., Moyles, D., Fisher, P., et al. (2009). Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 6473.Google Scholar
Forbes, E. E., May, J. C., Siegle, G. J., Ladouceur, C. D., Ryan, N. D., Carter, C. S., et al. (2006). Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: An fMRI study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 10311040.Google Scholar
Foti, D., Carlson, J. M., Sauder, C. L., & Proudfit, G. H. (2014). Reward dysfunction in major depression: Multimodal neuroimaging evidence for refining the melancholic phenotype. NeuroImage, 101, 5058.Google Scholar
Fowles, D. C. (1980). The three arousal model: Implications of Gray's two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 17, 87104.Google Scholar
Fowles, D. C. (1988). Psychophysiology and psychopathology: A motivational approach. Psychophysiology, 25, 373391.Google Scholar
Frodl, T., & Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125, 114126.Google Scholar
Frodl, T., Stauber, J., Schaaff, N., Koutsouleris, N., Scheuerecker, J., Ewers, M., et al. (2010). Amygdala reduction in patients with ADHD compared with major depression and healthy volunteers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121, 111118.Google Scholar
Furukawa, E., Bado, P., Tripp, G., Mattos, P., Wickens, J. R., Bramati, I. E., et al. (2014). Abnormal striatal BOLD responses to reward anticipation and reward delivery in ADHD. PLOS ONE, 9, e89129.Google Scholar
Gabbay, V., Johnson, A. R., Alonso, C. M., Evans, L. K., Babb, J. S., & Klein, R. G. (2015). Anhedonia, but not irritability, is associated with illness severity outcomes in adolescent major depression. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 25, 194200.Google Scholar
Gao, Y., Raine, A., Venables, P. H., Dawson, M. E., & Mednick, S. A. (2010). Reduced electrodermal fear conditioning from ages 3 to 8 years is associated with aggressive behavior at age 8 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 550558.Google Scholar
Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Kring, A. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 10861102.Google Scholar
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2011). Canary in the coal mine: Sensitivity of mesolimbic dopamine to environmental adversity during development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 794803.Google Scholar
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2007). Central nervous system substrates of impulsivity. In Coch, D., Dawson, G., & Fischer, K. W. (Eds.), Human behavior, learning, and the developing brain: Atypical development (pp. 239263). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Beauchaine, T. P., Shannon, K. E., Chipman-Chacon, J., Fleming, A. P., Crowell, S. E., et al. (2009). Neurological correlates of reward responding in adolescents with and without externalizing behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 203213.Google Scholar
Gau, S. S.-F., Ni, H.-C., Shang, C.-Y., Soong, W.-T., Wu, Y.-Y., Lin, L.-Y., et al. (2010). Psychiatric comorbidity among children and adolescents with and without persistent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 135143.Google Scholar
Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. S. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 313333.Google Scholar
Gizer, I., Otto, J. M., & Ellingson, J. M. (2016). Molecular genetics of the externalizing spectrum. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 149169). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 81748179.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Aksan, N., & Buss, K. A. (2000). Temperament substrates of personality development. In Molfese, V. J. & Molfese, D. L. (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life-span (pp. 132). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., Pollak, S. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Developmental neuroscience perspectives on emotion regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 132140.Google Scholar
Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 652669.Google Scholar
Gorwood, P. (2008). Neurobiological mechanisms of anhedonia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 10, 291299.Google Scholar
Gotlib, I. H., & Hamilton, J. P. (2008). Neuroimaging and depression: Current status and unresolved issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 159163.Google Scholar
Goto, Y., Otani, S., & Grace, A. A. (2007). The yin and yang of dopamine release: A new perspective. Neuropharmacology, 53, 583587.Google Scholar
Gottfried, J. A., O'Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science, 301, 11041107.Google Scholar
Grace, A. A. (2001) Psychostimulant actions on dopamine and limbic system function: Relevance to the pathophysiology and treatment of ADHD. In Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T, & Castellanos, F. X. (Eds.), Stimulant drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience (pp. 134147). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grace, A. A., & Bunney, B. S. (1995) Electrophysiological properties of midbrain dopamine neurons. In Borroni, E. & Kupfer, D. J. (Eds.), Psychopharmacology: The fourth generation of progress (pp. 163177). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1987a). The psychology of fear and stress. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1987b). Perspectives on anxiety and impulsivity: A commentary. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 493509.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Groenewold, N. A., Opmeer, E. M., de Jonge, P., Aleman, A., & Costafreda, S. G. (2013). Emotional valence modulates brain functional abnormalities in depression: Evidence from a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 152163.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G., McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2004). Error-related psychophysiology and negative affect. Brain and Cognition, 56, 189197.Google Scholar
Hamilton, J. P., Etkin, A., Furman, D. J., Lemus, M. G., Johnson, R. F., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis and new integration of baseline activation and neural response data. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 693703.Google Scholar
Hamilton, J. P., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Amygdala volume in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 9931000.Google Scholar
Hanson, J. L., Adluru, N., Chung, M. K., Alexander, A. L., Davidson, R. J., & Pollak, S. D. (2013). Early neglect is associated with alterations in white matter integrity and executive function. Child Development, 84, 15661578.Google Scholar
Hanson, J. L., Chung, M. K., Avants, B. B., Rudolph, K. D., Shirtcliff, E. A., Gee, J. C., et al. (2012). Structural variations in prefrontal cortex mediate the relationship between early childhood stress and spatial working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 79177925.Google Scholar
Hanson, J. L., Chung, M. K., Avants, B. B., Shirtcliff, E. A., Gee, J. C., Davidson, R. J., et al. (2010). Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: A tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 74667472.Google Scholar
Hanson, J. L., Hair, N., Shen, D. G., Shi, F., Gilmore, J. H., Wolfe, B. L., et al. (2013). Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PLOS ONE, 8, e80954.Google Scholar
Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron rod through the head. Boston Medical and Surgical Journal, 39, 389393.Google Scholar
Hart, H., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2012). Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of timing in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 22482256.Google Scholar
Harvey, P. O., Pruessner, J., Czechowska, Y., & Lepage, M. (2007). Individual differences in trait anhedonia: A structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study in non-clinical subjects. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 767775.Google Scholar
Haslam, N., Holland, E., & Kuppens, P. (2012). Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: A quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychological Medicine, 42, 903920.Google Scholar
Hasler, G., Fromm, S., Carlson, P. J., Luckenbaugh, D. A., Waldeck, T., Geraci, M., et al. (2008). Neural response to catecholamine depletion in unmedicated subjects with major depressive disorder in remission and healthy subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 521531.Google Scholar
Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363390.Google Scholar
Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 132139.Google Scholar
Heim, C., & Binder, E. B. (2012). Current research trends in early life stress and depression: Review of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Experimental Neurology, 233, 102111.Google Scholar
Heim, C., Newport, D. J., Mletzko, T., Miller, A. H., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2008). The link between childhood trauma and depression: Insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 693710.Google Scholar
Henriques, J. B., Glowacki, J. M., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). Reward fails to alter response bias in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 460466.Google Scholar
Herpertz, S. (1995). Self-injurious behavior: Psychopathological and nosological characteristics in subtypes of self-injurers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 91, 5768.Google Scholar
Herpertz, S. C., Huebner, T., Marx, I., Vloet, T. D., Fink, G. R., Stoecker, T., et al. (2008). Emotional processing in male adolescents with childhood-onset conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 781791.Google Scholar
Hesslinger, B., Van Elst, L. T., Thiel, T., Haegele, K., Hennig, J., & Ebert, D. (2002). Fronto-orbital volume reductions in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 328, 319321.Google Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P., Henker, B., Whalen, C. K., Erhardt, D., & Dunnington, Jr., R. E. (1989). Aggressive, prosocial, and nonsocial behavior in hyperactive boys: Dose effects of methylphenidate in naturalistic settings. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 636643.Google Scholar
Holmes, A., & Wellman, C. L. (2009). Stress-induced prefrontal reorganization and executive dysfunction in rodents. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 773783.Google Scholar
Hoogman, M., Aarts, E., Zwiers, M., Slaats-Willemse, D., Naber, M., Onnink, M., et al. (2011). Nitric oxide synthase genotype modulation of impulsivity and ventral striatal activity in adult ADHD patients and healthy comparison subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 10991106.Google Scholar
Howlett, D. R., & Nahorski, S. R. (1979). Acute and chronic amphetamine treatments modulate striatal dopamine receptor binding sites. Brain Research, 161, 173178.Google Scholar
Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., Werthamer, L., & Kellam, S. (2001). The distal impact of two first-grade preventive interventions on conduct problems and disorder in early adolescence. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 9, 146160.Google Scholar
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751.Google Scholar
Isen, J. D., Iacono, W. G., Malone, S. M., & McGue, M. (2012). Examining electrodermal hyporeactivity as a marker of externalizing psychopathology: A twin study. Psychophysiology, 49, 10391048.Google Scholar
Isgor, C., Kabbaj, M., Akil, H., & Watson, S. J. (2004). Delayed effects of chronic variable stress during peripubertal-juvenile period on hippocampal morphology and on cognitive and stress axis functions in rats. Hippocampus, 14, 636648.Google Scholar
Jensen, P. S. (2003). Comorbidity and child psychopathology: Recommendations for the next decade. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 293300.Google Scholar
Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Kraemer, H. C., Lenora, N., Newcorn, J. H., Abikoff, H. B., et al. (2001). ADHD comorbidity findings from the MTA study: Comparing comorbid subgroups. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 147158.Google Scholar
Jia, Z., Huang, X., Wu, Q., Zhang, T., Lui, S., Zhang, J., et al. (2010). High-field magnetic resonance imaging of suicidality in patients with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 13811390.Google Scholar
Johnstone, T., van Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Kalin, N. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Failure to regulate: Counterproductive recruitment of top-down prefrontal-subcortical circuitry in major depression. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 88778884.Google Scholar
Judd, L. L., Schettler, P. J., Coryell, W., Akiskal, H. S., & Fiedorowicz, J. G. (2013). Overt irritability/anger in unipolar major depressive episodes: Past and current characteristics and implications for long-term course. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 11711180.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (in press). High reactive temperament, behavioral inhibition, and vulnerability to psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kalivas, P. W. (2008). Addiction as a pathology in prefrontal cortical regulation of corticostriatal habit circuitry. Neurotoxicity Research, 14, 185189.Google Scholar
Kamata, K., & Rebec, G. V. (1983). Dopaminergic and neostriatal neurons: Dose-dependent changes in sensitivity to amphetamine following long-term treatment. Neuropharmacology, 22, 13771382.Google Scholar
Karcher, N. R., Martin, E. A., & Kerns, J. G. (2015). Examining associations between psychosis risk, social anhedonia, and performance of striatum-related behavioral tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 507518.Google Scholar
Katz, R. J., Roth, K. A., & Carroll, B. J. (1981). Acute and chronic stress effects on open field activity in the rat: Implications for a model of depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 5, 247251.Google Scholar
Kaufman, E., Crowell, S. E., & Stepp, S. D. (2016). Self-injury, borderline personality development, and the externalizing spectrum. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 6178). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keiley, M. K., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2000). A cross-domain growth analysis: Externalizing and internalizing behaviors during 8 years of childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 161179.Google Scholar
Keller, J., Young, C. B., Kelley, E., Prater, K., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2013). Trait anhedonia is associated with reduced reactivity and connectivity of mesolimbic and paralimbic reward pathways. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 13191328.Google Scholar
Kelly, M. A., Rubinstein, M., Phillips, T. J., Lessov, C. N., Burkhart-Kasch, S., Zhang, G., et al. (1998). Locomotor activity in D2 dopamine receptor-deficient mice is determined by gene dosage, genetic background, and developmental adaptations. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 34703479.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Aggen, S. H., Knudsen, G. P., Røysamb, E., Neale, M. C., & Reichborn-Kjeennerud, T. (2011). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for syndromal and subsyndromal common DSM-IV Axis I and Axis II disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 2939.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2002). Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 11331145.Google Scholar
Kerestes, R., Davey, C. G., Stephanou, K., Whittle, S., & Harrison, B. J. (2014). Functional brain imaging studies of youth depression: A systematic review. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 209231.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 617627.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 819.Google Scholar
Kim, D. (2008). Blues from the neighborhood? Neighborhood characteristics and depression. Epidemiologic Reviews, 30, 101117.Google Scholar
Klein, D. (1974). Endogenomorphic depression: A conceptual and terminological revision. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 447454.Google Scholar
Klein, D. N., Kotov, R., & Bufferd, S. J. (2011). Personality and depression: Explanatory models and review of the evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 269295.Google Scholar
Klein, D. N., & Riso, L. P. (1993). Psychiatric disorders: Problems of boundaries and comorbidity. In Costello, C. G. (Ed.), Basic issues in psychopathology (pp. 1966). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 15.Google Scholar
Knutson, B., Bhanji, J. P., Cooney, R. E., Atlas, L. Y., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Neural responses to monetary incentives in major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 686692.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, S., & Schultz, W. (2008). Influence of reward delays on responses of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 78377846.Google Scholar
Koenigs, M., & Grafman, J. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of depression: Distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 201, 239243.Google Scholar
Kovess-Masfety, V., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M., Bromet, E., de Girolamo, G., de Jonge, P., et al. (2013). Irritable mood in adult major depressive disorder: Results from the World Mental Health Surveys. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 395406.Google Scholar
Krach, S., Paulus, F. M., Bodden, M., & Kircher, T. (2010). The rewarding nature of social interactions. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 22.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1919). Dementia praecox and paraphrenia. New York: Huntington.Google Scholar
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691702.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 921926.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S. R., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 411424.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 111133.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2001). The higher-order structure of common DSM mental disorders: Internalization, externalization, and their connections to personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 12451259.Google Scholar
Kühn, S., & Gallinat, J. (2012). The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness. NeuroImage, 61, 289294.Google Scholar
Laakso, A., Wallius, E., Kajander, J., Bergman, J., Eskola, O., Solin, O., et al. (2003). Personality traits and striatal dopamine synthesis capacity in healthy subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 904910.Google Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Singh, A. L., Waldman, I. D., & Rathouz, P. J. (2011). Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 181189.Google Scholar
Lambert, G., Johansson, M., Ågren, H., & Friberg, P. (2000). Reduced brain norepinephrine and dopamine release in treatment-refractory depressive illness: Evidence in support of the catecholamine hypothesis of mood disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 787793.Google Scholar
Lee, S. Y., Burns, G. L., Beauchaine, T. P., & Becker, S. P. (2015). Bifactor latent structure of ADHD/ODD symptoms and first-order latent structure of sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms. Psychological Assessment. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Leibenluft, E., Cohen, P., Gorrindo, T., Brook, J. S., & Pine, D. S. (2006). Chronic versus episodic irritability in youth: A community-based, longitudinal study of clinical and diagnostic associations. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 16, 456466.Google Scholar
Leibenluft, E., & Stoddard, J. (2013). The developmental psychopathology of irritability. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 14731487.Google Scholar
Lemke, M. R., Puhl, P., Koethe, N., & Winkler, T. (1999). Psychomotor retardation and anhedonia in depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 99, 252256.Google Scholar
Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 718729.Google Scholar
Leung, H. C., & Cai, W. (2007). Common and differential ventrolateral prefrontal activity during inhibition of hand and eye movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 98939900.Google Scholar
Leventhal, A. M., Chasson, G. S., Tapia, E., Miller, E. K., & Pettit, J. W. (2006). Measuring hedonic capacity in depression: A psychometric analysis of three anhedonia scales. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 15451558.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2003). Comorbidity between and within childhood externalizing and internalizing disorders: Reflections and directions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 285291.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., Wager, T. D., Weber, J., & Barrett, L. F. (2015). The brain basis of positive and negative affect: Evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cerebral Cortex. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Liu, X., Hairston, J., Schrier, M., & Fan, J. (2011). Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 12191236.Google Scholar
Livesley, W. J., & Jang, K. L. (2005). Differentiating normal, abnormal, and disordered personality. European Journal of Personality, 19, 257268.Google Scholar
Loas, G. (1996). Vulnerability to depression: A model centered on anhedonia. Journal of Affective Disorders, 41, 3953.Google Scholar
Lodge, D. J. (2011). The medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices differentially regulate dopamine system function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 12271236.Google Scholar
Loth, A. K., Drabick, D. A., Leibenluft, E., & Hulvershorn, L. A. (2014). Do childhood externalizing disorders predict adult depression? A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 11031113.Google Scholar
Louilot, A., LeMoal, M., & Simon, H. (1989). Opposite influences of dopaminergic pathways to the prefrontal cortex or the septum on the dopaminergic transmission in the nucleus accumbens: An in vivo voltammetric study. Neuroscience, 29, 4556.Google Scholar
Luby, J. L., Mrakotsky, C., Heffelfinger, A., Brown, K., & Spitznagel, E. (2004). Characteristics of depressed preschoolers with and without anhedonia: Evidence for a melancholic depressive subtype in young children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 19982004.Google Scholar
Ludolph, A. G., Kassubek, J., Schmeck, K., Glaser, C., Wunderlich, A., Buck, A. K., et al. (2008). Dopaminergic dysfunction in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), differences between pharmacologically treated and never treated young adults: A 3, 4-dihdroxy-6-[18 F] fluorophenyl-l-alanine PET study. NeuroImage, 41, 718727.Google Scholar
Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: A review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 183213.Google Scholar
Luman, M., Tripp, G., & Scheres, A. (2010). Identifying the neurobiology of altered reinforcement sensitivity in ADHD: A review and research agenda. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 744754.Google Scholar
Luman, M., Van Meel, C. S., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Geurts, H. M. (2009). Does reward frequency or magnitude drive reinforcement-learning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Psychiatry Research, 168, 222229.Google Scholar
Lutz, K., & Widmer, M. (2014). What can the monetary incentive delay task tell us about the neural processing of reward and punishment? Neuroscience and Neuroeconomics, 3, 3335.Google Scholar
Macdonald, A., Goines, K., Novacek, D., & Walker, E. (2016). Prefrontal mechanisms of comorbidity from a transdiagnostic and ontogenic perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 1149–1177.Google Scholar
Makris, N., Biederman, J., Valera, E. M., Bush, G., Kaiser, J., Kennedy, D. N., et al. (2007). Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 13641375.Google Scholar
Marchand, W. R., Lee, J. N., Johnson, S., Thatcher, J., & Gale, P. (2013). Striatal circuit function is associated with prior self-harm in remitted major depression. Neuroscience Letters, 557, 154158.Google Scholar
Marchand, W. R., Lee, J. N., Johnson, S., Thatcher, J., Gale, P., Wood, N., et al. (2012). Striatal and cortical midline circuits in major depression: Implications for suicide and symptom expression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 36, 290299.Google Scholar
Marissen, M. A., Arnold, N., & Franken, I. H. (2012). Anhedonia in borderline personality disorder and its relation to symptoms of impulsivity. Psychopathology, 45, 179184.Google Scholar
Marmorstein, N. R., & Iacono, W. G. (2003). Major depression and conduct disorder in a twin sample: Gender, functioning, and risk for future psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 225233.Google Scholar
Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 454465.Google Scholar
Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., et al. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 712720.Google Scholar
Martin-Soelch, C., Leenders, K. L., Chevalley, A.-F., Missimer, J., Kunig, S., Magyar, A., et al. (2001). Reward mechanisms in the brain and their role in dependence: Evidence from neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies. Brain Research Reviews, 36, 139149.Google Scholar
Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Messer, J., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. (2004). Conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in a national sample: Developmental epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 606621.Google Scholar
McCabe, C. (2014). Neural correlates of anhedonia as a trait marker for depression. In Ritsner, M. (Ed.), Anhedonia: A comprehensive handbook (Vol. 2, pp. 159174). Amsterdam: Springer.Google Scholar
McCabe, C., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2009). Neural representation of reward in recovered depressed patients. Psychopharmacology, 205, 667677.Google Scholar
McCabe, C., Woffindale, C., Harmer, C. J., & Cowen, P. J. (2012). Neural processing of reward and punishment in young people at increased familial risk of depression. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 588594.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in adulthood (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N., & Corr, P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: Fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 285305.Google Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1962). Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. American Psychologist, 17, 827838.Google Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1973). Psychodiagnosis: Selected papers. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstraps taxometrics: Solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist, 50, 266275.Google Scholar
Meinzer, M. C., Lewinsohn, P. M., Pettit, J. W., Seeley, J. R., Gau, J. M., Chronis-Tuscano, A., et al. (2013). Attention–deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescence predicts onset of major depressive disorder through early adulthood. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 546553.Google Scholar
Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 12811310.Google Scholar
Meyer, J. H., Krüger, S., Wilson, A. A., Christensen, B. K., Goulding, V. S., Schaffer, A., et al. (2001). Lower dopamine transporter binding potential in striatum during depression. NeuroReport, 12, 41214125.Google Scholar
Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 463488.Google Scholar
Miller, A. H., Maletic, V., & Raison, C. L. (2009). Inflammation and its discontents: The role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 732741.Google Scholar
Miller, J. D., Lyman, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). Personality disorders as extreme variants of common personality dimensions: Can the five-factor model adequately represent psychopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253276.Google Scholar
Milner, P. M. (1991). Brain-stimulation reward: A review. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 136.Google Scholar
Morgan, J., Olino, T., McMakin, D., Ryan, N., & Forbes, E. (2013) Neural response to reward as a predictor of rise in depressive symptoms in adolescence. Neurobiological Disorders, 52, 6674.Google Scholar
Morrison, S. E., & Salzman, C. D. (2009). The convergence of information about rewarding and aversive stimuli in single neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1147111483.Google Scholar
MTA Cooperative Group. (1999). A 14th month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Generation Psychiatry, 56, 10731086.Google Scholar
Murray, E. A., Wise, S. P., & Drevets, W. C. (2011). Localization of dysfunction in major depressive disorder: Prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e43e54.Google Scholar
National Institute of Mental Health. (2014). Research Domain Criteria matrix. Retrieved on March 28, 2016, from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/research-domain-criteria-matrix.shtml.Google Scholar
Nestler, E. J., & Carlezon, Jr., W. A. (2006). The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 11511159.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist, 61, 622631.Google Scholar
Neuhaus, E., & Beauchaine, T. P. (in press). Impulsivity and vulnerability to psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Nikolas, M. A., & Burt, S. A. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 117.Google Scholar
Niv, Y. (2007). Cost, benefit, tonic, phasic: What do response rates tell us about dopamine and motivation? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104, 357376.Google Scholar
Nixon, M. K., Cloutier, P. F., & Aggarwal, S. (2002). Affect regulation and addictive aspects of repetitive self-injury in hospitalized adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 13331341.Google Scholar
Nock, M. K. (2010). Self-injury. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 339363.Google Scholar
Nock, M. K., Prinstein, M. J., & Sterba, S. K. (2009). Revealing the form and function of self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: A real-time ecological assessment study among adolescents and young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 816827.Google Scholar
Nordström, P., Schalling, D., & Asberg, M. (1995). Temperamental vulnerability in attempted suicide. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 92, 155160.Google Scholar
Norris, C. J., Larsen, J. T., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures. Psychophysiology, 44, 823826.Google Scholar
Oberlin, B. G., Dzemidzic, M., Bragulat, V., Lehigh, C. A., Talavage, T., O'Connor, S. J., et al. (2012). Limbic responses to reward cues correlate with antisocial trait density in heavy drinkers. NeuroImage, 60, 644652.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J. P. (2004). Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 769776.Google Scholar
O'Donnell, O., House, A., & Waterman, M. (2015). The co-occurrence of aggression and self-harm: Systematic literature review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175, 325350.Google Scholar
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 419427.Google Scholar
Olino, T. M., McMakin, D. L., Morgan, J. K., Silk, J. S., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., et al. (2014). Reduced reward anticipation in youth at high-risk for unipolar depression: A preliminary study. Developmental Social and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 5564.Google Scholar
Öngür, D., & Price, J. L. (2000). The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 206219.Google Scholar
Onnink, A. M. H., Zwiers, M. P., Hoogman, M., Mostert, J. C., Kan, C. C., Buitelaar, J., et al. (2014). Brain alterations in adult ADHD: Effects of gender, treatment and comorbid depression. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 24, 397409.Google Scholar
Pani, L., Porcella, A., & Gessa, G. L. (2000). The role of stress in the pathophysiology of the dopaminergic system. Molecular Psychiatry, 5, 1421.Google Scholar
Papousek, I., & Schulter, G. (2001). Associations between EEG asymmetries and electrodermal lability in low vs. high depressive and anxious normal individuals. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 41, 105117.Google Scholar
Paris, J. (1997). Antisocial and borderline personality disorders: Two separate diagnoses or two aspects of the same psychopathology? Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38, 237242.Google Scholar
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768774.Google Scholar
Pechtel, P., Dutra, S. J., Goetz, E. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Blunted reward responsiveness in remitted depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 18641869.Google Scholar
Peciña, S., Cagniard, B., Berridge, K. C., Aldridge, J. W., & Zhuang, X. (2003). Hyperdopaminergic mutant mice have higher “wanting” but not “liking” for sweet rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 93959402.Google Scholar
Pelizza, L., & Ferrari, A. (2009). Anhedonia in schizophrenia and major depression: State or trait. Annals of General Psychiatry, 8, 19.Google Scholar
Pendse, B., Westrin, Å., & Engström, G. (1999). Temperament traits in seasonal affective disorder, suicide attempters with non-seasonal major depression and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 54, 5565.Google Scholar
Perlis, R. H., Fava, M., Trivedi, M. H., Alpert, J., Luther, J. F., Wisniewski, S. R., et al. (2009). Irritability is associated with anxiety and greater severity, but not bipolar spectrum features, in major depressive disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 119, 282289.Google Scholar
Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16, 331348.Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 198202.Google Scholar
Philipsen, A. (2006). Differential diagnosis and comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and borderline personality disorder (BPD) in adults. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256, i42i46.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, D., Holmes, A., Dillon, D., Goetz, E., Birk, J., Bogdan, R., et al. (2009). Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 702710.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., & Fava, M. (2008). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: Evidence from a probabilistic reward task. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 7687.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, D. A., Jahn, A. L., & O'Shea, J. P. (2005). Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: A signal-detection approach. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 319327.Google Scholar
Plessen, K. J., Bansal, R., Zhu, H., Whiteman, R., Amat, J., Quackenbush, G. A., et al. (2006). Hippocampus and amygdala morphology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 795807.Google Scholar
Plichta, M. M., & Scheres, A. (2014). Ventral-striatal responsiveness during reward anticipation in ADHD and its relation to trait impulsivity in the healthy population: A meta-analytic review of the fMRI literature. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 38, 125134.Google Scholar
Plichta, M. M., Vasic, N., Wolf, R. C., Lesch, K. P., Brummer, D., Jacob, C., et al. (2009). Neural hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness during immediate and delayed reward processing in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 714.Google Scholar
Posner, J., Siciliano, F., Wang, Z., Liu, J., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Greenhill, L. (2014). A multimodal MRI study of the hippocampus in medication-naive children with ADHD: What connects ADHD and depression? Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 224, 112118.Google Scholar
Preskorn, S. H., & Baker, B. (2002). The overlap of DSM-IV syndromes: Potential implications for the practice of polypsychopharmacology, psychiatric drug development, and the human genome project. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 8, 170177.Google Scholar
Proudfit, G. H. (2014). The reward positivity: From basic research on reward to a biomarker for depression. Psychophysiology, 52, 449459.Google Scholar
Quay, H. C. (1993). The psychobiology of undersocialized aggressive conduct disorder: A theoretical perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 165180.Google Scholar
Rado, S. (1956). Psychoanalysis of behavior: Collected papers. New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
Rado, S. (1962). Psychoanalysis of behavior: Collected papers (Vol. 2). New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
Rawal, A., Collishaw, S., Thapar, A., & Rice, F. (2013). “The risks of playing it safe”: A prospective longitudinal study of response to reward in the adolescent offspring of depressed parents. Psychological Medicine, 43, 2738.Google Scholar
Rebec, G. V., & Segal, D. S. (1978). Dose-dependent biphasic alterations in the spontaneous activity of neurons in the rat neostriatum produced by d-amphetamine and methylphenidate. Brain Research, 150, 353366.Google Scholar
Reynolds, B., Ortengren, A., Richards, J. B., & de Wit, H. (2006). Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 305315.Google Scholar
Rhee, S. H., Lahey, B. B., & Waldman, I. D. (2015). Comorbidity among dimensions of childhood psychopathology: Converging evidence from behavior genetics. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 2631.Google Scholar
Ribot, T. (1896). La psychologie des sentiments. Paris: Felix Alcan.Google Scholar
Rice, F., Harold, G., & Thapar, A. (2002). The genetic aetiology of childhood depression: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 6579.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 3135.Google Scholar
Robinson, E. S. J., Eagle, D. M., Economidou, D., Theobald, D. E. H., Mar, A. C., Murphy, E. R., et al. (2009). Behavioural characterisation of high impulsivity on the 5-choice serial reaction time task: Specific deficits in “waiting” versus “stopping.” Behavioural Brain Research, 196, 310316.Google Scholar
Robinson, O. J., Cools, R., Carlisi, C. O., Sahakian, B. J., & Drevets, W. C. (2012). Ventral striatum response during reward and punishment reversal learning in unmedicated major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 152159.Google Scholar
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive–sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews, 18, 247291.Google Scholar
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 31373146.Google Scholar
Rosch, K. S., & Hawk, L. W. (2013). The effects of performance-based rewards on neurophysiological correlates of stimulus, error, and feedback processing in children with ADHD. Psychophysiology, 50, 11571173.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 207212.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rottenberg, J., & Gross, J. J. (2003). When emotion goes wrong: Realizing the promise of affective science. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 227232.Google Scholar
Rubia, K. (2011). “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” vetromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e69e87.Google Scholar
Rubia, K., Halari, R., Cubillo, A., Mohammad, M., & Taylor, E. (2009). Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naïve children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance task. Neuropharmacology, 57, 640652.Google Scholar
Rubia, K., Halari, R., Mohammad, A. M., Taylor, E., & Brammer, M. (2011). Methylphenidate normalizes frontocingulate underactivation during error processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 255262.Google Scholar
Rubia, K., Smith, A., Halari, R., Matukura, F., Mohammad, M., Taylor, E., et al. (2009). Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during sustained attention. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 8394.Google Scholar
Ruhé, H. G., Mason, N. S., & Schene, A. H. (2007). Mood is indirectly related to serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: A meta-analysis of monoamine depletion studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 331359.Google Scholar
Saddoris, M. P., Cacciapaglia, F., Wightman, R. M., & Carelli, R. M. (2015). Differential dopamine release dynamics in the nucleus accumbens core and shell reveal complementary signals for error prediction and incentive motivation. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 1157211582.Google Scholar
Sadeh, N., Javdani, S., Finy, M. S., & Verona, E. (2011). Gender differences in emotional risk for self- and other-directed violence among externalizing adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 106117.Google Scholar
Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E., Aase, H., & Russell, V. (2005). A dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 28, 397468.Google Scholar
Sah, P., Faber, E. L., De Armentia, M. L., & Power, J. (2003). The amygdaloid complex: Anatomy and physiology. Physiological Reviews, 83, 803834.Google Scholar
Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Farrar, A., & Mingote, S. M. (2007). Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology, 191, 461482.Google Scholar
Salamone, J. D., Cousins, M. S., & Bucher, S. (1994). Anhedonia or anergia? Effects of haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion on instrumental response selection in a T-maze cost/benefit procedure. Behavioural Brain Research, 65, 221229.Google Scholar
Sauder, C., Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Shannon, K. E., & Aylward, E. (2012). Neuroanatomical correlates of heterotypic comorbidity in externalizing male adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 346352.Google Scholar
Sauder, C. L., Derbidge, C. M., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2016). Neural responses to monetary incentives among self-injuring adolescent girls. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 277291.Google Scholar
Savage, J., Verhulst, B., Copeland, W., Althoff, R. R., Lichtenstein, P., & Roberson-Nay, R. (2015). A genetically informed study of the longitudinal relation between irritability and anxious/depressed symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 377384.Google Scholar
Scheres, A., Milham, M. P., Knutson, B., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 720724.Google Scholar
Schlüter, T., Winz, O., Henkel, K., Prinz, S., Rademacher, L., Schmaljohann, J., et al. (2013). The impact of dopamine on aggression: An [18F]-FDOPA PET Study in healthy males. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1688916896.Google Scholar
Schneider, M. F., Krick, C. M., Retz, W., Hengesch, G., Retz-Junginger, P., Reith, W., et al. (2010). Impairment of fronto-striatal and parietal cerebral networks correlates with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) psychopathology in adults—A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 183, 7584.Google Scholar
Schoenbaum, G., & Shaham, Y. (2008). The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: A review of preclinical studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 256262.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (1996). The experience-dependent maturation of a regulatory system in the orbital prefrontal cortex and the origin of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 5987.Google Scholar
Schrader, G. D. (1997). Does anhedonia correlate with depression severity in chronic depression? Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38, 260263.Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 127.Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 15931599.Google Scholar
Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., Makris, N., Monuteaux, M. C., Boriel, D. L., Kelkar, K., et al. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 10711080.Google Scholar
Seroczynski, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Coccaro, E. F. (1999). Etiology of the impusivity/aggression relationship: Genes or environment? Psychiatry Research, 86, 4157.Google Scholar
Seymour, K. E., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Iwamoto, D. K., Kurdziel, G., & MacPherson, L. (2014). Emotion regulation mediates the association between ADHD and depressive symptoms in a community sample of youth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 611621.Google Scholar
Shankman, S. A., Katz, A. C., DeLizza, A. A., Sarapas, C., Gorka, S. M., & Campbell, M. L. (2014). The different facets of anhedonia and their associations with different psychopathologies. In Ritsner, M. S. (Ed.), Anhedonia: A comprehensive handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 322). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Shankman, S. A., Nelson, B. D., Harrow, M., & Faull, R. (2010). Does physical anhedonia play a role in depression? A 20-year longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 120, 170176.Google Scholar
Shannon, K. E., Beauchaine, T. P., Brenner, S. L., Neuhaus, E., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2007). Familial and temperamental predictors of resilience in children at risk for conduct disorder and depression. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 701727.Google Scholar
Shannon, K. E., Sauder, C., Beauchaine, T. P., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2009). Disrupted effective connectivity between the medial frontal cortex and the caudate in adolescent boys with externalizing behavior disorders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 11411157 Google Scholar
Sharot, T., Shiner, T., Brown, A. C., Fan, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2009). Dopamine enhances expectation of pleasure in humans. Current Biology, 19, 20772080.Google Scholar
Shatz, C. B., & Rostain, A. L. (2006). ADHD with comorbid anxiety: A review of the current literature. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 141149.Google Scholar
Shaw, P., Lerch, J., Greenstein, D., Sharp, W., Clasen, L., Evans, A., et al. (2006). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 540549.Google Scholar
Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Sharp, W., Evans, A., & Greenstein, D. (2012). Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 191197.Google Scholar
Shaw, P., Stringaris, A., Nigg, J., & Leibenluft, E. (2014). Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 276293.Google Scholar
Shiels, K., & Hawk, L. W. (2010). Self-regulation in ADHD: The role of error processing. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 951961.Google Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Thompson, W., Carter, C. S., Steinhauer, S. R., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: Related and independent features. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 198209.Google Scholar
Skirrow, C., McLoughlin, G., Kuntsi, J., & Asherson, P. (2009). Behavioral, neurocognitive and treatment overlap between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and mood instability. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 9, 489503.Google Scholar
Smoski, M. J., Felder, J., Bizzell, J., Green, S. R., Ernst, M., Lynch, T. R., et al. (2009). fMRI of alterations in reward selection, anticipation, and feedback in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 118, 6978.Google Scholar
Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., Morley, S., Humayan, A., Hargreaves, D., & Trigwell, P. (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 99103.Google Scholar
Snaith, R. P., & Taylor, C. M. (1985). Irritability: Definition, assessment and associated factors. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 127136.Google Scholar
Sobanski, E., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Chen, W., Franke, B., et al. (2010). Emotional lability in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Clinical correlates and familial prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 915923.Google Scholar
Soloff, P. H., Lynch, K. G., Kelly, T. M., Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J. (2000). Characteristics of suicide attempts of patients with major depressive episode and borderline personality disorder: A comparative study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 601608.Google Scholar
Spurzheim, G. (1834). Outlines of phrenology (3rd ed.). Boston: Marsh, Caper & Lyon.Google Scholar
Stålenheim, E. G. (2001). Relationships between attempted suicide, temperamental vulnerability, and violent criminality in a Swedish forensic psychiatric population. European Psychiatry, 16, 386394.Google Scholar
Stringaris, A., Psych, M. R. C., Cohen, P., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2009). Adult outcomes of youth irritability: A 20-year prospective community-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 10481054.Google Scholar
Ströhle, A., Stoy, M., Wrase, J., Schwarzer, S., Schlagenhauf, F., Huss, M., et al. (2008). Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage, 39, 966972.Google Scholar
Strömbom, U. (1976). Catecholamine receptor agonists. Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's Archives of Pharmacology, 292, 167176.Google Scholar
Stuber, G. D., Sparta, D. R., Stamatakis, A. M., van Leeuwen, W. A., Hardjoprajitno, J. E., Cho, S., et al. (2011). Excitatory transmission from the amygdala to nucleus accumbens facilitates reward seeking. Nature, 475, 377380.Google Scholar
Stuhrmann, A., Suslow, T., & Dannlowski, U. (2011). Facial emotion processing in major depression: A systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders, 1, 10.Google Scholar
Sullivan, P. F., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Genetic epidemiology of major depression: Review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 15521562.Google Scholar
Taber, K. H., Black, D. N., Porrino, L. J., & Hurley, R. A. (2012). Neuroanatomy of dopamine: Reward and addiction. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 24, 14.Google Scholar
Tackett, J. L., Waldman, I. D., Van Hulle, C. A., & Lahey, B. B. (2011). Shared genetic influences on negative emotionality and major depression/conduct disorder comorbidity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 818827.Google Scholar
Tanji, J., & Hoshi, E. (2008). Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. Physiological Reviews, 88, 3757.Google Scholar
Taylor, J., Peterson, C. M., & Fischer, S. (2012). Motivations for self-injury, affect, and impulsivity: A comparison of individuals with current self-injury to individuals with a history of self-injury. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42, 602613.Google Scholar
Thomas, M. J., Beurrier, C., Bonci, A., & Malenka, R. C. (2001). Long-term depression in the nucleus accumbens: A neural correlate of behavioral sensitization to cocaine. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 12171222.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. In Thompson, R. A. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Vol. 36. Socioemotional development. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Thorell, L. H., Wolfersdorf, M., Straub, R., Steyer, J., Hodgkinson, S., Kaschka, W. P., et al. (2013). Electrodermal hyporeactivity as a trait marker for suicidal propensity in uni-and bipolar depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 19251931.Google Scholar
Tisch, S., Silberstein, P., Limousin-Dowsey, P., & Jahanshahi, M. (2004). The basal ganglia: Anatomy, physiology, and pharmacology. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 27, 757799.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1921). A text-book on psychology. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Tobler, P. N., Fiorillo, C. D., & Schultz, W. (2005). Adaptive coding of reward value by dopamine neurons. Science, 307, 16421645.Google Scholar
Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Focus: The Journal of Lifelong Learning in Psychiatry, 4, 378390.Google Scholar
Tomasi, D., & Volkow, N. D. (2012). Abnormal functional connectivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 71, 443450.Google Scholar
Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Schwartzman, A. N., Lambert, W. E., & Zald, D. H. (2009). Worth the “EEfRT”? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLOS ONE, 4, e6598.Google Scholar
Tremblay, L., Hollerman, J. R., & Schultz, W. (1998). Modifications of reward expectation-related neuronal activity during learning in primate striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 964977.Google Scholar
Tremblay, L. K., Naranjo, C. A., Graham, S. J., Herrmann, N., Mayberg, H. S., Hevenor, S., et al. (2005). Functional neuroanatomical substrates of altered reward processing in major depressive disorder revealed by a dopaminergic probe. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 12281236.Google Scholar
Tripp, G., & Wickens, J. R. (2008). Research review: Dopamine transfer deficit: A neurobiological theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 691704.Google Scholar
Tuvblad, C., Zheng, M., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2009). A common genetic factor explains the covariation among ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms in 9–10-year-old boys and girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 153167.Google Scholar
Urry, H. L., Van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N. H., Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 44154425.Google Scholar
Vaidya, C. J. (2012). Neurodevelopmental abnormalities in ADHD. In Stanford, C. & Tannock, R. (Eds.), Behavioral neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its treatment (pp. 4966). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
van Heeringen, C., Bijttebier, S., & Godfrin, K. (2011). Suicidal brains: A review of functional and structural brain studies in association with suicidal behaviour. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 688698.Google Scholar
Vasilev, C. A., Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., Mead, H. K., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2009). Correspondence between physiological and self-report measures of emotion dysregulation: A longitudinal investigation of youth with and without psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 13571364.Google Scholar
Vezina, P. (2004). Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity and the self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 827839.Google Scholar
Vles, J. S. H., Feron, F. J. M., & Hendriksen, J. G. M. (2003). Dopamine receptor and transporter system in children with attention deficit hyperkinetic disorder (ADHD). Neuropediatrics, 34, 7780.Google Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., & Wang, G. J. (2004). The addicted human brain viewed in light of imaging studies: Brain circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology, 47, 313.Google Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G., Ding, Y., & Gatley, S. J. (2002). Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: Insights from PET imaging studies. Journal of Attention Disorders, 6, S31S43.Google Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., Logan, J., Gatley, S. J., Gifford, A., et al. (1999). Prediction of reinforcing responses to psychostimulants in humans by brain dopamine D2 receptor levels. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 14401443.Google Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Newcorn, J. H., Telang, F., et al. (2009). Evaluating dopamine reward pathway in ADHD: Clinical implications. Journal of the American Medical Association, 302, 10841091.Google Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Newcorn, J., Fowler, J. S., Telang, F., Solanto, M. V., et al. (2007). Brain dopamine transporter levels in treatment and drug naive adults with ADHD. NeuroImage, 34, 11821190.Google Scholar
Völlm, B., Richardson, P., McKie, S., Elliott, R., Dolan, M., & Deakin, B. (2007). Neuronal correlates of reward and loss in Cluster B personality disorders: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 156, 151167.Google Scholar
Voorn, P., Vanderschuren, L. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Robbins, T. W., & Pennartz, C. M. (2004). Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 468474.Google Scholar
Wacker, J., Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). The role of the nucleus accumbens and rostral anterior cingulate cortex in anhedonia: Integration of resting EEG, fMRI, and volumetric techniques. NeuroImage, 46, 327337.Google Scholar
Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008). Prefrontal–subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron, 59, 10371050.Google Scholar
Wagner, G., Koch, K., Schachtzabel, C., Schultz, C. C., Sauer, H., & Schlösser, R. G. (2011). Structural brain alterations in patients with major depressive disorder and high risk for suicide: Evidence for a distinct neurobiological entity? NeuroImage, 54, 16071614.Google Scholar
Wahlstrom, D., Collins, P., White, T., & Luciana, M. (2010). Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: Behavioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain and Cognition, 72, 146159.Google Scholar
Walker, J. L., Lahey, B. B., Russo, M. F., Frick, P. J., Christ, M. A. G., McBurnett, K., et al. (1991). Anxiety, inhibition, and conduct disorder in children: I. Relations to social impairment. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 187191.Google Scholar
Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Watson, D. (2002). Positive affectivity. In Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 106119). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219235.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Walker, L. M. (1996). The long-term stability and predictive validity of trait measures of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 567577.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Clark, L. A., Strauss, M. E., & McCormick, R. A. (1995). Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 314.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A., Liu, H., Hajcak, G., & Shankman, S. A. (2015). Blunted neural response to rewards as a vulnerability factor for depression: Results from a family study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 878889.Google Scholar
Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Nomura, Y., Warner, V., Pilowsky, D., & Verdeli, H. (2006). Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 10011008.Google Scholar
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669689.Google Scholar
Wilbertz, G., van Elst, L. T., Delgado, M. R., Maier, S., Feige, B., Philipsen, A., et al. (2012). Orbitofrontal reward sensitivity and impulsivity in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage, 60, 353361.Google Scholar
Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., Brown, S., Tanguay, S., Monuteaux, M. C., Blake, C., et al. (2002). Psychiatric comorbidity and functioning in clinically referred preschool children and school-age youths with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 262268.Google Scholar
Willcutt, E. G. (in press). The etiology of ADHD: Behavioral and molecular genetic approaches. In Barch, D. (Ed.), Cognitive and affective neuroscience of psychopathology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Winer, E. S., Nadorff, M. R., Ellis, T. E., Allen, J. G., Herrera, S., & Salem, T. (2014). Anhedonia predicts suicidal ideation in a large psychiatric inpatient sample. Psychiatry Research, 218, 124128.Google Scholar
Winograd-Gurvich, C., Fitzgerald, P. B., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Bradshaw, J. L., & White, O. B. (2006). Negative symptoms: A review of schizophrenia, melancholic depression and Parkinson's disease. Brain Research Bulletin, 70, 312321.Google Scholar
Winstanley, C. A., Theobald, D. E., Cardinal, R. N., & Robbins, T. W. (2004). Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 47184722.Google Scholar
Wise, R. A. (2009). Roles for nigrostriatal—not just mesocorticolimbic—dopamine in reward and addiction. Trends in Neurosciences, 32, 517524.Google Scholar
Wrase, J., Schlagenhauf, F., Kienast, T., Wüstenberg, T., Bermpohl, F., Kahnt, T., et al. (2007). Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. NeuroImage, 35, 787794.Google Scholar
Wray, N. R., Pergadia, M. L., Blackwood, D. H. R., Penninx, B. W. J. H., Gordon, S. D., Nyholt, D. R., et al. (2012). Genome-wide association study of major depressive disorder: New results, meta-analysis, and lessons learned. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 3648.Google Scholar
Wright, A. G. C., Krueger, R. F., Hobbs, M. J., Markon, K. E., Eaton, N. R., & Slade, T. (2013). The structure of psychopathology: Toward an expanded quantitative empirical model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 281294.Google Scholar
Yamawaki, S., Okada, G., Okamoto, Y., & Liberzon, I. (2012). Mood dysregulation and stabilization: Perspectives from emotional cognitive neuroscience. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 15, 681694.Google Scholar
Yorbik, O., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D., Williamson, D. E., & Ryan, N. D. (2004). Clinical characteristics of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65, 16541659.Google Scholar
Zisner, A., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Midbrain neural mechanisms of trait impulsivity. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 184200). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zisner, A., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2016). Physiological methods and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Developmental neuroscience (3rd ed., pp. 832884). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar