Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-dlb68 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T15:00:28.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early adversity and children's emotion regulation: Differential roles of parent emotion regulation and adversity exposure

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2021

Helen M. Milojevich*
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA
Laura Machlin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Margaret A. Sheridan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Helen M. Milojevich, Department of Pediatrics, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, 940 NE 13th Street, 4th Floor Nicholson Tower, Suite 4946, Oklahoma City, OK73104; E-mail: helen-milojevich@ouhsc.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Exposure to early life adversity (ELA) is associated with increased rates of psychopathology and poor physical health. The present study builds on foundational work by Megan Gunnar identifying how ELA results in poor long-term outcomes through alterations in the stress response system, leading to major disruptions in emotional and behavioral regulation. Specifically, the present study tested the direct effects of ELA against the role of parent socialization to shed light on the mechanisms by which ELA leads to emotion regulation deficits. Children ages 4–7 years (N = 64) completed interviews about their experiences of deprivation and threat, a fear conditioning and extinction paradigm, and an IQ test. Parents of the children completed questionnaires regarding their own emotion regulation difficulties and psychopathology, their children's emotion regulation, and child exposure to adversity. At the bivariate level, greater exposure to threat and parental difficulties with emotion regulation were associated with poorer emotion regulation in children, assessed both via parental report and physiologically. In models where parental difficulties with emotion regulation, threat, and deprivation were introduced simultaneously, regression results indicated that parental difficulties with emotion regulation, but not deprivation or threat, continued to predict children's emotion regulation abilities. These results suggest that parental socialization of emotion is a robust predictor of emotion regulation tendencies in children exposed to early adversity.

Type
Special Section 2: Early Adversity and Development: Contributions from the Field
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

The field of early life adversity (ELA) has seen a recent surge of empirical work focused on understanding how ELA impacts children's developmental trajectories and ultimately the health and well-being of individuals across the lifespan. Moreover, clinicians and policy makers are placing increased attention on the critical role of ELA in the development of psychopathology and rates of morbidity and mortality (e.g., About Adverse Childhood Experiences, 2019). In part, this increased focus is the direct result of pioneering work by Megan Gunnar examining how ELA shapes biological risk, particularly the stress response system, leading to major disruptions in emotional and behavioral regulation (Gunnar, Frenn, Wewerka, & Van Ryzin, Reference Gunnar, Frenn, Wewerka and Van Ryzin2009; Gunnar & Quevedo, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2007, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2008; Gunnar & Vazquez, Reference Gunnar and Vazquez2001; Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long, & Griggs, Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009, Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2010; Hostinar, Sullivan, & Gunnar, Reference Hostinar, Sullivan and Gunnar2014; Loman & Gunnar, Reference Loman and Gunnar2010; Tarullo, Bruce, & Gunnar, Reference Tarullo, Bruce and Gunnar2007; Tarullo & Gunnar, Reference Tarullo and Gunnar2006; Tottenham et al., Reference Tottenham, Hare, Quinn, McCarry, Nurse, Gilhooly and Casey2010). Here we build on this groundwork laid by Megan Gunnar by examining specific forms of adversity exposure and how they differentially relate to deficits in emotion regulation, including those measured using physiology. We adopt a recent model intended to account for differential effects of adversity types, the Dimensional Model of Adversity and Psychopathology (DMAP; McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, Reference Sheridan and McLaughlin2014). Within this model, exposure to threat (e.g., physical abuse, violence) is expected to be associated with enhanced emotion reactivity and reduced emotion regulation. Conceptually within the DMAP model, threat exposure during childhood is expected to directly impact emotion regulation capabilities because it provides enhanced learning opportunities about violence exposure during early childhood when neural systems are maximally plastic (McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016; McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014). This early learning is expected to result in alterations in neural systems which support emotion reactivity and regulation (McLaughlin, Peverill, Gold, Alves, & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin, Peverill, Gold, Alves and Sheridan2015). However, a number of studies have posited that, particularly when threat takes the form of family violence, it is not the actual exposure to violence, per se, but the accompanying lack of appropriate socialization around emotion regulation which leads children to exhibit deficits in emotion regulation (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998). In this conceptualization, it is the implicit (modeling of emotion regulation) and explicit (teaching of emotion regulation strategies) behaviors whereby children learn when and how to regulate emotions, which matter most (Peisch, Dale, Parent, & Burt, Reference Peisch, Dale, Parent and Burt2019; Rogers, Halberstadt, Castro, MacCormack, & Garrett-Peters, Reference Rogers, Halberstadt, Castro, MacCormack and Garrett-Peters2016; Shipman & Zeman, Reference Shipman and Zeman2001). The present study tests the direct effects of adversity against the role of parent's own emotion regulation tendencies, as a marker of parental socialization, to shed light on the mechanisms by which ELA leads to emotion regulation deficits.

Emotion regulation deficits and early adversity exposure

Findings on emotion regulation in children exposed to adversity are almost universally consistent, with studies generally concluding that regardless of age, gender, or ethnicity, adversity-exposed children have poorer emotion regulation relative to nonexposed children (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, & McBride-Chang, Reference Chang, Schwartz, Dodge and McBride-Chang2003; Ellis, Alisic, Reiss, Dishion, & Fisher, Reference Ellis, Alisic, Reiss, Dishion and Fisher2014; Hébert, Langevin, & Oussaïd, Reference Hébert, Langevin and Oussaïd2018; Kim & Cicchetti, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2009, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2010; Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti, & Rogosch, Reference Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti and Rogosch2013; Maughan & Cicchetti, Reference Maughan and Cicchetti2002; Milojevich, Levine, Cathcart, & Quas, Reference Milojevich, Levine, Cathcart and Quas2018; Romens & Pollak, Reference Romens and Pollak2012; Shields & Cicchetti, Reference Shields and Cicchetti1998; Shields, Cicchetti, & Ryan, Reference Shields, Cicchetti and Ryan1994; Thabet, Tischler, & Vostanis, Reference Thabet, Tischler and Vostanis2004). Moreover, in studies that have measured children's use of specific emotion regulation strategies, findings indicate that adversity-exposed children tend to use maladaptive regulation strategies, such as disengagement, expressive suppression, and rumination, more frequently and use effective strategies, including cognitive reappraisal, less often than nonexposed children (Boyes, Hasking, & Martin, Reference Boyes, Hasking and Martin2016; Epstein-Ngo, Maurizi, Bregman, & Ceballo, Reference Epstein-Ngo, Maurizi, Bregman and Ceballo2013; Maughan & Cicchetti, Reference Maughan and Cicchetti2002; Milojevich et al., Reference Milojevich, Levine, Cathcart and Quas2018; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Morris, Heller, Scheeringa, Boris and Smyke2009). To date, most studies on early adversity exposure and emotion regulation have examined children exposed to maltreatment, with less research considering other types of adversity or comparing across exposure types. Within child maltreatment, studies often combine across subtypes (e.g., physical abuse, neglect) to create a single maltreated sample, which is then compared to a nonmaltreated comparison sample of matched controls (Alink, Cicchetti, Kim, & Rogosch, Reference Alink, Cicchetti, Kim and Rogosch2009; Kim-Spoon et al., Reference Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti and Rogosch2013; Milojevich et al., Reference Milojevich, Levine, Cathcart and Quas2018; Shields et al., Reference Shields, Cicchetti and Ryan1994). As such, little is known about the relative contribution of various forms of adversity exposure to children's emotion regulation or the impact of adjacent variables such as parent emotion socialization.

Direct effects of early adversity on children's emotion regulation

A useful conceptual framework for studying the direct effects of early adversity exposure is DMAP (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, Reference Sheridan and McLaughlin2014). Within DMAP, a central distinction is made between adverse experiences, which are characterized by deprivation and threat. Importantly, although exposure to deprivation and threat may co-occur for children, DMAP posits that these dimensions can be measured separately and have unique effects on developmental outcomes. Deprivation is the absence of species- or age-expectant cognitive and social inputs (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, Reference Sheridan and McLaughlin2014, Reference Sheridan and McLaughlin2016). Exposure to threat, on the other hand, refers to the presence of atypical events in which a child experiences actual or threatened physical harm. These dimensions encompass numerous adverse experiences that share an underlying exposure to threat or deprivation to a varying degree. For instance, threat is a primary feature of physical abuse, sexual abuse, and community violence, whereas deprivation is a primary feature of neglect, institutionalization, and the lack of psychosocial stimulation that can occur in poverty.

Of note, DMAP specifies unique emotional, cognitive, and neurobiological pathways linking early dimensions of experience with developmental outcomes. Exposure to deprivation is hypothesized to be selectively linked with age-specific reductions in complex cognitive function. In contrast, exposure to threat leads to changes in neural circuits that underlie emotion reactivity and regulation, resulting in changes in fear learning and enhanced reactivity to negative affective stimuli. Evidence suggests that, in models where both exposures are simultaneously included, exposure to threat is uniquely associated with deficits in automatic and deliberate emotion regulation processes, altered emotion reactivity, and disrupted fear learning, while exposure to deprivation is uniquely linked to poor cognitive control, working memory, and language ability (Busso, McLaughlin, & Sheridan, Reference Busso, McLaughlin and Sheridan2016; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, King, Monahan and McLaughlin2017; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Gold, Duys, Lambert, Peverill and Pine2016; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Sheridan, Hanson, McLaughlin, Bates, Lansford and Dodge2018; Milojevich, Norwalk, & Sheridan, Reference Milojevich, Norwalk and Sheridan2019; Sheridan, Peverill, Finn, & McLaughlin, Reference Sheridan, Peverill, Finn and McLaughlin2017). Many of these studies have used exposure to physical and sexual abuse as primary indicators of threat exposure.

Parent socialization of emotion regulation

Importantly, children exposed to physical and sexual abuse, and other forms of adversity, tend to have parents who display ineffective regulation strategies (Criss, Morris, Ponce-Garcia, Cui, & Silk, Reference Criss, Morris, Ponce-Garcia, Cui and Silk2016; Kliewer et al., Reference Kliewer, Parrish, Taylor, Jackson, Walker and Shivy2006; Shipman et al., Reference Shipman, Schneider, Fitzgerald, Sims, Swisher and Edwards2007), produce less prototypical facial expressions of emotions (Camras et al., Reference Camras, Ribordy, Hill, Martino, Spaccarelli and Stefani1988), report greater expression of negative emotions (Raver & Spagnola, Reference Raver and Spagnola2002), and are less accurate in recognizing facial expressions of emotions (Balge & Milner, Reference Balge and Milner2000). In maltreatment contexts, parents also often fail to teach their children effective ways to reduce distress and negative feelings, and instead respond by invalidating their children's feelings or neglecting them in emotional situations (Shipman et al., Reference Shipman, Schneider, Fitzgerald, Sims, Swisher and Edwards2007).

Parents are a crucial source of environmental input and, as such, have a profound impact on the ways in which children come to learn about emotions and how to alter emotional reactions (Brophy-Herb, Stansbury, Bocknek, & Horodynski, Reference Brophy-Herb, Stansbury, Bocknek and Horodynski2012; Chaplin, Cole, & Zahn-Waxler, Reference Chaplin, Cole and Zahn-Waxler2005; Denham, Zoller, & Couchoud, Reference Denham, Zoller and Couchoud1994; Dunsmore, Booker, & Ollendick, Reference Dunsmore, Booker and Ollendick2013; Dunsmore, Her, Halberstadt, & Perez-Rivera, Reference Dunsmore, Her, Halberstadt and Perez-Rivera2009; Dunsmore & Karn, Reference Dunsmore and Karn2001; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Halberstadt, Fox, & Jones, Reference Halberstadt, Fox and Jones1993; Katz & Rigterink, Reference Katz and Rigterink2012; Kliewer et al., Reference Kliewer, Parrish, Taylor, Jackson, Walker and Shivy2006; Meyer, Raikes, Virmani, Waters, & Thompson, Reference Meyer, Raikes, Virmani, Waters and Thompson2014; Perez Rivera & Dunsmore, Reference Perez Rivera and Dunsmore2011; Shaffer, Suveg, Thomassin, & Bradbury, Reference Shaffer, Suveg, Thomassin and Bradbury2012). For one, children constantly observe their parents’ emotional displays and interactions and how parents express and manage their emotions (Brophy-Herb et al., Reference Brophy-Herb, Stansbury, Bocknek and Horodynski2012; Halberstadt et al., Reference Halberstadt, Fox and Jones1993) and learn about emotion regulation through these observations (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Morris, Steinberg, Aucoin and Keyes2011). Children of parents who display effective emotion regulation are more likely to learn about successful emotion regulation strategies and the appropriateness of specific emotions across multiple contexts. In contrast, children of parents who utilize ineffective regulation strategies may struggle to appropriately regulate their emotions (Bariola, Gullone, & Hughes, Reference Bariola, Gullone and Hughes2011, 2012). Other forms of parental socialization of emotion regulation include parents actively teaching their children about emotion regulation through conversations about emotional events and strategies to cope with emotions (Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon, & Cohen, Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Alisic, Reiss, Dishion and Fisher2014; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Morris, Steinberg, Aucoin and Keyes2011; Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Suveg, Thomassin and Bradbury2012), as well as the overall emotional climate of the family (Davies & Cummings, Reference Davies and Cummings1994; Fosco & Grych, Reference Fosco and Grych2013; Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, & Lukon, Reference Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg and Lukon2002; Gottman & Katz, Reference Gottman and Katz1989; Milojevich & Haskett, Reference Milojevich and Haskett2018; Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, Reference Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti and Manning2012).

In sum, while direct exposure to family violence could shape future emotion reactivity and regulation via the basic learning mechanisms proposed by DMAP, it is also possible that children exposed to early adversity simply lack a crucial source of environmental input about emotions and may be taught poor emotion regulation strategies because of their parent's own compromised emotion regulation profile. In one study that examined parent socialization (specifically the family emotional climate) in a sample of exclusively threat-exposed children, parent socialization was a consistent predictor of children's emotional functioning both concurrently and longitudinally across a span of three years (Milojevich & Haskett, Reference Milojevich and Haskett2018).

Current study

The present study examined the relative contributions of parent socialization and the direct effects of adversity on children's emotion regulation in a sample of 4–7-year-olds. We focused on parental modeling, as one form of parent socialization, assessed via parent's own difficulties regulating emotions, in line with previous socialization research (Kliewer et al., Reference Kliewer, Parrish, Taylor, Jackson, Walker and Shivy2006; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Raikes, Virmani, Waters and Thompson2014). Parents who have difficulty regulating their own emotions model ineffective, maladaptive strategies, which their children may in turn imitate (Bariola, Hughes, & Gullone, Reference Bariola, Hughes and Gullone2012). Moreover, because young children may struggle to accurately report on their emotion regulation (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, Reference Cole, Martin and Dennis2004), we included both parent-report and physiological measures of children's emotion regulation.

Consistent with the extant adversity literature and DMAP predictions, we hypothesized that, at the bivariate level, parent socialization and threat, but not deprivation, would be associated with children's emotion regulation. To date, however, no study has examined the differential roles of parent socialization, deprivation, and threat on children's emotion regulation. We hypothesized that in models where parent socialization, deprivation, and threat were all simultaneously included, parent socialization, but not deprivation or threat, would continue to predict children's emotion regulation.

Method

Participants

Children (N = 64) aged 4–7 years old (M = 74.14 months, SD = 14.06; 57.1% female) and a parent or legal guardian participated in the current study, which was conducted in a rural and suburban region in the southeast United States. Table 1 presents descriptive data on the sample. Slightly more than half of the children were White and about a third were African American. The remaining children identified as multiracial or another race or ethnicity. Roughly 12% were Hispanic or Latinx. Nearly 35% of the families were living below 200% of the poverty line, as defined by an income-to-needs ratio below 2. In 25% of the families, parents had completed less than a high school degree.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the sample

Note. Parent Total Symptoms is the Positive Symptoms Total from the SCL-90 (Derogatis et al., Reference Derogatis, Lipman and Covi1973), which indicates the total number of self-reported symptoms of psychopathology. Parent Total DERS reflects parents’ total score on the Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004). Child Emotion Regulation and Negativity/Liability were children's scores from the parent-report Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields & Cicchetti, Reference Shields and Cicchetti1995).

Participants were recruited across 1.5 years using approaches to target families with low socioeconomic status (SES) through listservs, ad postings, other studies recruiting low socioeconomic status populations, and screening procedures previously detailed (Machlin, Miller, Snyder, McLaughlin, & Sheridan, Reference Machlin, Miller, Snyder, McLaughlin and Sheridan2019). Parents or legal guardians provided written consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board. If children were 7 years old at the time of participation, they provided written assent. Otherwise, they provided verbal assent to study procedures. Exclusion criteria for the study included major medical conditions, neurological illness, and diagnosis of a pervasive developmental disorder (e.g., autism, Down syndrome). Children were not excluded from the study for other diagnoses of psychopathology or psychological treatment.

Procedures

The study was completed in one three-hour visit to a psychology laboratory. After consent/assent procedures, parents of children in the study completed questionnaires assessing demographic information, symptoms of psychopathology in the parent, child exposure to threatening and depriving experiences. Children completed an IQ test, interviews about their experiences of deprivation and threat, and a fear conditioning and extinction paradigm. Following the study visit, parents completed additional questionnaires online using Qualtrics assessing parent emotion regulation and child emotion regulation abilities.

Out of 64 initial participants, one child who participated in the study was unable to complete any component of the study due to behavioral issues. In addition, parent and child emotion regulation questionnaire data were missing for seven children due to lack of completion of questionnaires at home. Thus, 56 children had complete questionnaire data. Regarding fear conditioning data, out of the 63 children with any usable data, seven children aborted the task or did not consent to complete the task. Due to technical problems, data from an additional six children were unusable. As a result, 50 children had usable data from fear extinction.

Measures

IQ

IQ was assessed through administration of the Kaufman brief intelligence test (KBIT-2), which is a brief measure of verbal and nonverbal cognitive abilities for children as young as 4 years old. Reliability of the composite IQ score from the KBIT-2 is 0.93 in a normative sample (Bain & Jaspers, Reference Bain and Jaspers2010).

Parent emotion regulation

Parent emotion regulation was measured by the total score on the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS), a widely used self-report measure of emotion regulation ability (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004). Prior research has found that the DERS has high internal consistency in normative and treatment-seeking populations and good test–retest reliability (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004; Hallion, Steinman, Tolin, & Diefenbach, Reference Hallion, Steinman, Tolin and Diefenbach2018). The DERS has high internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.81 in the current sample).

Parental psychopathology

Parental psychopathology was measured by the Symptom Checklist-90 (Derogatis, Lipman, & Covi, Reference Derogatis, Lipman and Covi1973), a 90-item self-report measure used to measure psychological symptoms with high internal consistency and reliability (Cronbach's alpha = 0.98 in the current sample).

Child emotion regulation

Child emotion regulation was assessed using the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC), which is a parent report of child emotion regulation with high internal consistency in prior studies (Shields & Cicchetti, Reference Shields and Cicchetti1995). The present study utilized the subscales for liability/negativity and emotion regulation (Cronbach's alpha = 0.87 for liability/negativity and 0.82 for emotion regulation in the current sample). Higher scores reflect greater liability/negativity and better emotion regulation, respectively.

Child exposure to threat

Threatening experiences were comprised of a score derived from three measures that were standardized by a z score and then summed to create a total threat score. First, the children's exposure to violence was measured using the Violence Exposure Scale for Children interview measure (VEX-R), which uses cartoon items to assess children's exposure to abuse, domestic violence, and witnessing community violence (Fox & Leavitt, Reference Fox and Leavitt1995). The current study utilized the sum of the total items in which children reported on exposure to violence perpetrated by a teenager or an adult (Cronbach's alpha = 0.81 in the current sample). Second, domestic violence was assessed using the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS-2), a parental report of domestic violence in the home (Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, Reference Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy and Sugarman1996). The sum of items on physical assault, psychological aggression, injury, and sexual coercion were used in the present study (Cronbach's alpha = 0.94 in the current sample). Third, the statistical likelihood of physical abuse was measured using the Child Abuse Potential Inventory (CAPI), a parent-report that screens for parental attitudes associated with child abuse (Milner, Charlesworth, Gold, Gold, & Friesen, Reference Milner, Charlesworth, Gold, Gold and Friesen1988). The total sum score of Child Abuse Scale was used in the present study (Cronbach's alpha = 0.90 in the current sample).

Child exposure to deprivation

Experiences of deprivation were comprised of a score derived from three measures that were standardized by a z score and then summed to create a total deprivation score. First, child neglect was assessed using the Multidimensional Neglectful Behavior Scale (MNBS-CR), which uses cartoon images to assess neglect. The reliability of the MNBS-CR ranges from 0.66 to 0.94 depending on the sample (Kantor et al., Reference Kantor, Holt, Mebert, Straus, Drach, Ricci and Brown2004). The current study utilized the sum of the emotional neglect, cognitive neglect, physical neglect, supervisory neglect, and abandonment items (Cronbach's alpha = 0.71 in the current sample). Second, cognitive stimulation was measured using the Home Screening Questionnaire, which is a parent-report based on the Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment (HOME) for use in a laboratory setting (Bradley, Caldwell, & Corwyn, Reference Bradley, Caldwell and Corwyn2003; Frankenburg & Coons, Reference Frankenburg and Coons1986). The HSQ has good test-retest reliability (Frankenburg & Coons, Reference Frankenburg and Coons1986). The current study used the sum of the HSQ with five items removed that assessed spanking and parental decision-making in the household (Cronbach's alpha = 0.77 in the current sample). Third, parental education was measured as the average of educational attainment for both caregivers of a child or one caregiver if there was one primary caregiver. Possible responses ranged from “Less than high school diploma (1)” to “Professional degree (5)” (Adler & Stewart, Reference Adler and Stewart2007).

Fear extinction paradigm

Children completed a block-design fear conditioning and extinction paradigm using a loud sound as the aversive stimuli (UCS) designed for young children. The paradigm has separate blocks of reinforced threat cues (UCS), nonreinforced threat cues (CS+), and stimuli unassociated with the unconditioned stimulus (CS−). Block-design fear conditioning paradigms have been successfully used to show differential fear conditioning in preschool-age children (Jovanovic et al., Reference Jovanovic, Nylocks, Gamwell, Smith, Davis, Norrholm and Bradley2014; Silvers et al., Reference Silvers, Lumian, Gabard-Durnam, Gee, Goff, Fareri and Tottenham2016). Children viewed 12 stimulus blocks during acquisition: four blocks of the reinforced threat cues (UCS) with a reinforcement rate of 80%, four blocks of the nonreinforced threat cues (CS+), and four blocks of the CS−. The first three blocks of fear acquisition were presented in the following order for all participants: CS−, UCS, and CS+. Blocks were randomized after the first three blocks. During extinction, children viewed eight randomized stimulus blocks: four blocks of the CS+ (non-reinforced threat cues) and four blocks of the CS−. On 2/10 of trials across blocks, children pressed to a dot to ensure attention during the task.

Fear learning was measured by the amplitude of skin conductance response (SCR) across fear acquisition and extinction. Skin conductance was measured through two electrodes filled with sodium chloride gel attached to the palm of the left hand. Data were sampled at 1,000 Hz. Data were filtered and preprocessed using Mindware. SCR was calculated with a minimum response of 0.05 μs (Braithwaite, Watson, Jones, & Rowe, Reference Braithwaite, Watson, Jones and Rowe2013). All data were visually inspected and edited if needed to verify peaks in skin conductance. A range correction was done to correct for inter-individual variance by dividing each skin conductance amplitude value by the maximum skin conductance value for each participant and conducting a square root transformation of the data after range-correction (Boucsein, Reference Boucsein2012; Lykken, Reference Lykken1972; Lykken & Venables, Reference Lykken and Venables1971). The four blocks of fear acquisition and fear extinction were divided into early blocks (Blocks 1–2) and late blocks (Blocks 3–4) based on prior literature (Jovanovic et al., Reference Jovanovic, Nylocks, Gamwell, Smith, Davis, Norrholm and Bradley2014; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Gold, Duys, Lambert, Peverill and Pine2016).

The present study utilized data from early and late blocks of fear extinction to evaluate emotion regulation following fear conditioning. Prior frameworks have identified fear extinction processes within implicit, automatic emotion regulation processes (Braunstein, Gross, & Ochsner, Reference Braunstein, Gross and Ochsner2017; Mauss, Bunge, & Gross, Reference Mauss, Bunge and Gross2007). Automatic emotion regulation processes typically involve affective learning, where a person updates information about a stimulus due to a shift in the context associated with a stimulus, as seen in fear extinction (Braunstein et al., Reference Braunstein, Gross and Ochsner2017). Data from the early and late blocks of fear acquisition have been published previously (Machlin et al., Reference Machlin, Miller, Snyder, McLaughlin and Sheridan2019).

Data analytic plan

Correlations were examined between all study variables. Then, multiple linear regression models were used to predict child emotion regulation separately for each measure of child emotion regulation. In the first set of models, child emotion regulation was measured by the two subscales of the ERC. In the second set of models, child emotion regulation was measured by the amplitude of SCR to the CS+ relative to the CS− during fear extinction. Separate regression models were conducted for early and late fear extinction. All models were conducted in a stepwise manner. First, we tested deprivation and threat as separate predictors of children's emotion regulation controlling for age, gender, IQ, and parental psychopathology. Second, we tested whether parents’ difficulties with emotion regulation predicted child emotion regulation, above and beyond children's exposure to deprivation and threat, again controlling for relevant covariates.

Results

Bivariate associations

Correlations among potential predictor variables and child emotion regulation scores are presented in Table 2. First, greater threat, higher IQ, and more parental difficulties with emotion regulation were associated with lower scores on the emotion regulation subscale of the ERC. Second, being male, higher IQ, and more parental difficulties with emotion regulation were associated with greater negativity/liability on the ERC. Finally, greater exposure to threat, more parental symptoms of psychopathology, and more parental difficulties with emotion regulation were associated with higher amplitudes of SCR to the CS+ versus CS− during early fear extinction, which suggests less extinction of the differential skin conductance response and is reflective of less automatic emotion regulation during fear extinction. No significant correlations were found for amplitudes of SCR during late fear extinction. Of note, exposure to deprivation and threat were not significantly correlated (r = .15, p = .23). Age, gender, IQ, and parent total symptoms of psychopathology were used as covariates in all analyses.

Table 2. Correlations among main predictor and child regulation scores

Note. Sex: female = 1, male = 0; *p < .05; **p < .01.

DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; SCR = skin conductance response

Parent socialization, dimensions of adversity, and children's emotion regulation

The primary goal of the present study was to examine whether dimensions of adversity exposure and parental socialization of emotion differentially predicted emotion regulation abilities in young children. In particular, we expected that, in models where parent socialization, threat, and deprivation are all simultaneously included, parent socialization, but not threat or deprivation, would predict children's emotion regulation. As illustrated in Table 3, when considering children's emotion regulation based on the parent-report ERC, controlling for demographic factors, scores on the emotion regulation subscale differed significantly by threat exposure, with higher threat exposure being associated with poorer emotion regulation. In this model we also observed that greater IQ predicted poorer emotion regulation. However, once parental difficulties with emotion regulation was added to the model, threat was no longer significant. Instead, scores on the emotion regulation subscale differed significantly by parental difficulties with emotion regulation, such that children had poorer emotion regulation when their parents with more difficulty regulating their emotions. IQ remained a significant predictor.

Table 3. Regression results for emotion regulation checklist

Note. Sex: female = 1, male = 0; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01.

DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale

For the negativity/liability subscale of the ERC, in Model 1, only sex and IQ emerged as significant predictors. Higher IQ was associated with more negativity/liability and boys were also rated as having more negativity/liability relative to girls. In Model 2, once again, parental difficulties with emotion regulation remained a significant predictor. Children had more negativity/liability when their parents reported greater difficulty regulating their own emotions.

Next, we examined whether adversity exposure and parental socialization of emotion differentially predicted physiological measures of emotion regulation. Given that the amplitude of SCR during late fear extinction was not associated with any of our main predictor variables at the bivariate level, we focused the regression analyses to early fear extinction. As shown in Table 4, in Model 1, extinction to the CS− was the only significant predictor. In Model 2, controlling for adversity exposure, demographic factors, and the amplitude of SCR to the CS−, amplitudes of SCR to the CS+ during early fear extinction differed significantly by parental difficulties with emotion regulation. Children had greater SCR amplitudes to the CS+ during early extinction when their parents with more difficulty regulating their emotions.Footnote 1

Table 4. Regression results for amplitude of SCR (skin conductance response) to the CS+ during fear extinction

Note. Sex: female = 1, male = 0; *p < .05; **p ≤ .01.

DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale

Discussion

In the present study, we examined the relative contributions of parent socialization and the direct effects of adversity on children's emotion regulation in a sample of 4–7-year-olds. Consistent with hypotheses, we found that parent socialization, in the form of parental difficulties with emotion regulation, consistently predicted parent-report and physiological measures of children's emotion regulation. Moreover, in line with expectations, threat exposure was associated with children's emotion regulation, but these associations did not hold after accounting for parent socialization. Finally, in keeping with our hypotheses, deprivation was unrelated to children's emotion regulation.

We hypothesized, and found, that parents’ difficulties with emotion regulation would be a consistent predictor of children's emotion regulation, over and above children's adversity exposure or parents’ symptoms of psychopathology. Ample evidence demonstrates the role of parental modeling of emotion regulation on the development of emotion regulation abilities in both community samples of children and adversity-exposed children (Bariola et al., Reference Bariola, Hughes and Gullone2012; Brophy-Herb et al., Reference Brophy-Herb, Stansbury, Bocknek and Horodynski2012; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Morris, Steinberg, Aucoin and Keyes2011). Furthermore, Milojevich and Haskett (Reference Milojevich and Haskett2018) found that, even in an exclusively threat-exposed sample, parental socialization is a strong, consistent predictor of children's emotional skills across development. One explanation for the robust link between parent socialization and children's emotion regulation is the chronic and pervasive nature of socialization – children see emotions modeled daily by their caregivers from the moment they are born and throughout their development. Threat and deprivation, though also potentially chronic, are likely not occurring at the same frequency. In addition, or perhaps alternatively, parents’ and children's difficulties with emotion regulation might have a genetic component that is being captured in our measure of parent socialization. Future work is needed to tease apart genetic from socialization contributions. On a final note, it is important to point out that the findings between parent socialization and children's emotion regulation held for both parent-report and physiological measures of children's emotion regulation. This consistency in findings reduces concern over shared method variance and suggests that parental socialization is a particularly strong predictor of emotion regulation in children exposed to early adversity.

In line with this argument, we found, as expected, that although threat was associated with children's emotion regulation, this association did not hold after accounting for parents’ emotion regulation difficulties. Threat exposure is undoubtedly detrimental to children's development and directly affects neural networks associated with emotion reactivity and regulation (Busso et al., Reference Busso, McLaughlin and Sheridan2016; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, King, Monahan and McLaughlin2017; McLaughlin, Sheridan, Alves, & Mendes, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Alves and Mendes2014; McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Sheridan, Hanson, McLaughlin, Bates, Lansford and Dodge2018; Milojevich et al., Reference Milojevich, Norwalk and Sheridan2019). However, our findings suggest that threat may not be the primary mechanism behind emotion regulation difficulties in children exposed to adversity. Rather, threat-exposed children may have difficulties regulating their emotions in large part because abusive parents often fail to teach their children a range of appropriate and adaptive regulation strategies and instead model maladaptive, ineffective regulation strategies (Shipman et al., Reference Shipman, Schneider, Fitzgerald, Sims, Swisher and Edwards2007). As such, threat-exposed children may not develop the regulation skills in emotionally-taxing situations and thereby experience deficits in emotion regulation.

Consistent with hypotheses from the DMAP and our previous work (Milojevich et al., Reference Milojevich, Norwalk and Sheridan2019), even at the bivariate level, deprivation was unrelated to children's emotion regulation. Previous studies have found associations between certain forms of deprivation (e.g., neglect) and emotion regulation deficits in children (Kim & Cicchetti, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Morris, Heller, Scheeringa, Boris and Smyke2009; Shipman, Edwards, Brown, Swisher, & Jennings, Reference Shipman, Edwards, Brown, Swisher and Jennings2005; Tottenham et al., Reference Tottenham, Hare, Quinn, McCarry, Nurse, Gilhooly and Casey2010). Here we examine a wider range of deprivation exposures, in line with the conceptualization in the DMAP model, which focuses on the absence of species-expected learning experiences in cognitive and social domains. In addition, previous studies have compared deprivation-exposed children to nonexposed children, with children exposed to deprivation demonstrating poorer emotion regulation (Kim & Cicchetti, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Morris, Heller, Scheeringa, Boris and Smyke2009; Shipman et al., Reference Shipman, Edwards, Brown, Swisher and Jennings2005; Tottenham et al., Reference Tottenham, Hare, Quinn, McCarry, Nurse, Gilhooly and Casey2010). In our study, however, deprivation was measured continuously with children experiencing a range of deprivation. A comparison of exposed to nonexposed children is a more extreme contrast and may not capture the range of emotion regulation abilities in children exposed to various levels of deprivation. By examining deprivation continuously, we were able to test whether difficulties in emotion regulation increased as severity of deprivation increased. Our results suggest that this is not the case – there was not a linear increase in emotion regulation difficulties as a function of deprivation exposure. To compare our results to previous studies with exposed versus nonexposed children, we conducted a mean-split of deprivation exposure and compared emotion regulation scores for high- versus low-deprivation children. Results indicated that high-deprivation children had lower scores on the emotion regulation subscale of the ERC (M = 25.31) relative to low-deprivation children (M = 27.80); t (37.68) = 37.65, p = .03,Footnote 2 consistent with previous studies. This finding highlights the need to carefully consider how deprivation (and adversity more broadly) are measured. By conceptualizing and measuring deprivation and threat as continuous dimensions and focusing on exposures which confer risk for a lack of learning experiences generally, we move away from a dichotomous categorization of adversity that treats all exposed children as the same and create a more nuanced assessment of deprivation.

Limitations and future directions

The present study is not without limitations that should be addressed in future work. First is the small sample size of included participants. Future work is needed to determine whether our findings hold in larger samples of children exposed to a range of adversities. Second, although we included multiple levels of analysis for our outcome of interest, future work should examine whether these associations emerge when using child-report or observational measures. Although child-reports of emotion regulation are challenging given linguistic and cognitive limitations (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Martin and Dennis2004), methodologies utilizing vignettes or play-based tasks (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009) could provide additional insight into the emotion regulation abilities of adversity-exposed children. Moreover, for parental socialization of emotion it would be particularly meaningful to include observational measures that do not rely on parental report. Parents prone to emotion dysregulation may not always recognize their emotional difficulties and observational measures might provide novel insight into their functioning. That being said, in our sample, parental reports on the DERS varied widely (range = 36–118), suggesting that some parents willingly reported high levels of emotional difficulties. Moreover, our results held even when controlling for parental psychopathology, which is often highly linked to emotion regulation tendencies (Sheppes, Suri, & Gross, Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015) and children's exposure to adversity (Berlin, Appleyard, & Dodge, Reference Berlin, Appleyard and Dodge2011; Stith et al., Reference Stith, Liu, Davies, Boykin, Alder, Harris and Dees2009).

Third, the present study sheds light on the difficulties in emotion regulation faced by children exposed to early adversity; however, future studies should further investigate the nature of these difficulties. Specifically, studies are needed to examine the types of emotion regulation strategies that adversity-exposed children use and how those strategies are differentially associated with parent socialization, deprivation, and threat. Previous studies indicate that adversity-exposed children may rely on certain emotion regulation strategies (e.g., disengagement, avoidance) more so than nonexposed children (Boyes et al., Reference Boyes, Hasking and Martin2016; Epstein-Ngo et al., Reference Epstein-Ngo, Maurizi, Bregman and Ceballo2013; Maughan & Cicchetti, Reference Maughan and Cicchetti2002; Milojevich et al., Reference Milojevich, Levine, Cathcart and Quas2018; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Morris, Heller, Scheeringa, Boris and Smyke2009). The findings of the present study should be extended to specific forms of emotion regulation.

Finally, future studies should examine the associations among parent socialization, dimensions of adversity, and children's emotion regulation across development. The present study focused on young children as early childhood is a time of rapid development of emotion regulation (Denham, Reference Denham1998). Moreover, in early childhood children rely on their parents for help regulating their emotions (Thompson & Meyer, Reference Thompson, Meyer and Gross2007). As such, the relative contribution of parent socialization and the direct effects of adversity may change with age, as children come to rely more on themselves and less on their parents when regulating their emotions.

Implications for interventions

The present study also has important implications for the development and implementation of evidence-based interventions. Many interventions designed to help when maltreatment has occurred or with parents who are exhibiting harsh discipline focus on identifying strategies other than physical punishment that parents can use when disciplining their child. The goal with these interventions is often to end the coercive cycle of physical punishment. While this is a worthwhile and meaningful goal, our work suggests that if we want to improve outcomes for children (e.g., emotional and behavioral functioning) we also need to teach parents how to better regulate their emotions and how to teach their children more adaptive, successful emotion regulation. By targeting parents’ own emotion regulation tendencies and the emotional messages that parents send to their children, we may be able to more robustly improve children's outcomes in addition to preventing further abuse. These implications are in need of direct testing, but hold promise for improving the well-being of children, parents, and families as a whole.

Financial Statement

This work was supported in part by a fellowship provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (T32-HD07376) through the Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill to L. Machlin and by a grant provided by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH115004) to M.A. Sheridan.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Footnotes

1 Of note, for all three regression models we tested relevant interactions. Specifically, we tested Gender x Deprivation (and Threat), as well as Deprivation (and Threat) x Parental difficulties with emotion regulation interactions. None of the interaction models were significant.

2 The difference between high-deprivation (M = 27.77) and low-deprivation (M = 24.90) scores on the negativity/liability subscale of the ERC was also trending in the expected direction, t (54) = −1.54, p = .13. The two groups did not differ in their early extinction amplitudes of skin conductance to the CS+ controlling for the CS− amplitude, F (1,44) = .31, n.s.

References

About Adverse Childhood Experiences. (2019). In Centers for Disease Control and Prevention online. Retrieved from https://www+.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/childabuseandneglect/acestudy/aboutace.htmlGoogle Scholar
Adler, N., & Stewart, J. (2007). The MacArthur scale of subjective social status. MacArthur Research Network on SES & Health. Retrieved from http://Www.Macses.Ucsf.Edu/Research/Psychosocial/Subjective.PhpGoogle Scholar
Alink, L. R. A., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J., & Rogosch, F. A. (2009). Mediating and moderating processes in the relation between maltreatment and psychopathology: Mother-child relationship quality and emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 831843. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9314-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bain, S. K., & Jaspers, K. E. (2010). Test review: Review of Kaufman brief intelligence test. Second Edition: Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson, Inc. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 28, 167174. doi:10.1177/0734282909348217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balge, K. A., & Milner, J. S. (2000). Emotion recognition ability in mothers at high and low risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 12891298. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00188-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bariola, E., Gullone, E., & Hughes, E. K. (2011). Child and adolescent emotion regulation: The role of parental emotion regulation and expression. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 198212. doi:10.1007/s10567-011-0092-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bariola, E., Hughes, E. K., & Gullone, E. (2012). Relationships between parent and child emotion regulation strategy use: A brief report. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21(3), 443448. doi:10.1007/s10826-011-9497-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, L. J., Appleyard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (2011). Intergenerational continuity in child maltreatment: Mediating mechanisms and implications for prevention: Intergenerational continuity. Child Development, 82, 162176. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01547.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucsein, W. (2012). Electrodermal activity. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyes, M. E., Hasking, P. A., & Martin, G. (2016). Adverse life experience and psychological distress in adolescence: Moderating and mediating effects of emotion regulation and rumination: Life experience, emotion regulation, rumination and psychological distress. Stress and Health, 32, 402410. doi:10.1002/smi.2635CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., & Corwyn, R. F. (2003). The Child Care HOME Inventories: Assessing the quality of family child care homes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18, 294309. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(03)00041-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R., & Rowe, M. (2013). A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) & skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. Psychophysiology, 49, 10171034.Google Scholar
Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2017). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A multi-level framework. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 15451557. doi:10.1093/scan/nsx096CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brophy-Herb, H. E., Stansbury, K., Bocknek, E., & Horodynski, M. A. (2012). Modeling maternal emotion-related socialization behaviors in a low-income sample: Relations with toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 352364. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busso, D. S., McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Dimensions of adversity, physiological reactivity, and externalizing psychopathology in adolescence: Deprivation and threat. Psychosomatic Medicine, 79(2), 162171. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Spaccarelli, S., & Stefani, R. (1988). Recognition and posing of emotional expressions by abused children and their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 24, 776781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 598606. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: Gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion, 5, 8088. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Dennis, T. A., Smith-Simon, K. E., & Cohen, L. H. (2009). Preschoolers' emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation. Social Development, 18, 324352. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00503.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317333. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Criss, M. M., Morris, A. S., Ponce-Garcia, E., Cui, L., & Silk, J. S. (2016). Pathways to adaptive emotion regulation among adolescents from low-income families: Pathways to adaptive emotion regulation. Family Relations, 65, 517529. doi:10.1111/fare.12202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 387411. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers’ emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 30, 928936. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.30.6.928CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., & Covi, L. (1973). SCL-90: An outpatient psychiatric rating scale–preliminary report. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 9, 1328.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunsmore, J. C., Booker, J. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2013). Parental emotion coaching and child emotion regulation as protective factors for children with oppositional defiant disorder: Emotion coaching with children with ODD. Social Development, 22, 444466. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00652.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunsmore, J. C., Her, P., Halberstadt, A. G., & Perez-Rivera, M. B. (2009). Parents’ beliefs about emotions and children's recognition of parents’ emotions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 121140. doi:10.1007/s10919-008-0066-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunsmore, J. C., & Karn, M. A. (2001). Mothers’ beliefs about feelings and children's emotional understanding. Early Education & Development, 12, 117138. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1201_7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241273. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. H., Alisic, E., Reiss, A., Dishion, T., & Fisher, P. A. (2014). Emotion regulation among preschoolers on a continuum of risk: The role of maternal emotion coaching. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 965974. doi:10.1007/s10826-013-9752-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein-Ngo, Q., Maurizi, L. K., Bregman, A., & Ceballo, R. (2013). In response to community violence: Coping strategies and involuntary stress responses among Latino adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 19, 3849. doi:10.1037/a0029753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fosco, G. M., & Grych, J. H. (2013). Capturing the family context of emotion regulation: A family systems model comparison approach. Journal of Family Issues, 34(4), 557578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Leavitt, L. A. (1995). The violence exposure scale for children-revised (VEX-R). College Park, MD: University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Frankenburg, W. K., & Coons, C. E. (1986). Home Screening Questionnaire: Its validity in assessing home environment. The Journal of Pediatrics, 108, 624626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 38, 222235. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1989). Effects of marital discord on young children's peer interaction and health. Developmental Psychology, 25, 373381. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 4154. doi:10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Frenn, K., Wewerka, S. S., & Van Ryzin, M. J. (2009). Moderate versus severe early life stress: Associations with stress reactivity and regulation in 10–12-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 6275. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145173. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & Quevedo, K. M. (2008). Early care experiences and HPA axis regulation in children: A mechanism for later trauma vulnerability. Progress in Brain Research, 167, 137149. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(07)67010-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & Vazquez, D. M. (2001). Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: Potential indices of risk in human development. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 515538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D., & Griggs, C. (2009). Developmental changes in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations with puberty. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 6985. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D., & Griggs, C. (2010). Developmental changes in hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations with puberty. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 237237. doi:10.1017/S095457940999037XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G., Fox, N. A., & Jones, N. A. (1993). Do expressive mothers have expressive children? The role of socialization in children's affect expression. Social Development, 2, 4865. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.1993.tb00004.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallion, L. S., Steinman, S. A., Tolin, D. F., & Diefenbach, G. J. (2018). Psychometric properties of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) and its short forms in adults with emotional disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 539. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hébert, M., Langevin, R., & Oussaïd, E. (2018). Cumulative childhood trauma, emotion regulation, dissociation, and behavior problems in school-aged sexual abuse victims. Journal of Affective Disorders, 225, 306312. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostinar, C. E., Sullivan, R. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2014). Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 256282. doi:10.1037/a0032671CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jovanovic, T., Nylocks, K. M., Gamwell, K. L., Smith, A., Davis, T. A., Norrholm, S. D., & Bradley, B. (2014). Development of fear acquisition and extinction in children: Effects of age and anxiety. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 113, 135142. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kantor, G. K., Holt, M. K., Mebert, C. J., Straus, M. A., Drach, K. M., Ricci, L. R., … Brown, W. (2004). Development and preliminary psychometric properties of the multidimensional neglectful behavior scale-child report. Child Maltreatment, 9, 409428. doi:10.1177/1077559504269530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, L. F., & Rigterink, T. (2012). Domestic violence and emotion socialization: Domestic violence and emotion socialization. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 5260. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00661.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim-Spoon, J., Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2013). A longitudinal study of emotion regulation, emotion lability-negativity, and internalizing symptomatology in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Child Development, 84, 512527. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01857.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2009). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology: Pathways linking maltreatment, emotion regulation, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 706716. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 706716. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliewer, W., Parrish, K. A., Taylor, K. W., Jackson, K., Walker, J. M., & Shivy, V. A. (2006). Socialization of coping with community violence: Influences of caregiver coaching, modeling, and family context. Child Development, 77, 605623. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00893.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, H. K., King, K. M., Monahan, K. C., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Differential associations of threat and deprivation with emotion regulation and cognitive control in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 929940. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000584CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loman, M. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 867876. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T. (1972). Range correction applied to heart rate and to GSR data. Psychophysiology, 9, 373379. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1972.tb03222.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: A proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 8, 656672. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1971.tb00501.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machlin, L. S., Miller, A. B., Snyder, J., McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2019). Differential associations between deprivation and threat with cognitive control and fear conditioning in early childhood. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 80. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maughan, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children's emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73, 15251542. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00488CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mauss, I. B., Bunge, S. A., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Automatic emotion regulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1, 146167. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00005.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., Peverill, M., Gold, A. L., Alves, S., & Sheridan, M. A. (2015). Child maltreatment and neural systems underlying emotion regulation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 753762. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2015.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 239245. doi:10.1177/0963721416655883CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Alves, S., & Mendes, W. B. (2014). Child maltreatment and autonomic nervous system reactivity: Identifying dysregulated stress reactivity patterns by using the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76, 538546. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Gold, A. L., Duys, A., Lambert, H. K., Peverill, M., … Pine, D. S. (2016). Maltreatment exposure, brain structure, and fear conditioning in children and adolescents. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41, 19561964. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578591. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, S., Raikes, H. A., Virmani, E. A., Waters, S., & Thompson, R. A. (2014). Parent emotion representations and the socialization of emotion regulation in the family. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 164173. doi:10.1177/0165025413519014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. B., Sheridan, M. A., Hanson, J. L., McLaughlin, K. A., Bates, J. E., Lansford, J. E., … Dodge, K. A. (2018). Dimensions of deprivation and threat, psychopathology, and potential mediators: A multi-year longitudinal analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127, 160170. doi:10.1037/abn0000331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, J. S., Charlesworth, J. R., Gold, R. G., Gold, S. R., & Friesen, M. R. (1988). Convergent validity of the child abuse potential inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 281285.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milojevich, H. M., & Haskett, M. E. (2018). Longitudinal associations between physically abusive parents’ emotional expressiveness and children's self-regulation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 77, 144154. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milojevich, H. M., Levine, L. J., Cathcart, E. J., & Quas, J. A. (2018). The role of maltreatment in the development of coping strategies. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 54, 2332. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2017.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milojevich, H. M., Norwalk, K. E., & Sheridan, M. A. (2019). Deprivation and threat, emotion dysregulation, and psychopathology: Concurrent and longitudinal associations. Development and Psychopathology, 31, 847857. doi:10.1017/S0954579419000294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Morris, M. D. S., Steinberg, L., Aucoin, K. J., & Keyes, A. W. (2011). The influence of mother–child emotion regulation strategies on children's expression of anger and sadness. Developmental Psychology, 47, 213225. doi:10.1037/a0021021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peisch, V., Dale, C., Parent, J., & Burt, K. (2019). Parent socialization of coping and child emotion regulation abilities: A longitudinal examination. Family Process. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/famp.12516Google Scholar
Perez Rivera, M. B., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2011). Mothers’ acculturation and beliefs about emotions, mother–child emotion discourse, and children's emotion understanding in Latino families. Early Education & Development, 22, 324354. doi:10.1080/10409281003702000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raver, C. C., & Spagnola, M. (2002). When My Mommy Was Angry, I Was Speechless: Children's perceptions of maternal emotional expressiveness within the context of economic hardship. Marriage & Family Review, 34, 6388. doi:10.1300/J002v34n01_04CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, L. R., Morris, A. S., Heller, S. S., Scheeringa, M. S., Boris, N. W., & Smyke, A. T. (2009). Relations between emotion regulation, parenting, and psychopathology in young maltreated children in out of home care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18, 421434. doi:10.1007/s10826-008-9246-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, M. L., Halberstadt, A. G., Castro, V. L., MacCormack, J. K., & Garrett-Peters, P. (2016). Maternal emotion socialization differentially predicts third-grade children's emotion regulation and lability. Emotion, 16, 280291. doi:10.1037/emo0000142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romens, S. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2012). Emotion regulation predicts attention bias in maltreated children at-risk for depression: Emotion regulation in maltreated children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 120127. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02474.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, A., Suveg, C., Thomassin, K., & Bradbury, L. L. (2012). Emotion socialization in the context of family risks: Links to child emotion regulation. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21, 917924. doi:10.1007/s10826-011-9551-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 379405. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2014). Dimensions of early experience and neural development: Deprivation and threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 580585. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Neurobiological models of the impact of adversity on education.. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 108113. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheridan, M. A., Peverill, M., Finn, A. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Dimensions of childhood adversity have distinct associations with neural systems underlying executive functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 29(5), 17771794. doi:10.1017/S0954579417001390CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 27, 381395. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, A. M., & Cicchetti, D. (1995). The development of an emotion regulation assessment battery: Reliability and validity among at-risk grade-school children. Poster Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.Google Scholar
Shields, A. M., Cicchetti, D., & Ryan, R. M. (1994). The development of emotional and behavioral self-regulation and social competence among maltreated school-age children. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 5775. doi:10.1017/S0954579400005885CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipman, K., Edwards, A, Brown, A, Swisher, L., & Jennings, E.. (2005). Managing emotion in a maltreating context: A pilot study examining child neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(9), 10151029. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipman, K. L., Schneider, R., Fitzgerald, M. M., Sims, C., Swisher, L., & Edwards, A. (2007). Maternal emotion socialization in maltreating and non-maltreating families: Implications for children's emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 268285. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00384.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipman, K. L., & Zeman, J. (2001). Socialization of children's emotion regulation in mother–child dyads: A developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 317336. doi:10.1017/S0954579401002073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvers, J. A., Lumian, D. S., Gabard-Durnam, L., Gee, D. G., Goff, B., Fareri, D. S., … Tottenham, N. (2016). Previous institutionalization is followed by broader amygdala–hippocampal–pfc network connectivity during aversive learning in human development. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 64206430. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0038-16.2016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stith, S. M., Liu, T., Davies, L. C., Boykin, E. L., Alder, M. C., Harris, J. M., … Dees, J. E. M. E. G. (2009). Risk factors in child maltreatment: A meta-analytic review of the literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 1329. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2006.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S. U. E., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2) development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283316. doi:10.1177/019251396017003001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., & Manning, L. G. (2012). Interparental violence, maternal emotional unavailability and children's cortisol functioning in family contexts. Developmental Psychology, 48, 237249. doi:10.1037/a0025419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarullo, A. R., Bruce, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2007). False belief and emotion understanding in post-institutionalized children. Social Development, 16, 5778. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00372.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2006). Child maltreatment and the developing HPA axis. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 632639. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thabet, A. A. M., Tischler, V., & Vostanis, P. (2004). Maltreatment and coping strategies among male adolescents living in the Gaza Strip. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 7791. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2002.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (2007). Socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.) Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 249268). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., … Casey, B. J. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation: Previous institutionalization. Developmental Science, 13, 4661. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlations among main predictor and child regulation scores

Figure 2

Table 3. Regression results for emotion regulation checklist

Figure 3

Table 4. Regression results for amplitude of SCR (skin conductance response) to the CS+ during fear extinction