Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:51:40.792Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion regulation deficits mediate childhood sexual abuse effects on stress sensitization and depression outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2020

Ilya Yaroslavsky*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Cleveland State University, Cleveland, OH, USA
Angela H. Bush
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Cleveland State University, Cleveland, OH, USA
Christopher M. France
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Cleveland State University, Cleveland, OH, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Ilya Yaroslavsky, Department of Psychology, Cleveland State University, 2121 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH44115, USA; E-mail: i.yaroslavsky@csuohio.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Child sexual abuse (CSA) is a notable risk factor for depressive disorders. Though multiply determined, increased sensitivity to stress (stress sensitization) and difficulty managing distress (emotion regulation) may reflect two pathways by which CSA confers depression risk. However, it remains unclear whether stress sensitization and emotion regulation deficits contribute to depression risk independently or in a sequential manner. That is, the frequent use of maladaptive emotion regulation responses and insufficient use of those that attenuate distress (adaptive emotion regulation) may lead to stress sensitization. We tested competing models of CSA, stress sensitization, and emotion regulation to predict depression symptoms and depressive affects in daily life among adults with and without histories of CSA. Results supported a sequential mediation: CSA predicted greater maladaptive repertoires that, in turn, exacerbated the effects of stress on depression symptoms. Maladaptive responses also exacerbated the effects of daily life stress on contemporaneous negative affect (NA) levels and their increase over time. Independent of stress sensitization, emotion regulation deficits also mediated CSA effects on both depressive outcomes, though the effect of maladaptive strategies was specific to NA, and adaptive responses to positive affect. Our findings suggest that emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization play key intervening roles between CSA and risk for depression.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Sexual abuse in the form of unwanted sexual contact or related sexual experiences (e.g., child pornography) during childhood and adolescence is common (Murray, Nguyen, & Cohen, Reference Murray, Nguyen and Cohen2014; World Health Organization, 1999), and globally affects between 18–19.7% of girls and 7.6–7.9% of boys (Stoltenborgh, Van Ijzendoorn, Euser, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference Stoltenborgh, Van Ijzendoorn, Euser and Bakermans-Kranenburg2011). These rates mirror those observed in the US (Merrick, Ford, Ports, & Guinn, Reference Merrick, Ford, Ports and Guinn2018) and likely underestimate the prevalence of child sexual abuse (CSA), given its stigmatizing nature and the reticence of those affected to report its occurrence (Katzenstein & Fontes, Reference Katzenstein and Fontes2017). Importantly, the adverse effects of CSA are far reaching and are associated with academic underachievement (Boden, Horwood, & Fergusson, Reference Boden, Horwood and Fergusson2007), underemployment (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Widom, Browne, Fergusson, Webb and Janson2009), and with difficulty forming and maintaining fulfilling social and romantic relationships (DiLillo & Long, Reference Dililio and Long1999; Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, & Smith, Reference Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis and Smith1990; Richards, Tillyer, & Wright, Reference Richards, Tillyer and Wright2017). Further, histories of CSA are linked to many adverse mental health outcomes across development (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Widom, Browne, Fergusson, Webb and Janson2009; Turner, Taillieu, Cheung, & Afifi, Reference Turner, Taillieu, Cheung and Afifi2017), of which depressive disorders are one common endpoint (Lindert et al., Reference Lindert, von Ehrenstein, Grashow, Gal, Braehler and Weisskopf2014). Indeed, CSA confers a three-to-eight fold increase in risk for developing depression before adulthood (Brown, Cohen, Johnson, & Smailes, Reference Brown, Cohen, Johnson and Smailes1999; Fergusson, Horwood, & Lynskey, Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Lynskey1996), and predicts more cumulative lifetime depressive episodes than histories of physical abuse (Gladstone et al., Reference Gladstone, Parker, Mitchell, Malhi, Wilhelm and Austin2004; Lindert et al., Reference Lindert, von Ehrenstein, Grashow, Gal, Braehler and Weisskopf2014). As functional impairment increases with the accumulation of depressive episodes (Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, Reference Lewinsohn, Rohde and Seeley1998), clarifying mechanisms that confer risk for depression among those with CSA may offset the far-reaching consequences of this recurrent disorder.

Mechanisms for depression risk among those with CSA

Though the relationship between CSA and depression is multiply determined, an increased sensitivity to stress (stress sensitization) (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Newport, Heit, Graham, Wilcox, Bonsall and Nemeroff2000; Luthar & Zigler, Reference Luthar and Zigler1991) and difficulty managing distressing emotions (emotion regulation) (Chang, Kaczkurkin, McLean, & Foa, Reference Chang, Kaczkurkin, McLean and Foa2018; Ullman, Peter-Hagene, & Relyea, Reference Ullman, Peter-Hagene and Relyea2014) may reflect two pathways by which depression risk arises for those with histories of CSA. Further, as emotion regulation deficits exacerbate adverse effects of stress (Extremera & Rey, Reference Extremera and Rey2015; Humbel et al., Reference Humbel, Messerli-Bürgy, Schuck, Wyssen, Garcia-Burgos, Biedert and Milos2018; Richardson, Reference Richardson2017), it is also feasible that the stress-sensitizing effects of CSA may in part be mediated by emotion regulation (ER) deficits. In the following sections, we examine the evidence for stress sensitization and ER deficits as key mechanisms for depression-related outcomes among individuals reporting CSA, and potential for the relationship between CSA and stress sensitization to be in part mediated by ER problems.

Stress sensitization

A diathesis for the adverse effects of stress is a component of many depression models (Hammen, Reference Hammen2005; Hankin, Reference Hankin2008), and has strong support in the empirical literature (Hammen, Reference Hammen2005; Kessler, Reference Kessler1997; Monroe & Harkness, Reference Monroe and Harkness2005). CSA may serve as one diathesis that sensitizes those with such histories to experience stress more keenly than their peers (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009), with stress sensitization evident across physiological (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Newport, Heit, Graham, Wilcox, Bonsall and Nemeroff2000) and subjective levels (Glaser, Van Os, Portegijs, & Myin-Germeys, Reference Glaser, Van Os, Portegijs and Myin-Germeys2006). For instance, studies link trauma during childhood, including CSA, with dysregulation in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, a system in the neuroendocrine stress response (Chida & Hamer, Reference Chida and Hamer2008; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, Reference Ulrich-Lai and Herman2009) that serves as a diathesis for stress in predicting suicidal ideation and self-harm behaviors (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Miller, Giletta, Hastings, Rudolph, Nock and Prinstein2018) and is associated with depression risk (Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin, & Altham, Reference Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin and Altham2000). In a similar vein, adults with histories of CSA demonstrate greater affective reactivity, worse health outcomes, and risk for depressive disorders than their peers at comparable stress levels (Glaser et al., Reference Glaser, Van Os, Portegijs and Myin-Germeys2006; Thakkar & McCanne, Reference Thakkar and McCanne2000). For example, adults who experienced unwanted sexual contact during childhood endorsed higher levels of negative affect (NA) in their daily lives when exposed to minor stressors than those without histories of CSA (Glaser et al., Reference Glaser, Van Os, Portegijs and Myin-Germeys2006) or when they perceived the day as stressful (Weltz, Armeli, Ford, & Tennen, Reference Weltz, Armeli, Ford and Tennen2016). In parallel, Thakkar and McCanne (Reference Thakkar and McCanne2000) observed a sensitivity to stress concerning self-reported symptoms of physical illness among women with histories of CSA whose daily hassles were monitored during a 1-month period. Though those with and without CSA reported similar levels of daily life hassles, somatic consequences were more severe for participants reporting CSA (Thakkar & McCanne, Reference Thakkar and McCanne2000). Importantly, findings also show that adults reporting CSA are at an increased risk for major depression than their peers when exposed to negative life events (Bandoli et al., Reference Bandoli, Campbell-Sills, Kessler, Heeringa, Nock, Rosellini and Stein2017). Taken together, these results, along with those from physiological studies, suggest that CSA may serve as a diathesis for stress, although the mechanisms by which CSA stress sensitization occurs are not entirely clear.

Emotion regulation

Considerable evidence suggests that unsuccessful efforts to downregulate distress are mechanisms for depression risk (Joormann & Gotlib, Reference Joormann and Gotlib2010). Such failures may arise from the insufficient or ineffective use of strategies that reduce distress in the short- and long-term (adaptive ER) and the abundant use of those that paradoxically exacerbate it (maladaptive ER) (Kovacs & Lopez-Duran, Reference Kovacs and Lopez-Duran2010; Kovacs, Rottenberg, & George, Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009). Abuse disrupts ER development that is believed to follow a maturational course (Heleniak, Jenness, Vander Stoep, McCauley, & McLaughlin, Reference Heleniak, Jenness, Vander Stoep, McCauley and McLaughlin2016; Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti, & Rogosch, Reference Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti and Rogosch2013; Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Lopez-Duran, George, Mayer, Baji, Kiss and Kapornai2019; Thompson, Reference Thompson2011). In support, CSA is associated with less effective ER repertoires across development (Kim-Spoon et al., Reference Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti and Rogosch2013; Séguin-Lemire, Hébert, Cossette, & Langevin, Reference Séguin-Lemire, Hébert, Cossette and Langevin2017; Ullman et al., Reference Ullman, Peter-Hagene and Relyea2014), including the frequent use of such maladaptive ER responses as rumination (Heleniak et al., Reference Heleniak, Jenness, Vander Stoep, McCauley and McLaughlin2016), avoidance (Hyman, Paliwal, & Sinha, Reference Hyman, Paliwal and Sinha2007; O'Mahen, Karl, Moberly, & Fedock, Reference O'Mahen, Karl, Moberly and Fedock2015), and substance use (Ullman et al., Reference Ullman, Peter-Hagene and Relyea2014). A small section of available literature also links CSA with the reduced use of adaptive ER responses (e.g., cognitive reappraisal, Carvalho Fernando et al., Reference Carvalho Fernando, Beblo, Schlosser, Terfehr, Otte, Löwe and Wingenfeld2014; but see Esposito & Clum, Reference Esposito and Clum2002) and deficits in processes that support adaptive ER (Kim & Cicchetti, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2010; Klanecky, Woolman, & Becker, Reference Klanecky, Woolman and Becker2015). In summary, a corpus of work associates CSA with ER deficits that may serve an intervening role between CSA and depression risk.

Stress sensitization via emotion regulation deficits

Though not examined in the literature, conceptual and empirical works support the possibility for ER deficits to mediate effects of CSA on stress sensitization. Like CSA, ER deficits have been put forward as a diathesis (Deater-Deckard, Li, & Bell, Reference Deater-Deckard, Li and Bell2016), with maladaptive ER responses exacerbating the adverse effects of stress (Extremera & Rey, Reference Extremera and Rey2015; Humbel et al., Reference Humbel, Messerli-Bürgy, Schuck, Wyssen, Garcia-Burgos, Biedert and Milos2018; Richardson, Reference Richardson2017) and adaptive responses reducing them (Troy, Wilhelm, Shallcross, & Mauss, Reference Troy, Wilhelm, Shallcross and Mauss2010). For example, those who frequently engage in emotional suppression report lower levels of positive affect (PA) when confronted with stressors in everyday life as compared to those who infrequently suppress their emotions (Richardson, Reference Richardson2017). Others found similar negative associations between state PA and emotional nonacceptance, low emotional clarity, and poor emotional awareness (Humbel et al., Reference Humbel, Messerli-Bürgy, Schuck, Wyssen, Garcia-Burgos, Biedert and Milos2018). Conversely, those who frequently engage in cognitive reappraisal have been shown to report less stress-related NA (Troy & Mauss, Reference Troy and Mauss2011) and to be protected against a reduction in PA (Deater-Deckard et al., Reference Deater-Deckard, Li and Bell2016). Such buffering effects were also noted for depression symptoms (Deater-Deckard et al., Reference Deater-Deckard, Li and Bell2016; Troy et al., Reference Troy, Wilhelm, Shallcross and Mauss2010). Therefore, as CSA is tied to ER deficits that, in turn, exacerbate the effects of stress, it is feasible that ER deficits and stress sensitization relate CSA to depression risk in a sequential manner. However, most studies that examine the relationship between CSA and depression risk focus on either stress sensitization or ER deficits, employ cross-sectional designs, and rely on self-report methods that are susceptible to reporting biases and limited self-knowledge (Simonich et al., Reference Simonich, Wonderlich, Crosby, Smyth, Thompson, Redlin and Haseltine2004).

Current study

The present study examined two conceptual models by which stress sensitization and ER deficits intervene between histories of CSA and two depression-related outcomes, contemporaneous depression symptoms and depressive affects marked by low PA and high NA (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991) across a week-long ecological momentary assessment (EMA). For each outcome, we investigated whether stress sensitization and ER deficits serve as independent, multiple mediators of CSA effects. Then, we tested a sequential mediation between CSA, ER deficits, stress sensitization, and depression-related outcomes.Footnote 1 Based on the extant literature, we hypothesized that histories of CSA would: (a) exacerbate the effects of stress on depression symptoms and depressive affects (i.e., stress sensitization), and (b) predict the reduced dispositional use of adaptive ER responses and the frequent use of maladaptive responses that, in turn, would mediate the relationship between CSA and depression-related outcomes. We also hypothesized that the effects of CSA on depression-related outcomes would be sequentially mediated via ER deficits and stress sensitization.

Method

Participants

Participants were 142 community-dwelling adults and undergraduate students (71% female, M = 26.63 years, SD = 10.81) who were recruited through online advertisements, referrals from outpatient treatment facilities, and from an undergraduate psychology student subject pool; 32 (23%) reported histories of sexual abuse before the age of 18 years. Participants’ racial composition was predominantly Caucasian (58%) and African American (23%), with some endorsing Middle Eastern (5%), multi-racial (4%), and “other” backgrounds (2%), and the remaining participants self-identifying as South Asian, South-East Asian, Native American, and Hawaiian. Of these participants, 109 (29 with histories of CSA) took part in the EMA protocol that is described later in this section; those who did not participate did not have access to cellular phones or had phones that were incompatible with our EMA software.

Seventy-one participants (50%) reported lifetime histories of depressive disorders (45% major depressive disorder, 4% dysthymic disorder, 1% depressive disorder nos), of whom 26 were in a major depressive episode at the time of the study. Participants also evidenced lifetime histories of generalized anxiety disorder (23%), social anxiety (14%), panic disorder (12%), specific phobia (12%) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (6%). Twenty-three participants’ (16%) histories were free of lifetime psychiatric disorders.

Psychosocial and clinical evaluation

Current and lifetime histories of psychiatric disorders were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (fourth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders) Disorders (SCID-I) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1994) by advanced graduate students and one of the authors (IY). The SCID-I is a well-validated measure of psychiatric disorders that evidenced good inter-rater reliability in this study (SCID-I, Fleiss’ κ = .91). Histories of “sexual abuse or rape” were ascertained during the clinical interview, as were the age and circumstance of their experience. A positive history of CSA reflects unwanted sexual contact from an adult that occurred before the participant reached the age of majority. All mandated reporting procedures were followed when instances of CSA were disclosed.

Measures

Depression symptoms

The Center for Epidemiological Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, Reference Radloff1977) was used to measure participants’ depression levels during the prior week. The CES-D is a well-validated 20-item scale that demonstrated good psychometric properties in this study (α = .90).

Perceived stress

The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983) is a well-validated 10-item survey that measures the degree to which respondents appraise events during the prior month as unpredictable, uncontrollable, and stressful. The PSS had good psychometric properties in this study (α = .93).

Emotion regulation

The Feelings and Me (FAM; Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009) questionnaire is a 54-item survey of adaptive and maladaptive cognitive, behavioral, and interpersonal responses that are dispositionally deployed in order to attenuate sadness and upset feelings. These responses measure mood repair, a component of ER dealing with downregulation of distress and dysphoria (Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009). Response repertoires are aggregated into adaptive and maladaptive ER indices that have shown excellent clinical validity in predicting incidents of depressive episodes and their recurrence (Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009, Reference Kovacs, Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg, George, Kiss, Halas and Makai2016). Both adaptive and maladaptive ER indices showed strong internal consistency in this study (α = .88–.89).

Depressive affects

EMA indices of PA and NA were measured using items drawn in part from the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988). Participants responded via a 5-point Likert scale (0 = very slightly/not at all and 4 = extremely) to adjectives indexing PA (happy & excited) and NA (sad & upset) concerning how they “feel at this moment.” Item pairs were aggregated to form PA and NA indices that evidenced good internal consistency at each EMA assessment across the measurement period (PA average α = .83; NA average α = .78).

Daily life stress

Stress in daily life reflected the degree to which respondents perceived stress at the time of the EMA prompt. Participants responded via a 5-point Likert scale (0 = very slightly/not at all and 4 = extremely) concerning how “stressed” they “feel at this moment.”

Overall proceduresFootnote 2

Study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board and executed in two parts: a laboratory visit and an EMA protocol. During the laboratory visit, participants completed psychosocial and clinical evaluations, survey measures, and experimental procedures, the data from which are not included in this study. Participants were then oriented to the EMA protocol, familiarized with the EMA questions, and signed up to receive text messaged links for the EMA survey on their cell phones using the SurveySignal software (Hofmann & Patel, Reference Hofmann and Patel2015) or software akin to SurveySignal that was developed for one of the authors (IY). Participants were compensated for their time following their lab visit and EMA completion.

EMA sampling generally occurred five times between 9 a.m. and 9 p.m. during the 7–8 contiguous days following the laboratory visit via fixed prompts that sampled participants’ affective states evenly across the morning, afternoon, and evening hours. Following best practices (Mehl & Conner, Reference Mehl and Conner2012), participants were sent a reminder prompt 15 min after receiving the initial text message within a given sampling period and were allowed a 30-min window to complete a given EMA survey before its deactivation.

Statistical analyses

Data analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 software. Missing values comprised less than 9% of cross-sectional survey data, with the exception of the PSS for which data were missing by design for 8% of respondents, and was partially completed by 8% of those who received the measure. Participants responded on average to 78% of EMA prompts (3,016 of the possible 3,870), which reflects a high level of compliance (Courvoisier, Eid, & Lischetzke, Reference Courvoisier, Eid and Lischetzke2012), and less than 2% of data were missing from incomplete or skipped responses. Missing value analyses revealed that cross-sectional and EMA data were missing completely at random, Little's χ2 (16–18) = 12.95–21.22, p = .17–.28. Following best practices (Enders, Reference Enders2010), we employed multiple imputation to recover missing values in order to reduce the analytic bias that is associated with their presence. Specifically, we imputed 200 data sets via the estimation–maximization (EM) algorithm that were then used in hypothesis testing: statistical models were fit to each data set, and the resulting parameter estimates were pooled across imputations (Enders, Reference Enders2010).

Depression symptoms models

We employed moderation, mediation, and moderated-mediation models using cross-sectional data from the full sample (N = 142) to test (a) stress sensitization effect of sexual abuse histories on depression symptoms, (b) the mediation of sexual abuse history effect on depression symptoms by adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation repertoires, and (c) the sequential mediation of sexual abuse effect on depression symptoms via emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization. In these models, stress sensitization reflects the CSA moderation of perceived stress effect on depression symptoms, a common methodological approach to modeling diathesis–stress relationships (e.g., Monroe & Harkness, Reference Monroe and Harkness2005; Morris, Ciesla, & Garber, Reference Morris, Ciesla and Garber2010).

First, we tested the effects of CSA on stress sensitization and the two ER indices to establish relationships between our predictor of interest and intervening variables. Then, akin to multiple mediation, we regressed depression symptoms on CSA, stress sensitization, and ER, and, where appropriate, calculated simple slopes and indirect effects of CSA onto our outcome of interest. Finally, akin to sequential mediation, we tested the indirect effects of CSA on stress sensitization and subsequent depression symptoms via adaptive and maladaptive ER indices. This model is mathematically equivalent to the moderated-mediation model, wherein the indirect effects of sexual abuse via ER are conditional (Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, Reference Preacher, Rucker and Hayes2007; K. Preacher, personal communication, October 26, Reference Preacher2018).

Following best practices, asymmetric confidence intervals (CIs) around indirect effects were estimated using the PRODCLIN method (Tofighi & Thoemmes, Reference Tofighi and Thoemmes2014) in the RMediation package (Tofighi & MacKinnon, Reference Tofighi and MacKinnon2011), as were the CIs for the conditional indirect effects that were modeled at the uncentered values of CSA, and at one standard deviation above and below the means of the ER indices.

Depressive affects models

Our general approach to testing our hypotheses with EMA data from the reduced sample (n = 109) mirrored that of the depression symptom models; we first tested the effects of CSA on stress sensitization and ER indices, as well as the multiple and sequential intervening effects of stress sensitization and ER between CSA and depressive affects. However, these models were conducted in a multilevel framework to accommodate the nesting of EMA responses (Level 1) within observation days (Level 2) and participants (Level 3), as well as the notable within-person stability of affective states (Level 3 ICCNA = .50 and ICCPA = .46, ps < .001) and the influence of observation days on both affects (Level 2 ICCNA = .09 and ICCPA = .21, ps < .001). Also, we tested our hypothesized associations between sexual abuse, ER and stress sensitization with respect to the contemporaneous effects of stress on NA and PA at the time of the EMA prompt (contemporaneous models), as well as their prospective effects on both affective outcomes (prospective models); separate models were fit for each affective outcome.

Following best practices, EMA-based stress measurements were decomposed into their time-invariant (participant's average stress level over the measurement period) and time-varying components (deviation from participant's average stress levels at a given EMA observation) (Algina & Swaminathan, Reference Algina, Swaminathan, Hox and Roberts2011; Enders & Tofighi, Reference Enders and Tofighi2007). Continuous Level 3 predictors were grand mean centered (i.e., age and ER indices) (Enders & Tofighi, Reference Enders and Tofighi2007) while dichotomous categorical variables (i.e., gender and CSA) retained their original metric (Singer & Willett, Reference Singer and Willett2003).

In contemporaneous models, stress sensitization effects reflect the Level 3 interaction between CSA and participants’ average stress levels and the cross-level interaction between CSA and within-person deviation from their average stress level at the time of the EMA prompt. In prospective models, stress sensitization is indicated by the previously described Level 3 interaction and cross-level interaction between CSA and within-person deviation from a given participant's average stress level at the prior EMA prompt. In these models, the effects of affective state at the prior EMA prompt, time since the prior EMA prompt, and their interaction were covaried to accommodate the unequal temporal distance between affect measurements. Random intercept and slopes were estimated for Level 1 variables as warranted (see Supplemental material for model equations).

Results

Sample characteristics

Demographic, psychiatric, and psychosocial characteristics of those with and without histories of CSA are presented in Table 1. Those who experienced CSA were approximately 11 years older and more likely to be women. They also reported higher levels of perceived stress and depression symptoms and were overrepresented among those with depression histories and those in the midst of a depressive episode. Those with CSA did not significantly differ from their peers in their racial distributions, nor histories of anxiety disorders. To account for differences in demographic characteristics, the effects of age and gender were statistically covaried in models.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics, psychiatric parameters, and perceived stress levels of those with and without childhood sexual abuse (CSA) histories

Note. Dep. D/O = depressive disorder (major depression, dysthymic disorder, or depressive disorder NOS), Anx. D/O = anxiety disorder (generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder, panic disorder, specific phobia, and posttraumatic stress disorder), Depression Sx = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale, Perceived Stress = Perceived Stress Scale.

a “Other” reflects aggregated racial categories to accommodate 0 frequency cells.

Do emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization mediate effects of CSA on depression symptoms?

Stress sensitization

As hypothesized, our results showed that, independent of demographic characteristics, histories of CSA and high levels of perceived stress were associated with concurrent elevation in depression symptoms, BCSA = 7.37, t (137) = 2.70, p = .008, BPSS = 10.73, t (137) = 3.56, p < .001. However, the association between perceived stress and depression symptoms did not vary as a function of abuse histories, as indicated by the nonsignificant second-order effect of the two variables (see Table 2).

Table 2. Multiple and sequential mediation of child sexual abuse history's effect on depression symptoms via stress sensitization and emotion regulation

Note. Sex (0 = female, 1 = male), Stress = Perceived Stress Scale, CSA = child sexual abuse (0 = absent, 1 = present), aER = Feeling and Me Scale, adaptive subscale, mER = Feeling and Me Scale, maladaptive subscale, Depression Symptoms = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.

***p ≤ .001, **p ≤ .01, *p ≤ .05, p ≤ .10.

Emotion regulation

Next, we tested whether ER deficits could serve as mediators of CSA by regressing dispositional adaptive and maladaptive ER repertoires on abuse group membership, and then adding both emotion regulation indices to the stress sensitization model described above (see Table 2). In support of their potential mediation effects, those with histories of sexual abuse reported deploying maladaptive ER responses more readily, BCSA = 6.97, t (138) = 3.56, p < .001, and engaging in adaptive ER responses less frequently during times of distress than their abuse-free peers, BCSA = - 4.78, t (138) = 2.13, p = .04. In turn, the frequent use of maladaptive ER responses predicted elevation in depression symptoms, BmER = .36, t (134) = 8.37, p < .001, while adaptive ER reduced depression symptoms, BaER = −.18, t (134) = 2.95, p = .004. Both ER indices mediated the direct effects of CSA on depression symptoms, BindmER = 2.54, 95% CI .70–5.01, BindaER = .84 95% CI .04–1.99.

Do emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization sequentially mediate effects of CSA on depression symptoms?

To test this possibility, second-order effects between each emotion regulation index and perceived stress were added to the previously described “multiple mediation” models in order to test the conditional indirect effects of abuse on depression symptoms via ER deficits and stress sensitization (see Table 2). Because stress sensitization reflects a moderation effect, indirect effects were examined via simple slopes at one standard deviation above and below the mean of a given ER index (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2003).

As hypothesized, maladaptive ER repertoires significantly moderated the effect of perceived stress on depression, BmERxPSS = .02, t (132) = 1.99, p = .05 (see Figure 1). Simple slopes analyses that probed the interaction revealed greater depression severity as a function of stress for those with elevated maladaptive ER levels, BmER,+1SD = .97, t (134) = 7.57, p < .001, relative to those with low maladaptive ER repertoires, BmER,−1SD = .61, t (134) = 4.84, p < .001. Tests of conditional indirect effects revealed significant mediation of CSA on depression via the moderation of perceived stress by maladaptive ER, BindmER,+1SD = 6.79, 95% CI 2.90–11.22, BindmER,−1SD = 4.27, 95% CI 1.66–7.54. In contrast to expectation, adaptive ER repertoires did not attenuate the adverse effect of perceived stress on depression, and therefore did not sequentially mediate the effects of CSA on depression via stress sensitization.

Figure 1. Maladaptive emotion regulation (mER) moderation of perceived stress effects on depression symptoms.

Do emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization mediate effects of CSA on depressive affects in daily life?

We examined this question in two ways, by testing the effects of CSA, stress sensitization, and ER repertoires on contemporaneous and prospective associations between stress and depressive affects (see Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3. Multiple and sequential mediation of child sexual abuse history's effects on contemporaneous negative and positive affects in daily life via stress sensitization and emotion regulation

Note. NA = negative affect, PA = positive affect, Sex (0 = female, 1 = male), wSt = deviation in average stress level at EMA prompt, PmSt = average stress level across EMA period, CSA = childhood sexual abuse (0 = absent, 1 = present), aER = Feeling and Me Scale, adaptive subscale, mER = Feeling and Me Scale, maladaptive subscale.

***p ≤ .001, **p ≤ .01, *p ≤ .05, p ≤ .10

Table 4. Multiple and sequential mediation of child sexual abuse history's effects on prospective negative and positive affects in daily life via stress sensitization and emotion regulation.

Note. NA = negative affect, PA = positive affect, Time = interval between current and prior EMA prompt, DVt−1 = NA or PA level at prior EMA prompt, Sex (0 = female, 1 = male), wStt−1 = deviation in average stress level at prior EMA prompt, PmSt = average stress level across EMA period, CSA = childhood sexual abuse (0 = absent, 1 = present), aER = Feeling and Me Scale, adaptive subscale, mER = Feeling and Me Scale, maladaptive subscale.

***p ≤ .001, **p ≤ .01, *p ≤ .05, p ≤ .10

In the contemporaneous models, CSA predicted NA elevation, γCSA = 1.38, t (103) = 5.07, p < .001), but was unrelated to PA. In a similar vein, participants’ average stress level during the measurement period predicted elevation in NA, γPmStress = 1.02, t (103) = 4.79, p < .001, and reduction in PA at a trend level, γPmStress = −.31, t (103) = 1.66, p = .10, as did momentary increases in stress at the time of the EMA prompt that significantly predicted both NA elevation, γwStress = .41, t (103) = 7.08, p < .001, and PA reduction, γwStress = −.64, t (103) = 11.81, p < .001.

In the prospective models, sexual abuse histories evidenced a trend for worsening NA over time, γCSA = .41, t (100) = 1.75, p = .08. Participants’ average stress level during the measurement period also predicted elevation in NA, γPmStress = .79, t (100) = 4.89, p < .001, as did momentary increases in stress at the time of the prior EMA prompt at a trend level, γwStress = .07, t (100) = 1.88, p = .06. Neither CSA nor the two stress indices predicted change in PA levels over time.

Stress sensitization

In support of their stress sensitization effect, sexual abuse histories evidenced a cross-level interaction with participants’ momentary deviations from their average stress levels to predict contemporaneous NA, γCSAxwStress = .41, t (103) = 3.42, p < .001 (see Figure 2, Panel A). Simple slopes analysis revealed a stronger association between stress and concurrent NA levels for those with sexual abuse histories, γCSA = .82, t (103) = 3.78, p < .001, than for their peers, γNoCSA = .41, t (103) = 6.83, p < .001. Sexual abuse histories did not moderate the relationship between participants’ average stress levels and NA, nor between both stress indices and PA.

Figure 2. Childhood sexual abuse moderation of within-subject stress fluctuations on contemporaneous negative affect (NA) (Panel A), and change (Δ) in NA (Panel B) and positive affect (Panel C) across successive ecological momentary assessment (EMA) prompts.

In the prospective models, sexual abuse histories and deviations from participants’ average stress levels during the prior EMA prompt jointly predicted significant change in subsequent PA, γCSAxwStress = −.19, t (98) = 2.13, p = .03, as well as change in NA at a trend level, BCSAxwStress = .13, t (98) = 1.74, p = .08 (see Figure 2, Panels B and C). Consistent with stress sensitization, CSA was associated with reduced PA across successive EMA observations, γwStress = −.13, t (98) = 1.98, p = .05, and increased NA, γwStress = .19, t (98) = 2.89, p = .005. Increased stress relative to average levels was unrelated to change in PA and NA for those without sexual abuse histories, γs = .06, t (98) = 1.09–1.40, ps = .17–.28.

Emotion regulation

In support of emotion regulation's role in distress among those with CSA, sexual abuse histories predicted elevated maladaptive, B = 7.03, t (105) = 3.21, p = .002, and reduced adaptive repertoires, B = −4.81, t (105) = 1.99, p = .05, across contemporaneous and prospective models. In turn, adaptive ER repertoires predicted elevations in contemporaneous PA levels, γ = .07, t (96) = 3.41, p = .002, that increased over successive EMA prompts, γ = .06, t (96) = 4.55, p < .001, as well as mediated the effects of CSA on PA contemporaneously γindaER = −.35 95% CI −.81–−.003, and prospectively, γindaER = −.29 95% CI −.65–−.002. Adaptive ER repertoires were unrelated to NA indices. Though unrelated in contemporaneous models, maladaptive ER repertoires predicted NA in the prospective model, γmER = .04, t (96) = 3.61, p < .001, and mediated the effect of CSA on increased NA across successive EMA observations, γindbER = .25 95% CI .11–.49. Maladaptive repertoires were unrelated to indices of PA.

Do emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization sequentially mediate effects of CSA on depression affects in daily life?

We tested this possibility by adding second-order effects of the adaptive and maladaptive ER repertoires with indices of stress in the contemporaneous and prospective models. In support of sequential mediation, maladaptive ER moderated contemporaneous effects of participants’ average stress levels and their momentary deviations on NA, γmERxPmStress = .04, t (95) = 2.08, p = .04, γmERxwStress = .02, t (95) = 2.44, p = .02, (see Figure 3, Panels A and B), and the effects of participants’ average stress levels in the prospective NA model, γmER = .04, t (92) = 2.18, p = .03 (see Figure 3, Panel C). Post hoc probes of the interaction terms revealed that tendencies to deploy maladaptive ER responses exacerbated the effects of stress on contemporaneous NA across participants’ average stress levels, γPmStress,+1SD = 1.08, t (95) = 4.25, p < .001, γPmStress, −1SD = .41, t (95) = 1.73, p = .09, and momentary deviations from those levels γwStress,+1SD = .59, t (95) = 5.78, p < .001, γwStress, −1SD = .25, t (95) = 4.08, p < .001. Similar effects emerged in the prospective model, whereby maladaptive ER potentiated the adverse effects of participants’ average stress levels on NA over time, BPmStress,+1SD = .94, t (92) = 4.29, p < .001, γPmStress, −1SD = .28, t (92) = 1.22, p = .22. Importantly, and as hypothesized, the frequent use of maladaptive ER responses mediated stress-sensitization effects of CSA on contemporaneous NA levels, PmStress: γindmER,+1SD = 7.60, 95% CI 2.46–14.22, γindmER,−1SD = 2.85, 95% CI −.34–7.20; wStress γindmER,+1SD = 4.15, 95% CI 1.49–7.35; γindmER,−1SD = 1.78, 95% CI .56–3.35, and their change over time, γmStress : γmER+1SD = 6.64, 95% CI 2.16–12.40; γmER−1SD = 1.96, 95% CI −1.16–5.93.

Figure 3. Maladaptive emotion regulation (mER) moderation of average stress level (Panel A) and within-subject stress fluctuations (Panel B) on concurrent negative affect (NA), and average stress levels on change (Δ) in NA (Panel C) across successive ecological momentary assessment prompts.

Surprisingly, though adaptive ER repertoires moderated the effects of within-participant stress fluctuations on PA, γaERxwStress = −.01, t (95) = 2.00, p = .05, the frequent use of adaptive responses was paradoxically associated with a reduction in PA as participants’ stress increased relative to average levels, γaER, +1SD = −.73, t (95) = 8.84, p < .001, γaER, −1SD = −.51, t (95) = 6.87, p < .001 (see Figure 4), and mediated stress-sensitization effects of sexual abuse on concurrent PA via contemporaneous stress fluctuations, wStress γindaER,+1SD = 3.49, 95% CI .05–7.18; γindaER,−1SD = 2.47, 95% CI .03–5.18. Adaptive ER did not moderate effects of either stress index on NA across contemporaneous or prospective models.

Figure 4. Adaptive emotion regulation (aER) moderation of within-subject stress fluctuations on contemporaneous hedonic affect.

Discussion

CSA confers a considerable risk across the life span for developing depressive disorders (Lindert et al., Reference Lindert, von Ehrenstein, Grashow, Gal, Braehler and Weisskopf2014; Turner et al., Reference Turner, Taillieu, Cheung and Afifi2017), with stress sensitization and emotion regulation deficits put forth as plausible mechanisms through which such risk is conferred (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Newport, Heit, Graham, Wilcox, Bonsall and Nemeroff2000; Ullman et al., Reference Ullman, Peter-Hagene and Relyea2014). Yet, though both are associated with CSA and depression (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009), it remains unclear whether stress sensitization and emotion regulation deficits contribute to depression risk independently or sequentially. We fit multiple and sequential mediation models to test whether stress sensitization and emotion regulation deficits mediate associations between histories of CSA and depression outcomes independently of one another, or whether the more pronounced effects of stress for those with CSA arise from a reliance on maladaptive responses and insufficient use of adaptive strategies.

Overall, results supported a sequential mediation between CSA and depression outcomes: histories of CSA predicted greater tendencies to deploy maladaptive emotion regulation responses that, in turn, exacerbated the effects of stress on depression severity. Further, maladaptive emotion regulation repertoires mediated the contemporaneous effects of daily life stress on NA levels and their increase across successive EMA observations. Though the reduction in adaptive emotion regulation repertoires mediated the direct effects of CSA on depression symptoms and PA in prospective EMA models, their intervening role between CSA and stress sensitization were inconsistent and paradoxical, as evidenced by the reduced PA in response to stress among those with CSA who frequently deployed adaptive responses.

Our results suggest that the relationship between CSA, stress, and depression is nuanced, as stress may have both transient and enduring components. Regarding the latter, we found higher perceived stress levels among those with CSA, for whom exploratory analyses also revealed higher average stress in daily life relative to their peers (M CSA = 2.17 vs. M NoCSA = 1.75, p = .03).Footnote 3 In turn, with few exceptions, elevation in stress levels predicted parallel increases in depression symptoms and depressive affects. These findings are in accord with results from a number of survey, laboratory, and experience sampling studies that connect CSA to elevated stress levels (Bandoli et al., Reference Bandoli, Campbell-Sills, Kessler, Heeringa, Nock, Rosellini and Stein2017; Glaser et al., Reference Glaser, Van Os, Portegijs and Myin-Germeys2006; Luthar & Zigler, Reference Luthar and Zigler1991), and with a large body of work associating stress withdepression (Hammen, Reference Hammen2005; Hankin, Reference Hankin2008). Though not of primary interest in this study, the strong relationship between CSA, stress, and depression outcomes may suggest that stress serves an intervening role between CSA and depression.

In contrast, and of primary importance, CSA-linked stress sensitization emerged only in response to fluctuations in participants’ stress levels. That is, as compared to their peers, those with CSA whose stress increased relative to their average levels reported more NA and less PA that worsened over time. Importantly, some CSA stress-sensitization effects were maintained when the emotion regulation indices were added to the model, suggesting that the vulnerability for depression conferred by a history of CSA exceeds that which is accounted for by dispositional emotion regulation repertoires.

The distinct pattern of associations between CSA and stress has both substantive and methodological implications. Our findings imply that mechanisms predisposing those with CSA to experience ongoing stress may be distinct from those that potentiate transient stress responses. Consistent with this possibility, empirical findings differentiate basal stress levels and their reactivity across physiological and subjective dimensions (Henckens et al., Reference Henckens, Klumpers, Everaerd, Kooijman, van Wingen and Fernández2016; Sliwinski, Almeida, Stawski, & Smyth, Reference Sliwinski, Almeida, Stawski and Smyth2009). For example, at the physiological level, resting cortisol, a neuroendocrine marker of perceived stress (van Eck & Nicolson, Reference van Eck and Nicolson1994), evidenced an inverse pattern of amygdala activity and emotion processing relative to cortisol reactivity in response to a psychological stress (Henckens et al., Reference Henckens, Klumpers, Everaerd, Kooijman, van Wingen and Fernández2016). Further, the two physiological stress indices were shown to be orthogonal and differed in associations with personality dimensions (Henckens et al., Reference Henckens, Klumpers, Everaerd, Kooijman, van Wingen and Fernández2016). In a similar vein, day-to-day fluctuations in perceived stress levels have been shown to differentially predict dysphoric states in daily life from more enduring indices of perceived stress (Sliwinski et al., Reference Sliwinski, Almeida, Stawski and Smyth2009). Given the complexity of the stress response, our results echo the call to take a multiple-levels-of-analyses perspective (Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002) to study shared and specific processes that underpin the stress experience of those with histories of CSA, both with respect to stress levels and their fluctuations.

Our findings also suggest that emotion regulation deficits play seminal, but complicated roles in the relationship between CSA and risk for depression. As in our study, CSA has been associated with the infrequent use of adaptive responses and reliance on maladaptive strategies and (Carvalho Fernando et al., Reference Carvalho Fernando, Beblo, Schlosser, Terfehr, Otte, Löwe and Wingenfeld2014; Heleniak et al., Reference Heleniak, Jenness, Vander Stoep, McCauley and McLaughlin2016) that prognosticate new incidence of depression (Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009, Reference Kovacs, Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg, George, Kiss, Halas and Makai2016), and that meta-analytic reviews tie to depression severity (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Webb, Miles, & Sheeran, Reference Webb, Miles and Sheeran2012) and depression status (Visted, Vøllestad, Nielsen, & Schanche, Reference Visted, Vøllestad, Nielsen and Schanche2018). However, as depression at the affective level may reflect a combination of high NA and low PA (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991), global measures of depression symptoms feasibly obscure the unique ties between emotion regulation deficits and the two affective domains. Indeed, our results and those of others suggest some specificity between emotion regulation repertoires and affective states, with adaptive responses linked to PA (Brans, Koval, Verduyn, Lim, & Kuppens, Reference Brans, Koval, Verduyn, Lim and Kuppens2013) and maladaptive ones with NA (Chaudhury et al., Reference Chaudhury, Galfalvy, Biggs, Choo, Mann and Stanley2017).

As in our study, deploying adaptive strategies in response to distress predicted increased PA in the daily lives of healthy adults (Brans et al., Reference Brans, Koval, Verduyn, Lim and Kuppens2013, Study 2; Brockman, Ciarrochi, Parker, & Kashdan, Reference Brockman, Ciarrochi, Parker and Kashdan2017) and patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD) (Chaudhury et al., Reference Chaudhury, Galfalvy, Biggs, Choo, Mann and Stanley2017), but was less or unrelated with NA downregulation (Brockman et al., Reference Brockman, Ciarrochi, Parker and Kashdan2017; Chaudhury et al., Reference Chaudhury, Galfalvy, Biggs, Choo, Mann and Stanley2017; but see Blalock, Kashdan, & Farmer, Reference Blalock, Kashdan and Farmer2016 and Nezlek & Kuppens, Reference Nezlek and Kuppens2008). Conversely, efforts to “find perspective,” a strategy akin to rumination, and to suppress NA predicted NA upregulation (Chaudhury et al., Reference Chaudhury, Galfalvy, Biggs, Choo, Mann and Stanley2017; Nezlek & Kuppens, Reference Nezlek and Kuppens2008; but see Blalock et al., Reference Blalock, Kashdan and Farmer2016 and Brockman et al., Reference Brockman, Ciarrochi, Parker and Kashdan2017), but not PA (Chaudhury et al., Reference Chaudhury, Galfalvy, Biggs, Choo, Mann and Stanley2017).

We do not imply a strong specificity between emotion regulation repertoires and affective domains. Rather, as sadness and dysphoria are more commonly observed symptom in depression than anhedonia (Baji et al., Reference Baji, Lopez-Duran, Kovacs, George, Mayer, Kapornai and Vetró2009; Smith, Joiner, Pettit, Lewinsohn, & Schmidt, Reference Smith, Joiner, Pettit, Lewinsohn and Schmidt2008), we believe that further inquiry into the link between adaptive strategies and PA may shed light on findings that suggest a weak and inconsistent association between adaptive emotion regulation repertoires and depression (Kovacs & Yaroslavsky, Reference Kovacs and Yaroslavsky2014; Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009, Reference Kovacs, Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg, George, Kiss, Halas and Makai2016; Yaroslavsky, Bylsma, Rottenberg, & Kovacs, Reference Yaroslavsky, Bylsma, Rottenberg and Kovacs2013).

Of consequence, our results suggest that emotion regulation repertoires play a critical role in stress sensitization that is observed among those with sexual abuse histories. In particular, the consistent and pernicious effects of maladaptive repertoires on the relationship between stress and depression outcomes across reporting methods, along with their direct effects, signals their key role in depression risk (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Extremera & Rey, Reference Extremera and Rey2015). This observation is aligned with literature that ties maladaptive emotion regulation to concurrent and prospective depression symptom elevation (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Just & Alloy, Reference Just and Alloy1997) and risk for incidents of depressive episodes (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema2000) and their recurrence (Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Rottenberg and George2009, Reference Kovacs, Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg, George, Kiss, Halas and Makai2016). These associations do not appreciably attenuate when the use of adaptive emotion regulation responses is statistically controlled.

The key mechanistic role of maladaptive repertoires is further bolstered by the limited and inconsistent predictive value of adaptive response repertoires that, though associated with depression severity (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010), are markedly weakened or drop below a level of significance in the presence of maladaptive responses. Our finding stress-potentiating effects of adaptive emotion regulation repertoires on concurrently measured PA adds to the uncertain role of adaptive responses in depression risk. How may stress-sensitizing effects of adaptive emotion regulation repertoires be understood? It is feasible that the observed effect masks a higher-order interaction wherein adaptive responses are iatrogenic rather than salubrious for those with CSA. For example, results from laboratory studies show that some vulnerable groups (e.g., depressed participants) experience worsening moods after deploying a putatively adaptive response (e.g., Joormann, Siemer, & Gotlib, Reference Joormann, Siemer and Gotlib2007). The same may be true for individuals with CSA. Future works that examine whether CSA moderates the effects of adaptive emotion regulation on affective outcomes would shed light on our findings.

Our results should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. There is evidence that sexual abuse during the childhood years can vary in intensity, frequency, source, and developmental timing, parameters that may affect stress sensitization, emotion regulation development, and depression risk. For example, age of onset, severity, source, and frequency of sexual abuse differentially predict difficulties in emotional (Clemmons, Walsh, Dilillo, & Messman-moore, Reference Clemmons, Walsh, Dilillo and Messman-moore2007; Kaplow & Widom, Reference Kaplow and Widom2007) and behavioral difficulties (Ruggiero, McLeer, & Dixon, Reference Ruggiero, McLeer and Dixon2000) across adulthood. Therefore, it is feasible that those whose abuse began early in life, was more severe, and chronic would evidence greater stress sensitization and emotion regulation deficits than those who experienced a single instance of abuse in late adolescence. Though detailed accounts of trauma were collected during the clinical interview, some participants were circumspect in their disclosure, thereby precluding an examination of several important CSA parameters in this study. Further, as those with CSA frequently experience other forms of abuse (Finkelhor, Turner, Shattuck, & Hamby, Reference Finkelhor, Turner, Shattuck and Hamby2015), it is feasible that accompanying histories of physical violence and neglect may have contributed to our results.

There is also some evidence that gender differences exist in the long-term consequences of CSA (Gray & Rarick, Reference Gray and Rarick2018), and in the stress response across physiological (Kudielka & Kirschbaum, Reference Kudielka and Kirschbaum2005) and subjective levels (Kelly, Tyrka, Anderson, Price, & Carpenter, Reference Kelly, Tyrka, Anderson, Price and Carpenter2008). Although our analyses controlled for gender, it is feasible that the relationship between CSA, emotion regulation, stress sensitization, and depression-related outcomes may vary between men and women, a possibility that we could not examine due to our small sample size of participants reporting CSA. Relatedly, emotion regulation outcomes are known to be contextual (Aldao, Sheppes, & Gross, Reference Aldao, Sheppes and Gross2015) and it is feasible that measuring state emotion regulation responses during EMA would have provided greater insights into the relationship between CSA and stress sensitization.

Although we proposed a sequential relationship between emotion regulation deficits and stress sensitization, a reverse effect is possible; CSA stress sensitization may lead to emotion regulation deficits by undermining normative emotion regulation development. We did not test this possibility because emotion regulation repertoires appear to slow in their development by mid-adolescence (see Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Lopez-Duran, George, Mayer, Baji, Kiss and Kapornai2019), and we used an adult sample in this study.

Finally, though not uncommon in EMA studies, our single-item measure of stress may have contributed to greater measurement error than what could be expected from the use of a longer survey. Future works that via longitudinal designs examine the effects of CSA parameters, co-occurring physical abuse and neglect histories, contextual effects of gender on emotion regulation across development, and measure stress during experience sampling via multiple items would do much to clarify the mechanisms through which CSA confers risk for depressive disorders.

Nevertheless, this study has a number of strengths. First, the use of mixed methodology enabled us to test concurrent and prospective effects of stress sensitization in the daily lives of adults with histories of CSA as they encountered ideographic stressors, which increased the generalizability of our findings beyond that offered through cross-sectional designs that are commonly seen in the literature. Second, by disambiguating daily life stress into its stable and time-varying components, we were able to show distinct ties between emotion regulation and stress sensitization, in particular, that stress sensitization emerges in response to perturbations in stress levels rather than the absolute stress magnitudes. Finally, to our knowledge, this study is the first to test the roles of putative adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation response repertoires in stress sensitization among those with histories of CSA.

Conclusion

In summary, our results have methodological and clinical implications as they point to emotion regulation deficits as key mechanisms for stress sensitization and depression-related outcomes, as well as a need to measure stress as a dynamic process that may precipitate depressive states differently depending on the nature of said deficits. Further, our findings may generalize to other disorders as CSA, stress sensitization, and emotion regulation deficits are transdiagnostic risk factors. This, in particular, may be the case for PTSD, which is associated with all three risk factors and depression (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Kaczkurkin, McLean and Foa2018; John, Cisler, & Sigel, Reference John, Cisler and Sigel2017; McLaughlin, Conron, Koenen, & Gilman, Reference McLaughlin, Conron, Koenen and Gilman2010). Experience sampling methods may therefore provide clinicians with an inexpensive means to detect and target specific emotion regulation deficits that likely differ across those with CSA. Such innovations may prove efficacious for treating PTSD and other emotional disorders that are associated with CSA.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942000098X.

Financial Support

This research was supported by Cleveland State University's Undergraduate Summer Research Award (PI: Yaroslavsky) and Faculty Research Development Award (PI: Yaroslavsky).

Footnotes

*

I. Yaroslavsky and A. H. Bush contributed equally to this work.

1 We treat stress sensitization as a single construct when describing its conceptual relationships with CSA, ER, and depression-related outcomes. Statistically, however, stress sensitization is measured as the moderation of stress by CSA. Therefore, though we refer to stress sensitization and ER deficits as intervening variables in mediation models, stress sensitization indexes the conditional effects of stress for those with histories of CSA in multiple mediation models, and the conditional indirect effects of CSA via ER deficits in the sequential mediation models.

2 Eleven participants were drawn from a study that involved an 8-day EMA measurement period and did not include the PSS. Data from the PSS were missing by design for these participants and was therefore not included in the Little's χ2 calculation (Enders, Reference Enders2010).

3 In an exploratory analysis, we tested whether akin to perceived stress (see Table 1), histories of CSA were associated with elevated average stress levels across the EMA measurement period. Independent of the effects of age (B = −.01, p = .08) and gender (B = −.07. p = .70), those with histories of CSA reported higher average stress levels than their peers (B = .43, p = .03).

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibility. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39, 263278. doi:10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Algina, J., & Swaminathan, H. (2011). Centering in two-level nested designs. In Hox, J. J. & Roberts, J. K. (Eds.), European association for methodology series. Handbook for advanced multilevel analysis (pp. 285312). New York, NY, USA: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 183191. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baji, I., Lopez-Duran, N. L., Kovacs, M., George, C. J., Mayer, L., Kapornai, K., … Vetró, Á. (2009). Age and sex analyses of somatic complaints and symptom presentation of childhood depression in a Hungarian clinical sample. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 14671472. doi:10.4088/JCP.08m04918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandoli, G., Campbell-Sills, L., Kessler, R. C., Heeringa, S. G., Nock, M. K., Rosellini, A. J., … Stein, M. B. (2017). Childhood adversity, adult stress, and the risk of major depression or generalized anxiety disorder in US soldiers: A test of the stress sensitization hypothesis. Psychological Medicine, 47, 23792392. doi:10.1017/S0033291717001064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blalock, D. V., Kashdan, T. B., & Farmer, A. S. (2016). Trait and daily emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 416425. doi:10.1007/s10608-015-9739-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, J. M., Horwood, L. J., & Fergusson, D. M. (2007). Exposure to childhood sexual and physical abuse and subsequent educational achievement outcomes. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 11011114. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.03.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emotion, 13, 926939. doi:10.1037/a0032400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., & Kashdan, T. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in daily life: Mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 46, 91113. doi:10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, J., Cohen, P., Johnson, J. G., & Smailes, E. M. (1999). Childhood abuse and neglect: Specificity of effects on adolescent and young adult depression and suicidality. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 14901496. doi:10.1097/00004583-199912000-00009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carvalho Fernando, S., Beblo, T., Schlosser, N., Terfehr, K., Otte, C., Löwe, B., … Wingenfeld, K. (2014). The impact of self-reported childhood trauma on emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder and major depression. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 15, 384401. doi:10.1080/15299732.2013.863262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C., Kaczkurkin, A. N., McLean, C. P., & Foa, E. B. (2018). Emotion regulation is associated with PTSD and depression among female adolescent survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 10, 319326. doi:10.1037/tra0000306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaudhury, S. R., Galfalvy, H., Biggs, E., Choo, T., Mann, J. J., & Stanley, B. (2017). Affect in response to stressors and coping strategies: An ecological momentary assessment study of borderline personality disorder. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 4, 19. doi:10.1186/s40479-017-0059-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chida, Y., & Hamer, M. (2008). Chronic psychosocial factors and acute physiological responses to laboratory-induced stress in healthy populations: A quantitative review of 30 years of investigations. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 829885. doi:10.1037/a0013342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Dawson, G. (2002). Multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 417420. doi:10.1017/S0954579402003012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316336. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clemmons, J. C., Walsh, K., Dilillo, D. K., & Messman-moore, T. L. (2007). Unique and combined contributions of multiple child abuse types and abuse severity to adult trauma symptomatology. Child Maltreatment, 12, 172181. doi:10.1177/1077559506298248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385396. doi:10.2307/2136404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courvoisier, D. S., Eid, M., & Lischetzke, T. (2012). Compliance to a cell phone-based ecological momentary assessment study: The effect of time and personality characteristics. Psychological Assessment, 24, 713720. doi:10.1037/a0026733CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deater-Deckard, K., Li, M., & Bell, M. A. (2016). Multifaceted emotion regulation, stress and affect in mothers of young children. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 444457. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1013087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dililio, D., & Long, P. J. (1999). Perceptions of couple functioning among female survivors of child sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 7(4), 5976.10.1300/J070v07n04_05CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Miller, A. B., Giletta, M., Hastings, P. D., Rudolph, K. D., Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018). HPA axis response and psychosocial stress as interactive predictors of suicidal ideation and behavior in adolescent females: A multilevel diathesis-stress framework. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43, 25642571. doi:10.1038/s41386-018-0206-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. Structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12, 121138. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esposito, C. L., & Clum, G. A. (2002). Social support and problem-solving as moderators of the relationship between childhood abuse and suicidality: Applications to a delinquent population. Journal of Traumatic Stress: Official Publication of The International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, 15, 137146. doi:10.1023/A:1014860024980CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Extremera, N., & Rey, L. (2015). The moderator role of emotion regulation ability in the link between stress and well-being. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 16321640. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. (1996). Childhood sexual abuse and psychiatric disorder in young adulthood: II. Psychiatric outcomes of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 13651374. doi:10.1097/00004583-199610000-00024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., Lewis, I., & Smith, C. (1990). Sexual abuse in a national survey of adult men and women: Prevalence, characteristics, and risk factors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 14, 1928. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(90)90077-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., Shattuck, A., & Hamby, S. L. (2015). Prevalence of childhood exposure to violence, crime and abuse: Results from the national survey of children's exposure to violence. JAMA Pediatrics, 169, 746754. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.0676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1994). Structured clinical interview for Axis I DSM-IV disorders. New York: Biometrics Research.Google Scholar
Gilbert, R., Widom, C. S., Browne, K., Fergusson, D., Webb, E., & Janson, S. (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. The Lancet, 373, 6881. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61706-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gladstone, G. L., Parker, G. B., Mitchell, P. B., Malhi, G. S., Wilhelm, K., & Austin, M. P. (2004). Implications of childhood trauma for depressed women: An analysis of pathways from childhood sexual abuse to deliberate self-harm and revictimization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 14171425. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.161.8.1417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaser, J. P., Van Os, J., Portegijs, P. J., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2006). Childhood trauma and emotional reactivity to daily life stress in adult frequent attenders of general practitioners. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 61, 229236. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodyer, I., Herbert, J., Tamplin, A., & Altham, P. (2000). Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 499504. doi:10.1192/bjp.177.6.499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, S., & Rarick, S. (2018). Exploring gender and Racial/Ethnic differences in the effects of child sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 27, 570587. doi:10.1080/10538712.2018.1484403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review Clinical Psychology, 1, 293319. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, B. L. (2008). Stability of cognitive vulnerabilities to depression: A short-term prospective multiwave study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 324333. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.324CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, C., Newport, D. J., Heit, S., Graham, Y. P., Wilcox, M., Bonsall, R., … Nemeroff, C. B. (2000). Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. JAMA, 284, 592597. doi:10.1001/jama.284.5.592CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heleniak, C., Jenness, J. L., Vander Stoep, A., McCauley, E., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Childhood maltreatment exposure and disruptions in emotion regulation: A transdiagnostic pathway to adolescent internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 394415. doi:10.1007/s10608-015-9735-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henckens, M. J. A. G., Klumpers, F., Everaerd, D., Kooijman, S. C., van Wingen, G. A., & Fernández, G. (2016). Interindividual differences in stress sensitivity: Basal and stress-induced cortisol levels differentially predict neural vigilance processing under stress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 663673. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., & Patel, P. V. (2015). SurveySignal: A convenient solution for experience sampling research using participants’ own smartphones. Social Science Computer Review, 33, 235253. doi:10.1177/0894439314525117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humbel, N., Messerli-Bürgy, N., Schuck, K., Wyssen, A., Garcia-Burgos, D., Biedert, E., … Milos, G. (2018). Self-reported emotion regulation difficulties are associated with mood but not with the biological stress response to thin ideal exposure. PLoS One, 13, e0199769. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0199769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, S. M., Paliwal, P., & Sinha, R. (2007). Childhood maltreatment, perceived stress, and stress-related coping in recently abstinent cocaine dependent adults. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21, 233238. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
John, S. G., Cisler, J. M., & Sigel, B. A. (2017). Emotion regulation mediates the relationship between a history of child abuse and current PTSD/depression severity in adolescent females. Journal of Family Violence, 32(6), 565575.10.1007/s10896-017-9914-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 281298. doi:10.1080/02699930903407948CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Mood regulation in depression: Differential effects of distraction and recall of happy memories on sad mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 484490. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.484CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression tests and an extension of the theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 221229. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplow, J. B., & Widom, C. S. (2007). Age of onset of child maltreatment predicts long-term mental health outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 176187. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.176CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katzenstein, D., & Fontes, L. A. (2017). Twice Silenced: The underreporting of child sexual abuse in orthodox Jewish communities. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 26, 752767. doi:10.1080/10538712.2017.1336505CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, M. M., Tyrka, A. R., Anderson, G. M., Price, L. H., & Carpenter, L. L. (2008). Sex differences in emotional and physiological responses to the trier social stress test. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 8798. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C. (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 191214. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.191CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim-Spoon, J., Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2013). A longitudinal study of emotion regulation, emotion lability-negativity, and internalizing symptomatology in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Child Development, 84, 512527. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01857.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 706716. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klanecky, A. K., Woolman, E. O., & Becker, M. M. (2015). Child abuse exposure, emotion regulation, and drinking refusal self-efficacy: An analysis of problem drinking in college students. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 41, 188196. doi:10.3109/00952990.2014.998365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M., & Lopez-Duran, N. (2010). Prodromal symptoms and atypical affectivity as predictors of major depression in juveniles: Implications for prevention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 472496. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02230.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M., Lopez-Duran, N. L., George, C., Mayer, L., Baji, L., Kiss, E., … Kapornai, K. (2019). The development of mood repair response repertories: I. Age-related changes among 7-to 14-year-old depressed and control children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 110.Google ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M., Rottenberg, J., & George, C. (2009). Maladaptive mood repair responses distinguish young adults with early-onset depressive disorders and predict future depression outcomes. Psychological Medicine, 39, 18411854. doi:10.1017/S0033291709005789CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M., & Yaroslavsky, I. (2014). Practitioner review: Dysphoria and its regulation in child and adolescent depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 55, 741757. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12172CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M., Yaroslavsky, I., Rottenberg, J., George, C. J., Kiss, E., Halas, K., … Makai, A. (2016). Maladaptive mood repair, atypical respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and risk of a recurrent major depressive episode among adolescents with prior major depression. Psychological Medicine, 46, 21092119. doi:10.1017/S003329171600057XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudielka, B. M., & Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: A review. Biological Psychology, 69, 113132. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J. R. (1998). Major depressive disorder in older adolescents: Prevalence, risk factors, and clinical implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(7), 765794.10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00010-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindert, J., von Ehrenstein, O. S., Grashow, R., Gal, G., Braehler, E., & Weisskopf, M. G. (2014). Sexual and physical abuse in childhood is associated with depression and anxiety over the life course: Systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Public Health, 59, 359372. doi:10.1007/s00038-013-0519-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434445. doi:10.1038/nrn2639CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., & Zigler, E. (1991). Vulnerability and competence: A review of research on resilience in childhood. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61, 622. doi:10.1037/h0079218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Conron, K. J., Koenen, K. C., & Gilman, S. E. (2010). Childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: A test of the stress-sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychological Medicine, 40, 16471658. doi:10.1017/S0033291709992121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehl, M. R., & Conner, T. S. (2012). Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Merrick, M. T., Ford, D. C., Ports, K. A., & Guinn, A. S. (2018). Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences from the 2011–2014 behavioral risk factor surveillance system in 23 states. JAMA Pediatrics, 172(11), 10381044. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.2537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2005). Life stress, the” kindling” hypothesis, and the recurrence of depression: Considerations from a life stress perspective. Psychological Review, 112, 417445. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, M. C., Ciesla, J. A., & Garber, J. (2010). A prospective study of stress autonomy versus stress-sensitization in adolescents at varied risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 341354. doi:10.1037/a0019036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L. K., Nguyen, A., & Cohen, J. A. (2014). Child sexual abuse. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics, 23, 321337. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2014.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nezlek, J. B., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of Personality, 76, 561580. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00496.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504511. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Mahen, H. A., Karl, A., Moberly, N., & Fedock, G. (2015). The association between childhood maltreatment and emotion regulation: Two different mechanisms contributing to depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 174, 287295. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J. (2018). Personal communication.Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185227. doi:10.1080/00273170701341316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, T. N., Tillyer, M. S., & Wright, E. M. (2017). Intimate partner violence and the overlap of perpetration and victimization: Considering the influence of physical, sexual, and emotional abuse in childhood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 67, 240248. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.02.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, C. M. (2017). Emotion regulation in the context of daily stress: Impact on daily affect. Personality and Individual Differences, 112, 150156. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.058CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruggiero, K. J., McLeer, S. V., & Dixon, J. F. (2000). Sexual abuse characteristics associated with survivor psychopathology. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24, 951964. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00144-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Séguin-Lemire, A., Hébert, M., Cossette, L., & Langevin, R. (2017). A longitudinal study of emotion regulation among sexually abused preschoolers. Child Abuse & Neglect, 63, 307316. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonich, H., Wonderlich, S., Crosby, R., Smyth, J. M., Thompson, K., Redlin, J., … Haseltine, B. (2004). The use of ecological momentary assessment approaches in the study of sexually abused children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 803809. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195152968.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sliwinski, M. J., Almeida, D. M., Stawski, R. S., & Smyth, J. (2009). Intraindividual change and variability in daily stress processes: Findings from two measurement-burst diary studies. Psychology and Aging, 24, 828840. doi:10.1037/a0017925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., Joiner, T. E., Pettit, J. W., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Schmidt, N. B. (2008). Implications of the DSM's emphasis on sadness and anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 159, 2530. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.05.010Google Scholar
Stoltenborgh, M., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Euser, E. M., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2011). A global perspective on child sexual abuse: Meta-analysis of prevalence around the world. Child Maltreatment, 16, 79101. doi:10.1177/1077559511403920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thakkar, R. R., & McCanne, T. R. (2000). The effects of daily stressors on physical health in women with and without a childhood history of sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 209221. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(99)00129-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (2011). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides of the developing coin. Emotion Review, 3, 5361. doi:10.1177/1754073910380969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tofighi, D., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2011). RMediation: An R package for mediation analysis confidence intervals. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 692700. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0076-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tofighi, D., & Thoemmes, F. (2014). Single-level and multilevel mediation analysis. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 34, 93119. doi:10.1177/0272431613511331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troy, A. S., & Mauss, I. B. (2011). Resilience in the face of stress: Emotion regulation as a protective factor. Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, 1, 3044. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511994791.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troy, A. S., Wilhelm, F. H., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B. (2010). Seeing the silver lining: Cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion, 10, 783795. doi:10.1037/a0020262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, S., Taillieu, T., Cheung, K., & Afifi, T. O. (2017). The relationship between childhood sexual abuse and mental health outcomes among males: Results from a nationally representative United States sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 66, 6472. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.01.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, S. E., Peter-Hagene, L. C., & Relyea, M. (2014). Coping, emotion regulation, and self-blame as mediators of sexual abuse and psychological symptoms in adult sexual assault. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 23, 7493. doi:10.1080/10538712.2014.864747CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 397409. doi:10.1038/nrn2647CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Eck, M. M., & Nicolson, N. A. (1994). Perceived stress and salivary cortisol in daily life. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 221227.Google Scholar
Visted, E., Vøllestad, J., Nielsen, M. B., & Schanche, E. (2018). Emotion regulation in current and remitted depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 756765. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 775808. doi:10.1037/a0027600CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weltz, S. M., Armeli, S., Ford, J. D., & Tennen, H. (2016). A daily process examination of the relationship between childhood trauma and stress-reactivity. Child Abuse & Neglect, 60, 19. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.08.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (1999). Report of the Consultation on Child Abuse Prevention, 29–31 March 1999, WHO, Geneva (No. WHO/HSC/PVI/99.1). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Yaroslavsky, I., Bylsma, L. M., Rottenberg, J., & Kovacs, M. (2013). Combinations of resting RSA and RSA reactivity impact maladaptive mood repair and depression symptoms. Biological Psychology, 94, 272–81. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics, psychiatric parameters, and perceived stress levels of those with and without childhood sexual abuse (CSA) histories

Figure 1

Table 2. Multiple and sequential mediation of child sexual abuse history's effect on depression symptoms via stress sensitization and emotion regulation

Figure 2

Figure 1. Maladaptive emotion regulation (mER) moderation of perceived stress effects on depression symptoms.

Figure 3

Table 3. Multiple and sequential mediation of child sexual abuse history's effects on contemporaneous negative and positive affects in daily life via stress sensitization and emotion regulation

Figure 4

Table 4. Multiple and sequential mediation of child sexual abuse history's effects on prospective negative and positive affects in daily life via stress sensitization and emotion regulation.

Figure 5

Figure 2. Childhood sexual abuse moderation of within-subject stress fluctuations on contemporaneous negative affect (NA) (Panel A), and change (Δ) in NA (Panel B) and positive affect (Panel C) across successive ecological momentary assessment (EMA) prompts.

Figure 6

Figure 3. Maladaptive emotion regulation (mER) moderation of average stress level (Panel A) and within-subject stress fluctuations (Panel B) on concurrent negative affect (NA), and average stress levels on change (Δ) in NA (Panel C) across successive ecological momentary assessment prompts.

Figure 7

Figure 4. Adaptive emotion regulation (aER) moderation of within-subject stress fluctuations on contemporaneous hedonic affect.

Supplementary material: File

Yaroslavsky et al. supplementary material

Yaroslavsky et al. supplementary material

Download Yaroslavsky et al. supplementary material(File)
File 19.7 KB