Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-v2ckm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T07:54:49.921Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental trends of hot and cool executive function in school-aged children with and without autism spectrum disorder: Links with theory of mind

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 March 2018

Evangelia-Chrysanthi Kouklari*
Affiliation:
University of Greenwich
Stella Tsermentseli
Affiliation:
University of Greenwich
Claire P. Monks
Affiliation:
University of Greenwich
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Evangelia-Chrysanthi Kouklari, Department of Psychology, Social Work, and Counselling, Bronte Building, University of Greenwich, Avery Hill Road, London, UK SE9 2UG; E-mail: e.kouklari@gre.ac.uk.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The development of executive function (EF) in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has been investigated using only “cool”-cognitive EF tasks while there is limited knowledge regarding the development of “hot”-affective EF. Although cool EF development and its links to theory of mind (ToM) have been widely examined, understanding of the influence of hot EF to ToM mechanisms is minimal. The present study introduced a longitudinal design to examine the developmental changes in cool and hot EF of children with ASD (n = 45) and matched (to age and IQ) controls (n = 37) as well as the impact of EF on ToM development over a school year. For children with ASD, although selective cool (working memory and inhibition) and hot (affective decision making) EF domains presented age-related improvements, they never reached the performance level of the control group. Early cool working memory predicted later ToM in both groups but early hot delay discounting predicted later ToM only in the ASD group. No evidence was found for the reverse pattern (early ToM predicting later EF). These findings suggest that improvements in some EF aspects are evident in school age in ASD and highlight the crucial role that both cool and hot EF play in ToM development.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a multifaceted neurodevelopmental disorder that significantly impairs children's verbal and nonverbal communication, social interactions, and behaviors (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Executive dysfunction has received increased attention, as deficits in aspects of executive function (EF) have been demonstrated consistently in ASD samples across the life span (see Hill's Reference Hill2004 review and Demetriou et al.'s Reference Demetriou, Lampit, Quintana, Naismith, Song, Pye and Guastella2017 meta-analysis). EF refers to a set of future-oriented and goal-directed cognitive skills, closely associated to the prefrontal cortex, that are crucial for problem solving and social behavior, as well as the ability to organize oneself (Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010). EF problems in ASD typically manifest as deficits in domains such as working memory (Alloway, Gathercole, Kirkwood, & Elliott, Reference Alloway, Gathercole, Kirkwood and Elliott2009; Geurts, de Vries, & van den Bergh, Reference Geurts, de Vries, van den Bergh, Goldstein and Naglier2014; Kercood, Grskovic, Banda, & Begeske, Reference Kercood, Grskovic, Banda and Begeske2014), inhibition (Christ, Holt, White, & Green, Reference Christ, Holt, White and Green2007; Happé, Booth, Charlton, & Hughes, Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes2006; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Xiao, Ke, Hong, Yang, Su and Liu2012), planning (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Chien, Wu, Shang, Wu and Gau2016; Kimhi, Kugelmas, Agam Ben Artzi, Ben Moshe, & Bauminger-Zviely, Reference Kimhi, Kugelmas, Agam Ben Artzi, Ben Moshe and Bauminger-Zviely2014; Verté, Geurts, Roeyers, Oosterlaanm & Sergeant, Reference Verté, Geurts, Roeyers, Oosterlaan and Sergeant2005) or cognitive flexibility (Gioia, Isquith, Kenworthy, & Barton, Reference Gioia, Isquith, Kenworthy and Barton2002; South, Ozonoff, & McMahon, Reference South, Ozonoff and McMahon2007; Yeung, Han, Sze, & Chan, Reference Yeung, Han, Sze and Chan2016). Although research has shown that impairments in specific components of EF (mostly cognitive flexibility and planning) have been conceptualized as being key aspects in discriminating ASD from other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; for a review, see Craig et al., Reference Craig, Margari, Legrottaglie, Palumbi, de Giambattista and Margari2016), the additive effect comorbid ADHD has been found to have on EF deficits, such as working memory (Andersen, Hovik, Skogli, Egeland, & Oie, Reference Andersen, Hovik, Skogli, Egeland and Oie2013; Yerys et al., Reference Yerys, Wallace, Sokoloff, Shook, James and Kenworthy2009) and inhibition (Sinzig, Morsch, Bruning, Schmidt, & Lehmkuhl, Reference Sinzig, Morsch, Bruning, Schmidt and Lehmkuhl2008) in ASD, should not be overlooked either.

EF development is generally considered to influence the development of children's social cognition (Devine, White, Ensor, & Hughes, Reference Devine, White, Ensor and Hughes2016) and is intimately tied to theory of mind (ToM), another cognitive domain known to develop atypically in ASD (Lantz, Reference Lantz2002). ToM is the ability to infer mental/emotional states in order to predict and explain behavior (Goldman, Reference Goldman, Margolis, Samuels and Stich2012). ToM is a multifaceted cognitive skill that develops gradually; its development commences in infancy and continues to improve throughout middle childhood and adolescence. The understanding of false belief (understanding that one's belief/representation about the world can contrast with reality), which is a critical aspect of ToM, measured by first-order false belief tasks, typically emerges at the age of 3–4 years (Schug, Takagishi, Benech, & Okada, Reference Schug, Takagishi, Benech and Okada2016). As children grow up, they present age-related performance gains and become capable of solving more complex, high-order ToM tasks (e.g., emotion understanding) across middle and later childhood (Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2013; Dumontheil, Apperly, & Blakemore, Reference Dumontheil, Apperly and Blakemore2010). Evidence from ASD suggests that some children with ASD fail to fully develop the prerequisites of ToM and that the ToM trajectory could follow either a delayed (Steele, Joseph, & Tager-Flusberg, Reference Steele, Joseph and Tager-Flusberg2003) or a deviant (Peterson, Wellman, & Liu, Reference Peterson, Wellman and Liu2005; Serra, Loth, van Geert, Hurkens, & Mineraa, Reference Serra, Loth, van Geert, Hurkens and Minderaa2002) pathway in ASD.

Evidence from both typical development and ASD has consistently shown that EF is strongly associated to ToM across childhood (Bock, Gallaway, & Hund, Reference Bock, Gallaway and Hund2015; Carlson, Claxton, & Moses, Reference Carlson, Claxton and Moses2013; Im-Bolter, Agostino, & Owens-Jaffray, Reference Im-Bolter, Agostino and Owens-Jaffray2016). Several developmental psychologists have argued that there is a more fundamental link between EF and ToM with functioning in one domain being a necessity for the emergence of the other. Perner (Reference Perner, Carruthers and Boucher1998) and Perner and Lang (Reference Perner and Lang1999) proposed that the acquisition of ToM is a prerequisite of children's ability to control their behavior (EF), while alternatively, Russell (Reference Russell1996) suggested that the ability to control one's actions (EF) must be attained first as it then crucially influences the development of ToM. Several longitudinal studies on the EF-ToM relationship in early childhood in typical development indicated that children's performance on EF measures predicted later performance on ToM false belief tasks (independent of age, verbal ability, and earlier ToM scores) but not vice versa (Carlson, Mandell, & Moses, Reference Carlson, Mandell and Williams2004; Flynn, Reference Flynn2007; Hughes, Reference Hughes1998; Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2007). These findings highlight that EF is a prerequisite for the development of ToM in early life as it scaffolds the emergence of ToM mechanisms (Apperly, Samson, & Humphreys, Reference Apperly, Samson and Humphreys2009; Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses, & Lee, Reference Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses and Lee2006). However, as this theoretical position does not account for potential EF-ToM associations beyond the fifth year, less is known about the extension of this developmental relationship in school-aged children with and without ASD. Relevant longitudinal research in school age is very limited (e.g., Austin, Groppe, & Elsner, Reference Austin, Groppe and Elsner2014) and has demonstrated weak evidence for the account that early ToM predicts later EF, but stronger support of the early EF influencing later ToM in school age. This could suggest that EF might play a substantial role in children's developmental outcomes, particularly in relation to social cognition.

The developmental nature of the EF-ToM relationship in ASD has been vastly theoretically debated due to this coexistence of deficits in both domains. Building on the initial propositions regarding the executive dysfunction influencing autism symptomatology (Pennington & Ozonoff, Reference Pennington and Ozonoff1996; Russell, Reference Russell and Russell1997), there is growing evidence showing that the ASD functional/social outcomes, such as ToM, may be due to differences in emerging EF abilities (Demetriou et al., Reference Demetriou, Lampit, Quintana, Naismith, Song, Pye and Guastella2017; Leung, Vogan, Powell, Anagnostou, & Taylor, Reference Leung, Vogan, Powell, Anagnostou and Taylor2016; Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2012; Russo et al., Reference Russo, Flanagan, Iarocci, Berringer, Zelazo and Burack2007). Performance on measures of EF and ToM (mainly false belief understanding tasks) have been found to be correlated in ASD (Kimhi et al., Reference Kimhi, Kugelmas, Agam Ben Artzi, Ben Moshe and Bauminger-Zviely2014; Kouklari, Thompson, Monks, & Tsermentseli, Reference Kouklari, Thompson, Monks and Tsermentseli2017; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, Reference Ozonoff, Pennington and Rogers1991; Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2007). Evidence regarding the link between EF and ToM in ASD mainly derives from the preschool period, and full consideration of this developmental issue has not yet been possible due to the limited number of studies analyzing this issue within a longitudinal design. Pellicano (Reference Pellicano2010) in a longitudinal study of two time points with children aged 4–7 years, (followed after 3 years), indicated that cool EF abilities (planning, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition) at the first time point were a significant predictor of the changes in children's ToM skills at the second time point (over and above the variance of age, verbal ability, and nonverbal ability) in ASD. Moreover, Tager-Flusberg and Joseph (Reference Tager-Flusberg, Joseph, Schneider, Schumann-Hengsteler and Sodian2005) found that ASD children's (5–14 years) performance on early EF tasks predicted later ToM abilities after 1 year independent of initial ToM scores and verbal ability. These studies show that in ASD, as in typical development, EF skills play a critical role in shaping the development of ToM. The important contribution of EF in typical development, together with these promising findings from studies in ASD, provide good reason to suspect that the development of EF might critically influence children's developmental trajectories of sociocognitive profiles, particularly ToM skills (with poor EF being a risk factor for poor developmental outcomes) in ASD.

It should be noted that the determination of the precise nature of the EF developmental pathway and its influence on ToM in ASD first requires research to shed more light on the actual nature of EF itself (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2012). EF is by nature a complex construct to characterize, as most attempts to determine its nature reflect that “EF is by no means a unitary concept” (Elliott, Reference Elliott2003). EF has traditionally been viewed through a purely cognitive lens. However, the theoretical distinction between cool and hot EF aspects, suggested by Zelazo and Müller (Reference Zelazo, Müller and Goswami2002), could aid in better understanding the effect of EF development on ToM and social cognition in general. According to this distinction, cool EF includes processes evoked in motivationally neutral, nonaffective situations such as inhibition, working memory, and planning (Zelazo & Carlson, Reference Zelazo and Carlson2012) that are usually tapped by decontextualized EF tasks, such as Go/No-Go, Tower of London, and Digit Span tasks (Anderson, Anderson, Jacobs, & Spencer-Smith, Reference Anderson, Anderson, Jacobs, Spencer-Smith, Anderson, Jacobs and Anderson2008). In contrast to cool EF, hot EF are elicited under motivationally significant, affective conditions such as delay discounting (the tendency to choose more immediate, smaller rewards) and affective decision making (mental processing occurring on the selection of one or more possible options under risk where one employs both rational and emotional processes). Hot EF is therefore mainly measured by tasks with meaningful rewards and losses for the individual (i.e., the Gambling task and/or Delay Discounting tasks).

EF emerges early in life, evidences critical changes in school age, and matures by adolescence (Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010). EF development has been mainly assessed by tasks assessing cool EF despite recent evidence supporting separate domains of cool and hot EF (Kim, Nordling, Yoon, Boldt, & Kochanska, Reference Kim, Nordling, Yoon, Boldt and Kochanska2014; Willoughby, Kupersmidt, Voegler-Lee, & Bryant, Reference Willoughby, Kupersmidt, Voegler-Lee and Bryant2011). Generally little is known about the developmental course of “hot”-affective EF processes and whether cool and hot EF present similar developmental changes. Hot EF is suggested as presenting rapid developments in the early years of life in typical development followed by age-related improvements across middle childhood and adolescence (Prencipe et al., Reference Prencipe, Kesek, Cohen, Lamm, Lewis and Zelazo2011; Zelazo & Müller, Reference Zelazo, Müller and Goswami2002). Only a few cross-sectional studies to date have investigated the development of hot EF (in conjunction with cool EF) mainly in typical development and have yielded mixed results. No significant differences have been found in the development of hot and cool EF in the preschool period (3–5 years) with both domains being correlated and exhibiting performance gains after the third year of life (Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee, & Zelazo, Reference Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee and Zelazo2005). Further evidence from middle childhood and adolescence (Hooper, Luciana, Conklin, & Yarger, Reference Hooper, Luciana, Conklin and Yarger2004; Prencipe et al., Reference Prencipe, Kesek, Cohen, Lamm, Lewis and Zelazo2011) suggested that the weakly correlated hot and cool EF develop independently, and hot EF is likely to follow a differentiated developmental trajectory beyond the 5 years of age. The potential differences in the developmental trends of cool and hot EF is an open topic of debate, and such examination in ASD could make it plausible for separate EF domains to be found specifically affected or have specific developmental relations to other outcomes such as ToM.

The limited number of longitudinal EF studies in ASD have focused only on cool aspects to date, and due to mixed results, it is not clear whether the development of EF in ASD follows the same pathway as that of typical development. Early studies (Griffith, Pennington, Wehner, & Rogers, Reference Griffith, Pennington, Wehner and Rogers1999; Ozonoff & McEvoy, Reference Ozonoff and McEvoy1994) indicated a lack of age-related improvements and poorer performance for children and adolescents with ASD relative to matched controls in measures of selective cool EF (i.e., planning and cognitive flexibility) across time. More recently, though, Pellicano (Reference Pellicano2010) found that EF planning of preschoolers with ASD (mean age 5.5 years) improved significantly over a 3-year period, surprisingly at an even faster rate than the controls. Identifying the developmental pathways of both cool and hot EF could provide a solid ground to overcome the limitations of current theories of EF development and lead to a better understanding of the heterogeneity in neurocognitive impairments in ASD.

Finally, it should be noted that research into the ToM-EF association in school-aged participants has mainly employed cool EF tasks, despite hot EF and ToM being considered as being mediated by the same medial regions of the prefrontal cortex (Chan, Shum, Toulopoulou, & Chen, Reference Chan, Shum, Toulopoulou and Chen2008; McDonald, Reference McDonald2013; Sabbagh, Bowman, Evraire, & Ito, Reference Sabbagh, Bowman, Evraire and Ito2009). For instance, social interactions that involve ToM abilities may require the control of behavior or thought under emotionally significant situations (hot EF; Zelazo & Müller, Reference Zelazo, Müller and Goswami2002). Thus, hot EF could be more central to the emergence of ToM. There is no study in ASD to date that has investigated whether hot EF influences ToM across time and how they may interact within the cognitive profile of school-aged children with ASD. The present study may hopefully enhance our understanding of the higher order cognitive deficits that underpin social interaction problems in ASD.

Current Objectives

Research on the development of hot EF both in typical development and in ASD lags behind that of cool EF and the understanding of the potential link between hot EF and ToM is quite limited. Longitudinal studies investigating the links between EF and ToM in the school-age period (>5 years) are very limited (Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2014). Extending the developmental EF-ToM research in school-aged children is critical in order to examine whether developmental patterns found in early life persist across the course of children's development (McAlister & Peterson, Reference McAlister and Peterson2013). Moreover, based on the evidence presented above (Hooper et al., Reference Hooper, Luciana, Conklin and Yarger2004; Prencipe et al., Reference Prencipe, Kesek, Cohen, Lamm, Lewis and Zelazo2011), distinct EF domains (hot and cool) are expected to follow different developmental trends and become more specialized across middle childhood and adolescence. The present study therefore attempted to address these gaps in the literature.

The first aim of this study was to compare the developmental changes in cool and hot EF and ToM abilities between school-aged children with and without ASD after a 1-year interval. Taken together, previous studies show there is no clear developmental framework of cool EF in ASD, with some studies reporting age-related improvements (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010) and others not (Griffith et al., Reference Griffith, Pennington, Wehner and Rogers1999; Ozonoff & McEvoy, Reference Ozonoff and McEvoy1994). Moreover, no research to date has investigated the development of hot EF in ASD. Due to these mixed findings and the minimal longitudinal evidence regarding the development of hot EF in ASD, the present study was exploratory, and specific predictions could not be made. We sought to determine whether there are similarities or deviance/delay relative to controls in the hot and cool EF developmental pathways followed in ASD.

The second aim was to shed more light on the longitudinal association between cool and hot EF and ToM in school-aged children with and without ASD. Based on previous research, specific predictions can be made only about the cool EF-ToM link, due to the current lack of relevant longitudinal hot EF research. Specifically, there is stronger evidence for the cool EF to predict the emergence of ToM rather than the opposite in early childhood and the preschool period in typical development (Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, O'Brien, Calkins, Leerkes, Weaver and Levine2014) and ASD (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010, Reference Pellicano2012). We hypothesized that early cool EF would predict later ToM also in school-aged children with and without ASD. Taking into consideration the theoretical notion that ToM may be more strongly related to hot EF than cool EF (Zelazo, Qu, & Müller, Reference Zelazo, Qu, Müller, Schneider, Schumann-Hengsteler and Sodian2005), we attempted to examine whether later ToM performance could be also predicted by early hot EF performance after controlling for potential covariates and cool EF and whether the association of hot EF variables to ToM was stronger in either controls or ASD, by including Hot EF × ASD Diagnosis interaction terms.

Method

Participants

Forty-five children with a clinical diagnosis of ASD (38 males; M = 9.07 years, SD = 1.42) and 37 controls (35 males; M = 9.03 years, SD = 1.17) aged 7–11 years old were recruited to participate in the present study. They were followed up 1 year after the initial assessment (two time points). At the second time point, all 82 children were followed up (0% attrition). All ASD participants were high functioning (IQ >70), held a clinical diagnosis by a qualified clinician using DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) or DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria, and qualified for a “broad ASD” on the Autism Diagnostic Interview/Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (Le Couteur et al., Reference Le Couteur, Rutter, Lord, Rios, Robertson, Holdgrafer and McLennan1989; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, Reference Lord, Rutter and Le Couteur1994) and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, Reference Lord, Rutter, DiLavore and Risi2000), in accordance to National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (2011) guidelines. They were also in receipt of a Statement of Special Educational Needs, a legal document that details the child's needs and services that the local authority has a duty to provide, which specified ASD as their primary need. All clinical records were inspected, and any individual lacking detailed information about the official source of diagnosis was excluded from the study. Additional exclusion criteria for the ASD group included the presence of a diagnosed psychiatric illness, comorbid conditions (i.e., ADHD, seizures, or color blindness), and full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) below 70 as determined by the abbreviated version of the Wechsler Intelligence scales (two subtests: vocabulary and matrix reasoning; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999). Typically developing participants were required to have no diagnosis, and no family history of ASD or other mental health disorders, dyslexia, or learning disability. Participants were matched for chronological age, t (80) = –0.13, p = .89, and FSIQ, t (80) = 1.73, p = .09. Most of the participants were White from mixed socioeconomic backgrounds and spoke English as their first language. Specific data on the socioeconomic status or educational attainment levels of their parents were not recorded. Ethical approval for the study was obtained and all participants’ parents/carers gave written informed consent (consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki) in compliance to the University Research Ethics Committee. Table 1 shows descriptive characteristics (means and standard deviations) of participants of both groups.

Table 1. Participants’ characteristics

Measures

Cool executive function

Inhibition

The “R” and “P” version of the Go/No-Go paradigm (Mueller & Piper, Reference Mueller and Piper2014) was used in the present study to assess participants’ response inhibition. An image of either the letter P or the letter R appeared in the center of the screen (for 1500 ms) on a black background. Participants were then instructed to press the button only when the letter P was shown (Go trials) and to avoid pressing it for the letter R (No-Go trials). On the second block of trials, the pattern was reversed, and the participants were asked to press the button when the letter R appeared (Go trials) and to avoid pressing it when P was presented (No-Go trials) this time. There was no feedback provided after a correct or incorrect response. Before each block, participants first completed 10 practice trials followed by the actual 320 test trials. In order to measure participants’ response inhibition, the proportion of incorrect No-Go trials was recorded. Lower scores indicated better performance.

Planning

Participants’ planning ability was measured by the Tower of London (ToL) task (Shallice, Reference Shallice1982). As a practice, participants were presented with 3 three-move problems, followed by the 12 actual trials of the original problem set (2 two-move tasks; 2 three-move tasks; 4 four-move tasks; and 4 five-move tasks). Following the procedure of Monks, Smith, and Swettenham (Reference Monks, Smith and Swettenham2005) and Poland, Monks, and Tsermentseli (Reference Poland, Monks and Tsermentseli2015), successful performance required participants to solve each problem moving only one bead each time and in the number of moves required; participants were given 2 min to complete each problem. The task was stopped when the participant completed all problems or failed two consecutively. In terms of the scoring, we measured the number of problems each participant completed successfully. One point was given to participants if they completed the problem successfully and 0 points if they failed to complete the problem. Scores ranged from 0 to 12.

Working memory

The digit span forward and backward subtests from the third edition of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children were employed to measure participants’ verbal working memory (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1991). Participants were asked to recall the sequence presented by the researcher (at a rate of one number per second) in the exact same order. In the backward digit recall task, the series of numbers should be repeated in reverse order. If participants responded successfully to all trials within a block, the researcher proceeded to the next block. Each block included 2 trials at each span length. In terms of scoring, participants were awarded 1 point for each correct trial, and the task was terminated when the participant failed both trials at any given span length. The sum of the points awarded for both the forward and backward subtests created a working memory score which was then converted into a standardized score.

Hot executive function

Affective decision making

A modified computerized version of the Iowa Gambling task (IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, Reference Bechara, Damasio, Damasio and Anderson1994) was employed to measure participants’ affective decision making. Participants were presented with four decks of cards (A, B, C, and D) and were told they should select a card from any of the four decks each time. Decks A and B were equivalent in terms of overall net loss, whereas Decks C and D were equivalent in terms of overall net winning. For each card selection, the wins and losses were set in a way that in every block of 20 cards from Decks A or B, there was a potential total gain of £1,000, interrupted by potential losses up to £1,250. Losses were less frequent but of a larger magnitude in Deck B, whereas in Deck A, losses were more frequent but in smaller amounts. For Decks C and D, the wins for each block were £500 totally while the potential net losses were £250. In Deck D, losses were less frequent and of higher magnitude relative to those in Deck C. Thus, Decks A and B were equally “disadvantageous” relative to Decks C and D, which were equally “advantageous.” We measured whether participants made mainly more advantageous or disadvantageous decisions. Based on the approach used by Verdejo-Garcia, Bechara, Recknor, and Perez-Garcia (Reference Verdejo-García, Bechara, Recknor and Perez-Garcia2006), scores were calculated by subtracting the number of disadvantageous choices (Decks A and B) from the number of advantageous choices (Decks C and D), divided then by the total number of trials.

Delay discounting

In line with previous research studying hot EF (Hongwanishkul et al., Reference Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee and Zelazo2005; Prencipe et al., Reference Prencipe, Kesek, Cohen, Lamm, Lewis and Zelazo2011), the Delay Discounting task was used in the present study in a computerized version to measure the extent to which participants do discount future rewards (Richards, Zhang, Mitchell, & Wit, Reference Richards, Zhang, Mitchell and Wit1999). This task originally included the forced choice between different amounts of money after different delays or with different chances. However, as the task was being given to school-aged participants, it was decided to modify it and completely remove the probability questions. Participants were told that they had to choose (hypothetically) between an immediate amount of money or £10 available after a delay. The test consisted of about 70 such questions, including “(a) Would you rather have £10 for sure in 30 days or (b) £2 for sure right now?” The amount of immediate money was adjusted across trials until an amount was reached that was determined by previous choices as being equivalent to a delayed £10 reward, until the participant was indifferent between the two choices (random adjusting procedure; for more details see Richards et al., Reference Richards, Zhang, Mitchell and Wit1999). For every participant, this indifference point (the amount of immediate money judged to be equivalent to £10) signified the subjective value of the delayed large reward (Richards et al., Reference Richards, Zhang, Mitchell and Wit1999). Delay discounting was determined by five delays (0, 10, 30, 180, and 365 days later). In terms of scoring, we followed the procedure described in Myerson, Green, and Warusawitharana (Reference Myerson, Green and Warusawitharana2001), where the indifference points were used to estimate delay discounting. Thus, indifference points were established within participants and were plotted against time (delay). Indifference points and delays were normalized, by expressing indifference points as proportions of the amount of the maximum delayed reward (£10) and the delays as proportions of the maximum delay (365 days). These normalized values were used as the x (delay) and y (indifference points) axes in order to plot the discounting function. Four separate trapezoids were then created by drawing vertical lines from each data point on the x-axis. The formula ([x2 – x1] × ([y1 + y2] / 2) was used to calculate the area of each trapezoid. The areas under these discounting curves were calculated by summing the resulting trapezoids.

ToM

False belief

In order to measure participants’ false belief understanding, the Sandbox task (Begeer, Bernstein, van Wijhe, Scheeren, & Koot, Reference Begeer, Bernstein, van Wijhe, Scheeren and Koot2012) was used. Participants were told that this task was about a father and a daughter (Sanne) planting flower bulbs in a sandbox. The researcher showed the participants the picture of a sandbox and told them the father decided to bury the flower bulb at the location of the cross. When the father went away to bring a watering can, Sanne decided to move the flower bulb and bury it in a different location. Before asking the false belief question, the researcher asked whether participants had a good look at the pictures and remembered where each character (Sanne and her dad) placed the flower bulb, in order to ensure that they have the requisite attentional or memory capacities necessary to demonstrate their ToM knowledge (control question). The researcher then asked the participants one false belief question: “When Sanne's dad comes back with the watering can, where will he give water to the flower bulb? You have to draw a cross.” In terms of scoring, the difference between the original hiding location of the flower bulb (0 mm) and the location where participants indicated Dad would look for it was measured (in millimeters). If participants indicated a location toward the direction of Sanne's hiding location of the flower bulb (63 mm), they received a positive bias score. If participants indicated a location in the opposite direction of the flower bulb, to the right of the original hiding location, they received a negative bias score. Lower scores indicated better performance. This paradigm was employed because, as the object is buried and reburied in the sandbox, a continuum is created between locations in contrast to the categorical approach of the classic false belief task that has been found to sometimes omit the subtle variance in false belief reasoning at different ages (Bernstein, Thornton, & Sommerville, Reference Bernstein, Thornton and Sommerville2011).

Emotion recognition

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes test (children's version; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Scahukk, Lawson, & Spong, Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Scahill, Lawson and Spong2001) was used to assess mental state/emotion recognition. This task was chosen as it is a widely used ToM test that measures the ability to decode the feelings and thoughts of others from the eyes. The test can also be considered an emotion recognition test (Vellante et al., Reference Vellante, Baron-Cohen, Melis, Marrone, Petretto, Masala and Preti2013). It consists of photographs of the eye regions of 28 faces. Participants were asked to make a choice between four words presented at the bottom of the page on which each picture appeared, choosing the one that best described what the person of the photograph was feeling or thinking. Successful performance required participants to select the correct mental and emotional state. Participants were asked to choose one of the terms even if they said that none of the terms was quite right, thus conforming to a forced-choice procedure. One point was given to each correctly reported response.

Procedure

The current study followed a longitudinal design with two time points, approximately 1 year apart. At both assessment points, children undertook the tests individually across two sessions (each lasted 40 min) with a female researcher in a quiet space at their school. All tasks were addressed in a fixed order across the two assessment sessions: Session 1: IQ test, Sandbox task, Digit Span, ToL, and Eyes Test; Session 2: Go/No-Go, IGT, Delay Discounting test.

Results

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS-23®. All variables were checked for normality and homogeneity assumptions of parametric tests. No extreme outliers were found. Analyses were conducted at two levels. The developmental changes in cool and hot EF and ToM of children with ASD relative to neurotypical controls across the 12 months were examined first. Second, the predictive relation between hot and cool EF and ToM across the two time points was investigated. No violations of multivariate assumptions for these variables were found. All tests were two-tailed and statistical significance was set at p < .05.

Comparison of the developmental changes in EF and ToM between the two groups

Table 2 presents descriptive statistics for hot and cool EF and ToM at each time point. The developmental changes in children's EF and ToM abilities across time points were examined by carrying out a mixed analysis of variance. Time was set as the within-subject factor (Time 1, Time 2) and group as the between-subject factor (ASD or control). Post hoc tests were not performed for group because there were fewer than three groups. Within-group comparisons were assessed by paired-sample t tests.

Table 2. Means and standard deviations for variables across the time points

Working memory (Digit Span)

Significant main effects of time, F (1, 80) = 50.6, p = .001, ŋp2 = .39, and group, F (1, 80) = 13.52, p < .001, ŋp2 = .15, were found. The interaction between time and group was also significant, F (1, 80) = 11.73, p = .001, ŋp2 = .13. Figure 1 presents mean working memory score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the ASD group showed poorer performance in working memory than neurotypicals both at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 17.09, p < .001, ŋp2 = .18, and at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 6.24, p = .02, ŋp2 = .07. Further analyses showed that children's performance improved significantly over time in both groups; ASD: t (45) = –7.96, p < .001; controls: t (37) = –2.45, p = .019. The significant interaction lays in the pattern of improvements over time. While both groups demonstrated developmental changes after 12 months, improvements for the ASD group were steeper (see Figure 1). The ASD group demonstrated a poorer performance on Digit Span scores throughout this developmental period.

Figure 1. Mean Cool EF scores across 12 months for ASD and control groups.

Planning (ToL)

The main effect of time, F (1, 80) = 86.65, p < .001, ŋp2 = .52, was found significant. Neither the main effect of group, F (1, 80) = 1.58, p = .21, ŋp2 = .2, nor the interaction between time and group, F (1, 80) = 1.67, p = .2, ŋp2 = .02, were found significant. Figure 1 presents mean planning score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the ASD group showed equal performance in planning to neurotypicals both at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 0.21, p = .65, ŋp2 = .003, and at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 3.62, p = .061, ŋp2 = .043. Further analyses showed that children's performance improved significantly over time in both groups; ASD: t (45) = –7.10, p < .001; controls: t (37) = –6.14, p < .001. Figure 1 reveals the similar developmental improvements after 12 months.

Inhibition (Go/No-Go)

The main effect of time on inhibition, F (1, 80) = 14.61, p < .001, ŋp2 = .15, and group, F (1, 80) = 9.68, p = .003, ŋp2 = .11, were found significant. No significant interaction between time and group, F (1, 80) = 2.48, p = .12, ŋp2 = .03, was found. Figure 1 presents mean inhibition score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the ASD group showed poorer performance in inhibition than neurotypicals both at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 7.32, p = .008, ŋp2 = .08, and at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 12.39, p = .001, ŋp2 = .13. Further analyses showed that children's performance improved significantly over time in both groups; ASD: t (45) = 4.3, p < .001; controls: t (37) = 2.52, p = .015. Both groups demonstrated developmental performance gains after 12 months, but the ASD group demonstrated a poorer performance on the Go/No-Go task throughout this developmental period.

Affective decision making (IGT)

The main effect of time, F (1, 80) = 17.33, p < .001, ŋp2 = .18, and group, F (1, 80) = 4.76, p = .03, ŋp2 = .06, were found to be significant. The interaction between time and group, F (1, 80) = 0.46, p = .49, ŋp2 = .01, was not significant. Figure 2 presents mean affective decision making score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the ASD group showed poorer performance in affective decision making than neurotypicals both at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 4.31, p = .041, ŋp2 = .05, and at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 4.03, p = .048, ŋp2 = .05. Further analyses showed that children's performance improved significantly over time in both groups; ASD: t (45) = –2.91, p = .006; controls: t (37) = –4.03, p = .001. Figure 2 shows that both groups demonstrated developmental performance improvements after 12 months, but with ASD children showing a poorer performance throughout this developmental period.

Figure 2. Mean Hot EF scores across 12 months for ASD and control groups.

Delay discounting (Delay Discounting task)

The effect of time, F (1, 80) = 0.09, p = .76, ŋp2 = .001, group, F (1, 80) = 3.34, p = .071, ŋp2 = .04, and the interaction between time and group, F (1, 80) = 1.91, p = .17, ŋp2 = .02, were not found significant. Figure 2 presents mean delay discounting score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the two groups showed equal performance in delay discounting both at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 5.15, p = .06, ŋp2 = .06, and at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 0.46, p = .49, ŋp2 = .006. Further analyses showed that children's performance did not improve significantly over time in either group; ASD: t (45) = 1.41, p = .17; controls: t (37) = –0.65, p = .52. Neither group presented developmental changes after 12 months.

False belief (Sandbox)

The main effect of time, F (1, 80) = 4.29, p = .04, ŋp2 = .05, was found significant. The effect of group, F (1, 80) = 2.12, p = .15, ŋp2 = .03, was not significant, while interaction between time and group, F (1, 80) = 8.54, p = .005, ŋp2 = .09, was found significant. Figure 3 presents mean false belief score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the ASD group showed poorer performance in false belief than neurotypicals only at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 6.18, p = .015, ŋp2 = .07, but equal scores at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 0.06, p = .8, ŋp2 = .001. Further analyses showed that children's performance improved significantly over time only in the ASD group; ASD: t (45) = 3.32, p = .002; controls: t (37) = –0.69, p = .49.

Figure 3. Mean ToM scores across 12 months for ASD and control groups.

Mental state/emotion recognition (Reading the Mind in the Eyes)

The main effects of time, F (1, 80) = 92.04, p < .001, ŋp2 = .54, group, F (1, 80) = 12.13, p = .001, ŋp2 = .13, and the interaction between time and group, F (1, 80) = 4.84, p = .03, ŋp2 = .06, were all found significant. Figure 3 presents mean mental state/emotion recognition score from Time 1 to Time 2 for each group. Planned comparisons demonstrated that the ASD group showed poorer performance in mental state/emotion recognition than neurotypicals both at Time 1, F (1, 80) = 16.43, p < .001, ŋp2 = .17, and at Time 2, F (1, 80) = 5.52, p = .021, ŋp2 = .07. Further analyses showed that children's performance improved significantly over time in both groups; ASD: t (45) = –8.62, p < .001; controls: t (37) = –5.11, p < .001. Figure 3 shows that both groups demonstrated an improved performance on mental state/emotion recognition after 12 months, but the ASD group showed a steeper developmental change (source of interaction). The ASD group's poorer performance persisted throughout this developmental period.

Longitudinal relations between EF and ToM in children with and without ASD

Pearson's correlations were conducted to investigate the relation between hot and cool EF and ToM across the two time points (see Table 3) in children with and without ASD. Table 3 shows that cool EF (Digit Span, ToL, and Go/No-Go scores) and both ToM tasks were significantly correlated at both time points. In terms of hot EF, only delay discounting was significantly related to ToM Eyes Test whereas performance on IOWA (affective decision making) was not significantly related to ToM at any of the time points.

Table 3. Correlations between ToM and EF tasks across the two time points

Note: T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2; *p < .05; **p < .01.

Following the correlational analysis, the predictive association between EF and ToM in children with and without ASD was examined by running two series of hierarchical regression models. The first series of regressions investigated whether early EF predicted later ToM (at Time 2). Block 1 of predictors included ASD diagnosis, concurrent age, concurrent FSIQ, and early ToM (control variables). Block 2 of predictors introduced the individual cool EF skills in order to examine the predictive role of EF on ToM over and above control variables. Block 3 included the hot EF (only delay discounting; IOWA scores were not included in the regression models as they did not correlate with any ToM task at either time point) in order to assess whether hot EF can predict ToM over and above control variables and cool EF. Finally, Block 4 of predictors included the Hot Delay Discounting × ASD Diagnosis interaction term, computed from the cross-product of effect-coded ASD (–1 = ASD, +1 = control) and the centered hot delay discounting scores (Aiken & West, Reference Aiken and West1991), in order to examine whether the association of hot EF to ToM was stronger in either controls or ASD. The second series of regressions investigated whether early ToM predicts later EF (at Time 2), after controlling for concurrent age and FSIQ, and early EF.

Due to the large number of predictors (n = 13) and the relatively small sample size (see Appendixes A and B for full results of regression analysis and reports of collinearity diagnostics; variance inflation factors [VIFs] and tolerance values), it was decided to set the cut off for significance at p < .01 rather than p < .05, for the following regressions, to counteract the possibility of Type I error (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Dunn, Nartova-Bochaver, Bochaver, Asadi, Khosravi and Yang2016). Although this threshold is not as stringent as a standard Bonferroni correction, the latter has been widely criticized for being problematically conservative, particularly for exploratory research such as the following regression analysis (Perneger, Reference Perneger1998). Thus, we assumed that p < .01 was satisfactory for this analysis; stringent but not overly so.

EF predicting later ToM mental state/ emotion recognition (Eyes Test at Time 2)

The first block of predictors (ASD diagnosis, concurrent age and FSIQ, and early Eyes Test scores (Time 1) contributed significantly to the variance of the later Eyes Test scores, F (4, 77) = 22.11, p < .001, explaining 53.5% of the variance. Neither for cool EF entered in Block 2, F (5, 72) = 0.97, p = .44, nor hot EF in Block 3, F (2, 70) = 0.66, p = .52, was significant additional variance explained. Finally, for the Hot Delay Discounting × ASD interaction terms entered in Block 4, the total variance explained rose to 66.2%, F (2, 68) = 9.07, p < .001. Later ToM Eyes Test scores (Time 2) were significantly predicted by the early (Time 1; p = .002) and later (Time 2; p = .002) Hot Delay Discounting × ASD Diagnosis interactions terms. Thus, early and later delay discounting predicted later ToM mental state/emotion recognition only in school-aged children with ASD.

EF predicting later ToM false belief (Sandbox scores at Time 2)

The first block of predictors (ASD diagnosis, concurrent age and FSIQ, and early false belief scores (Time 1) contributed significantly to the variance of the ToM false belief ability, F (4, 77) = 6.52, p < .001, explaining 25.3% of the variance. For cool EF entered in Block 2 the total variance explained rose to 39.8%, representing a significant increase of 14.5%, F (5, 72) = 3.48, p = .007, additional variance explained. Neither for hot EF entered in Block 3, F (2, 70) = 0.31, p = .74, nor for the Hot Delay Discounting × ASD interaction entered in Block 4, F (2, 68) = 2.07, p = .13, was significant additional variance explained. Later ToM false belief scores (Time 2) were significantly predicted by early working memory (Digit Span Time 1; p = .0004) and later working memory (Digit Span Time 2; p = .0002) overall in school-aged children with and without ASD.

Early ToM predicting later hot and cool EF skills

None of the regression models with early ToM skills predicting each one of the individual later hot and cool EF skills was significant. Early ToM skills did not predict later EF in school-aged children with and without ASD.

Discussion

The present study examined the developmental changes of cool and hot EF and their associations to ToM across 1 year in school-aged children with and without ASD. This longitudinal analysis demonstrated that for children with ASD, selective aspects of EF (working memory, inhibition, and affective decision making) presented significant age-related gains after 1 year, but their impairments, present from the initial assessment, remained throughout development without reaching the levels of neurotypicals. For “cool” planning, ASD participants showed equal performance and the same developmental gains relative to controls, while for “hot” delay discounting, there were no deficits or developmental changes found in ASD or typical development. With regard to ToM abilities, the mental state/emotion recognition presented age-related improvements but demonstrated a pattern deviant to controls as children's deficits remained present across time in ASD. For false belief, our results suggested it followed a delayed development in ASD. Furthermore, results of the longitudinal association between cool and hot EF and ToM in school-aged children with and without ASD revealed that selective early aspects of EF (working memory and delay discounting) predicted later ToM abilities, which supports the well-documented theoretical account of early EF predicting later ToM. No evidence was found to support the argument that ToM abilities predict later EF. Cool EF working memory predicted later ToM false belief overall in children with and without ASD, while hot delay discounting predicted later ToM mental state/emotion recognition over and above cool EF and control variables only in the ASD participants. This is the first study to date to investigate the developmental changes of both cool and hot EF across time and report that early hot delay discounting predicts later ToM above and beyond cool EF in school age in ASD. These findings highlight the multidimensional nature of EF and how its influence on other developmental landmarks such as ToM may increase our understanding of the higher order cognitive deficits that underpin social interaction problems in ASD.

Development of EF and ToM across time in school age

Our results demonstrated that children with ASD showed age-related improvements in all cool EF aspects, suggesting that during school age specific aspects of EF (working memory, inhibition, and planning) present developmental gains in ASD. These results support previous evidence of performance gains in EF during childhood both in typical development (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Claxton and Moses2013; Gur et al., Reference Gur, Richard, Calkins, Chiavacci, Hansen, Bilker and Gur2012) and in ASD (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010). These findings contradict two of the three previous longitudinal EF studies in ASD that reported no developmental improvements in EF, either in young children (Griffith et al., Reference Griffith, Pennington, Wehner and Rogers1999) or in adolescents (Ozonoff & McEvoy, Reference Ozonoff and McEvoy1994). Both of these studies indicated very few EF changes across time and suggested that probably there is a ceiling on the development of such cognitive abilities in ASD. In line with the third longitudinal EF study, though (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010), our findings paint a more positive picture of children with ASD's EF developmental trends, indicating perhaps the likelihood of a window of plasticity in ASD as well. Of note, the contradicting studies (Griffith et al., Reference Griffith, Pennington, Wehner and Rogers1999; Ozonoff & McEvoy, Reference Ozonoff and McEvoy1994) included ASD participants much less able than in our study that could account for their lack of significant developmental changes. However, it should be noted that despite the reported developmental changes in cool working memory and inhibition, children with ASD presented impairments in these aspects relative to matched neurotypicals, which remained present across development and never reached the performance level of the control group. This evidence in a way supports Happé, Ronald, and Plomin's (Reference Happé, Ronald and Plomin2006) proposition that there may be a particular profile of “coexisting cognitive atypicalities” in ASD that pertain across development. One could argue that this could suggest that children with ASD might eventually reach a performance ceiling in some EF aspects, as Ozonoff and McEvoy (Reference Ozonoff and McEvoy1994) implied in their study. Our data failed to provide more evidence about this issue as the present longitudinal design included only two time points and the sample did not include adolescents or young adults, which could shed more light on the maturity peaks of EF in ASD (if they ever develop up to the same level as controls). The present data indicate thus that despite the significant age-related improvements in working memory and inhibition, the performance of the ASD group never reached that of controls, which in a way implies deviant development across the age range of the present sample. The suggested deviant development should not allow for the present data to be overlooked. More specifically, it is worth considering the present data from the maturation processes perspective of Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew, and Sweeney (Reference Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew and Sweeney2007) that proposed that if deficits in EF persist across development, it could imply that impairments in the underlying EF brain mechanisms are not related to the brain developmental/maturation processes. This, in conjunction with the emerging developmental improvements we found here, could suggest that the developmental processes may be intact for ASD participants across this specific age range (middle childhood). The fact that planning ability presented an intact profile and similar developmental improvements to the control group (no deviance) as also shown in a previous study (Happé, Booth, et al., Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes2006) could actually add more support to this notion that, at least in school age, developmental/maturation processes of selective cool EF are intact in ASD. The developmental pattern of cool EF gains in school age could be explained in relation to the prefrontal cortex (the underlying brain region of EF) undergoing substantial maturation during this period (Otero & Barker, Reference Otero, Barker, Goldstein and Naglieri2014). School age is a crucial period of rapid developmental improvements and increased cognitive demands where children have to process and understand both their own sense of self and their sense of others as well as learning to interact effectively with the world around them (Siegel, Reference Siegel2013), which could justify these advances.

School age is an important developmental period demanding not only cool EF improvements but also hot EF gains, as the social contexts children are faced with involve advanced emotional and motivational processes. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the developmental changes of hot EF in ASD across time and showed that only affective decision making presented significant age-related gains in school-aged children with ASD. Our results are in line with previous research (Hooper et al., Reference Hooper, Luciana, Conklin and Yarger2004; Prencipe et al., Reference Prencipe, Kesek, Cohen, Lamm, Lewis and Zelazo2011) that reported age-related performance gains in affective decision making (IGT) across childhood and adolescence as well as with developmental theories proposing that the development of hot EF would be protracted to the extended development of the underlying brain region that is the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, across school age (Segalowitz & Davies, Reference Segalowitz and Davies2004). As with the cool EF developmental framework discussed above, despite the emerging developmental improvements, children with ASD presented deficits in affective decision making relative to the control group that did not become less marked with age (deviant to control group development across middle childhood). The emerging developmental gains in this hot EF aspect, despite the persisting deficits, highlight the importance of this finding since it suggests that the developmental/maturation processes of the brain structures underpinning selective hot systems in ASD continue across school age. The cognitive maturation processes of the areas of ventromedial prefrontal cortex regulating affective decision making seem to progress across school age in ASD. It should be noted at this point that our assumption that the persisting performance deficits of the ASD group (despite the developmental improvements found) suggest a deviant rather than a delayed pattern should be treated with caution. We have made sure to clarify at all relevant points in the discussion above that these suggested as deviant developmental patterns apply only to the specific age range employed in the present study. It is likely that group differences may eventually lessen with further age if performance was to be assessed at an older age, for example (late adolescence or young adulthood).

Contrary to the significant results of cool EF and hot affective decision making, hot delay discounting demonstrated an intact profile (in ASD) and nonsignificant developmental changes neither in ASD nor in typical development. These results contradict previous cross-sectional evidence having indicated age-related improvements in the Delay Discounting task (Scheres et al., Reference Scheres, Dijkstra, Ainslie, Balkan, Reynolds, Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos2006) across childhood and adolescence. Scheres et al. (Reference Scheres, Dijkstra, Ainslie, Balkan, Reynolds, Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos2006) employed a temporal and probabilistic discounting task that had a differentiated design with shorter time delays, smaller immediate monetary rewards, as well as levels of probability for the delayed reward. Besides this, the monetary awards they offered were real, contrary to ours being hypothetical due to the impractical cost and ethical issues raised within the school contexts. Such measures discrepancies could have perhaps made their older participants more motivated to wait for the larger rewards during the task relative to the younger ones resulting in the reported age-related gains. As the differentiated designs of Delay Discounting tasks are multifaceted and with levels of difficulty/complexity, we could assume that our Delay Discounting task dimensions were not developmentally sensitive enough to capture subtle age-related differences across school age in either group. Steinberg et al. (Reference Steinberg, Graham, O'Brien, Woolard, Cauffman and Banich2009) suggest that differentiated task designs do not likely follow the same developmental pathway. Besides task discrepancies though, the lack of significant developmental changes in hot delay discounting relative to the other hot aspect (affective decision making) is quite surprising and raises questions about their underlying brain structures and developmental course. One should expect the delay discounting trajectory to progress across school age as children are in greater need of their impulsivity control (tapped by delay discounting) within the more demanding social and educational settings. As findings from early childhood (Zelazo & Carlson, Reference Zelazo and Carlson2012) and adolescence (Scheres, Tontsch, Thoeny, & Sumiya, Reference Scheres, Tontsch, Thoeny and Sumiya2014) have both demonstrated age-related gains, it makes us consider the possibility that delay discounting may present a developmental pause during middle childhood (6/7–11) years before continuing to progression in adolescence. This assumption needs to be cautiously interpreted as the present design did not include a third or more time points across school age and adolescence that could clarify this issue. Finally, as several measures thought to tap hot EF have been criticized for lacking enough “heat” or not being ecologically valid (Welsh & Peterson Reference Welsh and Peterson2014), one potential explanation behind the lack of significant developmental changes here could be the Delay Discounting task not being so hot for this specific age range. One could argue that differing quantities of imaginary money is quite an abstract construct that failed to enhance young children's motivation or increase their sensitivity to money loss.

Regarding the developmental course of ToM in ASD, our discussion will not go in great depth as our main focus in the present study was its longitudinal association to EF across school age. ToM mental state/emotion recognition ability and false belief understanding ability both made substantial progress in ASD across the 1-year period and expand relevant findings from longitudinal studies in preschool period (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010; Steele et al., Reference Steele, Joseph and Tager-Flusberg2003) to school age as well. The ASD performance in mental state/emotion recognition (Eyes task) never reached the level of the control group despite the age-related gains, which implies that, as with cool EF described above, the developmental trend of this ToM ability for this specific age range could be argued as deviant of the typical development one. Once again, such an assumption is not warranted as group differences may eventually not be significant at an older age (after middle childhood). The small but significant changes, however, highlight that ToM gains, should they occur, may be present beyond the preschool period in ASD. In contrast to this, false belief understanding (Sandbox task) in ASD presented a differentiated developmental course as impairments were present only at the initial assessment point. Age-related gains emerged only in the ASD group, with false belief performance reaching the controls’ level at the second time point. This could imply that the false belief ability in ASD presents a delayed developmental pattern in our study. Failure to report developmental gains in the control group is due to ceiling effect (Sandbox task probably not sensitive enough to developmental trends of typical development). It seems that the ASD heterogeneity and the unique ToM profiles of distinct tasks cannot allow for neither the delayed nor the deviant development hypothesis to fully explain the ToM deficits in ASD. Our data support Baron-Cohen's (Reference Baron-Cohen1991) proposition that ToM development in ASD fits a hypothesis of both deviance and delay.

Longitudinal associations between hot and cool EF and ToM across time in school age

Our results showed that EF and ToM are developmentally linked across school age. In line with the vast majority of previous studies (see for a review Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2014), early EF predicted later ToM rather than the reverse pattern (early ToM predicting later EF). Therefore, these findings add more to the theoretical account suggesting that emerging EF in childhood is a potent, although not exclusive platform for the development of ToM both in typical development (Flynn, Reference Flynn2007; Hughes, Reference Hughes1998) and in ASD (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010). The emergence account of ToM posits that early EF skills predict later ToM; thus, children would first need to obtain sufficient EF skills and then understand and process ToM false beliefs or mental states (Russell, Reference Russell1996). After controlling for concurrent age, FSIQ, and prior ToM, we found that early working memory predicted later ToM false belief while early delay discounting predicted later ToM mental state/emotion recognition in ASD. These findings thus do not support Perner's (Reference Perner, Carruthers and Boucher1998) proposition that the acquisition of ToM is a prerequisite of children's EF, according to which, longitudinal predictions from earlier EF are not expected for a ToM task.

In line with our hypothesis and previous cross sectional and longitudinal studies that presented associations between cool EF and ToM in typical development and ASD (Carlson & Moses, Reference Carlson and Moses2001; Kimhi et al., Reference Kimhi, Kugelmas, Agam Ben Artzi, Ben Moshe and Bauminger-Zviely2014; Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2007), we found that later ToM false belief was predicted by early cool working memory. Children between 3 and 5 years of age present dramatic and rapid improvements in EF and ToM (Anderson, Reference Anderson, Anderson, Jacobs and Anderson2008), but the present findings suggest that developmental changes in ToM mechanisms across school age (beyond 5 years) may require EF to facilitate the emergence of more sophisticated ToM abilities. Advanced needs for cognitive executive control during school age are more than expected as children have to maintain and manipulate new, complex knowledge while socially interacting with their environments (Del Giudice, Reference Del Giudice2014). This evidence supports the working memory hypothesis according to which the working memory development is an important factor influencing children's developing understanding of false belief (Davis & Pratt, Reference Davis and Pratt1995) in early childhood, as replicated by other studies too (Gordon & Olson, Reference Gordon and Olson1998; Keenan, Olson, & Marini, Reference Keenan, Olson and Marini1998). It could be then argued that across school age, children with or without ASD need a heavier (improved) cognitive load on working memory toward a successful ToM development. Working memory and inhibition are generally considered central to the EF–false belief relation (Carlson, Moses, & Breton, Reference Carlson, Moses and Breton2002; Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2014), a notion for which we provided limited support. Contradicting prior studies in early childhood (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Mandell and Williams2004, Reference Carlson, Claxton and Moses2013), inhibition did not predict later ToM false belief in school age in our study. One could thus argue that inhibition may be more central to the emergence of false belief at the early years of childhood (Tillman, Brocki, Sørensen, & Lundervold, Reference Tillman, Brocki, Sørensen and Lundervold2015), and as ToM abilities progress across childhood, other EF may be more central to the development of ToM.

Another important finding of the present study was the significant longitudinal predictive association found between hot delay discounting and ToM mental state/emotion recognition, over and above cool EF and control variables in ASD. Current findings corroborate to an extent that there may be a developmental relation of the underlying brain mechanisms of selective hot processes and ToM present in ASD across time. Delay discounting could be linked with the emergence of ToM as, in order for children with ASD to understand the mental states of others, “hot” motivational or emotional processes need to be evoked (Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Qu, Müller, Schneider, Schumann-Hengsteler and Sodian2005) across school age. This emerging association between two seemingly unrelated constructs suggests that the ability of school-aged children with ASD to disengage from the present while considering more long-term goals/temporal perspectives (delay discounting) may provide a platform for the development of one's emotion understanding ability. Stolarski, Bitner, and Zimbardo (Reference Stolarski, Bitner and Zimbardo2011) has also suggested that emotional functioning is linked with the development of temporal perspectives (i.e., delay discounting). As this longitudinal association between delay discounting and mental state/emotion recognition was found only in the ASD group, it could imply there is a specificity in the relation between ToM and this hot executive process in ASD. However, as delay discounting did not predict later false belief understanding (the other ToM task), this assumption has to be examined cautiously. The developmental association between these delay-related motivational processes and ToM mechanisms being stronger in the ASD group needs to be tackled by future imaging studies investigating the structure of the underlying brain regions. For example, previous functional magnetic resonance imaging research in clinical population showed an association between ADHD and activity in the ventral striatum (the brain region responsible for preference for small sooner rewards over large later rewards; McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein, & Cohen, Reference McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein and Cohen2004) during reward anticipation in delay tasks (Scheres, Milham, Knutson, & Castellanos, Reference Scheres, Milham, Knutson and Castellanos2007; Ströhle et al., Reference Ströhle, Stoy, Wrase, Schwarzer, Schlagenhauf, Huss and Heinz2008). Relevant research could perhaps clarify if that could also be the case for ASD.

The present findings need to be interpreted cautiously in the light of limitations and be also corroborated with results from larger longitudinal studies with more than two time points across school age. As the present study followed the approach of the convenience sampling, the sample of the ASD participants was quite small (e.g., the between-groups IQ statistical difference of p = .09 indicates a medium effect size) and may not reflect the broader ASD population. Furthermore, the participants of the present study were aged between 7–11 years only. It thus remains to be examined whether these results can be generalized to younger children, adolescents, or adults across the spectrum. The fact that we did not include a validated screening measure to corroborate the clinical diagnostic reports was another important limitation. Finally, it should be noted that clear conclusions about the longitudinal association between hot EF and the broader ToM mechanism cannot be drawn as early hot delay discounting predicted only one of the two later ToM measures addressed here. Moreover, these two ToM measures tap only some of the various ToM skills and cannot be considered as the only crucial ToM measures. Future longitudinal research thus could investigate the impact of hot EF to several other ToM tasks such as the strange stories (Happé, Reference Happé1994), second-order ToM (Perner & Wimmer, Reference Perner and Wimmer1985), or the Faux Pas test (Baron-Cohen, O'Riordan, Jones, Stone, & Plaisted, Reference Baron-Cohen, O'Riordan, Jones, Stone and Plaisted1999).

In conclusion, the present study demonstrated that for children with ASD, selective cool and hot EF skills and ToM abilities presented significant developmental changes across time in school age. These data highlight the need to shed more light on the underlying brain structures as the reported impairments in EF are likely not related to the maturation processes. Furthermore, our data provided more to the theoretical account that cool EF influences the development of ToM and not vice versa in ASD and typical development, while expanding these longitudinal associations of ToM to hot EF as well suggesting that specific hot EF skills (delay discounting) also provide a platform for the emergence of ToM across school age in ASD. Although research into hot EF in childhood has recently received increased attention, knowledge of its developmental trajectory still lags behind that of cool EF, both in typical development and ASD, mainly due to the limited number of tasks that actually tap hot EF skills. Future research should direct more attention toward the development of more relevant tasks to measure these hot EF skills. Finally, findings of specific EF predicting later ToM contributed support to an emergence account (Russell, Reference Russell1996, Reference Russell and Russell1997) in typical development and ASD. Studying the developmental trends of hot and cool EF and their longitudinal associations to ToM may aid in gaining a greater understanding of the link between cognition and behavior in typical development and of the development of higher order cognitive impairments being a risk factor for poor developmental/social outcomes in children with ASD. Our findings highlight the need to address both hot and cool EF in clinical practice as they could contribute more toward future diagnosis or intervention projects.

Appendix A

Table A.1. Hierarchical regression analysis for later theory of mind false belief (Time 2) by group and executive function variables

Appendix B

Table B.1. Hierarchical regression analysis for later theory of mind mental state/emotion recognition (Time 2) by group and executive function variables

Footnotes

The authors are grateful for the Vice Chancellor Scholarship Funding from the University of Greenwich. The authors would like to acknowledge their gratitude to the participating schools and children. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Note: *Averaged inhibition in Block 2 represents the average score of early and later inhibition variables. It was decided to include this variable because the initial regression model with both inhibition variables suffered from multicollinearity issues due to those variables being highly correlate (r = .97). T1 = Time 1. VIF = variance inflation factor.

Note: *Averaged inhibition in Block 2 represents the average score of early and later inhibition variables. It was decided to include this variable because the initial regression model with both inhibition variables suffered from multicollinearity issues due to those variables being highly correlated (r = .97). T1 = Time 1. VIF = variance inflation factor.

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Kirkwood, H., & Elliott, J. (2009). The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80, 606621. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01282.xGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Andersen, P. N., Hovik, K. T., Skogli, E. W., Egeland, J., & Oie, M. (2013). Symptoms of ADHD in children with high-functioning autism are related to impaired verbal working memory and verbal delayed recall. PlOS ONE, 8, e64842. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064842Google Scholar
Anderson, P. J. (2008). Towards a developmental model of executive function. In Anderson, V., Jacobs, R., & Anderson, P. J. (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 323). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, V., Anderson, P. J., Jacobs, R., & Spencer-Smith, M. (2008). Development and assessment of executive function: From preschool to adolescence. In Anderson, V., Jacobs, R., & Anderson, P. J. (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 123155). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 45, 190201. doi:10.1037/a0014098Google Scholar
Austin, G., Groppe, K., & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 655. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00655Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The development of a theory of mind in autism: Deviance and delay? Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 3351.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., O'Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V. E., & Plaisted, K. (1999). A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407418.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Lawson, J., & Spong, A. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 4778.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 715. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3Google Scholar
Begeer, S., Bernstein, D. M., van Wijhe, J., Scheeren, A. M., & Koot, H. M. (2012). A continuous false belief task reveals egocentric biases in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 16, 357366. doi:10.1177/1362361311434545Google Scholar
Bernstein, D. M., Thornton, W. L., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Theory of mind through the ages: Older and middle-aged adults exhibit more errors than do younger adults on a continuous false-belief task. Experimental Aging Research, 37, 481502. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2011.619466Google Scholar
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 16411660. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.xGoogle Scholar
Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 509521. doi:10.1080/15248372.2014.888350Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Claxton, L. J., & Moses, L. J. (2013). The relation between executive function and theory of mind is more than skin deep. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 186197. doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.824883Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, J. L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 11051122. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 10321053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 7392. doi:10.1002/icd.298Google Scholar
Chan, R. C., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., & Chen, E. Y. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 201216. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010Google Scholar
Chen, S. F., Chien, Y. L., Wu, C. T., Shang, C. Y., Wu, Y. Y., & Gau, S. S. (2016). Deficits in executive functions among youths with autism spectrum disorders: An age-stratified analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46, 16251638. doi:10.1017/S0033291715002238Google Scholar
Christ, S. E., Holt, D. D., White, D. A., & Green, L. (2007). Inhibitory control in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 11551165. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0259-yGoogle Scholar
Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A. R., Palumbi, R., de Giambattista, C., & Margari, L. (2016). A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 11911202. doi:10.2147/NDT.S104620Google Scholar
Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1995). The development of children's theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 2531.Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M. (2014). Middle childhood: An evolutionary-developmental synthesis. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 193200. doi:10.1111/cdep.12084Google Scholar
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., … Guastella, A. J. (2017). Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi:10.1038/mp.2017.75Google Scholar
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2013). Silent films and strange stories: Theory of mind, gender and social experiences in middle childhood. Child Development, 84, 9891003. doi:10.1111/cdev.12017Google Scholar
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85, 17771794. doi:10.1111/cdev.12237Google Scholar
Devine, R. T., White, N., Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 758771. doi:10.1037/dev0000105Google Scholar
Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, 331338. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00888.xGoogle Scholar
Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders: Imaging in clinical neuroscience. British Medical Bulletin, 65, 4959. doi:10.1093/bmb/65.1.49Google Scholar
Flynn, E. (2007). The role of inhibitory control in false belief understanding. Infant and Child Development, 16, 5369. doi:10.1002/icd.500Google Scholar
Geurts, H. M., de Vries, M., & van den Bergh, S. F. (2014). Executive functioning theory and autism. In Goldstein, S. & Naglier, J. (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 121141). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Kenworthy, L., & Barton, R. M. (2002). Profiles of everyday executive function in acquired and developmental disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 121137. doi:10.1076/chin.8.2.121.8727Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (2012). Theory of mind. In Margolis, E., Samuels, R., & Stich, S. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of cognitive science (pp. 402424). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, A. C., & Olson, D. R. (1998). The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 7083. doi:10.1006/jecp.1997.2423Google Scholar
Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70, 817832. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00059Google Scholar
Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Calkins, M. E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, J. A, Bilker, W. B., … Gur, R. E. (2012). Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8–21. Neuropsychology, 26, 251265. doi:10.1037/a0026712Google Scholar
Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129154. doi:10.1007/BF02172093Google Scholar
Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61, 2539. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004Google Scholar
Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation of autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 12181220. doi:10.1038/nn1770Google Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189233. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001Google Scholar
Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617644. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4Google Scholar
Hooper, C. J., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., & Yarger, R. S. (2004). Adolescents’ performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: Implications for the development of decision-making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Psychology, 40, 11481158. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148Google Scholar
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233253. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00921.xGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43, 14471459. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447Google Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Agostino, A., & Owens-Jaffray, K. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood and early adolescence: Different from before? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 98115. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.006Google Scholar
Keenan, T., Olson, D. R., & Marini, Z. (1998). Working memory and children's developing understanding of mind. Australian Journal of Psychology, 50, 7682. doi:10.1080/00049539808257537Google Scholar
Kercood, S., Grskovic, J. A., Banda, D., & Begeske, J. (2014). Working memory and autism: A review of literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 13161332. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.06.011Google Scholar
Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J., & Kochanska, G. (2014). Effortful control in “hot” and “cool” tasks differentially predicts children's behaviour problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 4356. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4Google Scholar
Kimhi, Y., Kugelmas, D., Agam Ben Artzi, G., Ben Moshe, I., & Bauminger-Zviely, N. (2014). Theory of mind and executive function in preschoolers with typical development versus intellectually able pre-schoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 23412354. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2104-zGoogle Scholar
Kouklari, E. C., Thompson, T., Monks, C. P., & Tsermentseli, S. (2017). Hot and cool executive function and its relation to theory of mind in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Cognition and Development. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/15248372.2017.1339708Google Scholar
Lantz, J. (2002). Theory of mind in autism: Development, implications, and interventions. Reporter, 7, 1825.Google Scholar
Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., & McLennan, J. D. (1989). Autism Diagnostic Interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363387. doi:10.1007/BF02212936Google Scholar
Leung, R. C., Vogan, V. M., Powell, T. L., Anagnostou, E., & Taylor, M. J. (2016). The role of executive functions in social impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 22, 336344. doi:10.1080/09297049.2015.1005066Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2000). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659685. doi:10.1007/BF02172145Google Scholar
Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 474481. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.030Google Scholar
Marcovitch, S., O'Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Weaver, J. M., & Levine, D. W. (2014). A longitudinal assessment of the relation between executive function and theory of mind at 3, 4, and 5 years. Cognitive Development, 33, 4055. doi.10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.07.001Google Scholar
McAlister, A. R., & Peterson, C. C. (2013). Siblings, theory of mind, and executive functioning in children aged 3–6 years: New longitudinal evidence. Child Development, 84, 14421458. doi:10.1111/cdev.12043Google Scholar
McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503507. doi:10.1126/science.1100907Google Scholar
McDonald, S. (2013). Impairments in social cognition following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 231246. doi:10.1017/S1355617712001506Google Scholar
Monks, C. P., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (2005). Psychological correlates of peer victimisation in preschool: Social cognitive skills, executive function and attachment profiles. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 571588. doi:10.1002/ab.20099Google Scholar
Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250259. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024Google Scholar
Myerson, J., Green, L., & Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 76, 235243. doi:10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235Google Scholar
National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (2011). Autism: recognition, referral and diagnosis of children and young people on the autism spectrum (Clinical guideline 128). London: RCOG Press.Google Scholar
Otero, T. M., & Barker, L. A. (2014). The frontal lobes and executive functioning. In Goldstein, S. & Naglieri, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 2945). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415431. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006027Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 10811105. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.xGoogle Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology, 43, 974990. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.974Google Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46, 530544. doi:10.1037/a0018287Google Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2012). The development of executive function in autism. Autism Research and Treatment, 2012, 146132. doi:10.1155/2012/146132Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 5187. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.xGoogle Scholar
Perneger, T. V. (1998). What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. British Medical Journal, 316, 12361238.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1998). The meta-intentional nature of executive functions and theory of mind. In Carruthers, P. & Boucher, J. (Eds.), Language and thought (pp. 270283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337344. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01362-5Google Scholar
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that …” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437471. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7Google Scholar
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory of mind development for children with autism, deafness or typical development. Child Development, 76, 502517. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.xGoogle Scholar
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 621637. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008Google Scholar
Poland, S. E., Monks, C. P., & Tsermentseli, S. (2015). Cool and hot executive function as predictors of aggression in early childhood: Differentiating between the function and form of aggression. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 34, 181197. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12122Google Scholar
Richards, J. B., Zhang, L., Mitchell, S. H., & Wit, H. (1999). Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 71, 121143. doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.71-121Google Scholar
Robinson, O. C., Dunn, A., Nartova-Bochaver, S., Bochaver, K., Asadi, S., Khosravi, Z., … Yang, Y. (2016). Figures of admiration in emerging adulthood: A four-country study. Emerging Adulthood, 4, 8291. doi:10.1177/2167696815601945Google Scholar
Russell, J. (1996). Agency: Its role in mental development. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Russell, J. (1997). How executive disorders can bring about an adequate theory of mind. In Russell, J. (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 256304). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65, 7786. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007Google Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., Bowman, L. C., Evraire, L. E., & Ito, J. M. B. (2009). Neurodevelopmental correlates of theory of mind in preschool children. Child Development, 80, 11471162. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01322.xGoogle Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 7481. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.xGoogle Scholar
Scheres, A., Dijkstra, M., Ainslie, E., Balkan, J., Reynolds, B., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Castellanos, F. X. (2006). Temporal and probabilistic discounting of rewards in children and adolescents: Effects of age and ADHD symptoms. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20922103. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.012Google Scholar
Scheres, A., Milham, M. P., Knutson, B., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit⁄hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 720724. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.042Google Scholar
Scheres, A., Tontsch, C., Thoeny, A. L., & Sumiya, M. (2014). Temporal reward discounting in children, adolescents, and emerging adults during an experiential task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 711. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00711Google Scholar
Schug, J., Takagishi, H., Benech, C., & Okada, H. (2016). The development of theory of mind and positive and negative reciprocity in preschool children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00888Google Scholar
Segalowitz, S., & Davies, P. L. (2004). Charting the maturation of the frontal lobe: An electrophysiological strategy. Brain and Cognition, 55, 116133. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00283-5Google Scholar
Serra, M., Loth, F. L., van Geert, P. L. C., Hurkens, E., & Minderaa, R. B. (2002). Theory of mind in children with “lesser variants” of autism: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 885900. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00104Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 298, 199209.Google Scholar
Siegel, D. (2013). Brainstorm: The power and purpose of the teenage brain. New York: Jeremy Tarcher/Penguin.Google Scholar
Sinzig, J., Morsch, D., Bruning, N., Schmidt, M. H., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Inhibition, flexibility, working memory and planning in autism spectrum disorders with and without comorbid ADHD-symptoms. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 2. doi:10.1186/1753-2000-2-4Google Scholar
South, M., Ozonoff, S., & McMahon, W. M. (2007). The relationship between executive functioning, central coherence, and repetitive behaviors in the high functioning autism spectrum. Autism, 11, 437451. doi:10.1177/1362361307079606Google Scholar
Steele, S., Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Developmental change in theory of mind abilities in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 461467. doi:10.1023/A:1025075115100Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O'Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 2844. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.xGoogle Scholar
Stolarski, M., Bitner, J., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2011). Time perspective, emotional intelligence and discounting of delayed awards. Time and Society, 20, 346363. doi:10.1177/0961463X11414296Google Scholar
Ströhle, A., Stoy, M., Wrase, J., Schwarzer, S., Schlagenhauf, F., Huss, M., … Heinz, A. (2008). Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit⁄hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage, 39, 966972. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.044Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2005). Theory of mind, language, and executive functions in autism: A longitudinal perspective. In Schneider, W., Schumann-Hengsteler, R., & Sodian, B. (Eds.), Young children's cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 239258). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tillman, C., Brocki, K. C., Sørensen, L., & Lundervold, A. J. (2015). A longitudinal examination of the developmental executive function hierarchy in children with externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 496506. doi:10.1177/1087054713488439Google Scholar
Vellante, M., Baron-Cohen, S., Melis, M., Marrone, M., Petretto, D. R., Masala, C., & Preti, A. (2013). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: Systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18, 326354. doi:10.1080/13546805.2012.721728Google Scholar
Verdejo-García, A., Bechara, A., Recknor, E. C., & Perez-Garcia, M. (2006). Decision-making and the Iowa Gambling Task: Ecological validity in individuals with substance dependence. Psychologica Belgica, 46, 5578. doi:10.5334/pb-46-1-2-55Google Scholar
Verté, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Executive functioning in children with autism and Tourette syndrome. Developmental Psychopathology, 17, 415445. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050200Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale for Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Welsh, M., & Peterson, E. (2014). Issues in the conceptualization and assessment of hot executive functions in childhood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 152156. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001379Google Scholar
Willoughby, M., Kupersmidt, J., Voegler-Lee, M., & Bryant, D. (2011). Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 162180.Google Scholar
Xiao, T., Xiao, Z., Ke, X., Hong, S., Yang, H., Su, Y., … Liu, Y. (2012). Response inhibition impairment in high functioning autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from near-infrared spectroscopy data. PlOS ONE, 7, e46569. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046569Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Sokoloff, J. L., Shook, D. A., James, J. D., & Kenworthy, L. (2009). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms moderate cognition and behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 322333. doi:10.1002/aur.103Google Scholar
Yeung, M. K., Han, Y. M., Sze, S. L., & Chan, A. S. (2016). Abnormal frontal theta oscillations underlie the cognitive flexibility deficits in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 30, 281. doi:10.1037/neu0000231Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354360. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.xGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In Goswami, U. (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445470). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In Schneider, W., Schumann-Hengsteler, R., & Sodian, B. (Eds.), Young children's cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability and theory of mind (pp. 7195). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participants’ characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Means and standard deviations for variables across the time points

Figure 2

Figure 1. Mean Cool EF scores across 12 months for ASD and control groups.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Mean Hot EF scores across 12 months for ASD and control groups.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Mean ToM scores across 12 months for ASD and control groups.

Figure 5

Table 3. Correlations between ToM and EF tasks across the two time points

Figure 6

Table A.1. Hierarchical regression analysis for later theory of mind false belief (Time 2) by group and executive function variables

Figure 7

Table B.1. Hierarchical regression analysis for later theory of mind mental state/emotion recognition (Time 2) by group and executive function variables